
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Educ., 07 April 2025
Sec. Digital Education
Volume 10 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2025.1472385
This study aims to explore how e-rebuttal texts on Newton’s laws can reconstruct students’ mental models by altering existing misconceptions. A mixed methods approach (quantitative and qualitative) was used in the research. Participants were 31 students (15 boys and 16 girls, aged 15–16 years) in the 10th grade of one public high school in Sukabumi, West Java, Indonesia. The instrument used Multi-representation on Tier Instrument of Newton’s laws (MOTION), consists of 36 subject matter questions about Newton’s First Law, Newton’s Second Law, Newton’s Third Law, and the type of forces. The data were analyzed using the categories of conception, mental model, and correction of a mental model. The result shows positive changes in each mental model from the pre-test to the post-test. The rate of correction in students’ mental models from pre-test to post-test occurred primarily in the Acceptable Correction (ACo) category. It was concluded that e-rebuttal texts can be used to reconstruct students’ mental models in conceptual change to become Scientific (SC) models. Other researchers may use learning models and strategies to involve students in groups to facilitate the discussion process.
Many studies show that students in physics classes already have basic concepts that they acquire from everyday experience (Kulgemeyer and Wittwer, 2022; Ladachart et al., 2022; Ozkan and Selcuk, 2016). Nevertheless, often the initial conception does not match the scientific conception. This situation is known by several terms such as alternative conception, misconception, conceptual difficulty, initial conception, and initial framework (Gurel et al., 2015; Prinz et al., 2022; Yürük and Eroğlu, 2016).
Misconception can be caused by the student’s internal factors (mental models) and the student’s external factors (learning strategies, textbooks, language, and media) (Braasch et al., 2013; Hunsu et al., 2023; Ipek and Calik, 2008; Kaltakçi and Didiş, 2007; Kaya et al., 2022; Majid and Suyono, 2018; Nurdini et al., 2019). Regarding internal factors, mental models are one of the causes of students’ misconceptions. This is instigated by errors in modeling phenomena, especially abstract phenomena that are tough to grip with appropriate mental models, and students are unpredictable in their descriptions of physical phenomena. Mental models are ideas of student thinking used to describe, understand, and explain complex phenomena as a result of cognitive processes (Kurnaz and Eksi, 2015; Majid and Suyono, 2018; Wiji and Mulyani, 2018; Yildirir and Demirkol, 2018).
Mental models can be categorized as scientific, synthetic, and initial according to the student’s level of understanding (Kurnaz and Eksi, 2015; Sung et al., 2021). A scientific mental model is a mental model that conforms to scientific concepts, a synthetic mental model is a mental model that partially conforms to scientific concepts, and an initial mental model is a mental model that does not conform to scientific concepts. Scientific mental models occur when students’ conceptions are in concept understanding, partial understanding, or a combination of conceptual understanding and partial understanding for several questions that use the same concept. Synthetic mental models occur when students’ conceptions are in a combination of understanding the concept of understanding with misconceptions, not understanding, or non-coding for several questions that use the same concept. Initial mental models occur when students’ conceptions are misconceptions, do not understand, non-coding, or a combination of all three for several questions that use the same concept. Therefore, mental model identification can be done after identifying the student’s conception.
Mental models should be identified early on. Knowing a student’s mental model can help teachers find misconceptions, difficult knowledge for students, and initial concepts of students (Wiji and Mulyani, 2018). Since the learning process involves structuring mental models, this helps determine appropriate learning strategies to avoid misconceptions (Kurnaz and Eksi, 2015; Varela et al., 2020). Students who are able to develop more complete mental models have better learning outcomes and achievements. The structuring of mental models can occur through a process of conceptual change (Fratiwi et al., 2020; Podschuweit and Bernholt, 2018; Samsudin et al., 2015). Posner et al. (1982) suggested that changing students’ conceptions requires certain conditions, i.e., (1) dissatisfaction, namely students need to be faced with problems that make them aware that there are inconsistencies with their conceptions when faced with the process of explaining physical phenomena, (2) intelligibility, so that students can accommodate a new concept, the new concept must be clear and easy for students to understand, and not contain complexities, especially when the new concept is used to explain physical phenomena or solve physics problems, (3) plausibility, new concepts need to be seen as logical and acceptable by common sense (reasonable) by students. That logic must be shown by the suitability of the new conception with events that can be felt and observed or experienced in everyday life, and (4) fruitfulness, so that the new concept received by students can be accommodated, it must be shown that the new concept has better performance compared to the old conception when used to explain physical phenomena and solve physics problems. If this can happen, students will feel the usefulness of the new concept so that students will not hesitate to accommodate it into a new concept to replace the old concept.
Four conditions from Posner et al. (1982) can arise through the learning process. This is because one of the causes of alternative concepts lies in the student’s external factors (environment). Zhou et al. (2016) showed that science (including physics) learning should develop students’ concepts and help students overcome learning difficulties. However, in many cases, the learning process fails to improve the inadequate student’s initial conceptions and may even exacerbate students’ misconceptions. A number of studies have examined the effects of conceptual change approaches on alternative concepts and mental models in physical learning (Çil and Çepni, 2016; Gadgil et al., 2012; Mason and Zaccoletti, 2021; Suhandi et al., 2017; Wenning, 2008; Yürük and Eroğlu, 2016). The results show that the conceptual change approach is effective in changing conceptions and mental models.
