Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Educ., 24 January 2025
Sec. Digital Education
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Benefits of Technologies Applied to Learning in Education View all 18 articles

Video games and metacognition in the classroom for the development of 21st century skills: a systematic review

  • Department of Educational Sciences, Area of Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Alcalá, Alcalá de Henares, Spain

Introduction: Game-based learning is a methodology that has gained importance in the world of education due to the benefits of implementing ICT in the classroom. Due to the intellectual and competence demands that some commercial video games promote, they can be very valuable tools to stimulate and promote metacognitive abilities in students. Therefore, this paper aimed to analyze the use of commercial video games in educational contexts and their impact on the development of metacognitive skills.

Methods: For the analysis of the study, a systematic review of the literature was carried out following the PRISMA statement. Specialized databases such as Scopus, WoS, PsyInfo, PubMed, SciELO, and Eric were consulted, which helped capture the essence of the problem between 2004 and 2024.

Results: The review was then carried out on 10 selected articles. The results obtained show the following evidence: (a) commercial video games, together with the use of metacognitive strategies, enrich academic and social aspects as well as the metacognitive level, and (b) commercial video games are as effective in promoting metacognition as other more traditional methodologies or tools.

Discussion: Finally, there is a need to rethink the implications of the use of video games in the classroom if we want students to acquire skills that will be essential to successfully respond to the demands and challenges of their academic and professional future.

Introduction

Play and its cultural and educational impact

Play is essential to understanding what it means to be human. It has always been a fundamental part of life, present in every culture and situation. Through play, people have learned important life skills, and it has shaped the identity of each society while continuously contributing to the development of culture (Andrade, 2020; Delgado, 2011; Montero, 2017). In education, the role of games in the classroom is changing, largely due to advancements in information and communication technologies (ICT) and the need to improve learning processes. To address these changes, methods such as game-based learning (GBL) have emerged, integrating video games into educational settings to meet specific learning goals (Pegalajar Palomino, 2021; Torres et al., 2019). GBL uses video games as powerful tools to create engaging and innovative learning environments, especially as they are often the first digital technology that children access directly (Gee, 2007; Levis, 2013; Tokarieva et al., 2019) and are highly popular among this age group (Kahila et al., 2020).

Among the different types of video games, commercial games are the most commonly used by children and young people. They have several advantages over educational games, including being developed independently of specific educational goals, being easy to access, not requiring programming skills, allowing learning to happen outside the classroom, and being widely appealing due to their entertainment value (Connolly et al., 2012; Gee, 2011; Kahila et al., 2023). Beyond their ability to create engaging and innovative learning environments, commercial video games also have great potential to develop metacognitive skills. Metacognition—defined as the ability to reflect on, regulate, and plan one’s own thinking processes (Flavell, 1979)—is essential for independent and effective learning. These games present complex and dynamic scenarios where players must constantly evaluate their choices, plan strategies, and adapt to new challenges. This continuous process helps build key metacognitive skills, such as self-monitoring and flexible thinking (Schrader et al., 2020). Moreover, commercial video games are often the first type of digital technology that children interact with directly and personally (Gee, 2007; Levis, 2013; Tokarieva et al., 2019). Their widespread popularity among young users makes them an excellent tool for promoting autonomous, self-regulated learning—both essential 21st-century skills (Kahila et al., 2020). By encouraging players to reflect on their thinking during gameplay and adjust their strategies, these games play a key role in developing metacognitive abilities.

In this context, digital and playful environments have great potential to cultivate key 21st-century skills, as highlighted by recent scientific literature (Kickmeier-Rust and Albert, 2012; Sourmelis et al., 2017; Erstad and Voogt, 2018; Erstad et al., 2021; Zimmerman, 2008). Beyond fostering creativity, motivation, and social skills (Ames and Burrell, 2017; Hall et al., 2020; Sánchez-Rodríguez et al., 2010; Diaz-López et al., 2023), these digital environments play a pivotal role in enhancing metacognition. Studies show that engaging with video games helps players build cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring, and strategic thinking, all of which are core components of metacognitive growth (Ouellette, 2019; Squire, 2011; Taub et al., 2018; Yoon and Baek, 2018; Teng, 2024; Mitsea et al., 2024). Mastering digital literacy demands the development of sophisticated cognitive skills (Henderson, 2005; Weber and Greiff, 2023). Video games are particularly powerful tools in this regard because they provide dynamic scenarios where players must regulate their thought processes to succeed. This makes the field of metacognitive research, especially relevant, as it allows us to examine how players develop metacognitive strategies, such as planning, evaluating, and adjusting their actions to achieve in-game objectives (Bullard, 2013; Carter, 2015; Donaldson, 2017; Chatzivasileiou and Drigas, 2022; Pillay et al., 1999; Mayor et al., 1993).

