Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Nutr., 13 November 2018
Sec. Nutrition Methodology
This article is part of the Research Topic Emerging Topics in Dietary Assessment View all 7 articles

The Failure to Measure Dietary Intake Engendered a Fictional Discourse on Diet-Disease Relations

  • 1EvolvingFX, Jupiter, FL, United States
  • 2John Ochsner Heart and Vascular Institute, Ochsner Clinical School, The University of Queensland School of Medicine, New Orleans, LA, United States
  • 3Center for Human Nutrition at the University of Colorado, Aurora, CO, United States

Controversies regarding the putative health effects of dietary sugar, salt, fat, and cholesterol are not driven by legitimate differences in scientific inference from valid evidence, but by a fictional discourse on diet-disease relations driven by decades of deeply flawed and demonstrably misleading epidemiologic research. Over the past 60 years, epidemiologists published tens of thousands of reports asserting that dietary intake was a major contributing factor to chronic non-communicable diseases despite the fact that epidemiologic methods do not measure dietary intake. In lieu of measuring actual dietary intake, epidemiologists collected millions of unverified verbal and textual reports of memories of perceptions of dietary intake. Given that actual dietary intake and reported memories of perceptions of intake are not in the same ontological category, epidemiologists committed the logical fallacy of “Misplaced Concreteness.” This error was exacerbated when the anecdotal (self-reported) data were impermissibly transformed (i.e., pseudo-quantified) into proxy-estimates of nutrient and caloric consumption via the assignment of “reference” values from databases of questionable validity and comprehensiveness. These errors were further compounded when statistical analyses of diet-disease relations were performed using the pseudo-quantified anecdotal data. These fatal measurement, analytic, and inferential flaws were obscured when epidemiologists failed to cite decades of research demonstrating that the proxy-estimates they created were often physiologically implausible (i.e., meaningless) and had no verifiable quantitative relation to the actual nutrient or caloric consumption of participants. In this critical analysis, we present substantial evidence to support our contention that current controversies and public confusion regarding diet-disease relations were generated by tens of thousands of deeply flawed, demonstrably misleading, and pseudoscientific epidemiologic reports. We challenge the field of nutrition to regain lost credibility by acknowledging the empirical and theoretical refutations of their memory-based methods and ensure that rigorous (objective) scientific methods are used to study the role of diet in chronic disease.

Introduction

In an honest search for knowledge, you quite often have to abide by ignorance for an indefinite period. Instead of filing the gap by guesswork… however irksome the gap may be, its obliteration by a fake removes the urge to seek a tenable answer… The steadfastness in [this obligation], nay in appreciating it as a stimulus and a signpost to further quest, is a natural and indispensable disposition in the mind of a scientist.” Erwin Schrodinger (1)

The Success of Nutrition Science

The science of diet-disease relations has a long, illustrious and successful history dating to antiquity (2). One of the first clinical trials ever conducted was an examination of the effects of citrus fruits on scurvy in the eighteenth century (3). And since that time, the science of nutrition achieved clinical and public health successes that were unimaginable in the distant past. For example, in the early twentieth century, diet-related diseases such as beriberi, rickets, and goiter were major public health challenges. In the United States (US), pellagra (a disease of niacin deficiency) caused the death of more than 100,000 Americans and afflicted more than 3 million (4). In New York City, 75% of all infants and nearly 100% of African American children suffered from rickets, a painful disease of vitamin D deficiency (5). Yet by the turn of the twenty-first century, diet-related diseases were almost non-existent and biochemical analyses demonstrated that ~80% of Americans (aged ≥ 6 years) were not at risk of deficiencies in any of the 7 vitamins examined (i.e., vitamins A, B-6, B-12, C, D, and E) (6, 7); and almost 90 percent of women of childbearing age (i.e., 12–49 years of age) were not at risk of iron deficiency, while folate levels increased ~50 percent since 1998 (68). Thus, the improvement of the US food supply and the “American Diet” over the past century was one of the greatest public health success stories in history (9).

Speculations and the Shift to Implausible Anecdotal Evidence

By the mid- twentieth century, as diet-related diseases (e.g., scurvy, pellagra) and protein-energy malnutrition became increasingly rare in industrialized nations, a small but influential group of investigators began speculating that complex chronic non-communicable diseases (NCDs) were somehow causally related to diet [e.g., see (1014)] The evidence ostensibly supporting these conjectures was inferred from epidemiologic studies based on the naïve notion that a person's usual dietary intake (i.e., consumed foods and beverages) could be estimated simply by asking what he or she remembered eating and drinking (9, 1517). Despite the fact that the data collected by these memory-based dietary assessment methods (M-BMs) were non-falsifiable anecdotes (i.e., verbal or textual self-reports) and were repeatedly demonstrated to lack validity and reliability as far back as the 1950s (18, 19), studies employing M-BMs came to dominate the empiric, policy, and media landscapes (1517, 20). By the 1980s, tens of thousands of research reports based on M-BMs were published and some of these publications became the most highly-cited and widely publicized articles in the bio-medical literature.

Nevertheless, six decades of rigorous and highly-replicated research demonstrated unequivocally that M-BMs data were physiologically implausible (i.e., meaningless numbers) (9, 1517, 2125), often “incompatible with life,” (26) and had trivial relations to actual nutrient and caloric consumption (9, 1517, 2123, 2644). For example, in 2013 we used multiple methods to show that from 1971 to 2010, no human being could survive on the average reported caloric intake in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) (21). Furthermore, when hypotheses derived from nutrition epidemiologic research were tested using rigorous study designs, they failed to be supported (4549). For example, when over 50 nutrition claims were examined, “100% of the observational claims failed to replicate” and five conjectures were statistically significant “in the opposite direction” (50). These authors further stated “the public at large–are being deceived, and are being deceived in the name of science. This should not be allowed to continue” (50). While these authors wrote “The cause is elusive” (50), we showed that the refutation of diet-related hypotheses was due to the failure of nutrition epidemiologists to actually measure the variable of interest: dietary-intake.

This failure, in concert with the fact that the diet-disease associations did not meet Bradford Hill's criteria for causation (51), engendered a fictional discourse on the health effects of sugar, salt, fat, and cholesterol. This fictional discourse was widely disseminated by the popular press and exacerbated by governmental and non-governmental health organizations, and commercial interests (e.g., weight-loss industries and manufacturers of “heart-healthy” foods). The wide-spread publication of spurious diet-related speculations had significant adverse public health consequences (15, 20) that included public confusion (15, 52), regressive and misdirected public policy (20, 53, 54), the misallocation of research resources (9), scientifically illiterate recommendations on sugar (15, 55), and potentially harmful recommendations on salt (20, 5660) that may have led to “deaths due to hyponatremia” (20) p. 22.

Purpose of This Critical Analysis

Given the fictional discourse on diet-disease relations and the escalating debate over the validity of M-BMs (16, 17, 6164), the purpose of this critical analysis is to present evidence that the current controversies regarding diet-disease relations are not driven by legitimate differences in scientific inference on the physiologic effects of dietary intake (i.e., consumed foods and beverages). Rather, we contend that current confusion on the putative health effects of dietary sugar, salt, fat, and cholesterol were engendered by a fictional discourse on diet-disease relations created by deeply flawed, demonstrably misleading, and pseudoscientific nutrition epidemiologic reports (9, 1517, 20, 21, 23, 55). Herein, we argue that the confusion created by the fictional discourse and use of pseudoscientific methods to inform public policy led to the field of nutrition science losing credibility and scientific authority. Thus, to regain the public's trust, it is necessary for the field to acknowledge the empirical and theoretical refutations of M-BMs and ensure that in the future, rigorous scientific methods (e.g., randomized control trials, RCTs) are used to study the role of diet in chronic disease.

The Fatal Flaws of Nutrition Epidemiology and the Use of M-BMs

M-BMs, such as 24-h recall interviews (24 HR) and food frequency questionnaires (FFQs), are the predominant protocols in nutritional epidemiologic research. These methods continue to be funded and employed despite several facts and 60+ years of highly replicated, rigorous evidence that invalidate their use (9, 16, 17). In this critical analysis, we present this evidence and several arguments detailing the fatal flaws of M-BMs.

