- Sensory Science and Eating Behavior Group, Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University and Research, Wageningen, Netherlands
Sensory properties inform likes and dislikes, but also play an important functional role in guiding food choice and intake behavior. Odors direct food choice and stimulate sensory-specific appetites and taste helps to anticipate calorie and nutrient content of food. Food textures moderate eating rate and the energy consumed to satiation and post-ingestive metabolism. We summarize how sensory cues moderate intake, and highlight opportunities to apply sensory approaches to improve dietary behavior. Salt, sweet and savory taste influence liking, but also influence energy intake to fullness, with higher taste intensity and duration linked to lower intake. Psycho-physical studies show it is relatively easy to rank taste intensities at different concentrations but more challenging to discriminate fat contents, and fat discrimination declines further when combined with high-taste intensity. Fat has low impact on sensory intensity, but makes significant contributions to energy content. Combinations of high taste and fat-content can promote passive energy over-consumption, and adding fat also increases energy intake rate (kcals/min), reducing opportunities to orally meter consumption. Consumers adapt their oral processing behaviors to a foods texture, which can influence the rate and extent of energy intake. Understanding how texture influences eating behaviors and bolus formation, affords new opportunities to impact eating rate, energy intake and metabolic response to food. Food formulation has traditionally focused on composition and sensory appeal. Future research needs to consider the role of sensory properties in moderating consumer interaction with their food environment, and how they influence calorie selection, and shape our eating behaviors and intake.
Introduction; Sensory Cues as Functional Food Properties
A foods sensory appeal is largely determined by the physical and chemical properties that are sensed before and during consumption, which informs initial acceptance and the degree to which a food will be consumed (1). High sensory appeal is proposed as the main reason for excessive energy intakes, yet dietary energy intake patterns are not dominated only by highly palatable foods, and most energy is consumed from staple foods and meals with diverse sensory properties. This suggests that palatability is only one dimension of food intake, and that the sensory properties of food play an important functional role in guiding intake behavior, beyond simply promoting “liking”. The senses of vision and olfaction are involved in the anticipation of food intake and direct sensory specific appetites and food choice. By contrast, hearing, taste, retro-nasal olfaction, texture perception and trigeminal stimulation are directly involved during consumption, and collectively inform the onset of satiation and the termination of energy intake (2). These sensory signals are integrated to form a dynamic perceptual impression of a food, which determines both our liking and intake behavior to the point of satiation (3). Sensory cues are operational before and during a meal, having a direct effect on satiation, with less of an impact on satiety (4).
This mini-review provides a summary of recent findings on the role of odor, taste and texture on calorie selection and energy intake. We highlight the differential role of smell and taste and texture perception in the initiation and termination of eating, and the sensory impact of fat in promoting higher energy intake rates. Food texture can also influence the oral phase of digestion and subsequent metabolic responses, and we highlight opportunities to apply an empirical understanding of the role of sensory cues to moderate food choice, intake and the eating behaviors associated with the healthiness of the food supply.
Impact of Palatability, Odors, and Taste on Energy Selection and Intake
High palatability is a powerful incentive to eat, and the ingestion of “good tasting foods” has been linked to a multitude of positive emotions (5). A higher palatability increases the probability that a particular food will be chosen amongst a set of alternatives (6), and research has also shown that higher palatability leads to an increase in energy intake [i.e., (7)]. However, it is an oversimplification to assume that we only choose and consume foods based on palatability, and food intake decisions are influenced by a complex set of factors that go beyond palatability (1). Food choice and intake are influenced by a multitude of factors and we do not only consume our “most liked” foods (8). Highly palatable foods are often regarded as “treats”, and are consumed infrequently and so do not contribute disproportionately to higher energy intakes. For example, treat foods such as ice cream contribute a relatively small proportion (5%) to consumed calories (9). The majority of daily energy intakes comes from “savory-fatty” foods (10, 11) and staple foods (12) which often have relatively low hedonic valence, such as rice, potato or pasta, rather than indulgent “treat” products often characterized as junk foods or empty calories (13). Whereas, liking may predict choice, the effect of higher palatability on intake is not a linear relationship, with research showing the predictive relationship between liking and intake tends to diminish at higher levels of palatability. For example, in a field study with US soldiers, soldiers consumed 100% of served portions when the liking rating on the 9-point hedonic scale was either a “8” or a “9”, yet the relationship between liking and intake tended to plateau at ratings above score of 5–6 (6). Beyond their role in liking, research in from numerous controlled sensory-feeding behavior studies has highlighted the role of food sensory properties in calorie selection and intake.