The role of video games in developing metacognitive skills

Metacognition refers to the knowledge and control individuals have over their cognitive and behavioral processes (Flavell, 1979; Ford et al., 1998; Hughes et al., 2013; Fleming, 2024). It can be divided into two key dimensions: metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive control. Metacognitive knowledge includes three components: (1) declarative knowledge—what one knows about their cognitive abilities and strategies, (2) procedural knowledge—how to apply these strategies, and (3) conditional knowledge—when and why specific strategies should be used (Baker and Brown, 1984; Efklides, 2008; Pérez and González, 2020). Metacognitive control, on the other hand, involves three processes: (1) planning, which occurs before starting a task and involves setting goals and selecting strategies, (2) monitoring, which entails tracking progress and adjusting strategies while performing the task, and (3) evaluation, where the effectiveness of strategies is assessed after the task is completed.

Metacognitive development also encompasses behavioral aspects, including emotional and social factors. Flavell (1979) recognized the role of affect in metacognitive processes, proposing that individuals can develop their cognitive skills autonomously by exercising control over their mental states and processes. This highlights the critical role of self-awareness and self-regulation in metacognition. Metacognition has been defined as an individual’s ability to actively monitor, regulate, and organize cognitive processes to achieve specific goals (García Madruga et al., 2002). Within this framework, two essential concepts emerge: (1) knowledge of one’s cognitive system, which is divided into awareness of one’s abilities (person), understanding task objectives and complexity (task), and identifying the necessary strategies to succeed (strategies) (Gandini, 2018); and (2) metacognitive control, which focuses on the actions needed to complete a task effectively. Moreover, Brown (1978) introduced the concept of conditional knowledge, emphasizing its importance in recognizing the optimal conditions for applying strategies. For example, understanding when and why to use specific study techniques when preparing for an examination enhances strategic learning. This integration of knowledge and regulation underscores the central role of metacognition in fostering independent, effective learning.

Recent research underscores the importance of how cognitive processes interact with specific tasks, which often evoke emotions such as familiarity, confidence, and satisfaction (Pérez and González, 2020; Fernández Velasco and Loev, 2024). In this context, metacognitive models currently integrate emotional self-regulation alongside knowledge and cognitive control. Emotional self-regulation involves managing affective factors during planning, monitoring, and evaluation processes (Efklides, 2009; Pintrich et al., 2000; Zimmerman, 2000; Zhu et al., 2024). In particular, ineffective regulation of emotions can contribute to cognitive-attentional syndrome (CAS), a condition where maladaptive patterns, such as persistent negative thoughts and avoidance of positive ones, dominate mental processes, potentially leading to psychological disorders such as anxiety and depression (Wells, 2009). Research suggests that metacognitive beliefs about uncontrollability and perceived danger are central indicators of these conditions (Cano-López et al., 2022).

In this sense, play is a powerful tool for developing metacognitive skills. It involves rules, objectives, feedback, problem-solving, competition, and challenges—all of which require players to engage in cognitive processes such as planning, monitoring, and adjusting strategies. These activities activate critical cognitive functions, including working memory, attention, processing speed, and abstract reasoning (Wilson et al., 2023). Video games, as a form of play, further amplify these metacognitive processes by providing immersive, interactive environments that encourage learners to apply their knowledge in enjoyable and meaningful ways (Valencia-Molina et al., 2017). The bidirectional relationship between play and metacognition means that each gaming experience strengthens learner’s ability to regulate and reflect on their cognitive processes. In the social dimension, metacognition has traditionally focused on individual self-awareness (Hadwin and Järvellä, 2011; Pérez and González, 2020). However, studies show that collaboration within peer groups enhances the metacognitive processes of planning, monitoring, and evaluating knowledge (McCaslin and Hickey, 2001; Schunk, 2001; Zimmerman, 2000; Sun et al., 2022). Group interactions provide opportunities for learners to develop a shared understanding, refine strategies, and co-regulate their learning, thereby strengthening both individual and collective metacognitive skills.

Research on commercial video games and metacognitive processes in education

The implementation of tools such as video games can effectively target and enhance the previously mentioned metacognitive factors. From a sociocultural perspective on development (Gauvain, 2020; Hu and Luo, 2024), metacognitive processes related to knowledge (e.g., what do I know and how do I know it) and control (the regulation of what I know) can be enhanced, as the use of video games necessitates ongoing digital literacy (Henderson, 2005). Moreover, factors such as interest, satisfaction, engagement (Bond et al., 2020), and perceived difficulty play a crucial role in shaping learning experiences. Interest drives curiosity and sustained attention, satisfaction reinforces motivation and a sense of achievement, and engagement ensures active participation and immersion in the activity. However, perceived difficulty can influence the learner’s emotional state—moderate difficulty may encourage persistence, whereas excessive difficulty might induce anxiety, thereby hindering metacognitive processes (Misak, 2018; Moncarz, 2011; Monem, 2015; Naeimi and Rafezi, 2023).

