Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 09 January 2025
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Neural and Behavioral Mechanisms of Social Learning View all 4 articles

Social context as a source of variability in the psychological sciences

  • 1Department of Neuroscience, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States
  • 2Aix Marseille Université, CNRS, LIS, Marseille, France
  • 3Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States

Introduction

Poor reproducibility in the psychological sciences is often attributed to systemic factors such as publication bias and lack of financial support sources for replication studies (Open Science Collaboration, 2015). While such factors undoubtedly contribute to the problem, controllable issues such as variability in testing methodology, laboratory environment, and subject characteristics may serve as other possible sources of non-replication (Van Bavel et al., 2016; Crabbe et al., 1999; Sorge et al., 2014). One source of variability which is rarely accounted for in either human or animal studies is social context, i.e., the environment formed as a result of the behavioral and biological characteristics of the conspecifics with whom the subject interacts or coexists. Alongside more direct social influences (e.g., conspecific aggression), social context may also be influenced by broader, indirect influences arising from trends in conspecific behavior/beliefs (e.g., cultural norms) or the subject's place in the broader social order (e.g., dominance status). Here, we will cover some of the ways in which preventable variations in social context might influence behavioral measures in human and non-human animal research. We will then discuss strategies to account for social context in future research.

Effects of social context on behavior in non-human animals

(See Figure 1 for section overview).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Social contextual factors and some of their influences on behavior in non-human animals.

First, we consider the overall population density of the home cage. It is well-established that in social species such as mice, rats, and non-human primates, extended periods of social isolation produce a range of marked behavioral abnormalities (Valzelli, 1973; McKinney, 1974; Love and Zelikowsky, 2020). Social isolation has been found to impair various forms of learning (Einon, 1980; Lander et al., 2017), induce abnormal social behaviors (McKinney, 1974; Koike et al., 2009; Mitchell et al., 1966; Keesom et al., 2017; Rivera-Irizarry et al., 2020), increase locomotion (Lander et al., 2017; Ieraci et al., 2016), alter behavioral drug responsivity (Lander et al., 2017; Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996), increase aggressive behavior toward conspecifics (Koike et al., 2009; Mitchell et al., 1966; Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996), and exacerbate behavioral markers of anxiety and depression (Lander et al., 2017; Koike et al., 2009; Ieraci et al., 2016; Weiss et al., 2004; Lukkes et al., 2009). These effects vary depending on species, sex, and the age at social isolation. On the opposite end of the spectrum, overcrowding may also serve as a source of stress and behavioral abnormalities. In mice, high population density (< 8–15 in2 surface area in the cage/mouse) [National Research Council (US) Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 2011] has been found to increase adiposity, produce an anxiety-like phenotype, alter behavioral responsivity to ethanol and ethanol sensitization, and—in some strains—induce social avoidance (Lin et al., 2015; Delaroque et al., 2021; Lee et al., 2018; van Ingelgom et al., 2024; Laber et al., 2008).

An unstable social context can also serve as a source of stress resulting in behavioral changes. For example, mice subjected to chronic social instability (CSI) stress by having cage mates repeatedly replaced with novel conspecifics over the course of multiple weeks display impaired recognition memory (Featherstone et al., 2022), spatial memory (Schmidt et al., 2010), and social memory (Saavedra-Rodríguez and Feig, 2013). Additionally, CSI subjugated mice display social avoidance (Saavedra-Rodríguez and Feig, 2013; dos Santos Guilherme et al., 2022), increased social aggression (Schmidt et al., 2007), and behavioral patterns consistent with anhedonia (Featherstone et al., 2022; Schmidt et al., 2010; Dadomo et al., 2018; Haller et al., 1999; Koert et al., 2021; de Lima and Massoco, 2017) and increased anxiety (Schmidt et al., 2010; Saavedra-Rodríguez and Feig, 2013; dos Santos Guilherme et al., 2022; Koert et al., 2021; Yohn et al., 2019) (excepting in the open field test, see Featherstone et al., 2022; Dadomo et al., 2018; de Lima and Massoco, 2017; Sturman et al., 2021; Díez-Solinska et al., 2022). In rats, CSI produces long-term spatial and social/object recognition memory deficits (Green and McCormick, 2013; McCormick et al., 2010, 2012; Hodges et al., 2017), impairs fear learning (Morrissey et al., 2011) and extinction (McCormick et al., 2013b), reduces social approach (Hodges et al., 2017; Green et al., 2013; Graf et al., 2023; Hodges et al., 2018), impairs sexual behavior (McCormick et al., 2013a), and increases defensive social behavior (Graf et al., 2023). As with social isolation, behavioral effects of CSI vary based on species, sex, and the timing of the procedure/behavioral testing (see Koert et al., 2021 for review). Notably, while the vast majority of experiments involve multiple weeks of CSI, some of the behavioral changes could be observed as early as 2 days into the CSI procedure (Dadomo et al., 2018). Furthermore, mice moved to a new social context after an hour of isolation display higher levels of corticosterone compared to controls (McCormick et al., 2007; Hodges et al., 2014). This suggests that some of the behavioral effects observed following CSI could manifest acutely after even minor shuffling of research subjects.