M-BMs Do Not Measure Dietary Intake

Most nutrition epidemiologists do not measure dietary intake (i.e., consumed foods and beverages) or the nutrient and caloric consumption of their participants. Instead, these investigators use M-BMs to collect unverified self-reported memories of what Willett in his popular textbook on nutritional epidemiology called “perceptions of usual intake” (65) p.111. Stated simply, the data collected via M-BMs are not quantitative estimates of dietary intake, but are mere guesstimates of whatever the respondents are willing and able to remember and report about what they think they ate or drank in the past, or want the investigators to think they consumed (9, 16, 17, 21, 23, 32, 37, 44). Thus, because the information collected is founded exclusively upon the respondent's honesty, memory, willingness, and ability to estimate and report past dietary intake (9, 15, 21, 23), investigators have no control over the quality, quantity, or error of the data collected via M-BMs.

Accordingly, there is a large disparity between anecdotal and objective evidence of dietary intake (9, 16, 17, 2123, 2644, 62). This disparity is largely explained by myriad intentional and non-intentional distorting factors, such as reactivity, confabulation, lying, forgetting, false memories, mis-estimation, and social desirability (9, 23, 32, 37). In rigorous scientific fields such as chemistry, biology, and physiology, anecdotal data are not used, and uncorroborated self-reported data are considered “inadmissible” as valid evidence (9). As such, we think that if nutrition epidemiologists wish to regain lost credibility, the use of M-BMs should be discontinued and not be used to inform public policy (9, 15, 20, 23).

Ten Million Jelly-Beans: M-BMs Collect Numerically Labeled Qualitative Data, Not Quantitative Data

Verbal and textual reports of dietary intake are numerically labeled qualitative data (i.e., guesstimates). These data are not quantitative measurements of dietary intake. For example, if a person reports consuming ten million jelly beans, it should be apparent that this guesstimate is not a quantitative measure of actual food consumption but is merely a numerically labeled qualitative report (i.e., an anecdote). This remains a fact even if the person's guesstimate is more plausible (e.g., 50 jelly beans) because neither self-report was based on an objective measurement protocol. Therefore, without objective corroboration these “data” are not falsifiable and therefore must be considered pseudo-scientific (9, 16, 17, 22, 23).

Human Memory and Recall Are Not Valid Tools for Data Collection

Human memory and recall are not valid tools for scientific data collection (9), and it is well-established that subjective reports of past events (i.e., anecdotes) are not representative of objective facts (9, 6670). There is a vast body of evidence demonstrating that “one fact stands out more than any of the others—the very worthlessness of human testimony…” (71) p.13–14. For example, after reviewing the validity of self-reported data in nutrition, health-care, anthropology, communications, criminal justice, economics, and psychology, over three decades ago Bernard et al., concluded “on average, about half of what informants report is probably incorrect…” (66).

Deception: People Lie When Asked to Report Their Dietary Intake

Deception is an inherent part of human nature and the majority of people intentionally misreport (i.e., lie) when reporting their dietary intake (16, 17, 29, 30, 72). For example, when asked to report their dietary intake, 78% of clinical and 64% of non-clinical participants “declared an intention to misreport” (37) p. 209. This is a common finding (39, 41, 72), yet epidemiologists make no attempt to address the reality of intentional misreporting in their research. This failure is a major contributor to the fictional discourse on diet-disease relations because the participants' actual dietary intake is unknown and what is reported may be “pure” fiction (73).

Reactivity: Participants Alter Behavior

It is well-established that participants alter their consumption when asked to report their dietary intake (29, 31, 35, 37, 39, 41). For example, when asked, participants offer socially desirable reports based on current dogma, such as consuming less “fatty foods…[and consuming more]…fruits and vegetables” (30) p. 792. This body of work unequivocally demonstrates that self-reported dietary data are not representative of usual dietary intake, and therefore contribute to the fictional discourse. Yet more importantly, given that health-conscious participants are more likely to engage in exercise, not smoke, and report their dietary intake in a manner consistent with current dietary dogma (e.g., eat more fruits and vegetables), current nutritional epidemiologic studies simply reinforce past recommendations without providing any information regarding the validity of those recommendations. Defenders of M-BMs fail to grasp the significance of this bias (i.e., dietary recommendations are a self-fulfilling prophecy) and often cite the allegedly positive effects of past dietary recommendations as support for the validity of M-BMs [e.g., see (62, 7476)].

Credulousness Is Antithetical to Scientific Data Collection

It is unequivocal that a person's reported memories of perceptions of a past event are not an accurate qualitative or quantitative representation of that event (68, 70, 77, 78). This reality, in confluence with the fact that people often misrepresent their perceptions to influence the opinion of others, demonstrates that reported memories of perceptions of consumed foods and beverages are not valid. In a scathing critique of self-reported data, Lewontin wrote, “It is frightening to think that…science is in the hands of professionals so deaf to human nuance that they believe that people do not lie…and that they have no interest in manipulating the impression that strangers have of them” (79) p. 28. Nevertheless, nutrition epidemiologic investigators treat their participant's anecdotes (i.e., dietary self-reports) as accurate and truthful despite voluminous evidence to the contrary (9, 16, 17, 21, 23). Thus, given that blind-faith is antithetical to scientific data collection, the credulousness of epidemiologists, and their failure to account for basic human behaviors (e.g., deception and reactivity) were major drivers of the fictional discourse on diet-disease relations.

M-BMs Are Founded Upon a “Category Error” and the Logical Fallacy of “Misplaced Concreteness”

Category Error and Incommensurability

A “Category Error” is an ontological mistake. The standard exemplar is the conflation of abstract phenomena with physical objects (80). Mental phenomena, such as memories and perceptions, do not exist outside the mind of the perceiver and therefore cannot be observed nor measured. Conversely, physical objects (e.g., foods and beverages) exist independent of the perceiver, and are therefore, observable, and potentially measurable. Thus, actual dietary intake and memories of perceptions of dietary intake are distinct and incommensurable (i.e., nonequivalent) and a large body of rigorous evidence demonstrated this non-equivalence (9, 2123, 2644, 62). Thus, when employing M-BMs, nutrition epidemiologists are attempting to “measure the unmeasurable” (16) and continue to commit a fatal “Category Error” by erroneously assuming that reported memories of “perceptions of usual intake” (65) p. 50 are the equivalent of actual dietary intake.

Reification: “Misplaced Concreteness”

Reification is the fallacious practice of treating abstract entities as if they were physical objects and is the behavioral extension of a “Category Error.” For example, memories of perceptions of dietary intake are mental phenomena about food and beverages that may or may not have been consumed in the past (9, 68, 81). It should be obvious that these purely mental phenomena do not, and cannot, contain nutrients or calories. Thus, when epidemiologists assign nutrient and caloric values to memories of perceptions of dietary intake they are “mistaking the abstract for the concrete” (82) p. 73. Therefore, when verbal or textual reports of memories of perceptions of dietary intake are presented as estimates of actual intake or nutrient and caloric consumption, these data are both pseudo-scientific and misleading (9, 20).

M-BMs Data Rely on Pseudo-Quantification

Self-reported perceptions of consumed foods and beverages are not estimates of nutrient or caloric consumption. Therefore, statistical analyses of diet-disease relations require the creation of proxy-estimates of nutrient and caloric consumption via the post-hoc assignment of reference nutrient and energy values to the verbal or textual reports. This process is known as pseudo-quantification because it transforms qualitative (i.e., nominal/anecdotal) self-reports into quantitative (i.e., ratio) data via the assignment of numerals. As will be discussed in the following section, pseudo-quantification differs from scientific measurement because measurement is the discovery, not assignment of numeric relations (8385).