According to Sarwar et al. (2024), one approach to change the conception is to use text mode. One of them is rebuttal texts (refutational texts). A rebuttal texts is a text that refutes misconceptions by presenting a general misconception, an explicit disclaimer that the description is a misconception, and a scientific explanation. The effectiveness of rebuttal text is related to making students aware of their concept’s limitations, the differences between their concepts and scientific concepts, and understanding the truth of scientific concepts (Mason et al., 2019; Prinz et al., 2022). Disclaimers also draw students’ attention to new information they need to learn, trigger cognitive conflict, and integrate correct information. In addition, Braasch et al. (2013) argue that rebuttal texts are effective guides for contextualizing prior knowledge and its use.
Rebuttal texts differ from traditional texts (such as textbooks) that only explain concepts, they are often difficult for students to understand and cannot effectively change students’ concepts. Several studies have shown that the use of rebuttal text is effective in changing students’ conceptions (Caleon and Subramaniam, 2013; Will et al., 2019). Mason et al. (2019) stated a rebuttal text presents the possibility that students hold misconceptions, the refutation that the misconceptions are not scientific concepts, and an explanation consistent with scientific concepts so can help students change their conceptions. Additionally, using rebuttal text can make students’ mental models more scientific (Schroeder and Kucera, 2022; Tippett, 2010).
Based on the research of Bunawan et al. (2015), the use of texts does not hold students’ attention, so they are less active in the learning process. Students expect learning media that are interesting, easy to understand, easy to use, and encourage students to actively participate in the learning process (Kahnbach et al., 2024; Li et al., 2024; Mondragon-Estrada et al., 2023). Therefore, teachers can use digital media in the learning process, including the texts they use. Digital media that arouse student interest can be effectively used to correct student misconceptions (Karaoglan Yilmaz et al., 2018; Nurdini et al., 2020; Suhandi et al., 2017). The use of digital media also makes it easier for students to understand abstract concepts because they can be visualized (Lechner et al., 2024; Li et al., 2023; Sakurai and Goos, 2023). Integrating the use of rebuttal text into digital media and inserting videos, animations or simulations can make the used text more interactive. Simulations also enable students to conduct comprehensive investigations (control variables) to more effectively change misconceptions and change students’ mental models. The integration of interactive media-based rebuttal text is called e-rebuttal texts. Figure 1 shows the difference between the structure of e-rebuttal texts and common rebuttal texts.
Each section of the e-rebuttal texts includes instructions that prompt students to engage in specific activities. These activities are aligned with the structure of the e-rebuttal texts. Additionally, the e-rebuttal texts integrate various multimedia elements, such as graphics, text, videos, simulations, animations, and images. These elements simulate experiments and physical phenomena, making the learning content more engaging. This approach encourages active student participation and enhances understanding of physical concepts (Georgiou et al., 2021; Jian-hua and Hong, 2012). Research by Jiang et al. (2018) demonstrated that students are more likely to accept conflicting evidence from integrated studies (e.g., control variables) compared to observational studies when revising their initial concepts. Simulations integrated into rebuttal texts can facilitate this process. Moreover, interactive media have been found to save time and resources while enabling the visualization of abstract physical concepts by presenting dynamic information not available in traditional text (Ozkan and Selcuk, 2015). Studies by Aslan and Demircioğlu (2014) further highlight that computer-based media support knowledge construction, assist in learning challenging and abstract concepts, and enhance students’ conceptual understanding, particularly in physics.
When teaching physics, we often see misconceptions about some concepts such as force and motion (Bayraktar, 2009; Liu and Fang, 2016; Poutot and Blandin, 2015; Saglam-Arslan and Devecioglu, 2010), optical geometry (Kaltakci-Gurel et al., 2017), electricity and magnetism (Leppavirta, 2012; Peşman and Eryilmaz, 2010), and fluids (Nurdini et al., 2020; Purwanto et al., 2018; Samsudin et al., 2018). However, the concept of force is a fundamental concept in the study of physics, especially the concept of force in Newton’s laws. Saglam-Arslan and Devecioglu (2010) showed that many students struggled to understand basic concepts such as force, acceleration, displacement, and gravitational acceleration. Nevertheless, Newton’s laws are important because they can be easily applied by students in their daily lives and provide a basis for further study of physics (Ferreira et al., 2017). Previous research has revealed several alternative concepts that appear in Newton’s laws material. That is, (1) if a force is acting on an object, the object will move in the direction of the acting force, (2) when the force acting on the object is constant, objects move at a constant speed, (3) objects in motion eventually come to a stop when no force acts, (4) the acceleration of a falling object is affected by the mass of the object, (5) that only gravity acts on falls object (Wenning, 2008).