Research has shown that food related ambient odors can increase sensory specific appetites and directly influence food choice (14, 15). For example, when customers at an experimental restaurant were asked to choose their meals from a menu, the proportion selecting a fruit dessert increased when choices were made in the presence of a non-attentively perceived “pear” odor, than when choices were made without an odor (16). The implication is that our response, particularly to unattended odors, are likely to play an important role in food choices. Similarly, when exposed to odors signaling a specific “taste” (i.e., sweet/savory) participants had a greater appetite for congruent sweet/savory food, compared to incongruent products, suggesting odors can induce “sensory specific appetites” that influence choice, independently of liking (17, 18). Although this effect has been reproduced many times, it seems limited to choice and despite early findings to the contrary (19, 20), odors do not seem to have a direct impact of energy intake in realistic consumption conditions (21, 22).
By contrast taste quality and intensity have been shown to moderate energy selection and intake. Consuming foods with higher umami intensity has been shown to reduce subsequent energy intake (23, 24), and foods with congruent savory-taste and protein content have been shown to enhance post-meal satiety (25, 26). Taste quality and intensity reflect the concentration of the taste substrate in the food environment, such that “sweeter” foods tend to contain more mono- and di-saccharides and salty foods contain more NaCl. However, there are also exceptions to this relationship, where fat (which is usually present as triacylglycerol), has a low sensory impact, but a large impact on the energy content of foods. Fat sensation affects mouthfeel, flavor release, and can directly impact the rate of the energy intake (27). In many cases the widespread use of low and no-calorie sweeteners now means there can be a strong taste signal in the absence of any sugar. Humans are largely blind to the primary macronutrients sources of energy we consume including starch, protein and triacylglycerol, which have little or no taste activity (13). As such, the sense of taste is influential in linking what is perceived during consumption with the positive post-ingestive consequences of food intake, and through repeated exposure, taste acquires a predictive capacity where we learn to imprint preferences and habitual eating habits via a reciprocal effect of flavor-consequence learning (28, 29).
Whether one taste quality is more satiating than another has been investigated based on anecdotal reports that “sweet” foods were wrongly believed to be less satiating than savory foods on a kcal for kcal basis, and may therefore promote increased energy intake. Early research on this topic showed no difference in the short term effect of sweet/non-sweet carbohydrates on subsequent satiety (30) and later findings support this showing that energy compensation is no different whether the energy taste quality was sweet or savory (31). This is further supported by research on taste and satiation, which showed that ad libitum intakes were equivalent for sweet and salty/savory tasting versions of the same meal (32).
Within a meal, the combined duration and intensity or “magnitude” of a taste may also affect the onset of satiation. Studies have investigated the impact of taste duration on ad libitum intakes, while maintaining a constant intake rate using a peristaltic pump. In one study, researchers measured individual concentration-pleasantness curves for salt in tomato soup, and exposed subjects to equally palatable low and high intensity salt during separate ad libitum test meals (33, 34). A longer oro-sensory exposure time and higher salt intensity combined to decreased food ad libitum intake by ~9%, though the oro-sensory exposure had a stronger impact than taste intensity. Vickers et al. showed that high yogurt sweetness was liked more than a low sweetness, but consumption showed the opposite effect, indicating that higher sweetness intensity led to earlier satiation (35). Lasschuijt et al. showed that higher taste intensity led to earlier meal termination, but as with previous findings, the effect of oro-sensory duration had a stronger impact than taste intensity (36).