Pseudo-Quantification: The Converse of Scientific Measurement

Scientific measurement is the empirical process of discovery in which a known unit of an observable phenomenon (e.g., a milligram of Vitamin C) is compared to the magnitude (i.e., amount) of that unit in an observable entity (e.g., an orange). The mere assignment of numerals is not the scientific equivalent of measurement because the assigned number may or may not have an empirically supported relation to the observed entity (83, 84). Nevertheless, rather than measuring the actual consumption of participants, epidemiologic investigators merely assign “reference” nutrient and caloric values to self-reported foods and beverages to create proxy-estimates of consumption. This process of pseudo-quantification is literally the converse of scientific measurement because the actual nutrient and caloric consumption of the participants are never “discovered” and remain unknown.

Impermissible Transformation: Nominal Data + Assigned Numerals ≠ Quantitative Data

Valid inferences on diet-disease relations necessitate measuring both consumed foods and beverages, and concomitant nutrient and caloric intake. M-BMs measure neither. The mere assignment of numerals to anecdotal evidence does not transform the qualitative (nominal) reports into quantitative (ratio) data. Thus, the process of pseudo-quantification is the impermissible transformation of incommensurable phenomena (i.e., converting abstractions into concrete entities). As described in the prior “Jelly-Bean” example, numerically labeled verbal or textual reports are not the equivalent of quantitative data obtained from measurement protocols. As such, there is no verifiable relation between the dietary reports and the actual foods and beverage consumed. More importantly, given the lack of validity and comprehensiveness of nutrient databases (as discussed below), there are no verifiable relations between the assigned nutrient and caloric values and the actual nutrient and caloric consumption of the participant. These fatal conceptual and measurement flaws explain why the tens of millions of extremely precise proxy-estimates created by epidemiologists over the past six decades were repeatedly demonstrated to be physiologically implausible (i.e., meaningless numbers) (9, 21, 2325).

Nutrient Database Issues

The databases used for the pseudo-quantification of FFQs and 24HRs, such as the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), contain <8,000 unique foods (86). Yet it was estimated that more than 85,000 unique items exists in the ever-expanding US food supply (86) and over 200,000 unique food codes were published in the US Department of Agriculture's (USDA) Food Composition Databases (24, 87). Thus, given that FFQs collect “a finite list of foods/portions with little detail” (62) p. 2 and include only 75–200 items, it is highly unlikely that the extremely precise nutrient and caloric values assigned to FFQ or 24HR data are representative of what was actually consumed (16, 17, 24, 25). Given these facts, both FFQs and 24-HRs lack face validity (16, 17).

Importantly, unlike the standard coefficients used in chemistry, standardized reference nutrient or caloric values for foods and beverages do not exist. Inherent variability and the “rapidly changing landscape of the food supply” (86) prevent the creation and publication of accurate reference values. For example, variation in farming practices (e.g., seed and soil quality), as well as time, temperature, and other changes induced via storage and processing (e.g., cooking), and industry reformulations render the accuracy and reliability of published reference values uncertain, if not wholly invalid (86, 88, 89). For example, Merchant et al. stated, “there may be large discrepancies in nutrient content of foods between the USDA database and what is found in the field because of manufacturing practices,” (90) p. 7 and Phillips et al., stated, “Reliable methods are simply not currently available for some components in all foods (e.g., folate, Vitamin D)” (91) p. 1354. Similarly, Deharveng et al. stated, “…due to the high natural variation in foods…[and]…the use of several sources which may mean that the nutritional values are not comparable within the same table…[and] “common methods and definitions…or modes of expression (energy, protein, carbohydrates, carotenes, vitamin A and E) have not yet been agreed upon, so values are not comparable [between or within databases, and there are] …values produced over 20 years ago with outdated analytical methods” (92) p.60. Moreover, there are numerous nutrient databases with varying degrees of accuracy (e.g., incorrect values) and completeness (e.g., missing values) (86, 88, 90, 9299), and the investigators' choice of database affects results and conclusions (89, 91, 92, 100). As Natarajan et al., wrote, “self-report measures could be strongly biased by the inherent errors in the nutrient databases' ability to estimate true…intake” (100) p. 776.

These results demonstrate there can be no standardized or valid “reference” caloric and nutrient values. As such, the pseudo-quantification of self-reported memories, digital images of foods and beverages, or other recent advances [e.g., “eating” sensors and instrumented utensils (101)] offer no valid or verifiable quantitative information on the actual caloric or nutrient consumption of participants. Therefore, there are no valid nutrient and energy data from which to examine diet-disease relations. Thus, pseudo-quantification is a major driver of both the implausibility of M-BM data and the fictional discourse on diet-disease relations.

Implausible Proxy-Estimates of Nutrient and Energy Consumption

The process of pseudo-quantification does not produce accurate or even believable proxy-estimates of nutrient or caloric consumption. Six decades of rigorous research demonstrated unequivocally that the proxy-estimates created by investigators were often physiologically implausible (i.e., meaningless numbers), (9, 21, 2325) “incompatible with life” (26) p. 347, and have trivial relations to actual nutrient and caloric consumption (9, 2123, 2644, 62). For example, severe, systematic, and intentional under- and over-reporting of specific foods and beverages (e.g., sugar, vegetables, alcohol), and absolute caloric and nutrient consumption (e.g., protein, sodium) are omnipresent (9, 2123, 2644, 62). These highly-replicated results demonstrate that any conclusions regarding diet-disease relations inferred from M-BM-based research are not valid.

The Non-quantifiability of Measurement Error

Falsifiability: Discerning Fact From Fiction

The discrimination between scientific and pseudoscientific (i.e., non-falsifiable) data is contingent upon the ability to discern fact from fiction. To accomplish this task in nutrition, it is first necessary to ascertain if the reported foods and beverages match the respondent's actual intake; and second, it is necessary to quantify the disparity between the proxy-estimates created via pseudo-quantification and the respondent's actual caloric and nutrient consumption.

Thus, accurate estimations of the types and amounts of consumed foods and beverages is required. Yet, as we demonstrated previously (9), this is not possible because “without objective corroboration it is impossible to quantify what percentage of the recalled foods and beverages are completely false, grossly inaccurate, or somewhat congruent with actual consumption” (9) p. 919 and “neither the researchers nor the participants know the validity or reliability of the reported food and beverage consumption…” (9) p. 918. Similarly, it is not possible to quantify the disparity between the proxy-estimates of nutrient and caloric consumption and the respondent's actual consumption because the nutrient and caloric intakes of participants were never measured; these values were merely assigned to the reported foods and beverages. Importantly, the errors in the original verbal or textual reports will be propagated unpredictably via pseudo-quantification in a non-quantifiable manner. This renders the final estimates of nutrient and caloric consumption essentially meaningless. Thus, statements that 24 HR, “have known measurement errors…” (102) p. 922 and that 24HR data can be “adjusted for total energy [to] reduce measurement error” (103) p. 2552 are false and contribute to the ongoing fictional discourse on diet-disease relations.

In summary, M-BMs cannot estimate the participants' true consumption of foods and beverages. Therefore, the participants' actual nutrient and caloric intake are unknown and unknowable; and because the measurement error associated with M-BMs is non-quantifiable and non-falsifiable, self-reported dietary data are inadmissible as scientific evidence (9, 23). Thus, six decades of statistical analyses of pseudo-quantified (i.e., reified) dietary anecdotes were a major contributor of the fictional discourse on diet-disease relations (1517, 55).

Misleading Publications and the Fictional Discourse on Diet-Disease Relations

Failure to Cite Contrary Evidence

Over the past six decades, epidemiologists published tens of thousands of research reports in which millions of self-reported memories of perceptions of dietary intake were presented as the equivalent of data on the actual dietary intake of participants. Nevertheless, and despite a century of research from multiple domains (e.g., psychology, sociology, and cognitive neuroscience) demonstrating that this presentation was patently false and misleading (9, 16, 17, 6670), epidemiologists often failed to cite, acknowledge or address the overwhelming contrary evidence.