Furthermore, a study by Kurnaz and Eksi (2015) suggests that most students (56.28%) have a synthetic mental model of the concept of friction. In line with this, 10% of students have a scientific mental model, 63% of students have a synthetic mental model and 27% of students have an early mental model, based on preliminary research by researchers. This indicates that up to 63% of students do not fully understand Newton’s laws. Students can only use concepts of certain phenomena, but cannot use those concepts to explain other, more complex phenomena. For example, students can establish that act and reaction forces are exaggerated not only by the mass of an object but also by the acceleration of the object. However, students cannot apply the concept of action and reaction forces to two objects that interact but do not cause displacement. Additionally, up to 27% of students cannot use concepts to explain phenomena. For example, students cannot use the concept of action and reaction forces for two objects colliding, two objects interacting but not causing displacement, or two objects interacting with each other causing displacement.
This research highlights the untapped potential of e-rebuttal texts in addressing misconceptions and reconstructing students’ mental models. Although previous studies have shown the effectiveness of rebuttal texts and computer-based media in mitigating misconceptions and improving conceptual understanding, their integration into a cohesive, interactive digital format specifically tailored for teaching Newton’s Laws remains relatively unexplored. This study pioneers an innovative approach by combining e-rebuttal texts with multimedia elements, including videos, animations, and simulations, designed to enhance engagement and comprehension. By centering on the reconstruction of students’ mental models, this research seeks to fill a critical gap in leveraging interactive digital tools for conceptual change in physics education. Accordingly, this study aims to investigate the extent to which e-rebuttal texts on Newton’s Laws can reshape students’ mental models by addressing and correcting existing misconceptions. Therefore, this study aims to explore how e-rebuttal texts on Newton’s Laws can reconstruct students’ mental models by altering existing misconceptions.
The research method used is a mixed method of collecting quantitative and qualitative data (Dawadi et al., 2021). Quantitative research methods used to determine the students’ conceptions and the rate of change in students’ mental models after e-rebuttal texts, while qualitative methods used to determine the process of students’ mental models change. Research design used embedded mixed methods. The embedded mixed methods design simultaneously combines quantitative and qualitative research methods. Quantitative data were obtained based on pre-test and post-test results. Then, qualitative data were obtained when the learning process was based on student responses to e-rebuttal texts. Additionally, the process of changing mental models is also used as a qualitative data analysis. Quantitative and qualitative data obtained from studies were used simultaneously to interpret the results.
Participants in this study were 31 students (15 boys and 16 girls, aged 15–16 years) in the 10th grade of one public high school in Sukabumi, West Java, Indonesia. These students followed the Indonesian national curriculum, specifically the 2013 curriculum, which includes physics as a mandatory subject. The physics curriculum for 10th-grade students consists of four hours per week (each session lasting 45 min). Participants study using an e-rebuttal texts on Newton’s laws material. Their prior knowledge was assessed through a diagnostic test before the intervention. Learning is held for three weeks in January 2020 (before the Covid-19 pandemic spreads in Indonesia).
The instrument in this study, in the form of a diagnostic test, was used to identify students’ mental models. This test was developed from a standard test, the Force Concept Inventory (FCI). In addition, the test was developed in two-tier tests, the first tier being the usual multiple-choice procedure and the second tier being the reason in the form of open-ended tests. Students give reasons for possible answers in the first tier. The student’s answers are collected, then the most common reasons are used as the choice of reasons for the four-tier test. For the purpose of pre-test and post-test, this instrument was also developed in the form of multiple representations in verbal, pictorial and mathematical form. Based on the test results, the easiest representation for students will be used as a pre-test and the most difficult will be used as a post-test for each sub-material. This is to prevent students from perceiving similarities in the problems they are working on. This test is hereafter referred to as Multi-representation on Tier Instrument of Newton’s laws (MOTION). MOTION consists of 36 sub-material questions about Newton’s First Law, Newton’s Second Law, Newton’s Third Law and type of forces. Each subject matter consists of nine questions in three formats. An example of MOTION is shown in Figure 2.
From Figure 2, it can be seen that the first tier is a multiple-choice answer format. The second tier is the student’s confidence in the first-tier response options, in the form of “sure” and “not sure.” The third tier is the choice of reasons for the first-tier answers, and the fourth tier is the student’s level of confidence in the choice of reasons in the fourth tier, in the form of “sure” and “not sure.”
The instrument was first tested before being used in research. This instrument was distributed to 92 students, and 23 students each worked on sub-materials of Newton’s First Law, Newton’s Second Law, Newton’s Third Law, and the type of forces in three expressions. After that, an instrument analysis was carried out which included validity and reliability tests. Test validity and reliability through Rasch analysis (Samsudin et al., 2020). The validity test shows that the sub-materials of Newton’s First Law and the Types of Forces fall under the ‘excellent’ category with a value raw variance explained by measures greater than 60%, while the sub-materials of Newton’s Second Law and Newton’s Third Law are categorized as ‘good’ with values raw variance explained by measures above 40%. Meanwhile, the reliability test reveals that the Cronbach’s alpha values for Newton’s First Law, Newton’s Second Law, Newton’s Third Law, and Types of Forces are 0.89, 0.94, 0.87, and 0.95, respectively. These values indicate that all sub-materials meet the ‘very good’ criteria, as they exceed the threshold of 0.8. The results of this validity and reliability test show that MOTION can be used effectively and reliably.
Before analyzing the student’s mental model, first, the students’ conceptions must be analyzed. Based on the results of student responses to MOTION, student conceptions can be classified into several categories identified by Gurel et al. (2015) as shown in Table 1.