Sensory Contribution of “Fat”; Perceptually Benign but Energetically Potent
New techniques have been developed in recent years to profile dietary energy-intake behaviors based on the predominant taste properties of the foods consumed. This has produced new insights on consumer sensory-patterns of dietary intake based on the preferred taste quality of energy and nutrients consumed (37). This approach has been described as “sensory-epidemiology” and it enables comparison of daily energy intakes by clustering foods by their predominant taste quality, and then comparing the relative contribution of taste clusters to higher or lower energy consumed. Whereas, much attention has been directed to role of sweet foods and added sugar to high energy intakes, findings suggest that the excess energy intakes are mostly associated with greater intakes of “savory–fatty” tasting foods, which are consistently associated with increased energy intakes and higher rates of overweight (11). The implication is that foods high in “savory-fat” combinations make a significant contribution to daily energy intakes. Previous research has shown that our ability to discriminate between fat levels in food is reasonably linear at low taste intensity, but this ability dramatically decreases when fat is presented alongside higher intensity of sweet (38) or salty (39, 40) tastes. The implication is that when we are unable to detect increases in energy density due to higher fat, it becomes more difficult to adjust portion selected or later energy intakes in response to higher energy consumed from fat (41). It is therefore relatively easy to “hide” fats in foods without the fat being sensed, yet it makes a significant contribution to energy intakes. For example, ad libitum energy intake was ~2,100 kcal on a diet with 15–20% energy from fat compared to 2,600 kcal in a diet of equal palatability which derived 45–50% energy from fat (42).
Foods with a higher fat content can also lubricate and agglomerate more rapidly during consumption, which enhances bolus formation and increases eating rate and the extent of energy intake (43–45). As we summarize in the following section, this dual impact of increased eating rate and energy density can also promote excessive energy intakes.
Food Texture, Eating Rate, Energy Intake, and Metabolism
Eating behaviors emerge in response to the texture challenges encountered during consumption, where consumers adapts their microstructural patterns of oral processing to prepare the initial structure for safe swallow (46). The effect of texture on satiation/food intake is mainly operational through eating rate, where harder, chunkier, more viscous textures result in lower eating rates (47). Previous research on liquid and semi-solid foods has shown that ad libitum intake of a liquid was ~30% higher than that of a semi-solid food (48). Difference in intake between liquid and semi-solids disappear when eating rates are set equal, with the help of peristaltic pump (49). There is wide natural in the eating rate of foods commonly encountered, with recent comparisons highlighting a range of between 10 and 120 g/min for solid foods, and rates of up to 400–600 g/min for liquids (50). Energy dense liquids pose a double-risk as they can be easily over-consumed, but also deliver poor satiety on a kcal for kcal basis (51).
Significant progress has been made in our understanding of how food texture influences oral processing (47, 52) and the specific influence of food textures on food intake (53). Food texture has been shown to drive eating rate (44, 54, 55) which in turn can influence ad libitum energy intake to satiation (56), and several studies have shown that faster eating is associated with the transition to overweight and obesity and poor cardio-metabolic health (55, 57–59). A meta-analysis of the food physical and sensory properties that affect intake concluded that people tend to consume less when solid-foods were harder, chunkier and more viscous (60). Evidence from numerous studies (53, 54, 61) now suggest that with a 20% reduction in eating rate, it is possible to reduce ad libitum energy intake by 1–14% without a loss in subsequent feelings of satisfaction (62). Food form and mode of consumption can also influence the rate and extent of intake, and solids have been shown to have a higher satiating efficiency than semi-solids and liquids, unless consumed slowly (as a soup) (63). Similarly, intakes were ~100 g lower each day when a semi-solid food was consumed with a spoon than a straw, highlighting that slower eating rate can support the onset of satiation for fewer calories (64). Food can influence eating rate but also oral processing and saliva-bolus uptake during the oral phase of digestion. Differences in food oral processing behavior have been shown to contribute to temporal changes in post-prandial glucose and insulin, and post-meal satiety responses (65–67). Slower eating rates result in greater bolus surface area, saliva uptakes and may have an incretin effect as early glucose release stimulates greater early insulin release (36, 65). Taken together, these findings indicate that food texture contributes much more simply “sensory appeal”, and can effect satiation and satiety by moderating eating rate, but can also exerts influence on the oral phase of digestion and the subsequent metabolic response to ingested nutrients. Further research is needed to understand how food texture based differences in eating rate can influence food intake control and support healthy metabolic responses to ingested nutrients.