The individual experiences of group members also influence behavior in their cage mates via emotional contagion, i.e., the psychological phenomenon whereby observing a change in another individual's behavior activates this same change in behavior in the viewer (Panksepp and Lahvis, 2011). In rodents, it is well established that directly observing a conspecific in distress causes acute physiological and behavioral changes, such as increases in fear-related/defensive behavior (Jeon et al., 2010; Bruchey et al., 2010; Andraka et al., 2021; Keysers and Gazzola, 2021), enhanced startle responding, and hyperalgesia (Langford, 2006; Li et al., 2014). Moreover, observing a conspecific in distress can induce extended effects such as long-term pain sensitization (Raber and Devor, 2002) and fear responses to stimuli that accompanied conspecific distress (Jeon et al., 2010; Bruchey et al., 2010; Kavaliers et al., 2001). Observing pain or distress in familiar or related conspecifics often produces more potent behavioral effects and is sometimes necessary for long-term effects to be observed in both mice and rats (Jeon et al., 2010; Langford, 2006; Li et al., 2014; Agee et al., 2019; Jones et al., 2014; Kavaliers et al., 2005) (though see Hernandez-Lallement et al., 2022). This suggests a keen sensitivity toward the emotional state of cage mates. In this way, the treatment of a given subject may be sufficient to alter their cage mates' behavior either acutely (e.g., if animals within a single cage are run sequentially and allowed to interact between testing) or over the long term (e.g., if a cage mate is subjected to surgical or testing procedures that cause enduring stress/pain).

Finally, we consider the influence of social rank on behavior. Most socially housed laboratory species are known to maintain dominance hierarchies to some degree (Williamson et al., 2016, 2019; Varholick et al., 2019; Schuhr, 1987; Blanchard et al., 1984; Ziporyn and McClintock, 1991; Sterck and Steenbeek, 1997; Blanchard et al., 1988; Jones and Monfils, 2016; Seese et al., 2024; Monfils and Agee, 2019), but to simplify our discussion we will focus on mice. The results of the dominance literature regarding the effect of social rank on behavior are quite inconsistent. A recent meta-analysis (Varholick et al., 2021) found no clear effect of dominance rank across studies in open field exploration, elevated plus maze open arm time, or immobility during the forced swim test. Indeed, results were often directly contradictory. One explanation for this lack of consensus is variability in the type of dominance hierarchy formed within a group. In triads of mice, variation is observed both in the stability of a hierarchy (i.e., the degree to which rank is maintained) and the linearity of the hierarchical structure (Varholick et al., 2019). Reports of overall hierarchical stability vary between studies, with some researchers finding high stability (Williamson et al., 2016, 2019) and others reporting frequent reshuffling of rank order (Varholick et al., 2019). The degree of alpha despotism, i.e., the ability to suppress aggressive behavior in lower ranked counterparts, also varies (Williamson et al., 2016). This variance is important to consider, as recent research has found that the often-inconsistent findings regarding endocrine function and behavior in dominant vs. subordinate animals may be explained by interactions between dominance rank and hierarchical characteristics. For example, while past research has found contradictory results on the relative testosterone levels in dominant and subordinate mice (Machida et al., 1981; Ely, 1981; Selmanoff et al., 1977; Barnard et al., 1996; Hilakivi et al., 1989), recent evidence suggests that high despotism may serve as the determining factor for this difference (Williamson et al., 2017). Further research considering hierarchical characteristics in conjunction with social rank will hopefully resolve some of these contradictions.