For example, in 2013, we demonstrated via multiple methods that over the past five decades the average caloric intake reported in the NHANES could not support human life (21) and that >40% of NHANES participants' reported caloric intakes were below the level needed to support a comatose patient's survival (9, 21, 23, 104). Yet despite the clear refutations and empirically supported rebukes of M-BMs, the 2015 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee (DGAC) (105) falsely wrote that the implausible NHANES data, “provide national and group level estimates of dietary intakes of the U.S. population, on a given day…” (105) Part C, p 13, Similarly, the Dietary Guidelines for Americans (DGA) presented these implausible (i.e., meaningless) NHANES dietary data as “Current Eating Patterns in the United States.” (106) Chapter 2.

Furthermore, 80% of the studies in the US Department of Agriculture's National Evidence Library (107) used by the 2015 DGAC to establish the DGA employed M-BMs. We contend that the unremitting use of this large body of refuted evidence in concert with the failure to cite or even acknowledge contrary evidence exacerbated the fictional discourse and led to the “Disease-Mongering of the American Diet” (108) and the “demonization” of dietary sugar (15, 55). For example, despite biochemical analyses demonstrating that the vast majority of Americans were not at risk of vitamin and mineral deficiencies (7), the 2015 DGAC report stated that “several nutrients are underconsumed”18(PartA,p20) (i.e., vitamins A, C, D, E, and folate) and that for women of childbearing age, “iron also is a shortfall nutrient.”18(PartA,p2). Clearly Americans could not have adequate serum levels (as demonstrated by biochemical analyses), if these vitamins and minerals were “underconsumed.”

The reason for the apparent underconsumption of minerals and vitamins was the well-established fact that self-reported dietary intakes are severely and systematically under-reported via M-BMs. Thus, by failing to acknowledge the existence of contrary biochemical data and reporting only the implausible NHANES M-BM data, the 2015 DGAC presented a false and alarmist perspective on the nutritional status of Americans. This misleading presentation was an exemplar of “disease-mongering” (15, 108) because it distorted the scientific record and misled both the public and policy makers by erroneously suggesting that Americans were at risk for nutritional deficiencies when objective evidence demonstrated they were not (15, 55).

More recently, the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine examined the process by which the Dietary Guidelines for Americans were created. Their report, “Redesigning the Process for Establishing the Dietary Guidelines for Americans” (109) was ostensibly intended to review the ongoing controversy surrounding the rigor and validity of the evidence employed. Nevertheless, the authors failed to cite any of our numerous peer-reviewed publications refuting the validity of the NHANES dietary data, and in direct opposition to extant evidence, the authors of the report wrote, “After extensive evaluation, we found that the current methods being used in the DGA process…are indeed appropriate” (109) p. x; Preface [and] “Self-report dietary intake [M-BMs] data are central to the development of dietary guidelines” (109) p. 4–13. Thus, by summarily excluding our large body of contrary evidence and failing to inform readers of the empiric, theoretic and conceptual refutations of their methods and data, the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine exacerbated the fictional discourse on the putative health effects of dietary sugar, salt, fat, and cholesterol. This biased presentation has significant public-health consequences (e.g., public confusion, ineffective, and regressive public policy, and misallocation of research resources) (9, 15, 20).

Recent Contributions to the Fictional Discourse on Diet-Disease Relations

Most recently, the Journal of Clinical Epidemiology published a series of “Controversy and Debate” articles on the “Fatal Flaws of Food Frequency Questionnaires…” (16, 17, 63, 64). In our target paper (17), we presented a number of very specific challenges to the status quo in nutrition epidemiology. Nevertheless, our esteemed opponents in the debate failed to address the issues and chose to offer mere ipse dixit statements and fallacious arguments (e.g., ignoratio elenchi, ad hominems, ad populum) (63, 64). Thus, in our closing statement we wrote that improving nutrition science and public health policy will be achieved only if the epidemiologic research community acknowledges and addresses contrary evidence and empirical refutations (16).

Challenges and Relevancy of Estimating Dietary Intake

Challenges to Measuring or Estimating Dietary Intake

Given that deception and reactivity are inherent components of human relations, acquiring accurate information on behaviors that are subject to social approbation or stigma is extremely challenging. Thus, it is highly unlikely that data derived from uncorroborated self-reports and other forms of information controlled exclusively by the participant will ever be valid. For example, while there is emerging interest in digital food imaging, bite counters, instrumented utensils and other movement sensors to estimate dietary intake (101, 110, 111), it should be apparent that these technology-based methods do not address the major issues presented herein (e.g., deception, reactivity, pseudo-quantification, and invalidity of reference nutrient and energy values). For example, individuals wishing to “game” the system can merely remove the devices when eating or take digital images of their dining companion's salad while consuming a high-calorie dessert. Thus, despite their novelty and inventiveness, these technological developments currently offer little progress in estimating habitual dietary consumption because they do not account for basic human behaviors (e.g., intentional deception and reactivity).

Diet-Centrism: “Let Food Be Thy Medicine…”

Diet-disease relations were posited since antiquity and it has been asserted for millennia that individuals should “Let food be thy medicine and medicine be thy food” (112). While this ancient advice was relevant to individuals consuming nutritionally inadequate diets, for individuals and populations consuming biochemically superior diets [e.g., the average US citizen (68)] this advice is archaic and misleading (15).

Recently, we coined the term “Diet-Centrism” to describe the “the naïve tendency of both researchers and the public to attribute a wide-range of negative health outcomes exclusively to dietary factors while neglecting the essential and well-established role of individual differences in nutrient-metabolism” (15). As we demonstrated, the explicit conflation of “diet” with both nutritional status and health ignores the fact that the human body is a complex physiologic system in which dietary factors are merely one of myriad factors that affect health. And more importantly, the effects of dietary intake are entirely dependent on the physiologic context of the consuming individual (15). Thus, with respect to diet-related health, it is not what is eaten that affects health and disease, but what one's body does with what was eaten (113, 114). Therefore, as we previously detailed (15, 55, 113), the idea that “you are what you eat” is demonstrative of prescientific thinking [i.e., magico-religious reasoning (55)], “physiologic illiteracy” (15, 114), and flouts centuries of progress in medical science because dietary components cannot have effects independent of the physiologic context of the consuming individual (15, 55, 113, 114).

Does “Diet” Have a Non-trivial Impact on Health?

Modern scientific investigations established that if a person habitually fails to consume sufficient calories or protein to meet metabolic demands, that person will die due to protein-energy malnutrition (i.e., starvation). Similarly, if a person fails to consume adequate levels of nutrients, then he or she will suffer diseases specific to the dietary deficiency (e.g., scurvy from insufficient Vitamin C). Nevertheless, it is extremely important to note that in general populations the established causal effects of “diet” are limited to protein-energy malnutrition and nutrient deficiencies.

Contrary to current conjectures, there are no valid data demonstrating that “diet” per se is causal to increased mortality from obesity, NCDs, and metabolic diseases (15, 113). First, diet-centric speculations based on mere statistical associations provide no evidence of causation, and when tested via rigorous (i.e., objective) methods these hypotheses were repeatedly demonstrated to be false (50, 115, 116). Second, diet-disease conjectures on sugar, salt, and fat consumption fail to meet Bradford Hill's criteria for causality (e.g., strength, consistency, biological gradient, and specificity) (15, 51, 113). Third, the vast majority of diet-disease relations rely upon the validity of M-BMs. If as demonstrated herein, M-BMs are not valid, then ~80% of the research in the US Department of Agricultures' National Evidence Library (107) and most diet-centric speculations have no valid empirical support. Fourth, clinical trials examining intermediate biomarkers (e.g., serum lipids) and other surrogate risk factors are often irrelevant to the actual risk of mortality and morbidity (117). For example, it is well-established that while biomarkers may reflect the short-term physiologic effects of an intervention, these alterations are not necessarily indicative of changes in risk (118, 119). Fifth, myriad paradoxes [e.g., Australian, French, Finish, Irish, Israeli, Indian, Spanish, Masai, and Japanese (120128)] and rigorous analyses (15, 113, 129, 130) suggest that sugar, salt, and fat are merely necessary for health and well-being but have no major impacts on chronic diseases (15, 113). For example, there are populations that consume as much as 80% of their caloric intake from added sugars with no obesity and metabolic diseases. For a review please see (15, 113). Finally, research dating to the 1950s demonstrated that the “over-consumption” that leads to obesity and metabolic disease is not driven by dietary factors per se, but by physical inactivity-induced increments in energy intake (113, 114, 131134) and non-genetic evolutionary processes (i.e., accumulative maternal effects) (113, 114, 135137) that lead to the asymmetric and adipogenic partitioning of nutrient-energy (114).