Among other benefits, such as fostering emotional awareness and promoting reflective thinking, this approach would enhance students’ ability to recognize their emotional states, enabling them to more effectively address behavioral aspects in the classroom and guiding them toward improved emotional self-regulation (Broadbent and Poon, 2015). Additionally, video games possess an interactive capability that promotes coordination among students when collaborating to achieve a common goal (Vlachopoulos and Makri, 2017). Research on video games for educational purposes or “serious games” and metacognition has covered various aspects, with the most notable focusing on motivation and feedback (Kapp et al., 2019; Zumbach et al., 2020; Singh and Muis, 2024), robotics, design, and collaborative learning (Yoon and Baek, 2018; Li et al., 2024), scientific reasoning and self-regulated learning (Taub et al., 2018; Dörrenbächer-Ulrich et al., 2024), or English language acquisition (Mahayanti et al., 2020; Tan et al., 2022; Zhang, 2024).

However, the application of commercial video games in the classroom to enhance metacognitive processes remains insufficiently explored in the scientific literature. Limited research has examined the presence of dynamic stimuli within these multimodal, interactive, and multisensory tools, which can promote both self-scaffolding and metacognitive awareness (Monem, 2015), as well as support self-regulation, planning, and continuous monitoring of one’s actions (Lickteig, 2020). Therefore, it seems necessary to deepen our understanding of the current state of research, and a systematic review of the existing literature would be a good approach.

The general objective of this study was to analyze the role of video games in fostering metacognitive skills within classroom settings and their contribution to the development of essential 21st-century skills, such as critical thinking, problem-solving, and self-regulated learning, through a comprehensive review of existing literature and evidence-based practices. This general objective is specified in two specific objectives: (1) to analyze the use of commercial video games in education and how they stimulate metacognition and (2) to explore whether commercial video games are capable of fostering metacognitive abilities in the same way as other more traditional tools or methodologies. To answer these objectives, the following research question has been posed: (1) What impact do commercial video games have on metacognitive skills?

Methodology

To achieve the research objectives and provide answers to the questions formulated, a systematic review of the literature was carried out (Medina et al., 2020; Páramo, 2020; Sánchez-Meca, 2010). This review is proposed with the purpose of analyzing and specifying those studies that analyze the inclusion of commercial video games in the classroom and their impact on metacognitive skills and competencies necessary for the citizens of the 21st century (Prendes Espinosa et al., 2017).

Protocol and record-keeping

A series of guidelines and guidelines proposed by the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) methodology (Liberati et al., 2009; Page et al., 2020) for scientific systematic reviews were established, as well as some of the definitions and elements recommended by the Cochrane Collaboration (Urrútia and Bonfill, 2010), in order to carry out methodical planning (Hutton et al., 2015).

Procedure

The literature search completed during the months of June and July 2024, consulted the following electronic databases: Scopus©, Web of Science©, PsycInfo©, PubMed©, Scielo©, and Eric©. In addition, the different thesauruses provided by the different databases were considered, which helped broaden the search and better synthesize the results. The search for published scientific articles began in 2004 and ended in May 2024, with the purpose of extracting as many documents as possible on the research problem addressed.

Search strategy

To optimize the search and target the selection more specifically, a series of inclusion and exclusion criteria were used (Table 1). Some of these filters were provided by the databases themselves (article, year of publication, book, conference proceedings, press, or language), and others were proposed to narrow down the overview of the study as much as possible (methodology, sample, subject matter, and duplicate articles).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Study selection

As a result of applying these search strategies, a total of 545 articles were obtained from the different databases mentioned above. As can be seen in Figure 1, all these articles went through different vetting phases, eliminating any article that was affected by the exclusion criteria and admitting those that matched the inclusion criteria and the research topic (Table 1). Articles addressing aspects of metacognition, the use of digital educational games, or variations of analytics based on a learning or cognitive framework were collected. To define the target population, labels indicating the intended medium (e.g., educational video games) were applied. The outcome section was defined by keywords aimed at capturing the overarching cognitive and learning focus of this review (e.g., cognition and learning). Finally, the comparison component, which involves directly contrasting the effectiveness of commercial video games with traditional educational tools or methodologies, could not be included in this review because no specific empirical procedures, such as experimental or control group setups, had been pre-established; instead, this component served as a focal point of interest for the review. In order to simplify the search in each of the databases consulted, the following keywords and Boolean operators were used to obtain as many articles as possible, as well as to narrow the search in order and structure: (“videogame” or “game” or “digital games”) and (“metacognition” or “metacognitive” or “metagame”).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the systematization of the review.

The final result of the whole process produced a total of 19 items for detailed review and analysis.

Data analysis

A database was generated to record and systematize the following information of the selected articles: title of the study, author/s, year and place of publication, study sample, objectives, methodology, design and data collection instruments, and results of the study, thus following the PICoS strategy (Pertegal-Vega et al., 2019) through Rayyan Intelligent Systematic Review© software. We then used Review Manager 5.4© software to examine, code, and categorize in detail the most relevant information units of the final sample selected.