Future Opportunities for Sensory Nutrition
Public health guidelines recommend reductions in sugar, salt and fat but rarely consider the functional role of a foods sensory properties on choice and intake, or opportunities to incorporate an understanding of sensory cues in guiding reformulation or eating behavior changes. This review provides an overview of data that consistently shows how sensory cues have a reproducible influence on how we select, consume and feel satisfied from the foods in our diet. Further research is need to understand whether sensory properties can support sustained changes in eating behavior and promote healthier dietary patterns in the longer-term. Future product development and renovation requires significant reductions in several public sensitive nutrients (i.e., salt, sugar and fat) alongside enhanced nutrient density, to support better health and reduce the risk of diet related chronic disease. Understanding how consumers perceive and consume a food is central to the success of efforts to improve dietary behavior. We outline three potential opportunities for future applications of “sensory-nutrition” approaches to support improved eating behavior, dietary patterns and health.
Using food odors to promote healthy food choice: Research has highlighted the food odors stimulate sensory specific appetites, and are associated with recalled energy content and memory for foods. This may influence “foraging” behavior and is likely to support how we navigate the food environment when making choices on what to consume (68). Limited research to date has focused on the application of odor primes to encourage sensory-appetites and choice for healthier food products, and future research should aim to explore whether odor cues can be applied to stimulate consumer appeal and reinforce positive elements of healthy food choice and consumption.
Application of low-calorie taste compounds to sustain the appeal reduced energy foods: Extensive research has shown the impact of no- and low-calorie sweeteners (non-nutritive sweeteners, NNS) to support sugar reduction, and this has been particularly effective in removing sugar from soft-drinks. Numerous meta-analyses of experimental evidence highlight that applying non-nutritive sweeteners to reduce the sugar content of the diet can both lower dietary energy density and support clinically significant reductions in body weight [i.e., see (69)]. However, as demonstrated earlier, “savory-fatty” foods make a significant contribution to daily energy intakes (11), yet less research has been focused on how to sustain sensory appeal of savory foods with reduced energy density. In addition to the potential application of umami savory enhancers highlighted in the current review, recent findings suggest that kokumi may have the potential to enhance sensory appeal, increase calorie estimates, while supporting energy density reduction (70). These kokumi compounds are low calorie taste enhancers, often comprising tri-peptides and yeast extracts, that are known to enhance sensations of mouthfulness, continuity and complexity, often mimicking the sensory impact of fat. Preliminary findings demonstrate that addition of kokumi compounds can enhance sensory dimensions linked to calorie expectations, and promote higher estimated calories and expected fullness across a series of equi-caloric broths. Future research should further explore the potential of Kokumi compounds to support calorie reduction while maintaining product sensory appeal.
Texture and Energy Density to reduce intake to satiation; Evidence from several controlled feeding studies has demonstrated that energy density (71) and food texture (53) can independently and in combination influence the rate and extent of energy intake within meals. Findings from a recent RCT on ultra-processed foods highlights that higher energy intake rates (kcals/min) support sustained increases in ad libitum energy intake (72). These energy intake rates have been shown to vary widely within the food environment (27). Enhancing food texture in combination with energy density reductions combine to produce an 10–14% reduction in energy intakes, with no loss in meal palatability or post-meal satisfaction (53, 62). Further research is now needed to demonstrate the sustained effect of texture-energy density interventions on habitual energy intakes and subsequent energy balance.
Conclusions
Knowing that the sensory properties of food influence choice and intake behavior is important, but this knowledge will have little impact if we do not apply sensory cues to encourage the consumption of healthier diets. As illustrated above, a number of proof of principle studies have clearly shown that it is possible to change sensory cues in the food environment in such a way that people consume less calories while maintaining the palatability of diets. These approaches require further research to understand the longitudinal impact of sensory properties on energy intake in the food environment and across a wider population of consumers. Controlled “sensory-nutrition” intervention studies are required to further understand how effective these longer term approaches are in producing sensory optimized foods that help to moderate the flow of energy though our diets.