Effects of social context on participant behavior in human studies

The social context of human subjects will virtually always be more complex than that of lab animals confined to a fixed community of only a few conspecifics. As such, human researchers can realistically only hope to assess participants based on broad differences in social context in which individuals can be easily categorized or scored. We thus restrict this discussion to a few facets of an individual's social context that can reasonably be ascertained from basic participant surveys. Additionally, we discuss how more immediate aspects of the social context during testing (e.g., the presence of an experimenter or other subjects) might affect responding.

As in many lab species, social isolation in human subjects has been shown to be associated with a variety of physiological and behavioral effects. For example, individuals reporting high subjective social isolation display higher levels of depression (Fiordelli et al., 2020; Steptoe et al., 2013; Layden et al., 2017), increased mortality (Steptoe et al., 2013; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015), and generally interpret social interactions more negatively (Duck et al., 1994; Anderson and Martin, 1995; Hawkley et al., 2003). Notably, in humans, perceived social isolation (i.e., loneliness) is measurable and distinct from objective social isolation (i.e., actual social network size), and the two measures correlate only weakly to moderately (Fiordelli et al., 2020; Steptoe et al., 2013; Hawkley et al., 2008) (see also Layden et al., 2017). Additionally, the quality of social ties—not the number of ties—appears to exert a greater protective influence on loneliness levels (Lee and Ko, 2018). As such, simply gathering demographic data may not be an accurate gauge of social context.

The immediate social environment during testing also has the potential to alter participant responding. When studying social behavior, lab studies have traditionally used non-participatory settings, where people observe stimuli of others without being part of the interaction. While this research is valuable in documenting human social biases in general, it fails to account for people's true social behavior outside the lab (Risko et al., 2016; Pfeiffer et al., 2012). For example, in the case of social attention, non-participatory settings tend to overemphasize face gazing as an information-gathering tool; however, whether we know that our gaze is available to others has significant consequences on how much we look at them (Laidlaw et al., 2011; Gobel et al., 2015). More specifically, non-participatory lab experiments overlook the effect of gaze as a signaling tool in natural social interactions (Risko et al., 2016). Crucially, this effect is modulated by cultural norms, relationships between interactants (e.g., familiar person vs. stranger), and the nature of their interaction (e.g., cooperative vs. collaborative), emphasizing the necessity to factor socio-contextual features in studies (Dalmaso et al., 2020).

This observation has broad implications beyond the study of social behavior. Human research generally involves interactions with a human experimenter in some way or another. This is particularly the case in child development research, since young children cannot, for example, read instructions off a computer screen. This makes the experimental outcome partly dependent on the experimenter-child social dynamics. Take, for example, the so-called Marshmallow task (Mischel et al., 1972) introduced to test children's delayed gratification management. Kidd et al. (2013) showed that children's perception of the experimenter's trustworthiness influences their strategy. In fact, the experimenter's identity alone (e.g., perceived as in-group vs. outgroup) had a significant impact on the wait time in the task (Pierre et al., 2023; Strickland, 1972). Furthermore, children's performance depends on their cultural background. For instance, societies that emphasize hierarchy vs. autonomy lead the child to adopt different self-regulatory strategies (Lamm et al., 2018). Similarly, children tend to adopt strategies that are consistent with their cultural norms related to waiting and food (Yanaoka et al., 2022). Failing to consider the social context (or lack thereof) can impact both the external validity of behavioral tasks as well as their internal validity, potentially contributing to the replicability crisis in human research.

Discussion

In the preceding sections we highlighted some of the ways in which social context influences behavioral and physiological measures. While we do not have space to cover all components of social context here, what we have reviewed hopefully makes a compelling case for the idea that even experiments not focused on social behavior should be designed with certain aspects of social context in mind. Controlling all aspects of social context is not feasible, but some basic measures can be taken to limit social confounds. In non-human animal studies, social context can be standardized across experimental groups and between studies (if replication is the goal) by careful housing practices aimed at minimizing social stressors. In practice, however, this is rarely straightforward. For example, emotional contagion can theoretically be minimized by keeping subjects in single housing, but this exposes subjects to the behavioral and physiological changes that accompany social isolation stress. In such cases, alternative solutions—e.g., ensuring a balanced distribution of members of each experimental group between cages—should also be considered. Critically, details on social housing conditions and experimental group distribution between cages should be explicitly stated in the methods section and recorded on publicly available datasheets. Having this information readily available will help in interpreting inconsistent results and could assist researchers conducting meta-analyses.