Students’ mental models can be seen based on the conception of each sub-material (Kurnaz and Eksi, 2015) as shown in Table 2. Correction of a student’s mental model is an important analysis because it allows one to see if a student has corrected their mental model. Mental model improvement categories can be divided into three categories shown in Figure 3, Acceptable Correction (ACo), No Acceptable Correction (NAC), and No Correction (NCo).
The ACo category is an acceptable correction, namely from the initial (IN) or synthetic (SY) model to scientific (SC) and from the IN model to SY. The NAC category is an unacceptable repair, namely from SY or SC to IN and from SC to SY. While the NCo category is a model that does not change from beginning to end. The mental model correction category has several possibilities as shown in Figure 3.
E-rebuttal texts is different from rebuttal texts in general, with a structure as shown in Figure 1. First, students predict what will happen to the presented phenomenon. By predicting activities, student’s initial conceptions (mental models) can be examined. Misconceptions are then presented that often appear with the refutation proposition. Students then observe what happens to the phenomenon. Observational activities can lead to cognitive conflict when students’ predictions and observations do not match. This activity is performed using simulations and videos that the students can operate themselves. Finally, a scientific explanation of the concept is presented. This activity can build the conceptions of the students. In addition, students can also write their own answers on e-rebuttal texts.
E-rebuttal texts is web-based and can be accessed through the quiz.tioc1.com site. E-rebuttal texts can be opened from a computer, laptop or mobile phone. An example display (in Indonesian) from a laptop is shown in Figure 4.
The readings listed on the e-rebuttal texts display in Figure 4 is:
A child is about to go on an excursion using a pickup truck as shown in Figure 1. The car is initially at rest.
Predict what will happen to the child if the car that was initially at rest suddenly moves and the car that was initially moving suddenly stops!
Students’ mental models can be analyzed based on the category of students’ conceptions. The distribution of students’ conceptions is shown in Table 3.
Mental models can be divided into three, namely Scientific (SC), Synthetic (SY), and Initial (IN). The percentage of students’ mental models for the pre-test and post-test is presented in Figure 5.
Based on Figure 5, it can be seen the comparison of the percentage of students’ mental models. During the pre-test, the highest percentage was in the Synthetic (SY) category at 68%, Initial (IN) at 26%, and Scientific (SC) at 6%. At the time of post-test, the highest percentage of mental models was in the SY category at 57%, SC at 37%, and IN at 6%. The percentages for the Scientific (SC) model improved from pre-test to post-test, whereas the percentages for the SY and IN models reduced. Based on this comparison, e-rebuttal texts can be used to construct students’ mental models into SC models. This is in accordance with the research of Lem et al. (2017) which states that rebuttal texts is effective for changing students’ mental models. The interactive media used allows students to carry out integrated investigations, can simulate various experiments and physical phenomena and their processes, and visualize abstract concepts (Jian-hua and Hong, 2012; Jiang et al., 2018; Nurdini et al., 2020; Ozkan and Selcuk, 2015; Wang et al., 2021).
The distribution of students for each mental model at the pre-test and post-test for each subject matter is shown in Table 4. Based on Table 4, the distribution of students for each mental model at the pre-test and post-test for each sub-material of Newton’s Law was obtained. Based on the percentage of each mental model, there is an improvement from the pre-test to the post-test. Furthermore, it was analyzed regarding the correction of students’ mental models during the pre-test and post-test. This is done to obtain information about the mental model after the learning process with e-rebuttal texts. The description of the overall mental model correction from pre-test to post-test is shown in Table 5.
Based on Table 5, it can be seen that the most frequent corrections occurred in SY with 45 corrections, followed by corrections from SY to SC with 35 changes. Corrections from the pre-test to the post-test are not yet fully SC. Overall, the percentage of students’ mental model correction categories can be seen in Figure 6.
Based on Figure 6, it can be seen that after the implementation of e-rebuttal texts, there was a correction in students’ mental models. The most correction occurred in the Acceptable Correction (ACo) category. That is, students’ mental models can be corrected through e-rebuttal texts. However, there are still students who maintain the IN and SY mental models which may contain misconceptions. This is because misconceptions are difficult to change in a short time. As revealed by Liu and Fang (2016), Samsudin et al. (2019), and Will et al. (2019) that misconceptions are difficult to change and require a long process if these conceptions are closely attached to students’ thinking.
Based on Figure 7, the highest total correction in students’ mental models occurred in the Acceptable Correction (ACo) category of 52%. That is after the implementation of e-rebuttal texts, students’ mental models experienced acceptable corrections. Corrections from IN to SY are the most common corrections. However, the SY mental model is a mental model that some still have a conception in the NU, MC, or NC categories. In the No Acceptable Correction (NAC) category, 6% occurred. In the No Correction (NCo) category, the most occurred in SY at 39%.
Figure 7. Correction of students’ mental model on Newton’s first law sub-material from pre-test to post-test.