Evaluation of methodological quality

Independently selected articles (n = 348) were evaluated in the eligibility phase through Rayyan Intelligent Systematic Review© software. In situations of disagreement, the judgment of a third person from the research team was requested. For the methodological review of the studies, the articles were evaluated based on satisfactory compliance with the criteria indicated.

Results

Publications

In addressing the research problem and adhering to the proposed eligibility criteria, the final sample size (n = 19) demonstrates that the use of commercial video games as a tool to develop metacognitive skills in the classroom remains a relatively unexplored area in the scientific literature. However, this limited number of studies should not be interpreted as a lack of relevance or interest in the subject. On the contrary, the emerging data suggest a growing awareness of the potential that these digital tools hold in educational contexts. As illustrated in Figure 2, the number of articles published on the topic has grown exponentially in recent years (n = 348). This trend underscores an increasing recognition of the critical role video games may play in fostering metacognitive processes. Although the existing literature is still in its infancy, the surge in academic publications signals a shift toward exploring how video games can serve not only as entertainment but as powerful vehicles for developing essential 21st-century skills, such as critical thinking, problem-solving, and self-regulated learning.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Temporal evolution of the use of video games in education.

The significance of this study lies precisely in the current gap within literature. The intersection of metacognition and video games is an innovative area with untapped potential, where empirical evidence is gradually emerging but remains insufficient to fully understand the depth and scope of this relationship. This underrepresentation in the scientific discourse highlights the necessity for further research and makes this study particularly relevant. By contributing to this nascent field, the present research sheds light on how commercial video games can be harnessed to promote metacognitive skills, thereby enriching both educational theory and practice. Moreover, the growing body of work emphasizes that the use of commercial video games goes beyond mere engagement. It intersects with cognitive and emotional dimensions, providing students with opportunities to practice planning, monitoring, and evaluating their learning processes in real time. Therefore, the importance of continuing to explore this relationship cannot be overstated, as it holds the potential to transform traditional educational methodologies and foster more holistic learning experiences.

Specifically, with regard to the systematic and individual analysis of each of the articles that form the basis of this study, the most important aspects are analyzed in detail below, in accordance with the inclusion criteria of the study: objectives, research design, instruments used, main results and conclusions, limitations, and practical applications in the educational setting. The process of obtaining the sample was carried out by generating a database to record and systematize the most relevant information contained in the selected articles (Table 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Selection of articles to be analyzed in the systematic review.

Sample characteristics

The studies reviewed encompass a wide range of sample sizes, ages, and designs, reflecting the diversity of research on metacognition and cognitive skills in video game contexts. Sample sizes varied significantly, from single-case studies such as Monem (2015), who observed a 16-year-old player’s self-scaffolding in an MMORPG, to large-scale studies such as Hardy et al. (2015), with 4,715 participants exploring the impact of cognitive training versus crossword puzzles on neuropsychological performance. Most studies targeted adolescents and young adults, with average ages often ranging from 10 to 20 years, including a notable sample of 285 university students in Shaw (2022) who used Minecraft to examine cognitive predictors of creativity. Only a few studies focused on children, such as Harris et al. (2008), who analyzed goal-oriented problem-solving in a sample of 48 nine-year-olds, and Ricker and Richert (2021), who assessed metacognitive awareness in 103 children averaging 8 years of age. Additionally, Valentine and Jensen (2021) used a phenomenological approach with 30 Pokémon Go players over a period of 4 to 12 months, examining how augmented reality plays influences social and cultural interactions.

Across these studies, research designs varied from qualitative approaches, such as Luang-Ng et al. (2022), observing vocabulary learning in four MMORPG players, to experimental and quasi-experimental designs, exemplified by Tangkui and Keong (2023) with a sample of five students learning fractions using Minecraft. Mixed-methods designs were also prevalent, enabling researchers to capture both quantitative and qualitative aspects of gameplay. Certain studies, like Wilson et al. (2022) with The Sims 4, used pre-test/post-test evaluations to assess changes in vocabulary and metacognitive awareness among 65 students. In contrast, some studies focused on specific cognitive assessments, such as Imanian et al. (2024), which evaluated attention, working memory, and cognitive flexibility in 40 participants through FIFA, noting differences between single-player and cooperative play modes. This diverse array of samples and methodologies highlights the breadth of current research exploring how video games impact metacognitive and cognitive skills across varied populations and gaming environments.

Research design and methodology overview

Regarding the aim of the studies, one of these is to use metacognitive strategies and commercial video games as a support to improve relevant educational aspects, such as academic performance, social problem-solving, narrative skills, or motivation (Valentine and Jensen, 2021; Shaw, 2022; Garcia Martinez, 2014; Kim et al., 2009). Other studies establish correlations between the metacognitive construct, the difficulty of video game practice, and participants’ mental ability (Tangkui and Keong, 2023; Hardy et al., 2015; Hughes et al., 2013) or analyze the process students use to achieve an educational goal with respect to their level of metacognitive awareness and regulation (Piédalue, 2022; Lickteig, 2020; Michael et al., 2012; Harris et al., 2008). In terms of research design, the sample of studies used both qualitative and quantitative methodology, as well as mixed methodology designs, with no clear predominance, as can be seen in Figure 3.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Representation in percentages of the methodology applied.