Thus, we posit that while dietary intake is an obvious and essential component of health, it is a trivial risk factor for obesity, metabolic, and chronic diseases (15, 55, 73, 113, 114). Our position is rapidly acquiring support given the “tiny” effect sizes and “massive confounding” inherent in nutrition research (61, 138140). For example, when compared to the relative risk estimates of smoking tobacco, estimates for dietary factors are an order of magnitude smaller (140). In fact, these estimates are so trivial, “crude and imprecise” that most diet-disease associations may be considered spurious (141). As such, we posit that measuring “diet” per se is tangential if not irrelevant to the major public health issues faced by industrialized nations (113, 114).

Forward Progress

Because identical diets consumed by different individuals result in divergent metabolic and health effects (15, 114, 142144), measuring “diet” without accurate and detailed knowledge of the metabolic phenotype of the consuming individual is pointless. And given that accurate metabolic phenotyping in large samples is prohibitively expensive in terms of resources and participant burden, valid epidemiologic (i.e., population-level) investigations of diet-disease relations may simply be unachievable.

Nevertheless, the metabolic fate of consumed foods and beverages are accessibly to quantification via laboratory settings because all dietary components are fully metabolized or excreted in experimentally relevant time-frames (i.e., minutes to days). Thus, the non-trivial physiologic effects of dietary intake can be ascertained via RCTs. Given this reality, it is important to note that the notion that “diet” has long-term physiologic or health effects independent of the metabolic fate of consumed bio-active molecules is a form of magico-religious reasoning (15, 55) and an impediment to scientific progress.

We realize that some may perceive our conclusions as both contrary and controversial. Nevertheless, we posit that the decades-long fictional discourse on the effects of dietary sugar, salt, and fat led to an extreme form of diet-centrism that obscured well-established evidence and engendered the proliferation of misleading and demonstrably false research programs and failed public health initiatives (15, 20, 113). Thus, given the evidence presented herein, it is incumbent upon nutrition epidemiologists to provide valid scientific support for their “diet-centric” speculations and demonstrate that the average “Western diet” (6, 7) has non-trivial effects on obesity and NCDs in industrialized nations (114).

Summary and Conclusions

Since its first clinical trial in the eighteenth century, the field of nutrition relied on the observable effects of an individual's dietary intake on his or her health. This scientific process led to the elimination of diet-related deficiencies and substantial improvements in public health. Nonetheless, beginning in the 1950s, the field allowed rigorous research to be obscured by the sensational but implausible results and conclusions generated by the pseudo-quantified anecdotal data generated via M-BMs. The devolution from rigorous scientific observation to anecdotal (i.e., self-reported) evidence led to a fictional discourse on diet-disease relations that resulted in both public and policy confusion, and a major loss of credibility for the nutrition sciences. We challenge the field to acknowledge the inherent flaws and empirical and theoretical refutations of M-BMs, and ensure that in the future, rigorous scientific methods (e.g., RCTs) are used to study the role of diet in chronic disease.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

EA is employed by EvolvingFX.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Schrödinger E. ‘Nature and the Greeks’ and ‘Science and Humanism’. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (1954/1996).

2. Hippocrates. Hippocrates collected works I. In: Jones WHS, editor. Digital Hippocrates Collection. Cambridge: Harvard University Press (1868). Available online at: https://daedalus.umkc.edu/hippocrates/HippocratesLoeb1/index.html (Accessed October 24, 2018).

3. Baron JH. Sailors' scurvy before and after James Lind – a reassessment. Nutr Rev. (2009) 67:315–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2009.00205.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Rajakumar K. Pellagra in the United States: a historical perspective. South Med J. (2000) 93:272–7. doi: 10.1097/00007611-200093030-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rajakumar K, Thomas SB. Reemerging nutritional rickets: a historical perspective. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2005) 159:335–41. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.4.335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cdc. Second National Report on Biochemical Indicators of Diet and Nutrition in the U.S. Population. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2012). Available online at: http://www.cdc.gov/nutritionreport/pdf/Nutrition_Book_complete508_final.pdf (Accessed August 20, 2015).

7. Pfeiffer CM, Sternberg MR, Schleicher RL, Haynes BM, Rybak ME, Pirkle JL. The CDC's Second National Report on biochemical indicators of diet and nutrition in the U.S. population is a valuable tool for researchers and policy makers. J Nutr. (2013) 143:938S−47S. doi: 10.3945/jn.112.172858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. CDC. National Report on Biochemical Indicators of Diet and Nutrition in the US Population 1999–2002. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Environmental Health (2008). Available online at http://www.cdc.gov/nutritionreport

9. Archer E, Pavela G, Lavie CJ. The inadmissibility of what we eat in America and NHANES dietary data in nutrition and obesity research and the scientific formulation of national dietary guidelines. Mayo Clin Proc. (2015) 90:911–26. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2015.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Keys A. Diet and the epidemiology of coronary heart disease. J Am Med Assoc. (1957) 164:1912–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.1957.62980170024007e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Keys A, Aravanis C, Blackburn HW, Van Buchem FS, Buzina R, Djordjevic BD, et al. Epidemiological studies related to coronary heart disease: characteristics of men aged 40-59 in seven countries. Acta Med Scand Suppl. (1966) 460:1–392. doi: 10.1111/j.0954-6820.1966.tb04737.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hegsted DM. Dietary goals–a progressive view. Am J Clin Nutr. (1978) 31:1504–9. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/31.9.1504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hegsted DM. Serum-cholesterol response to dietary cholesterol: a re-evaluation. Am J Clin Nutr. (1986) 44:299–305. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/44.2.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Keys A, Menotti A, Karvonen MJ, Aravanis C, Blackburn H, Buzina R, et al. The diet and 15-year death rate in the seven countries study. Am J Epidemiol. (1986) 124:903–915. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Archer E. In defense of sugar: a critique of diet-centrism. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. (2018) 61:10–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2018.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Archer E, Marlow ML, Lavie CJ. Controversy and Debate: Memory-Based Dietary Assessment Methods Paper #3. J Clin Epidemiol. (2018). doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.07.013. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text

17. Archer E, Marlow ML, Lavie CJ. Controversy and debate: memory-based methods paper 1: the fatal flaws of food frequency questionnaires and other memory-based dietary assessment methods. J Clin Epidemiol. (2018). doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.08.003. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Beaudoin R, Mayer J. Food intakes of obese and non-obese women. J Am Diet Assoc. (1953) 29:29–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

19. Stunkard AJ. The Management of obesity. N Y State J Med. (1958) 58:79–87.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

20. Archer E, Marlow M, Williams R. Government Dietary Guidelines: Uncertain Science Leads to Questionable Public Health Policy. Mercatus Center (2017). Available online at: https://www.mercatus.org/publications/government-dietary-guidelines-public-health-policy (Accessed April 30, 2017).