One example of the answer S08 on e-rebuttal texts for Newton’s First Law is question number 1 (N1). In N1 is the concept of inertia, namely on the occasion that a glass of water without a cover is in a bus and the bus suddenly brakes. At the pre-test, S08 did not have an misconception, but in the post-test, S08 had an misconception. During the pre-test, S08 said that the water in the glass would spill forward when the bus suddenly braked. This is due to the reaction of the water. The answer choice S08 is correct but the reasons given are wrong, and S08 is sure of the answer choices and reasons. Therefore, S08 is in the PU category. However, during the post-test, S08 said that the water in the glass would spill forward and then backwards due to the action of the bus driver. The answer choices and reasons S08 are wrong and are sure of the answer choices and reasons so that S08 has an misconception. When viewed from S08’s answer on e-rebuttal texts, S08 correctly predicts inertia occasions. However, when explaining the results of observations for the concept of a moving object that will continue to move as long as there is no external force that affects it, the answer S08 does not fit the context presented.
When explaining the results of predictions and observations, S08 answered that there was an inertia characteristic, but did not explain further which object had an inertia characteristic of the event. In addition, this can also be caused by different representations at pre-test and post-test. The representation used during the pre-test was pictorial while the post-test was verbal. In addition to having a difficulty level in the “difficult” category, verbal representations are also possible to generate misconceptions (Braasch et al., 2013; Majid and Suyono, 2018; Yumuşak et al., 2015). In accordance with the statement of Soysal and Yilmaz-Tuzun (2021) there are many physical terms that have different meanings from everyday terms such as the terms force, power and work. In addition, in line with the research of Fithrathy (2019), after the application of physics learning with multimedia, students’ verbal representation abilities had a smaller increase than pictorial representation (graphics).
The process of correcting students’ mental models in Newton’s Second Law sub-material is shown in Figure 8. Based on Figure 8, the highest total correction in students’ mental models occurred in the Acceptable Correction (ACo) category of 55%. That is, after the implementation of e-rebuttal texts, students’ mental models experienced acceptable corrections. Corrections from SY to SC are the most common corrections. In the No Acceptable Correction (NAC) category, 7% occurred. In the No Correction (NCo) category, most occurred in SY at 35%.
Figure 8. Correction of students’ mental model in Newton’s second law sub-material from pre-test to post-test.
The process of correcting students’ mental models in the types of forces is shown in Figure 9. Based on Figure 9, the highest total corrections in students’ mental models occurred in the Acceptable Correction (ACo) category of 58%. That is, after the implementation of e-rebuttal texts, students’ mental models experienced acceptable corrections. Corrections from SY to SC are the most common corrections. In the No Acceptable Correction (NAC) category, 7% occurred. In the No Correction (NCo) category, most occurred in SY at 35%.
Figure 9. Correction of students’ mental model in sub-material types of forces from pre-test to post-test.
The process of correcting students’ mental models in Newton’s Third Law sub-material is shown in Figure 10. Based on Figure 10, the highest total correction in students’ mental models occurred in the NCo category of 49%. Most mental models occur in SY. This means that students’ mental models remain in SY after the application of e-rebuttal texts. In the ACo category, the correction from IN to SY occurred the most. That is, the mental model of students is not complete after the learning process. In the No Acceptable Correction (NAC) category, 6% occurred.
Figure 10. Correction of students’ mental model in Newton’s third law sub-material from pre-test to post-test.
Based on research conducted to reconstruct students’ mental model in conceptual change through e-rebuttal texts on Newton’s Laws, it was concluded that e-rebuttal texts can be used to reconstruct students’ mental models in conceptual change to become Scientific (SC). Overall, positive changes were seen from pre-test to post-test on each mental model. The rate of correction in students’ mental models from pre-test to post-test occurred primarily in the Acceptable Correction (ACo) category. Insertion of prediction and observation sections into rebuttal texts creating new structures in e-rebuttal texts: predictions, presenting current misconceptions, sentence of refute, observations and scientific explanations.
Moreover, there are some recommendations made by researchers based on the studies that have been conducted. Before using e-rebuttal texts, the teacher or researcher should check device and internet connectivity availability, as the media used is web-based. Other researchers may add parts of the text, such as animated force diagrams on inclined planes, motion during free fall, simulated action and reaction forces on nearby objects, etc. Additionally, the learning process used does not involve students participating in groups. Therefore, researchers may also use learning models and strategies to involve students in groups to facilitate the discussion process.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving humans were approved by Universitas Pendidikan Indonesia. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
AnS: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. AcS: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. NF: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. NN: Project administration, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. SF: Writing – review & editing. MP: Writing – review & editing. SL: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. BC: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This material is based on work supported by the Directorate General of Higher Education, Research and Technology, Ministry of Education, Culture, Research and Technology, Indonesia, number 0667/E5/AL.04/2024.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Ministry of Education, Culture, Research and Technology.