Author Contributions
CF and KG conceived and wrote the manuscript. Both authors revised and approved the submitted version.
Funding
The writing of this article was partially funded by the Dutch TKI-Agri-food Project Restructure; 2020-221.
Conflict of Interest
CF is currently a member of the Global Scientific Advisory Committee of the Kerry Health and Nutrition Institute. KG is a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of Sensus BV (NL) and a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of the Cosun Nutrition Center (NL).
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. McCrickerd K, Forde C. Sensory influences on food intake control: moving beyond palatability. Obes Rev. (2016) 17:18–29. doi: 10.1111/obr.12340
2. Boesveldt S, de Graaf K. The differential role of smell and taste for eating behavior. Perception. (2017) 46:307–19. doi: 10.1177/0301006616685576
3. Forde CG. Measuring Satiation and Satiety. In: Methods in Consumer Research. Vol. 2. Elsevier: Woodhead Publishing (2018). p. 151–82. doi: 10.1016/C2015-0-06109-3
4. De Graaf C, De Jong LS, Lambers AC. Palatability affects satiation but not satiety. Physiol Behav. (1999) 66:681–8. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00335-7
5. Schifferstein HN, Desmet PM. Hedonic asymmetry in emotional responses to consumer products. Food Qual Prefer. (2010) 21:1100–4. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.07.004
6. de Graaf C, Kramer FM, Meiselman HL, Lesher LL, Baker-Fulco C, Hirsch ES, et al. Food acceptability in field studies with US army men and women: relationship with food intake and food choice after repeated exposures. Appetite. (2005) 44:23–31. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.08.008
7. Yeomans MR. Taste, palatability and the control of appetite. Proc Nutr Soc. (1998) 57:609–15. doi: 10.1079/PNS19980089
8. Mela DJ. Food choice and intake: the human factor. Proc Nutr Soc. (1999) 58:513–21. doi: 10.1017/S0029665199000683
9. CDC. Available online at: https://www.cdc.gov/nutrition/data-statistics/added-sugars.html 2021 (accessed December 9, 2021).
10. van Langeveld AW, Gibbons S, Koelliker Y, Civille GV, de Vries JH, de Graaf C, et al. The relationship between taste and nutrient content in commercially available foods from the United States. Food Qual Prefer. (2017) 57:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.10.012
11. Teo PS, Tso R, van Dam RM, Forde CG. Taste of modern diets: the impact of food processing on nutrient sensing and dietary energy intake. J Nutr. (2022) 152:200–10. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxab318
12. Slimani N, Deharveng G, Southgate D, Biessy C, Chajes V, Van Bakel M, et al. Contribution of highly industrially processed foods to the nutrient intakes and patterns of middle-aged populations in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition study. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2009) 63:S206–25. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2009.82
13. Mattes RD. Taste, teleology and macronutrient intake. Curr Opin Physiol. (2021) 19:162–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.11.003
14. Proserpio C, de Graaf C, Laureati M, Pagliarini E, Boesveldt S. Impact of ambient odors on food intake, saliva production and appetite ratings. Physiol Behav. (2017) 174:35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.02.042
15. Gaillet M, Sulmont-Rossé C, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Chambaron S. Priming effects of an olfactory food cue on subsequent food-related behaviour. Food Qual Prefer. (2013) 30:274–81. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.06.008
16. Gaillet-Torrent M, Sulmont-Rossé C, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Chambaron S. Impact of a non-attentively perceived odour on subsequent food choices. Appetite. (2014) 76:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.01.009
17. Zoon HF, de Graaf C, Boesveldt S. Food odours direct specific appetite. Foods. (2016) 5:12. doi: 10.3390/foods5010012
18. Zoon HFA, He W, De Wijk RA, De Graaf C, Boesveldt S. Food preference and intake in response to ambient odours in overweight and normal-weight females. Physiol Behav. (2014) 133:190–6. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.026