Naturally, controlling for social context in human research is a more complicated prospect. While experimenters have no control over the broader aspects of their human participant's social context, variability in social context can be at least considered in analyses. Basic details of participants' social relationships might be gleamed via pre-screening or post-testing questionnaires and assessed as possible response mediators, but more detailed questions about the quality of these relationships may be necessary to properly categorize participants. Additionally, careful consideration must be given to the nature of interactions between participants and other individuals present during testing. As with animal research, thorough documentation of these interactions is essential for later interpretation of inconsistent results. While accounting for social context in research presents a formidable challenge, it is essential to consider for improving reproducibility and the validity of behavioral studies. By prioritizing the standardization and documentation of social variables, researchers can mitigate potential confounds and contribute to a more reliable body of scientific knowledge.

Author contributions

LA: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AF: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. M-HM: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Agee, L. A., Jones, C. E., and Monfils, M. H. (2019). Differing effects of familiarity/kinship in the social transmission of fear associations and food preferences in rats. Anim. Cogn. 22, 1013–1026. doi: 10.1007/s10071-019-01292-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C. M., and Martin, M. M. (1995). The effects of communication motives, interaction involvement, and loneliness on satisfaction: a model of small groups. Small Group Res. 26, 118–137. doi: 10.1177/1046496495261007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Andraka, K., Kondrakiewicz, K., Rojek-Sito, K., Ziegart-Sadowska, K., Meyza, K., Nikolaev, T., et al. (2021). Distinct circuits in rat central amygdala for defensive behaviors evoked by socially signaled imminent versus remote danger. Curr. Biol. 31, 2347–2358.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.03.047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnard, C. J., Behnke, J. M., and Sewell, J. (1996). Social status and resistance to disease in house Mice (Mus musculus): status-related modulation of hormonal responses in relation to immunity costs in different social and physical environments. Ethology 102, 63–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01104.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, D. C., Fukunaga-Stinson, C., Takahashi, L. K., Flannelly, K. J., and Blanchard, R. J. (1984). Dominance and aggression in social groups of male and female rats. Behav. Processes 9, 31–48. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(84)90006-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, R. J., Flannelly, K. J., and Blanchard, D. C. (1988). Life-span studies of dominance and aggression in established colonies of laboratory rats. Physiol. Behav. 43, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90089-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruchey, A. K., Jones, C. E., and Monfils, M. H. (2010). Fear conditioning by-proxy: social transmission of fear during memory retrieval. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 80–84. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crabbe, J. C., Wahlsten, D., and Dudek, B. C. (1999). Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environment. Science 284, 1670–1672. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5420.1670

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dadomo, H., Gioiosa, L., Cigalotti, J., Ceresini, G., Parmigiani, S., Palanza, P., et al. (2018). What is stressful for females? Differential effects of unpredictable environmental or social stress in CD1 female mice. Horm. Behav. 98, 22–32. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.11.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L., and Galfano, G. (2020). Social modulators of gaze-mediated orienting of attention: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 27, 833–855. doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01730-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Lima, A. P. N., and Massoco, C. O. (2017). Passive adaptation to stress in adulthood after short-term social instability stress during adolescence in mice. Stress 20, 329–332. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2017.1313223

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delaroque, C., Chervy, M., Gewirtz, A. T., and Chassaing, B. (2021). Social overcrowding impacts gut microbiota, promoting stress, inflammation, and dysglycemia. Gut Microbes. 13:2000275. doi: 10.1080/19490976.2021.2000275

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Díez-Solinska, A., Lebeña, A., Garmendia, L., Labaka, A., Azkona, G., Perez-Tejada, J., et al. (2022). Chronic social instability stress down-regulates IL-10 and up-regulates CX3CR1 in tumor-bearing and non-tumor-bearing female mice. Behav. Brain Res. 435:114063. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

dos Santos Guilherme, M., Tsoutsouli, T., Chongtham, M. C., Winter, J., Gerber, S., Müller, M. B., et al. (2022). Selective targeting of chronic social stress-induced activated neurons identifies neurogenesis-related genes to be associated with resilience in female mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 139:105700. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105700