Aslan, A., and Demircioğlu, G. (2014). The effect of video-assisted conceptual change texts on 12 th grade students’ alternative conceptions: the gas concept. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 116, 3115–3119. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.718
Bayraktar, S. (2009). Misconceptions of Turkish pre-service teachers about force and motion. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 7, 273–291. doi: 10.1007/s10763-007-9120-9
Braasch, J. L. G., Goldman, S. R., and Wiley, J. (2013). Influences of text and reader characteristics on learning from refutations in science texts. J. Educ. Psychol. 105, 561–578. doi: 10.1037/a0032627
Bunawan, W., Setiawan, A., and Rusli, A. (2015). Penilaian pemahaman representasi grafik materi optika geometri menggunakan tes diagnostik. Jurnal Cakrawala Pendidikan 2, 257–267. doi: 10.21831/cp.v2i2.4830
Caleon, I., and Subramaniam, R. (2013). Addressing students’ alternative conceptions on the propagation of periodic waves using a refutational text. Phys. Educ. 48, 657–663. doi: 10.1088/0031-9120/48/5/657
Çil, E., and Çepni, S. (2016). The effectiveness of conceptual change texts and concept clipboards in learning the nature of science. Res. Sci. Technol. Educ. 34, 43–68. doi: 10.1080/02635143.2015.1066323
Dawadi, S., Shrestha, S., and Giri, R. A. (2021). Mixed-methods research: a discussion on its types, challenges, and criticisms. J. Pract. Stud. Educ. 2, 25–36. doi: 10.46809/jpse.v2i2.20
Ferreira, A., Lemmer, M., and Gunstone, R. (2017). Alternative conceptions: turning adversity into advantage. Res. Sci. Educ. 49, 657–678. doi: 10.1007/s11165-017-9638-y
Fithrathy, A. (2019). Developing physics learning multimedia to improve graphic and verbal representation of high school students. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 1233:012071. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/1233/1/012071
Fratiwi, N. J., Samsudin, A., Ramalis, T. R., Saregar, A., Diani, R., Irwandani, R., et al. (2020). Developing memori on Newton’s laws: for identifying students’ mental models. Europ. J. Educ. Res. 9-2020, 699–708. doi: 10.12973/eu-jer.9.2.699
Gadgil, S., Nokes-Malach, T. J., and Chi, M. T. H. (2012). Effectiveness of holistic mental model confrontation in driving conceptual change. Learn. Instr. 22, 47–61. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.06.002
Georgiou, Y., Tsivitanidou, O., and Ioannou, A. (2021). Learning experience design with immersive virtual reality in physics education. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 69, 3051–3080. doi: 10.1007/s11423-021-10055-y
Gurel, D. K., Eryilmaz, A., and McDermott, L. C. (2015). A review and comparison of diagnostic instruments to identify students’ misconceptions in science. Eurasia J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 11, 989–1008. doi: 10.12973/eurasia.2015.1369a
Hunsu, N. J., Adesope, O., and McCrudden, M. T. (2023). The effects of text structure on students’ use of comprehension strategies and cognitive outcomes during science text processing. Front. Educ. 8, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1112804
Ipek, H., and Calik, M. (2008). Combining different conceptual change methods within four-step constructivist teaching model: a sample teaching of series and parallel circuits. Int. J. Environ. Sci. Educ. 3, 143–153.
Jiang, T., Wang, S., Wang, J., and Ma, Y. (2018). Effect of different instructional methods on students’ conceptual change regarding electrical resistance as viewed from a synthesized theoretical framework. Eurasia J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 14, 2771–2786. doi: 10.29333/ejmste/90592
Jian-hua, S., and Hong, L. (2012). Explore the effective use of multimedia Technology in College Physics Teaching. Energy Procedia 17, 1897–1900. doi: 10.1016/j.egypro.2012.02.329
Kahnbach, L., Hase, A., Kuhl, P., and Lehr, D. (2024). Explaining primary school teachers’ intention to use digital learning platforms for students’ individualized practice: comparison of the standard UTAUT and an extended model. Front. Educ. 9, 1–17. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1353020
Kaltakçi, D., and Didiş, N. (2007). Identification of pre-service physics teachers’ misconceptions on gravity concept: a study with a 3-tier misconception test. AIP Conf. Proc. 499–500. doi: 10.1063/1.2733255
Kaltakci-Gurel, D., Eryilmaz, A., and McDermott, L. C. (2017). Development and application of a four-tier test to assess pre-service physics teachers’ misconceptions about geometrical optics. Res. Sci. Technol. Educ. 35, 238–260. doi: 10.1080/02635143.2017.1310094
Karaoglan Yilmaz, F. G., Özdemir, B. G., and Yasar, Z. (2018). Using digital stories to reduce misconceptions and mistakes about fractions: an action study. Int. J. Math. Educ. Sci. Technol. 49, 867–898. doi: 10.1080/0020739X.2017.1418919
Kaya, Z., Kaya, O. N., Aydemir, S., and Ebenezer, J. (2022). Knowledge of student learning difficulties as a plausible conceptual change pathway between content knowledge and pedagogical content knowledge. Res. Sci. Educ. 52, 691–723. doi: 10.1007/s11165-020-09971-5
Kulgemeyer, C., and Wittwer, J. (2022). Misconceptions in physics explainer videos and the illusion of understanding: an experimental study. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 21, 417–437. doi: 10.1007/s10763-022-10265-7
Kurnaz, M. A., and Eksi, C. (2015). An analysis of high school students’ mental models of solid friction in physics. Kuram ve Uygulamada Egitim Bilimleri 15, 787–795. doi: 10.12738/estp.2015.3.2526
Ladachart, L., Cholsin, J., Kwanpet, S., Teerapanpong, R., Dessi, A., Phuangsuwan, L., et al. (2022). Using reverse engineering to enhance ninth-grade students’ understanding of thermal expansion. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 31, 177–190. doi: 10.1007/s10956-021-09940-1
Lechner, M., Moser, S., Pander, J., Geist, J., and Lewalter, D. (2024). Learning scientific observation with worked examples in a digital learning environment. Front. Educ. 9, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1293516
Lem, S., Onghena, P., Verschaffel, L., and Van Dooren, W. (2017). Using refutational text in mathematics education. ZDM, 49, 509–518.