Quantitative designs also played a central role, with many studies implementing pre-test/post-test structures to assess the impact of video games on cognitive and metacognitive abilities. Wilson et al. (2022) used such a design to measure the effects of The Sims 4 on vocabulary acquisition and metacognitive awareness in an undergraduate sample. In another quantitative approach, Piédalue (2022) employed a questionnaire and observational measures to analyze executive functioning in children with ADHD using Minecraft. Mixed-methods designs, such as the quasi-experimental structure by Tangkui and Keong (2023), allowed for a comprehensive comparison of traditional teaching methods with video game-based learning in a small sample of Year 5 students. Additionally, Hardy et al. (2015) utilized a randomized control design to compare cognitive training in the Lumosity program with a crossword puzzle control group, examining neuropsychological outcomes across a large sample of over 4,000 participants. This methodological diversity highlights the adaptable nature of video game research in capturing complex cognitive and metacognitive phenomena across varied contexts and participant groups.

The remaining studies aim to understand the metacognitive dimension. For instance, Wilson et al. (2023) found that using The Sims 4: Get to Work in a Business English course enhanced students’ metacognitive skills—such as planning, monitoring, and evaluating—while also improving their use of Business English in practical, goal-oriented tasks; Lickteig (2020) defined the phenomenon known as metagaming, highlights how players leverage external information and strategies beyond the game itself to optimize their experience. A compelling example is the introduction of patient-accessible online medical records, which function as a metacognitive extension of the game, transforming player interaction while enhancing self-regulation and strategic planning—skills essential not only in gameplay but also in real-world applications; in Monet’s study (2015), the focus was on how an adolescent male gamer in an MMORPG developed metacognitive skills, self-awareness, and a virtual identity, highlighting the use of self-scaffolding strategies within immersive digital environments. On the other hand, other studies validate an instrument in relation to metacognition and a video game (Imanian et al., 2024; Li et al., 2015). In the study by Imanian et al. (2024), the effects of esports on cognitive skills were analyzed with 40 undergraduate students. Participants played FIFA for 8 weeks, in either single-player or co-player mode. Results showed improvements in attention and working memory for both groups, while cognitive flexibility improved only in the co-player group. These findings suggest that esports can enhance cognitive skills with potential applications in learning and instruction. Finally, highly interactive digital games enhance metacognitive awareness in children aged 6–10 years, while less interactive games show no such effect. Game preferences do not vary by age or sex, highlighting differences in cognitive opportunities across game types (Ricker and Richert, 2021).

Within the quantitative studies, pre-test/post-test designs predominate (Shaw, 2022; Piédalue, 2022; Hardy et al., 2015; Harris et al., 2008; Hughes et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2009), using control and experimental (Wilson et al., 2022; Harris et al., 2008) or quasi-experimental groups (Tangkui and Keong, 2023).

In terms of qualitative studies, we find an observational case study (Monem, 2015), an action research study (Imanian et al., 2024; Wilson et al., 2023), and a study based on focus group interviews (Lickteig, 2020). The forms of qualitative data collection are audio (Luang-Ng et al., 2022), journals, or reflections (Von Gillern and Stufft, 2023). With respect to the target population of the different studies, it is noteworthy that the male university population occupies a large part of the total sample. The predominance of male university students in the sample aligns with the objective of the study because men are generally more engaged in commercial video games, particularly in highly interactive genres (Gisbert-Pérez et al., 2024).

Analysis of video game types and genres used

The range of video games used across the studies reviewed reflects a diverse approach to examining the effects of different game types on metacognitive and cognitive skills. Popular sandbox and simulation games such as Minecraft and The Sims 4 were frequently employed due to their open-ended structures, which encourage problem-solving, strategic planning, and cognitive flexibility. Minecraft was notably used in studies by Piédalue (2022), focusing on executive functioning in children with ADHD, and by Shaw (2022), examining the relationships between cognitive abilities, personality traits, and creativity. Similarly, The Sims 4 was implemented by Wilson et al. (2022) and Wilson et al. (2023) to assess its effects on vocabulary acquisition, metacognitive awareness, and Business English learning, given its potential for simulating real-world scenarios that require both language skills and metacognitive regulation.