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duck, S., Pond, K., and Leatham, G. (1994). Loneliness and the evaluation of relational events. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 11, 253–276. doi: 10.1177/0265407594112006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Einon, D. (1980). Spatial memory and response strategies in rats: age, sex and rearing differences in performance. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 473–489. doi: 10.1080/14640748008401840

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ely, D. L. (1981). Hypertension, social rank, and aortic arteriosclerosis in CBA/J mice. Physiol. Behav. 26, 655–661. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90140-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Featherstone, R. E., Gifford, R. L., Crown, L. M., Amirfathi, F., Alaniz, J. P., Yi, J., et al. (2022). Early life social instability stress causes lasting cognitive decrement and elevated hippocampal stress-related gene expression. Exp. Neurol. 354:114099. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2022.114099

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiordelli, M., Sak, G., Guggiari, B., Schulz, P. J., and Petrocchi, S. (2020). Differentiating objective and subjective dimensions of social isolation and apprasing their relations with physical and mental health in italian older adults. BMC Geriatr. 20:472. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01864-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobel, M. S., Kim, H. S., and Richardson, D. C. (2015). The dual function of social gaze. Cognition 136, 359–364. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Graf, A., Murray, S. H., Eltahir, A., Patel, S., Hansson, A. C., Spanagel, R., et al. (2023). Acute and long-term sex-dependent effects of social instability stress on anxiety-like and social behaviours in Wistar rats. Behav. Brain Res. 438:114180. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. R., Barnes, B., and McCormick, C. M. (2013). Social instability stress in adolescence increases anxiety and reduces social interactions in adulthood in male long–evans rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 55, 849–859. doi: 10.1002/dev.21077

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. R., and McCormick, C. M. (2013). Effects of social instability stress in adolescence on long-term, not short-term, spatial memory performance. Behav. Brain Res. 256, 165–171. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.08.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haller, J., Fuchs, E., Halász, J., and Makara, G. B. (1999). Defeat is a major stressor in males while social instability is stressful mainly in females: towards the development of a social stress model in female rats. Brain Res. Bull. 50, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(99)00087-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 105–120. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Masi, C. M., Thisted, R. A., Cacioppo, J. T., et al. (2008). From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 63, S375–S384. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.6.S375

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernandez-Lallement, J., Gómez-Sotres, P., and Carrillo, M. (2022). Towards a unified theory of emotional contagion in rodents—a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 132, 1229–1248. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilakivi, L. A., Lister, R. G., Duncan, M. J., Ota, M., Eskay, R. L., Mefford, I., et al. (1989). Behavioral, hormonal and neurochemical characteristics of aggressive α-mice. Brain Res. 502, 158–166. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90471-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, T. E., Baumbach, J. L., Marcolin, M. L., Bredewold, R., Veenema, A. H., McCormick, C. M., et al. (2017). Social instability stress in adolescent male rats reduces social interaction and social recognition performance and increases oxytocin receptor binding. Neuroscience 359, 172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, T. E., Baumbach, J. L., and McCormick, C. M. (2018). Predictors of social instability stress effects on social interaction and anxiety in adolescent male rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 60, 651–663. doi: 10.1002/dev.21626

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, T. E., Green, M. R., Simone, J. J., and McCormick, C. M. (2014). Effects of social context on endocrine function and Zif268 expression in response to an acute stressor in adolescent and adult rats. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 35, 25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., and Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ieraci, A., Mallei, A., and Popoli, M. (2016). Social isolation stress induces anxious-depressive-like behavior and alterations of neuroplasticity-related genes in adult male mice. Neural. Plast. 2016:6212983. doi: 10.1155/2016/6212983

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeon, D., Kim, S., Chetana, M., Jo, D., Ruley, H. E., Lin, S. Y., et al. (2010). Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 482–488. doi: 10.1038/nn.2504

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, C. E., and Monfils, M. H. (2016). Dominance status predicts social fear transmission in laboratory rats. Anim. Cogn. 19, 1051–1069. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-1013-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, C. E., Riha, P. D., Gore, A. C., and Monfils, M. H. (2014). Social transmission of Pavlovian fear: fear-conditioning by-proxy in related female rats. Anim. Cogn. 17, 827–834. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0711-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavaliers, M., Choleris, E., and Colwell, D. D. (2001). Learning from others to cope with biting flies: social learning of fear-induced conditioned analgesia and active avoidance. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 661–674. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.3.661