Leppavirta, J. (2012). Assessing undergraduate students’ conceptual understanding and confidence of electromagnetics. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 10, 1099–1117. doi: 10.1007/s10763-011-9317-9
Li, K. C., Wong, B. T. M., and Chan, H. T. (2023). Teaching and learning innovations for distance learning in the digital era: a literature review. Front. Educ. 8, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1198034
Li, B., Yu, J., Sun, L., and Yang, H. (2024). Impact of active learning instruction in blended learning on students’ anxiety levels and performance. Front. Educ. 9. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1332778
Liu, G., and Fang, N. (2016). Student misconceptions about force and acceleration in physics and engineering mechanics education. Int. J. Eng. Educ. 32, 19–29.
Majid, A., and Suyono, S. (2018). Misconception analysis based on students mental model in atom structure materials. In Seminar Nasional Kimia-National Seminar on Chemistry (SNK 2018). Atlantis Press. pp. 244–247.
Mason, L., and Zaccoletti, S. (2021). Inhibition and conceptual learning in science: a review of studies. In. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 33, 181–212. doi: 10.1007/s10648-020-09529-x
Mason, L., Zaccoletti, S., Carretti, B., Scrimin, S., and Diakidoy, I. A. N. (2019). The role of inhibition in conceptual learning from refutation and standard expository texts. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 17, 483–501. doi: 10.1007/s10763-017-9874-7
Mondragon-Estrada, E., Kirschning, I., Nolazco-Flores, J. A., and Camacho-Zuñiga, C. (2023). Fostering digital transformation in education: technology enhanced learning from professors’ experiences in emergency remote teaching. Front. Educ. 8, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1250461
Nurdini, N., Ramalis, T. R., and Samsudin, A. (2019). Exploring K-11 students’ conception using a four-tier diagnostic test on static fluid: a case study. RSU Conf., 672–681. doi: 10.14458/RSU.res.2019.83
Nurdini, N., Suhandi, A., Ramalis, T., Samsudin, A., Fratiwi, N. J., and Costu, B. (2020). Developing multitier instrument of fluids concepts (MIFO) to measure Student’s conception: a Rasch analysis approach. J. Adv. Res. Dyn. Control Syst. 12, 3069–3083. doi: 10.5373/JARDCS/V12I6/S20201273
Ozkan, G., and Selcuk, G. S. (2015). Effect of technology enhanced conceptual change texts on students’ understanding of buoyant force. Univ. J. Educ. Res. 3, 981–988. doi: 10.13189/ujer.2015.031205
Ozkan, G., and Selcuk, G. S. (2016). Facilitating conceptual change in students’ understanding of concepts related to pressure. Eur. J. Phys. 37:055702. doi: 10.1088/0143-0807/37/5/055702
Peşman, H., and Eryilmaz, A. (2010). Development of a three-tier test to assess misconceptions about simple electric circuits. J. Educ. Res. 103, 208–222. doi: 10.1080/00220670903383002
Podschuweit, S., and Bernholt, S. (2018). Composition-effects of context-based learning opportunities on students’ understanding of energy. Res. Sci. Educ. 48, 717–752. doi: 10.1007/s11165-016-9585-z
Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., and Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: toward a theory of conceptual change. Sci. Educ. 66, 211–227. doi: 10.1002/sce.3730660207
Poutot, G., and Blandin, B. (2015). Exploration of students’ misconceptions in mechanics using the FCI. Am. J. Educ. Res. 3, 116–120. doi: 10.12691/education-3-2-2
Prinz, A., Kollmer, J., Flick, L., Renkl, A., and Eitel, A. (2022). Refuting student teachers’ misconceptions about multimedia learning. Instr. Sci. 50, 89–110. doi: 10.1007/s11251-021-09568-z
Purwanto, M. G., Nurliani, R., Kaniawati, I., and Samsudin, A. (2018). Promoting the hydrostatic conceptual change test (HCCT) with four-tier diagnostic test item. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 1013, 012035–012036. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/1013/1/012035
Saglam-Arslan, A., and Devecioglu, Y. (2010). Student teachers’ levels of understanding and model of understanding about Newton’s laws of motion. Asia-Pacific Forum Sci. Learn. Teach. 11, 1–20.
Sakurai, J., and Goos, M. (2023). Revisiting tools in numeracy learning: the role of authentic digital tools. Front. Educ. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1291407
Samsudin, A., Fratiwi, N., Amin, N., Wiendartun, S., Wibowo, F., Faizin, M., et al. (2018). Improving students’ conceptions on fluid dynamics through peer teaching model with PDEODE (PTM-PDEODE). J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 1013:012040. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/1013/1/012040
Samsudin, A., Fratiwi, N. J., Ramalis, T. R., Aminudin, A. H., Costu, B., and Nurtanto, M. (2020). Using rasch analysis to develop multi-representation of tier instrument on newton’s law (motion). Int. J. Psychosoc. Rehabil. 24, 8542–8556. doi: 10.37200/IJPR/V24I6/PR260865
Samsudin, A., Suhandi, A., Rusdiana, D., Kaniawati, I., and Costu, B. (2015). Fields conceptual change inventory: a diagnostic test instrument on the electric field and magnetic field to diagnose students’ conceptions. Int. J. Indust. Electr. Elect. Eng. 3, 74–77.