21. Archer E, Hand GA, Blair SN. Validity of U.S. nutritional surveillance: national health and nutrition examination survey caloric energy intake data, 1971-2010. PLoS ONE (2013) 8:e76632. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Archer E, Blair SN. Reply to LS Freedman et al. Adv Nutr. (2015) 6:489–90. doi: 10.3945/an.115.009183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Archer E, Pavela G, Lavie CJ. A discussion of the refutation of memory-based dietary assessment methods (M-BMs): the rhetorical defense of pseudoscientific and inadmissible evidence. Mayo Clin Proceed. (2015) 90:1736–8. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2015.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Archer E. The use of implausible data without caveats is misleading. Am J Clin Nutr. (2017) 106:949–50. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.116.150870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Archer E. The NHANES dietary data are physiologically implausible and inadmissible as scientific evidence. Am J Clin Nutr. (2017) 106:951–2. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.116.150946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ioannidis JPA. Implausible results in human nutrition research. BMJ (2013) 347:6698. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f6698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Goldberg GR, Black AE, Jebb SA, Cole TJ, Murgatroyd PR, Coward WA, et al. Critical evaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 1. Derivation of cut-off limits to identify under-recording. Eur J Clin Nutr. (1991) 45:569–81.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

28. Lichtman SW, Pisarska K, Berman ER, Pestone M, Dowling H, Offenbacher E, et al. Discrepancy between self-reported and actual caloric intake and exercise in obese subjects. N Engl J Med. (1992) 327:1893–8. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199212313272701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Macdiarmid JI, Blundell JE. Dietary under-reporting: what people say about recording their food intake. Eur J Clin Nutr. (1997) 51:199–200. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Mela DJ, Aaron JI. Honest but invalid what subjects say about recording their food intake. J Acad Nutr Dietetics (1997) 97:791–3. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(97)00195-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Goris AH, Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Westerterp KR. Undereating and underrecording of habitual food intake in obese men: selective underreporting of fat intake. Am J Clin Nutr. (2000) 71:130–4. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/71.1.130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Heitmann BL, Lissner L, Osler M. Do we eat less fat, or just report so? Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. (2000) 24:435–42. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Schaefer EJ, Augustin JL, Schaefer MM, Rasmussen H, Ordovas JM, Dallal GE, et al. Lack of efficacy of a food-frequency questionnaire in assessing dietary macronutrient intakes in subjects consuming diets of known composition. Am J Clin Nutr. (2000) 71:746–51. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/71.3.746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ferrari P, Slimani N, Ciampi A, Trichopoulou A, Naska A, Lauria C, et al. Evaluation of under- and overreporting of energy intake in the 24-hour diet recalls in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC). Public Health Nutr. (2002) 5:1329–45. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Westerterp KR, Goris AH. Validity of the assessment of dietary intake: problems of misreporting. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care (2002) 5:489–93. doi: 10.1097/00075197-200209000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Scagliusi FB, Polacow VO, Artioli GG, Benatti FB, Lancha AHJr. Selective underreporting of energy intake in women: magnitude, determinants, and effect of training. J Am Diet Assoc. (2003) 103:1306–13. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(03)01074-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lara JJ, Scott JA, Lean ME. Intentional mis-reporting of food consumption and its relationship with body mass index and psychological scores in women. J Hum Nutr Diet. (2004) 17:209–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-277X.2004.00520.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Lissner L, Troiano RP, Midthune D, Heitmann BL, Kipnis V, Subar AF, et al. OPEN about obesity: recovery biomarkers, dietary reporting errors and BMI. Int J Obes. (2007) 31:956–61. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Macdiarmid J, Blundell J. Assessing dietary intake: who, what and why of under-reporting. Nutr Res Rev. (2007) 11:231–53. doi: 10.1079/NRR19980017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Forrestal SG. Energy intake misreporting among children and adolescents: a literature review. Matern Child Nutr. (2011) 7:112–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8709.2010.00270.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Stubbs RJ, O'reilly LM, Whybrow S, Fuller Z, Johnstone AM, Livingstone MBE, et al. Measuring the difference between actual and reported food intakes in the context of energy balance under laboratory conditions. Br J Nutr. (2014) 111:2032–43. doi: 10.1017/S0007114514000154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Mercado CI, Cogswell ME, Valderrama AL, Wang CY, Loria CM, Moshfegh AJ, et al. Difference between 24-h diet recall and urine excretion for assessing population sodium and potassium intake in adults aged 18-39 y. Am J Clin Nutr. (2015) 101:376–86. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.081604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Orcholski L, Luke A, Plange-Rhule J, Bovet P, Forrester TE, Lambert EV, et al. Under-reporting of dietary energy intake in five populations of the African diaspora. Br J Nutr. (2015) 113:464–72. doi: 10.1017/S000711451400405X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Sugimoto M, Asakura K, Masayasu S, Sasaki S. Relatively severe misreporting of sodium, potassium, and protein intake among female dietitians compared with nondietitians. Nutr Res. (2016) 36:818–26. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2016.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Prentice RL. Clinical trials and observational studies to assess the chronic disease benefits and risks of multivitamin-multimineral supplements. Am J Clin Nutr. (2007) 85:308S−313S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.1.308S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Schatzkin A, Mouw T, Park Y, Subar AF, Kipnis V, Hollenbeck A, et al. Dietary fiber and whole-grain consumption in relation to colorectal cancer in the NIH-AARP diet and health study. Am J Clin Nutr. (2007) 85:1353–1360. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.5.1353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Tatsioni A, Bonitsis NG, Ioannidis JP. Persistence of contradicted claims in the literature. JAMA (2007) 298:2517–26. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.21.2517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Druesne-Pecollo N, Latino-Martel P, Norat T, Barrandon E, Bertrais S, Galan P, et al. Beta-carotene supplementation and cancer risk: a systematic review and metaanalysis of randomized controlled trials. Int J Cancer (2010) 127:172–84. doi: 10.1002/ijc.25008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Moorthy D, Chung M, Lee J, Yu WW, Lau J, Trikalinos TA. Concordance Between the Findings of Epidemiological Studies and Randomized Trials in Nutrition: An Empirical Evaluation and Citation Analysis: Nutritional Research Series, Vol. 6. Rockville MD (2013).

Google Scholar

50. Young SS, Karr A. Deming, data and observational studies. Significance (2011) 8:116–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-9713.2011.00506.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hill AB. The environment and disease: association or causation? Proc R Soc Med. (1965) 58:295–300.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

52. Rowe S, Alexander N. Are nutrition scientists communicating worse or has the situation just gotten more complicated? Nutr Today (2013) 48:251–4. doi: 10.1097/NT.0000000000000005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Edwards RD. Sugar-sweetened beverage taxes raise demand for substitutes and could even raise caloric intake. Prevent Med. (2012) 54:284–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.12.003

CrossRef Full Text

54. Cornelsen L, Green R, Dangour A, Smith R. Why fat taxes won't make us thin. J Public Health (2014) 37:18–23. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdu032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Archer E. The demonization of ‘Diet' is nothing new. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 61. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2018.07.013. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

56. Hite AH, Feinman RD, Guzman GE, Satin M, Schoenfeld PA, Wood RJ. In the face of contradictory evidence: report of the Dietary Guidelines for Americans Committee. Nutrition (2010) 26:915–24. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2010.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lawrence GD. Dietary fats and health: dietary recommendations in the context of scientific evidence. Adv Nutr. (2013) 4:294–302. doi: 10.3945/an.113.003657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Cohen E, Cragg M, Defonseka J, Hite A, Rosenberg M, Zhou B. Statistical review of US macronutrient consumption data, 1965-2011: Americans have been following dietary guidelines, coincident with the rise in obesity. Nutrition (2015) 31:727–32. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2015.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Hite AH, Schoenfeld P. Open Letter to the Secretaries of the U.S. Departments of Agriculture and Health and Human Services on the creation of the 2015 Dietary Guidelines for Americans. Nutrition (2015) 31:776–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2014.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Teicholz N. The scientific report guiding the US dietary guidelines: is it scientific? BMJ (2015) 351:h4962. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h4962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Dhurandhar NV, Schoeller D, Brown AW, Heymsfield SB, Thomas D, Sorensen TI, et al. Energy balance measurement: when something is not better than nothing. Int J Obes. (2014) 39:1109–13. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Subar AF, Freedman LS, Tooze JA, Kirkpatrick SI, Boushey C, Neuhouser ML, et al. Addressing current criticism regarding the value of self-report dietary data. J Nutr. (2015) 145:2639–45. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.219634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Martín-Calvo N, Martínez-González MÁ. Controversy and debate: memory-based methods paper 4. J Clin Epidemiol. (2018). doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.08.004. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

64. Martín-Calvo N, Martínez-González MÁ. Controversy and debate: memory-based dietary assessment methods paper 2. J Clin Epidemiol. (2018). [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

65. Willett W. Nutritional Epidemiology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (1998).

Google Scholar

66. Bernard HR, Killworth P, Kronenfeld D, Sailer L. The problem of informant accuracy: the validity of retrospective data. Ann Rev Anthropol. (1984) 13:495–517. doi: 10.1146/annurev.an.13.100184.002431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Schacter D. The Seven Sins of Memory. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt (2001).