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavaliers, M., Colwell, D. D., and Choleris, E. (2005). Kinship, familiarity and social status modulate social learning about “micropredators” (biting flies) in deer mice. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 58, 60–71. doi: 10.1007/s00265-004-0896-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keesom, S. M., Finton, C. J., Sell, G. L., and Hurley, L. M. (2017). Early-life social isolation influences mouse ultrasonic vocalizations during male-male social encounters. PLoS ONE 12:e0169705. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169705

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keysers, C., and Gazzola, V. (2021). Emotional contagion: improving survival by preparing for socially sensed threats. Curr. Biol. 31, R728–R730. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.03.100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidd, C., Palmeri, H., and Aslin, R. N. (2013). Rational snacking: young children's decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition 126, 109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koert, A., Ploeger, A., Bockting, C. L. H., Schmidt, M. V., Lucassen, P. J., Schrantee, A., et al. (2021). The social instability stress paradigm in rat and mouse: a systematic review of protocols, limitations, and recommendations. Neurobiol. Stress. 15:100410. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100410

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koike, H., Ibi, D., Mizoguchi, H., Nagai, T., Nitta, A., Takuma, K., et al. (2009). Behavioral abnormality and pharmacologic response in social isolation-reared mice. Behav. Brain Res. 202, 114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laber, K., Veatch, L. M., Lopez, M. F., Mulligan, J. K., and Lathers, D. M. (2008). Effects of housing density on weight gain, immune function, behavior, and plasma corticosterone concentrations in BALB/c and C57BL/6 Mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 47, 16–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Laidlaw, K. E. W., Foulsham, T., Kuhn, G., and Kingstone, A. (2011). Potential social interactions are important to social attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 5548–5553. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1017022108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, B., Keller, H., Teiser, J., Gudi, H., Yovsi, R. D., Freitag, C., et al. (2018). Waiting for the second treat: developing culture-specific modes of self-regulation. Child Dev. 89, e261–e277. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lander, S. S., Linder-Shacham, D., and Gaisler-Salomon, I. (2017). Differential effects of social isolation in adolescent and adult mice on behavior and cortical gene expression. Behav. Brain Res. 316, 245–254. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langford, D. J. (2006). Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science 312, 1967–1970. doi: 10.1126/science.1128322

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Layden, E. A., Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Cappa, S. F., Dodich, A., Falini, A., et al. (2017). Perceived social isolation is associated with altered functional connectivity in neural networks associated with tonic alertness and executive control. Neuroimage 145, 58–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y., and Ko, Y. (2018). gun. Feeling lonely when not socially isolated: social isolation moderates the association between loneliness and daily social interaction. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 35, 1340–1355. doi: 10.1177/0265407517712902

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. A., Obora, T., Bondonny, L., Toniolo, A., Mivielle, J., Yamaguchi, Y., et al. (2018). The effects of housing density on social interactions and their correlations with serotonin in rodents and primates. Sci. Rep. 8:3497. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-21353-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Lu, Y. F., Li, C. L., Wang, Y., Sun, W., He, T., et al. (2014). Social interaction with a cagemate in pain facilitates subsequent spinal nociception via activation of the medial prefrontal cortex in rats. PAIN 155, 1253–1261. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.03.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, E. J. D., Sun, M., Choi, E. Y., Magee, D., Stets, C. W., During, M. J., et al. (2015). Social overcrowding as a chronic stress model that increases adiposity in mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51, 318–330. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Love, J., and Zelikowsky, M. (2020). Stress varies along the social density continuum. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 14:582985. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.582985

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukkes, J. L., Watt, M. J., Lowry, C. A., and Forster, G. L. (2009). Consequences of post-weaning social isolation on anxiety behavior and related neural circuits in rodents. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 3:18. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.018.2009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Machida, T., Yonezawa, Y., and Noumura, T. (1981). Age-associated changes in plasma testosterone levels in male mice and their relation to social dominance or subordinance. Horm. Behav. 15, 238–245. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(81)90013-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., Green, M. R., Cameron, N. M., Nixon, F., Levy, M. J., Clark, R. A., et al. (2013a). Deficits in male sexual behavior in adulthood after social instability stress in adolescence in rats. Horm. Behav. 63, 5–12. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., Merrick, A., Secen, J., and Helmreich, D. L. (2007). Social instability in adolescence alters the central and peripheral hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to a repeated homotypic stressor in male and female rats. J. Neuroendocrinol. 19, 116–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2006.01515.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., Mongillo, D. L., and Simone, J. J. (2013b). Age and adolescent social stress effects on fear extinction in female rats. Stress 16, 678–688. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2013.840283