Samsudin, A., Suhandi, A., Rusdiana, D., Kaniawati, I., Fratiwi, N. J., Zulfikar, A., et al. (2019). Optimizing students’ conceptual understanding on electricity and magnetism through cognitive conflict-based multimode teaching (CC-BMT). J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 1204:012027. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/1204/1/012027
Sarwar, M. N., Shahzad, A., Ullah, Z., Raza, S., Wasti, S. H., Shrahili, M., et al. (2024). Concept mapping and conceptual change texts: a constructivist approach to address the misconceptions in nanoscale science and technology. Front. Educ. 9, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1339957
Schroeder, N. L., and Kucera, A. C. (2022). Refutation text facilitates learning: a Meta-analysis of between-subjects experiments. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 34, 957–987. doi: 10.1007/s10648-021-09656-z
Soysal, Y., and Yilmaz-Tuzun, O. (2021). Relationships between teacher discursive moves and middle school students’ cognitive contributions to science concepts. Res. Sci. Educ. 51, 325–367. doi: 10.1007/s11165-019-09881-1
Suhandi, A., Hermita, N., Samsudin, A., Maftuh, B., and Coştu, B. (2017). Effectiveness of visual multimedia supported conceptual change texts on overcoming students’ misconception about boiling concept. Turkish Online J. Educ. Technol., 1012–1022.
Sung, S. H., Li, C., Chen, G., Huang, X., Xie, C., Massicotte, J., et al. (2021). How does augmented observation facilitate multimodal representational thinking? Applying deep learning to decode complex student construct. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 30, 210–226. doi: 10.1007/s10956-020-09856-2
Tippett, C. D. (2010). Refutation text in science education: a review of two decades of research. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 8, 951–970. doi: 10.1007/s10763-010-9203-x
Varela, B., Sesto, V., and García-Rodeja, I. (2020). An investigation of secondary students’ mental models of climate change and the greenhouse effect. Res. Sci. Educ. 50, 599–624. doi: 10.1007/s11165-018-9703-1
Wang, Y. J., Lee, S. W. Y., Liu, C. C., Lin, P. C., and Wen, C. T. (2021). Investigating the links between students’ learning engagement and modeling competence in computer-supported modeling-based activities. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 30, 751–765. doi: 10.1007/s10956-021-09916-1
Wenning, C. (2008). Dealing more effectively with alternative conceptions in science. J. Physics Teach. Educ. Online 5, 11–19. Available at: http://www2.phy.ilstu.edu/ptefiles/publications/dealing_alt_con.pdf
Wiji, W., and Mulyani, S. (2018). Student’s mental model, misconceptions, troublesome knowledge, and threshold concept on thermochemistry with DToM-POE. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 1013:012098. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/1013/1/012098
Will, K. K., Masad, A., Vlach, H. A., and Kendeou, P. (2019). The effects of refutation texts on generating explanations. Learn. Individ. Differ. 69, 108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.12.002
Yildirir, H. E., and Demirkol, H. (2018). Identifying mental models of students for physical and chemical change. J. Balt. Sci. Educ. 17, 986–1004. doi: 10.33225/jbse/18.17.986
Yumuşak, A., Maraş, I., and Şahin, M. (2015). Effects of computer-assisted instruction with conceptual change texts on removing the misconceptions of radioactivity. J. Educ. Gifted Young Sci. 3, 23–50. doi: 10.17478/JEGYS.2015214277
Yürük, N., and Eroğlu, P. (2016). The effect of conceptual change texts enriched with metaconceptual processes on pre-service science teachers’ conceptual understanding of heat and temperature. J. Balt. Sci. Educ. 15, 693–705. doi: 10.33225/jbse/16.15.693
Keywords: conceptual change, e-rebuttal texts, mental model, misconceptions, Newton’s laws
Citation: Suhandi A, Samsudin A, Fratiwi NJ, Nurdini N, Feranie S, Purwanto MG, Linuwih S and Coştu B (2025) Altering misconceptions: how e-rebuttal texts on Newton’s laws reconstructs students’ mental models. Front. Educ. 10:1472385. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2025.1472385
Received: 29 July 2024; Accepted: 04 March 2025;
Published: 07 April 2025.
Edited by:
Clifford A. Shaffer, Virginia Tech, United StatesReviewed by:
Kaan Bati, Hacettepe University, TürkiyeCopyright © 2025 Suhandi, Samsudin, Fratiwi, Nurdini, Feranie, Purwanto, Linuwih and Coştu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Andi Suhandi, YW5kaV9zaEB1cGkuZWR1; Nuzulira Janeusse Fratiwi, bnV6dWxpcmEuamFuZXVzc2UuZnJhdGl3aUB1cGkuZWR1; Nurdini Nurdini, bnVyZGluaUB1cGkuZWR1
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.