Other studies explored the impact of specific genres such as massively multiplayer online role-playing games (MMORPGs) and strategy games, as these genres are known for their emphasis on complex, multi-layered decision-making and social interactions. For example, Guild Wars 2 was used by Luang-Ng et al. (2022) to study vocabulary acquisition, highlighting how MMORPGs foster social and collaborative learning environments. Unreal Tournament 2004, used by Michael et al. (2012), allowed for the examination of error framing in a fast-paced gaming context, providing insight into motivational mechanisms and self-efficacy. Additionally, games with distinct educational and problem-solving elements, such as Logical Journey of the Zoombinis by Harris et al. (2008) and Pandemic by Prisms of Reality by Sobocinski et al. (2024), were chosen for their capacity to facilitate cognitive monitoring and metacognitive awareness. Even classic games such as FIFA (Imanian et al., 2024) and Pokémon Go (Valentine and Jensen, 2021) were analyzed for their unique contributions to cognitive skills such as attention, working memory, and spatial awareness. This variety of game types and genres highlights the tailored approach researchers take in selecting games that align with specific cognitive and metacognitive constructs under investigation, thus offering insights into how different gaming environments can enhance or influence metacognitive processes and learning outcomes.

Assessments instruments and tools

Some of the assessment tools used in each of the investigations were Sperling’s Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (Jr. MAI; Sperling et al., 2002) (Wilson et al., 2022; Ricker and Richert, 2021), General Mental Ability (GMA): Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM) (Raven et al., 1998; Michael et al., 2012), Alternative Uses Task (AUT; Guilford, 1967), Consensual Assessment Technique (CAT; Amabile, 1988, 1996), to assess Divergent Thinking Task (Shaw, 2022) as well as the modification of others (Hardy et al., 2015; Li et al., 2015) to adapt the procedure, methodology, sample, etc., to the object of research. In qualitative studies, there is an almost unique use of techniques such as interviews, field or reflective diaries (Lickteig, 2020; Monem, 2015), modeling, think-alouds, and recordings (Harris et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2009; Misak, 2018).

Main findings by study focus

Metacognitive awareness and self-regulation enhancement

Many studies demonstrated that video games effectively enhance metacognitive awareness, helping players become more conscious of their thought processes and learning strategies. Wilson et al. (2022) found that The Sims 4 facilitated significant gains in metacognitive awareness among undergraduate students, particularly in vocabulary acquisition, by requiring players to continuously monitor and adjust their in-game actions to achieve goals. Similarly, Piédalue (2022) reported that children with ADHD displayed improved metacognitive regulation when using Minecraft, as the game encouraged them to plan, track progress, and make strategic adjustments. These findings underscore the role of video games in fostering self-awareness and self-regulation—skills critical for effective learning in educational contexts.

Integration of metacognitive strategies within gameplay

Studies indicated that games designed with complex problem-solving or open-ended tasks promote active engagement with metacognitive strategies, such as planning, evaluating, and revising approaches. For instance, Kim et al. (2009) found that players using strategies such as thinking aloud and self-modeling in Gersang showed significant improvements in social problem-solving skills. This demonstrates how integrating explicit metacognitive strategies within gameplay can create an environment where players naturally engage in cognitive monitoring, which reinforces learning processes.

Decision-making and problem-solving as vehicles for metacognitive growth

Games that require players to make decisions in real time or solve multifaceted problems provide ideal conditions for practicing metacognitive regulation. Harris et al. (2008), who used the Logical Journey of the Zoombinis with children, observed that participants with mastery goals engaged more in reflective problem-solving discussions, enhancing metacognitive control. Additionally, studies on RPGs and MMORPGs, such as Monem (2015) and Luang-Ng et al. (2022), showed that these genres facilitate self-scaffolding as players manage complex in-game scenarios, make decisions under pressure, and seek resources collaboratively. Such dynamic problem-solving environments allow learners to apply and refine metacognitive skills, making them more adept at managing cognitive challenges in both gaming and educational settings.

Role of game difficulty and error feedback in metacognitive engagement

Research on game difficulty and error management suggests that carefully calibrated challenges and error feedback within games can foster metacognitive reflection and adaptation. Michael et al. (2012) highlighted that positive error framing in Unreal Tournament 2004 helped participants develop resilience and self-efficacy, key components of metacognitive regulation. The need to confront and learn from errors creates a cycle of self-monitoring and strategy adjustment, which mirrors effective learning practices. This controlled introduction of error-based learning within games can thus cultivate a mindset beneficial for academic learning, where resilience and adaptive thinking are essential.

Metacognitive benefits of collaborative gameplay

The social aspect of certain games enhances metacognitive engagement through peer interaction, feedback, and collective problem-solving. Luang-Ng et al. (2022) showed that Guild Wars 2 encouraged players to adopt metacognitive strategies through collaboration, with players gaining insights into their own learning processes while observing others’ strategies. This aligns with findings from Harris et al. (2008), where collaborative problem-solving in children led to higher levels of metacognitive engagement, particularly for those oriented toward mastery goals. These social dynamics promote reflective thinking, mutual regulation, and shared goal-setting, all of which are foundational metacognitive skills relevant to group-based academic tasks.