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., Nixon, F., Thomas, C., Lowie, B., and Dyck, J. (2010). Hippocampal cell proliferation and spatial memory performance after social instability stress in adolescence in female rats. Behav. Brain Res. 208, 23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C. M., Thomas, C. M., Sheridan, C. S., Nixon, F., Flynn, J. A., Mathews, I. Z., et al. (2012). Social instability stress in adolescent male rats alters hippocampal neurogenesis and produces deficits in spatial location memory in adulthood. Hippocampus 22, 1300–1312. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20966

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McKinney, W. T. Jr. (1974). Primate social isolation: psychiatric implications. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 31, 422–426. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760150122018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., and Raskoff Zeiss, A. (1972). Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 21, 204–218. doi: 10.1037/h0032198

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, G. D., Raymond, E. J., Ruppenthal, G. C., and Harlow, H. F. (1966). Long-term effects of total social isolation upon behavior of rhesus monkeys. Psychol. Rep. 18, 567–80. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1966.18.2.567

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Monfils, M. H., and Agee, L. A. (2019). Insights from social transmission of information in rodents. Genes Brain Behav. 18:e12534. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrissey, M. D., Mathews, I. Z., and McCormick, C. M. (2011). Enduring deficits in contextual and auditory fear conditioning after adolescent, not adult, social instability stress in male rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 95, 46–56. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.10.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

National Research Council (US) Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (2011). Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 8th Edn. Washington, DC: National Academies Press (US).

Google Scholar

Open Science Collaboration (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 349:aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. B., and Lahvis, G. P. (2011). Rodent empathy and affective neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1864–1875. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeiffer, U., Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Jording, M., Bente, G., Vogeley, K., et al. (2012). Eyes on the mind: investigating the influence of gaze dynamics on the perception of others in real-time social interaction. Front. Psychol. 3:537. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00537

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierre, T. S., White, K. S., and Johnson, E. K. (2023). Who is running our experiments? The influence of experimenter identity in the marshmallow task. Cogn. Dev. 65:101271. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101271

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raber, P., and Devor, M. (2002). Social variables affect phenotype in the neuroma model of neuropathic pain. Pain 97, 139–150. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00013-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Risko, E. F., Richardson, D. C., and Kingstone, A. (2016). Breaking the fourth wall of cognitive science: real-world social attention and the dual function of gaze. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 25, 70–74. doi: 10.1177/0963721415617806

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera-Irizarry, J. K., Skelly, M. J., and Pleil, K. E. (2020). Social isolation stress in adolescence, but not adulthood, produces hypersocial behavior in adult male and female C57BL/6J mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:129. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saavedra-Rodríguez, L., and Feig, L. A. (2013). Chronic social instability induces anxiety and defective social interactions across generations. Biol. Psychiatry. 73, 44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M. V., Scharf, S. H., Sterlemann, V., Ganea, K., Liebl, C., Holsboer, F., et al. (2010). High susceptibility to chronic social stress is associated with a depression-like phenotype. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 635–643. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M. V., Sterlemann, V., Ganea, K., Liebl, C., Alam, S., Harbich, D., et al. (2007). Persistent neuroendocrine and behavioral effects of a novel, etiologically relevant mouse paradigm for chronic social stress during adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 417–429. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.02.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuhr, B. (1987). Social structure and plasma corticosterone level in female albino mice. Physiol. Behav. 40, 689–693. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90269-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seese, S., Tinsley, C. E., Wulffraat, G., Hixon, J. G., and Monfils, M. H. (2024). Conspecific interactions predict social transmission of fear in female rats. Sci. Rep. 14:7804. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-58258-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Selmanoff, M. K., Goldman, B. D., and Ginsburg, B. E. (1977). Serum testosterone, agonistic behavior, and dominance in inbred strains of mice. Horm. Behav. 8, 107–119. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(77)90026-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorge, R. E., Martin, L. J., Isbester, K. A., Sotocinal, S. G., Rosen, S., Tuttle, A. H., et al. (2014). Olfactory exposure to males, including men, causes stress and related analgesia in rodents. Nat. Methods. 11, 629–632. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2935