Google Scholar

68. Loftus E. Our changeable memories: legal and practical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2003) 4:231–4. doi: 10.1038/nrn1054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Schacter DL, Slotnick SD. The cognitive neuroscience of memory distortion. Neuron (2004) 44:149–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Schacter DL, Loftus EF. Memory and law: what can cognitive neuroscience contribute? Nat Neurosci. (2013) 16:119–23. doi: 10.1038/nn.3294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Ryan C. The Second National Colloquium on Oral History at Arden House. New York, NY: Oral History Association (1968).

72. Blundell JE. What foods do people habitually eat? A dilemma for nutrition, an enigma for psychology. Am J Clin Nutr. (2000) 71:3–5. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/71.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Archer E, Lavie CJ. Is the PURE study ‘pure’ fiction? Eur Heart J. (in press).

74. Hébert J, Hurley TG, Steck SE, Miller DR, Tabung FK, Peterson KE, et al. Considering the value of dietary assessment data in informing nutrition-related health policy. Adv Nutr. (2014) 5:447–55. doi: 10.3945/an.114.006189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Davy BM, Estabrooks PA. The validity of self-reported dietary intake data: focus on the “what we eat in america” component of the national health and nutrition examination survey research initiative. Mayo Clinic Proceed. (2015) 90:845–7. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2015.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Satija A, Yu E, Willett WC, Hu FB. Understanding nutritional epidemiology and its role in policy. Adv Nutr. (2015) 6:5–18. doi: 10.3945/an.114.007492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Schooler JW. The distinctions of false and fuzzy memories. J Exp Child Psychol. (1998) 71:130–43. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Schacter DL. Constructive memory: past and future. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2012) 14:7–18.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

79. Lewontin RC. Sex, lies, and social science. The New York Review of Books. Westminster, MD: Penguin Random House (1995).

Google Scholar

80. Ryle G. The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinsons University Library (1951).

Google Scholar

81. Bernstein DM, Loftus EF. The consequences of false memories for food preferences and choices. Persp Psychol Sci. (2009) 4:135–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01113.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Whitehead AN. Science and the Modern World. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster (1997/1925).

Google Scholar

83. Euclid, Heath TL. The Thirteen Books of the Elements, Vol. 2. New York, NY: Dover Publications (1956).

84. Michell J. Measurement in Psychology: A Critical History of a Methodological Concept. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (1999).

Google Scholar

85. Michell J. Measurement: a beginner's guide. J Appl Meas. (2003) 4:298–308.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

86. Ng SW, Popkin BM. Monitoring foods and nutrients sold and consumed in the United States: dynamics and Challenges. J Acad Nutr Dietetics 112:41–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

87. USDA. USDA Food Composition Databases. United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service. Available online at: https://ndb.nal.usda.gov/ndb/search/list (Accessed April 20, 2017).

88. Ahuja JKC, Lemar L, Goldman JD, Moshfegh AJ. The impact of revising fats and oils data in the US Food and Nutrient Database for Dietary Studies. J Food Composit Anal. (2009) 22 (Supplement):S63–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jfca.2009.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Ng SW, Dunford E. Complexities and opportunities in monitoring and evaluating US and global changes by the food industry. Obes Rev. (2013) 14 (Suppl 2):29–41. doi: 10.1111/obr.12095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Merchant AT, Dehghan M. Food composition database development for between country comparisons. Nutr J. (2006) 5:2. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-5-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Phillips KM, Patterson KY, Rasor AS, Exler J, Haytowitz DB, Holden JM, et al. Quality-control materials in the USDA national food and nutrient analysis program (NFNAP). Anal Bioanal Chem. (2006) 384:1341–55. doi: 10.1007/s00216-005-0294-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Deharveng G, Charrondiere UR, Slimani N, Southgate DA, Riboli E. Comparison of nutrients in the food composition tables available in the nine European countries participating in EPIC. Eur Prosp Invest Cancer Nutr Eur J Clin Nutr. (1999) 53:60–79. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Guenther PM, Perloff BP, Vizioli TLJr. Separating fact from artifact in changes in nutrient intake over time. J Am Diet Assoc. (1994) 94:270–5. doi: 10.1016/0002-8223(94)90367-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Bazzano LA, He J, Ogden LG, Loria CM, Vupputuri S, Myers L, et al. Agreement on nutrient intake between the databases of the first national health and nutrition examination survey and the ESHA food processor. Am J Epidemiol. (2002) 156:78–85. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Davis DR, Epp MD, Riordan HD. Changes in USDA food composition data for 43 garden crops, 1950 to 1999. J Am Coll Nutr. (2004) 23:669–82. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2004.10719409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Ishihara J, Inoue M, Kobayashi M, Tanaka S, Yamamoto S, Iso H, et al. Impact of the revision of a nutrient database on the validity of a self-administered food frequency questionnaire (FFQ). J Epidemiol. (2006) 16:107–16. doi: 10.2188/jea.16.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Urban LE, Mccrory MA, Dallal GE, Das SK, Saltzman E, Weber JL, et al. Accuracy of stated energy contents of restaurant foods. JAMA (2012) 306:287–93. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Maalouf J, Cogswell ME, Gunn JP, Curtis CJ, Rhodes D, Hoy K, et al. Monitoring the sodium content of restaurant foods: public health challenges and opportunities. Am J Public Health (2013) 103:e21–30. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. De Bruyn J, Ferguson E, Allman-Farinelli M, Darnton-Hill I, Maulaga W, Msuya J, et al. Food composition tables in resource-poor settings: exploring current limitations and opportunities, with a focus on animal-source foods in sub-Saharan Africa. Br J Nutr. (2016) 116:1709–19. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516003706

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Natarajan L, Flatt SW, Sun X, Gamst AC, Major JM, Rock CL, et al. Validity and systematic error in measuring carotenoid consumption with dietary self-report instruments. Am J Epidemiol. (2006) 163:770–8. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Schoeller DA, Westerterp M. Advances in the Assessment of Dietary Intake. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press (2017).

Google Scholar

102. Micha R, Peñalvo JL, Cudhea F, Imamura F, Rehm CD, Mozaffarian D. Association between dietary factors and mortality from heart disease, stroke, and type 2 diabetes in the united states. JAMA (2017) 317:912–24. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.0947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Rehm CD, Penalvo JL, Afshin A, Mozaffarian D. Dietary intake among US adults, 1999-2012. JAMA (2016) 315:2542–53. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.7491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Archer E, Blair SN. Implausible data, false memories, and the status quo in dietary assessment. Adv Nutr. (2015) 6:229–30. doi: 10.3945/an.114.007799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Dgac. Scientific Report of the 2015 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services and U.S. Department of Agriculture (2015).

106. Dga. Dietary Guidelines for Americans. Washington, DC: U.S. Departments of Health and Human Services (HHS) and of Agriculture (USDA) (2015). Available online at http://health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2015/guidelines/ (Last accessed April 16, 2016).

107. USDA. National Evidence Library. United States Department of Agriculture (2015). Available online at: http://www.nel.gov/ (Accessed June 12, 2016).

108. Archer E. The disease-mongering of the american diet. In: Priorities. American Council of Science and Health. (2018). Available online at: https://www.acsh.org/news/2018/05/23/disease-mongering-american-diet-13002 (Accessed September 27, 2018).