Transfer of metacognitive skills to academic contexts

Several studies suggest that video games serve as a bridge for transferring metacognitive skills into academic settings. For example, Tangkui and Keong (2023) demonstrated that students using Minecraft for fraction problem-solving developed higher order thinking skills that are directly applicable to classroom tasks. This transferability was also evident in studies using The Sims 4, where students not only improved vocabulary but also gained a heightened sense of self-regulation, aiding their learning in formal settings (Wilson et al., 2023). These findings emphasize that video games can be powerful tools for cultivating metacognitive skills with real-world educational applications, promoting strategic thinking, persistence, and adaptability.

Limitation of the studies

Regarding the limitations of the different studies, the most significant ones were found with respect to the measurement of the metacognitive construct (Hardy et al., 2015), due to the use of a type of questionnaire that attended more to metacognitive knowledge than to metacognitive regulation, the latter option being more relevant to the objective of the study. In this sense, there could be some mismatch in the results if the same psychometric items were applied to participants of different ages (Li et al., 2015). On the other hand, it is not possible to clarify, based on the results of the different studies, whether subjects with high metacognitive levels tend to choose a particular type of video game or, on the contrary, it is the video game itself that develops these metacognitive skills in the subject (Ricker and Richert, 2021). However, there seems to be a correlation between the type of video game chosen and the metacognitive level developed (Moncarz, 2011). Similarly, the authors acknowledge some sociodemographic limitations within their studies such as sex (Hardy et al., 2015; Hughes et al., 2013; Lickteig, 2020), sample representation (Harris et al., 2008; Monem, 2015; Ricker and Richert, 2021), or age (Harris et al., 2008; Hughes et al., 2013; Moncarz, 2011).

Finally, when considering future practical applications on the use of video games in formal educational contexts, the articles highlight the importance and difficulty of including these cultural tools in classrooms (Hughes et al., 2013; Monem, 2015). The causes of this reluctance could be explained by the teachers’ need for specific training in these tools, which are essential for them to be able to value the positive educational aspects of video games (Martín del Pozo et al., 2017; De Sousa and Rasmussen, 2019). In this way, methodological initiatives could be promoted where video games are the protagonists, with the aim of achieving both curricular objectives and the development of transversal cognitive and metacognitive competencies within their teaching practice (Kim et al., 2009; Lickteig, 2020). This prior training would also be useful to select which video game is the most appropriate to include in classrooms (Hughes et al., 2013; Lickteig, 2020), thus establishing a forecast on what cognitive or metacognitive abilities each one develops and thus making the best choice within the large existing offer (Moncarz, 2011; Ricker and Richert, 2021).

Discussion and conclusions

One of the primary outcomes of this systematic review is the identification of limited studies explicitly examining the relationship between commercial video games and metacognitive skill development, despite a growing body of research on video games in other educational contexts. This scarcity may stem from the challenges associated with integrating commercial games in educational settings (Marín-Díaz, 2019; Quesada and Tejedor, 2016) and the lack of consensus on defining and measuring metacognitive constructs (Gaviria, 2019; Huertas et al., 2014; Li et al., 2015; Mariano and González, 2020). However, the studies reviewed provide insights into how specific game genres and in-game features can stimulate metacognitive processes, offering clear but preliminary answers to the research questions.

For the first objective—understanding how commercial video games in education might foster metacognition—findings indicate that games facilitate a range of metacognitive activities, such as planning, self-monitoring, and evaluation (Wilson et al., 2022; Kim et al., 2009; Lickteig, 2020). Several studies focused on integrating metacognitive strategies such as reflective journaling, modeling, and verbalization (Kim et al., 2009; Li et al., 2015; Misak, 2018), while others found that orienting educational goals toward mastery supports deeper metacognitive engagement (Harris et al., 2008). Additionally, some researchers highlighted the impact of game genres and specific mechanics (e.g., RPGs, MMORPGs, and sandbox games) in promoting metacognitive skills through decision-making and collaborative problem-solving (Monem, 2015; Ricker and Richert, 2021). These findings suggest that well-chosen commercial games can support metacognitive development, though targeted research on genre-specific effects is needed to strengthen this conclusion.

In relation to the second objective—assessing whether commercial video games can cultivate both metacognitive and other cognitive skills—the evidence suggests that video games indeed support broader educational outcomes. Games were shown to enhance critical thinking, problem-solving, self-regulation, and engagement in learning tasks (Coutinho and Neuman, 2008; Kleitman and Gibson, 2011; Klimenko and Alvares, 2009; Schraw et al., 2006; Sungur, 2007). Specific examples, such as the increased self-monitoring and regulation observed in players of The Sims 4 and Minecraft (Wilson et al., 2022; Piédalue, 2022), align with Flavell’s (1979) definition of metacognition as active monitoring and control of cognition, suggesting that commercial games do not hinder but rather promote valuable learning and motivational benefits in educational contexts.

However, this review reveals notable limitations, as highlighted in some studies (Hughes et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2009; Ricker and Richert, 2021), including the lack of standardized psychometric tools for assessing metacognitive processes in gaming contexts. Although six major databases were searched, the few studies meeting the inclusion criteria reflect limited research on the impact of commercial video games on metacognition, suggesting a need for further exploration, potentially including doctoral dissertations and unpublished studies. Another limitation concerns the reliability of existing measures for metacognition, underscoring the importance of validating tools that capture metacognitive elements specific to interactive gaming environments.