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Shankar, A., Demakakos, P., and Wardle, J. (2013). Social isolation, loneliness, and all-cause mortality in older men and women. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 110, 5797–5801. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219686110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterck, E. H. M., and Steenbeek, R. (1997). Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric thomas langur and long-tailed Macaque. Behaviour 134, 749–774. doi: 10.1163/156853997X00052

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strickland, B. R. (1972). Delay of gratification as a function of race of the experimenter. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 22, 108–12. doi: 10.1037/h0032385

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sturman, O., von Ziegler, L., Privitera, M., Waag, R., Duss, S., Vermeiren, Y., et al. (2021). Chronic adolescent stress increases exploratory behavior but does not appear to change the acute stress response in adult male C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiol. Stress 15:100388. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100388

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valzelli, L. (1973). The “isolation syndrome” in mice. Psychopharmacologia 31, 305–320. doi: 10.1007/BF00421275

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Bavel, J. J., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Brady, W. J., and Reinero, D. A. (2016). Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 113, 6454–6459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521897113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ingelgom, T., Didone, V., Godefroid, L., and Quertemont, É. (2024). Effects of social housing conditions on ethanol-induced behavioral sensitization in Swiss mice. Psychopharmacology 241, 987–1000. doi: 10.1007/s00213-024-06527-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Varholick, J. A., Bailoo, J. D., Jenkins, A., Voelkl, B., and Würbel, H. A. (2021). Systematic review and meta-analysis of the relationship between social dominance status and common behavioral phenotypes in male laboratory mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:624036. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.624036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Varholick, J. A., Pontiggia, A., Murphy, E., Daniele, V., Palme, R., Voelkl, B., et al. (2019). Social dominance hierarchy type and rank contribute to phenotypic variation within cages of laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 9:13650. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49612-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, I. C., Pryce, C. R., Jongen-Rêlo, A. L., Nanz-Bahr, N. I., and Feldon, J. (2004). Effect of social isolation on stress-related behavioural and neuroendocrine state in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 152, 279–295. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.10.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, C. M., Lee, W., and Curley, J. P. (2016). Temporal dynamics of social hierarchy formation and maintenance in male mice. Anim. Behav. 115, 259–272. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, C. M., Lee, W., DeCasien, A. R., Lanham, A., Romeo, R. D., Curley, J. P., et al. (2019). Social hierarchy position in female mice is associated with plasma corticosterone levels and hypothalamic gene expression. Sci. Rep. 9:7324. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-43747-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, C. M., Lee, W., Romeo, R. D., and Curley, J. P. (2017). Social context-dependent relationships between mouse dominance rank and plasma hormone levels. Physiol. Behav. 171, 110–119. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.12.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wongwitdecha, N., and Marsden, C. A. (1996). Social isolation increases aggressive behaviour and alters the effects of diazepam in the rat social interaction test. Behav. Brain Res. 75, 27–32. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00181-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yanaoka, K., Michaelson, L. E., Guild, R. M., Dostart, G., Yonehiro, J., Saito, S., et al. (2022). Cultures crossing: the power of habit in delaying gratification. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1172–1181. doi: 10.1177/09567976221074650

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yohn, C. N., Ashamalla, S. A., Bokka, L., Gergues, M. M., Garino, A., Samuels, B. A., et al. (2019). Social instability is an effective chronic stress paradigm for both male and female mice. Neuropharmacology 160:107780. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.107780

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziporyn, T., and McClintock, M. K. (1991). Passing as an indicator of social dominance among female wild and domestic Norway rats. Behaviour 118, 26–41. doi: 10.1163/156853991X00184

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social context, emotional contagion, reproducibility of results, dominance—rank orders, social instability

Citation: Agee LA, Fourtassi A and Monfils M-H (2025) Social context as a source of variability in the psychological sciences. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 18:1507010. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1507010

Received: 15 October 2024; Accepted: 24 December 2024;
Published: 09 January 2025.

Edited by:

Katharina J. Rohlfing, University of Paderborn, Germany

Reviewed by:

Manoel Jorge Nobre, Centro Universitário Municipal de Franca (UNIFACEF), Brazil

Copyright © 2025 Agee, Fourtassi and Monfils. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura A. Agee, bGFhZ2VlQHV0ZXhhcy5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.