19. Ruijschop RM, Boelrijk AE, De Ru JA, De Graaf C, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Effects of retro-nasal aroma release on satiation. Br J Nutr. (2008) 99:1140–8. doi: 10.1017/S0007114507837482
20. Ruijschop RMAJ, Boelrijk AE, de Graaf C, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Retronasal aroma release and satiation: a review. J Agric Food Chem. (2009) 57:9888–94. doi: 10.1021/jf901445z
21. Ramaekers MG, Boesveldt S, Lakemond CMM, Van Boekel MAJS, Luning PA. Odors: appetizing or satiating? Development of appetite during odor exposure over time. Int J Obes. (2014) 38:650–6. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.143
22. Ramaekers MG, Luning PA, Ruijschop RMAJ, Lakemond CMM, Bult JHF, Gort G, et al. Aroma exposure time and aroma concentration in relation to satiation. Br J Nutr. (2014) 111:554–62. doi: 10.1017/S0007114513002729
23. Imada T, Hao SS, Torii K, Kimura E. Supplementing chicken broth with monosodium glutamate reduces energy intake from high fat and sweet snacks in middle-aged healthy women. Appetite. (2014) 79:158–65. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.011
24. Miyaki T, Imada T, Shuzhen Hao S, Kimura E. Monosodium l-glutamate in soup reduces subsequent energy intake from high-fat savoury food in overweight and obese women. Br J Nutr. (2016) 115:176–84. doi: 10.1017/S0007114515004031
25. Masic U, Yeomans MR. Does monosodium glutamate interact with macronutrient composition to influence subsequent appetite? Physiol Behav. (2013) 116–7:23–9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.017
26. Masic U, Yeomans MR. Umami flavor enhances appetite but also increases satiety. Am J Clin Nutr. (2014) 100:532–8. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.080929
27. Forde CG, Mars M, De Graaf K. Ultra-processing or oral processing? A role for energy density and eating rate in moderating energy intake from processed foods. Curr Dev Nutr. (2020) 4:nzaa019. doi: 10.1093/cdn/nzaa019
28. Turner BL, Thompson AL. Beyond the paleolithic prescription: incorporating diversity and flexibility in the study of human diet evolution. Nutr Rev. (2013) 71:501–10. doi: 10.1111/nure.12039
29. Yeomans MR. The role of learning in development of food preferences. Front Nutr Sci. (2006) 3:93. doi: 10.1079/9780851990323.0093
30. De Graaf C, Schreurs A, Blauw YH. Short-term effects of different amounts of sweet and nonsweet carbohydrates on satiety and energy intake. Physiol Behav. (1993) 54:833–43. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90290-V
31. Tey SL, Salleh N, Henry CJ, Forde CG. Effects of consuming preloads with different energy density and taste quality on energy intake and postprandial blood glucose. Nutrients. (2018) 10:161. doi: 10.3390/nu10020161
32. Griffioen-Roose S, Mars M, Finlayson G, Blundell JE, De Graaf C. Satiation due to equally palatable sweet and savory meals does not differ in normal weight young adults. J Nutr. (2009) 139:2093–8. doi: 10.3945/jn.109.110924
33. Bolhuis DP, Lakemond CMM, De Wijk RA, Luning PA, De Graaf C. Effect of salt intensity on ad libitum intake of tomato soup similar in palatability and on salt preference after consumption. Chem Senses. (2010) 35:789–99. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq077
34. Bolhuis DP, Lakemond C, De Wijk R, Luning P, De Graaf C. Both longer oral sensory exposure to and higher intensity of saltiness decrease ad libitum food intake in healthy normal-weight men. J Nutr. (2011) 141:2242–8. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.143867
35. Vickers Z, Holton E, Wang J. Effect of ideal–relative sweetness on yogurt consumption. Food Qual Prefer. (2001) 12:521–6. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(01)00047-7
36. Lasschuijt M, Mars M, de Graaf C, Smeets PA. How oro-sensory exposure and eating rate affect satiation and associated endocrine responses—a randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr. (2020) 111:1137–49. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqaa067