109. NASEM Redesigning the Process for Establishing the Dietary Guidelines for Americans. Washington, DC: National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine; The National Academies Press (2017).

110. Scisco JL, Muth ER, Dong Y, Hoover AW, O'neil P, Fishel-Brown SR. Usability and acceptability of the “bite counter” device. Proc Hum Factors and Ergon Soc Annu Meet. (2011). 55:1967–9. doi: 10.1177/1071181311551410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Martin CK, Correa JB, Han H, Allen HR, Rood J, Champagne CM, et al. Validity of the Remote Food Photography Method (RFPM) for estimating energy and nutrient intake in near real-time. Obesity (2012) 20:891–9. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Hippocrates (1849). The Genuine Works of Hippocrates; translated from the Greek With a Preliminary Discourse and Anotations. New York, NY: W. Wood and company.

113. Archer E, Lavie CJ, Hill JO. The contributions of ‘diet’, ‘genes’, and physical activity to the etiology of obesity: contrary evidence and consilience. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 61:89–102.

PubMed Abstract

114. Archer E, Pavela G, Mcdonald S, Lavie CJ, Hill JO. Cell-specific “competition for calories” drives asymmetric nutrient-energy partitioning, obesity, and metabolic diseases in human and non-human animals. Front Physiol. (2018) 9:1053. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Bjelakovic G, Nikolova D, Gluud LL, Simonetti RG, Gluud C. Antioxidant supplements for prevention of mortality in healthy participants and patients with various diseases. Cochr Datab Syst Rev. (2012) 3:CD007176. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007176.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Ye Y, Li J, Yuan Z. Effect of antioxidant vitamin supplementation on cardiovascular outcomes: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS ONE (2013) 8:e56803. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Psaty BM, Weiss NS, Furberg CD, Koepsell TD, Siscovick DS, Rosendaal FR, et al. Surrogate end points, health outcomes, and the drug-approval process for the treatment of risk factors for cardiovascular disease. JAMA (1999) 282:786–90. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.8.786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Ball JR, Micheel CM. Evaluation of Biomarkers and Surrogate Endpoints in Chronic Disease, Washington, DC: National Academies Press (2010).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

119. Krumholz HM. Biomarkers, risk factors and risk: clarifying the controversy about surrogate endpoints and clinical outcomes. Circ Cardiovasc Quality Outcomes (2015) 8:457–9. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.115.002245

CrossRef Full Text

120. Mann GV, Shaffer RD, Anderson RS, Sandstead HH. Cardiovascular disease in the masai. J Atheroscler Res. (1964) 4:289–312. doi: 10.1016/S0368-1319(64)80041-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Malhotra SL. Epidemiology of ischaemic heart disease in India-geographic aspects, dietary and environmental factors in railway population of known constitution. Ind J Ind Med. (1968) 14:219–41.

Google Scholar

122. Artaud-Wild SM, Connor SL, Sexton G, Connor WE. Differences in coronary mortality can be explained by differences in cholesterol and saturated fat intakes in 40 countries but not in France and Finland. A Paradox. Circulation (1993) 88:2771–9. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.88.6.2771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

123. Koga Y, Hashimoto R, Adachi H, Tsuruta M, Tashiro H, Toshima H. Recent Trends in Cardiovascular Disease and Risk Factors in the Seven Countries Study. Tokyo: Springer (1994). p. 63–74.

Google Scholar

124. Serra-Majem L, Ribas L, Tresserras R, Ngo J, Salleras L. How could changes in diet explain changes in coronary heart disease mortality in Spain? The Spanish paradox. Am J Clin Nutr. (1995) 61:1351S−9S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/61.6.1351S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Ravnskov U. The questionable role of saturated and polyunsaturated fatty acids in cardiovascular disease. J Clin Epidemiol. (1998) 51:443–60. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(98)00018-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Taubes G. The soft science of dietary fat. Science (2001) 291:2536–45. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5513.2536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Barclay AW, Brand-Miller J. The Australian paradox: a substantial decline in sugars intake over the same timeframe that overweight and obesity have increased. Nutrients (2011) 3:491–504. doi: 10.3390/nu3040491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Evans A. The French paradox and other ecological fallacies. Int J Epidemiol. (2011) 40:1486–9. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Graudal NA, Hubeck-Graudal T, Jurgens G. Effects of low sodium diet versus high sodium diet on blood pressure, renin, aldosterone, catecholamines, cholesterol, and triglyceride. Cochr Database Syst Rev. (2017) 4:Cd004022. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004022.pub4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Graudal N, Jürgens G. Conflicting evidence on health effects associated with salt reduction calls for a redesign of the salt dietary guidelines. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. (2018) 61:20–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2018.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Mayer J. Decreased activity and energy balance in the hereditary obesity-diabetes syndrome of mice. Science (1953) 117:504–5. doi: 10.1126/science.117.3045.504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Mayer J, Marshall NB, Vitale JJ, Christensen JH, Mashayekhi MB, Stare FJ. Exercise, food intake and body weight in normal rats and genetically obese adult mice. Am J Physiol. (1954) 177:544–8. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1954.177.3.544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Mayer J, Roy P, Mitra KP. Relation between caloric intake, body weight, and physical work: studies in an industrial male population in west bengal. Am J Clin Nutr. (1956). 4:169–175.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

134. Shook RP, Hand GA, Drenowatz C, Hebert JR, Paluch AE, Blundell JE, et al. Low levels of physical activity are associated with dysregulation of energy intake and fat mass gain over 1 year. Am J Clin Nutr. (2015) 102:1332–8. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.115360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Archer E. The childhood obesity epidemic as a result of nongenetic evolution: the maternal resources hypothesis. Mayo Clinic Proc. (2015) 90:77–92. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2014.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Archer E. The mother of all problems. New Scientist. (2015) 225:32–3. doi: 10.1016/S0262-4079(15)60404-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Archer E, Mcdonald SM. The maternal resources hypothesis and childhood obesity. In: MS Patel and JS Nielsen, editors. Fetal and Early Postnatal Programming and its Influence on Adult Health, New York: CRC Press; Taylor and Francis Group, (2017) 17–32.

PubMed Abstract

138. Siontis GC, Ioannidis JP. Risk factors and interventions with statistically significant tiny effects. Int J Epidemiol. (2011) 40:1292–307. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Ioannidis JA. The challenge of reforming nutritional epidemiologic research. JAMA (2018) 320:969–70. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.11025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Trepanowski JF, Ioannidis JP. Perspective: limiting dependence on nonrandomized studies and improving randomized trials in human nutrition research: why and how. Adv Nutr. (2018) 9:367–77. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmy014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Shapiro S. Bias in the evaluation of low-magnitude associations: an empirical perspective. Am J Epidemiol. (2000) 151:939–45. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a010135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Vrolix R, Mensink RP. Variability of the glycemic response to single food products in healthy subjects. Contemporary Clin Trials (2010) 31:5–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2009.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Krogh-Madsen R, Pedersen M, Solomon TP, Knudsen SH, Hansen LS, Karstoft K, et al. Normal physical activity obliterates the deleterious effects of a high-caloric intake. J Appl Physiol. (2014) 116:231–9. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00155.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Zeevi D, Korem T, Zmora N, Israeli D, Rothschild D, Weinberger A, et al. Personalized nutrition by prediction of glycemic responses. Cell (2015) 163:1079–94. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: nutrition, diet, epidemiology, implausible, fallacy, category error

Citation: Archer E, Lavie CJ and Hill JO (2018) The Failure to Measure Dietary Intake Engendered a Fictional Discourse on Diet-Disease Relations. Front. Nutr. 5:105. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2018.00105

Received: 26 May 2018; Accepted: 17 October 2018;
Published: 13 November 2018.

Edited by:

Emily Jane Dhurandhar, Texas Tech University, United States

Reviewed by:

Hollie Raynor, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, United States
Melanie Hingle, University of Arizona, United States

Copyright © 2018 Archer, Lavie and Hill. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Edward Archer, YXJjaGVyLmVkd2FyZGNAZ21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.