37. Teo PS, van Langeveld AW, Pol K, Siebelink E, de Graaf C, Martin C, et al. Training of a Dutch and Malaysian sensory panel to assess intensities of basic tastes and fat sensation of commonly consumed foods. Food Qual Prefer. (2018) 65:49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.11.011
38. Drewnowski A, Schwartz M. Invisible fats: sensory assessment of sugar/fat mixtures. Appetite. (1990) 14:203–17. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(90)90088-P
39. Bolhuis DP, Costanzo A, Newman LP, Keast RS. Salt promotes passive overconsumption of dietary fat in humans. J Nutr. (2016) 146:838–45. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.226365
40. Bolhuis DP, Newman LP, Keast RS. Effects of salt and fat combinations on taste preference and perception. Chem Senses. (2016) 41:189–95. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv079
41. Brunstrom JM, Drake AC, Forde CG, Rogers PJ. Undervalued and ignored: are humans poorly adapted to energy-dense foods? Appetite. (2018) 120:589–95. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.10.015
42. Lissner L, Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ, Kalkwarf HJ, Roe DA. Dietary fat and the regulation of energy intake in human subjects. Am J Clin Nutr. (1987) 46:886–92. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/46.6.886
43. Van Eck A, Wijne C, Fogliano V, Stieger M, Scholten E. Shape up! how shape, size and addition of condiments influence eating behavior towards vegetables. Food Funct. (2019) 10:5739–51. doi: 10.1039/C9FO01206K
44. Viskaal-van Dongen M, Kok FJ, de Graaf C. Eating rate of commonly consumed foods promotes food and energy intake. Appetite. (2011) 56:25–31. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.11.141
45. Forde CG, van Kuijk N, Thaler T, de Graaf C, Martin N. Oral processing characteristics of solid savoury meal components, and relationship with food composition, sensory attributes and expected satiation. Appetite. (2013) 60:208–19. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.015
46. Hutchings JB, Lillford PJ. The perception of food texture - the philosophy of the breakdown path. J Texture Stud. (1988) 19:103–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-4603.1988.tb00928.x
47. Bolhuis DP, Forde CG. Application of food texture to moderate oral processing behaviors and energy intake. Trends Food Sci Technol. (2020) 106:445–56. doi: 10.1016/j.tifs.2020.10.021
48. De Wijk R, Zijlstra N, Mars M, De Graaf C, Prinz J. The effects of food viscosity on bite size, bite effort and food intake. Physiol Behav. (2008) 95:527–32. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.07.026
49. De Graaf C. Texture and satiation: the role of oro-sensory exposure time. Physiol Behav. (2012) 107:496–501. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.05.008
50. van den Boer J, Werts M, Siebelink E, de Graaf C, Mars M. The availability of slow and fast calories in the Dutch diet: the current situation and opportunities for interventions. Foods. (2017) 6:87. doi: 10.3390/foods6100087
51. de Graaf C. Why liquid energy results in overconsumption. Proc Nutr Soc. (2011) 70:162–70. doi: 10.1017/S0029665111000012
52. Wee MSM, Goh AT, Stieger M, Forde CG. Correlation of instrumental texture properties from textural profile analysis (TPA) with eating behaviours and macronutrient composition for a wide range of solid foods. Food Funct. (2018) 9:5301–12. doi: 10.1039/C8FO00791H
53. McCrickerd K, Lim CM, Leong C, Chia EM, Forde CG. Texture-based differences in eating rate reduce the impact of increased energy density and large portions on meal size in adults. J Nutr. (2017) 147:1208–17. doi: 10.3945/jn.116.244251
54. Forde CG, van Kuijk N, Thaler T, de Graaf C, Martin N. Texture and savoury taste influences on food intake in a realistic hot lunch time meal. Appetite. (2013) 60:180–6. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.10.002
55. Teo PS, Forde CG. The Impact of Eating Rate on Energy Intake, Body Composition, and Health. Handbook of Eating and Drinking: Interdisciplinary Perspectives. Cham: Springer (2020). p. 715–40. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-75388-1
56. Robinson E, Almiron-Roig E, Rutters F, de Graaf C, Forde CG, Tudur Smith C, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effect of eating rate on energy intake and hunger. Am J Clin Nutr. (2014) 100:123–51. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.081745
57. Fogel A, McCrickerd K, Aris IM, Goh AT, Chong Y-S, Tan KH, et al. Eating behaviors moderate the associations between risk factors in the first 1000 days and adiposity outcomes at 6 years of age. Am J Clin Nutr. (2020) 111:997–1006. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqaa052
58. Teo PS, van Dam RM, Whitton C, Tan LWL, Forde CG. Association between self-reported eating rate, energy intake, and cardiovascular risk factors in a multi-Ethnic Asian Population. Nutrients. (2020) 12:1080. doi: 10.3390/nu12041080
59. Teo PS, van Dam RM, Forde CG. Combined impact of a faster self-reported eating rate and higher dietary energy intake rate on energy intake and adiposity. Nutrients. (2020) 12:3264. doi: 10.3390/nu12113264
60. Appleton KM, Newbury A, Almiron-Roig E, Yeomans MR, Brunstrom JM, de Graaf K, et al. Sensory and physical characteristics of foods that impact food intake without affecting acceptability: systematic review and meta-analyses. Obes Rev. (2021) 22:e13234. doi: 10.1111/obr.13234
61. Bolhuis DP, Forde CG, Cheng Y, Xu H, Martin N, de Graaf C. Slow food: sustained impact of harder foods on the reduction in energy intake over the course of the day. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e93370. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093370
62. Forde C. From perception to ingestion; the role of sensory properties in energy selection, eating behaviour and food intake. Food Qual Prefer. (2018) 66:171–7. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.01.010
64. Hogenkamp PS, Mars M, Stafleu A, de Graaf C. Intake during repeated exposure to low-and high-energy-dense yogurts by different means of consumption. Am J Clin Nutr. (2010) 91:841–7. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28360
65. Goh AT, Choy JYM, Chua XH, Ponnalagu S, Khoo CM, Whitton C, et al. Increased oral processing and a slower eating rate increase glycaemic, insulin and satiety responses to a mixed meal tolerance test. Eur J Nutr. (2020) 60:2719–33.
66. Choy J, Goh A, Chatonidi G, Ponnalagu S, Wee S, Stieger M, et al. Impact of food texture modifications on oral processing behaviour, bolus properties and postprandial glucose responses. Curr Res Food Sci. (2021) 4:891–9. doi: 10.1016/j.crfs.2021.11.018
67. Goh AT, Chatonidi G, Choy M, Ponnalagu S, Stieger M, Forde CG. Impact of individual differences in eating rate on oral processing, bolus properties and post-meal glucose responses. Physiol Behav. (2021) 238:113495. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113495
68. de Vries R, de Vet E, de Graaf K, Boesveldt S. Foraging minds in modern environments: high-calorie and savory-taste biases in human food spatial memory. Appetite. (2020) 152:104718. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104718
69. Rogers P, Hogenkamp P, De Graaf C, Higgs S, Lluch A, Ness A, et al. Does low-energy sweetener consumption affect energy intake and body weight? A systematic review, including meta-analyses, of the evidence from human and animal studies. Int J Obes. (2016) 40:381. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.177
70. Tang CS, Tan VW, Teo PS, Forde CG. Savoury and 70 enhancement increases perceived calories and expectations of fullness in equicaloric beef broths. Food Qual Prefer. (2020) 83:103897. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103897
71. Rolls BJ. The relationship between dietary energy density and energy intake. Physiol Behav. (2009) 97:609–15. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.011
Keywords: sensory, food choice, energy intake, texture, odor, taste
Citation: Forde CG and de Graaf K (2022) Influence of Sensory Properties in Moderating Eating Behaviors and Food Intake. Front. Nutr. 9:841444. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.841444
Received: 22 December 2021; Accepted: 27 January 2022;
Published: 21 February 2022.
Edited by:
Melanie Charron, Soremartec Italia Srl, ItalyReviewed by:
Mei Peng, University of Otago, New ZealandCopyright © 2022 Forde and de Graaf. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ciarán G. Forde, ciaran.forde@wur.nl