In conclusion, the metacognitive dimension, with its potential to enhance educational outcomes, aligns well with the engaging and dynamic environments created by commercial video games. Integrating these tools could significantly support metacognitive development alongside traditional academic skills, advocating a pedagogical shift toward more reflective and controlled learning experiences (Osses Bustingorry and Jaramillo Mora, 2008; Roque-Herrera et al., 2018). To fully leverage this potential, further research is necessary to identify effective game genres, validate psychometric tools, and establish instructional frameworks that incorporate video games into curricula, ultimately fostering a participatory and self-regulated learning process that meets contemporary educational needs.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

MC-R: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JG-L: Investigation, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the Program “Stimulus to excellence for tenured university professors,” funded by the University of Alcalá de Henares and the Community of Madrid, EPU-INV/2020/005.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

RPG, Role-playing game.

References

Amabile, T. M. (1988). “A model of creativity and innovation in organizations” in Research in organizational behavior, vol. 10. eds. B. M. Staw and L. L. Cummings (Greenwich, CT: JAI Press), 123–167.

Google Scholar

Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: update to "the social psychology of creativity.". Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Google Scholar

Ames, M. G., and Burrell, J. (2017) ‘Connected learning’ and the equity agenda: a microsociology of Minecraft play. In Proceedings of the 2017 ACM conference on computer supported cooperative work and social computing, 446–457.

Google Scholar

Andrade, A. (2020). El juego y su importancia cultural en el aprendizaje de los niños en educación inicial. J. Sci. Res. 5, 132–149.

Google Scholar

Baker, L., and Brown, A. (1984). “Metacognitive skills and reading” in Handbook of Reading research. eds. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, and P. Mosenthal (New York, EE.UU: Longman), 353–394.

Google Scholar

Bell, B. S., and Kozlowski, W. J. (2002). Goal orientation and ability: Interactive effects on self-efficacy, performance, and knowledge. J. Appl. Psychol. 87:497. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.497

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bond, M., Buntins, K., Bedenlier, S., Zawacki-Richter, O., and Kerres, M. (2020). Mapping research in student engagement and educational technology in higher education: a systematic evidence map. Int. J. Educ. Technol. High. Educ. 17, 1–30. doi: 10.1186/s41239-019-0176-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, J., and Poon, W. (2015). Self-regulated learning strategies & academic achievement in online higher education learning environments: a systematic review. Internet High. Educ. 27, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2015.04.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. (1978). “Knowing when, where, and how to remember: a problem of metacognition” in Advances in instructional psychology. ed. R. Glaser (Hillsdale, Michigan: Erlbaum), 77–165.

Google Scholar

Bullard, J. (2013). Playfully serious information for serious play: the integration of community values in an information resource. iConference 2013 Proceedings (pp. 389–397).

Google Scholar

Cano-López, J. B., García-Sancho, E., Fernández-Castilla, B., and Salguero, J. M. (2022). Empirical evidence of the metacognitive model of rumination and depression in clinical and nonclinical samples: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cogn. Ther. Res. 46, 367–392. doi: 10.1007/s10608-021-10260-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, M. (2015). Emitexts and Paratexts: propaganda in EVE online. Games Culture 10, 311–342. doi: 10.1177/1555412014558089

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatzivasileiou, P., and Drigas, A. (2022). ICTs for the development of the cognitive and metacognitive abilities of the students with specific learning disorder in mathematics. Technium Soc. Sci. J. 31, 131–152. doi: 10.47577/tssj.v31i1.6484

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, T. M., Boyle, E. A., MacArthur, E., Hainey, T., and Boyle, J. M. (2012). A systematic literature review of empirical evidence on computer games and serious games. Comput. Educ. 59, 661–686. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.03.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coutinho, S., and Neuman, G. (2008). A model of metacognition, achievement goal orientation, learning style and self-efficacy. Learning Environ. Res. 11, 131–151. doi: 10.1007/s10984-008-9042-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Zurilla, T. J., Nezu, A. M., and Maydeu-Olivares, A. (2002). Social problem-solving inventory - revised (SPSI-R™): Manual. Washington, DC: APA PsycTests.

Google Scholar

De Sousa, F., and Rasmussen, I. (2019). Productive disciplinary engagement and videogames: a teacher’s educational design to engage students with ethical theories in citizenship education. Nordic J. Digit. Lit. 14, 99–116. doi: 10.18261/issn.1891-943x-2019-03-04-02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado, I. (2011). Juego infantil y su metodología. Paraninfo 2011.

Google Scholar

Diaz-López, A., Maquilón-Sánchez, J. J., and Mirete-Ruiz, A. B. (2023). Validation of the Ud-TIC scale on the problematic use of mobile phones and video games as mediators of social skills and academic performance. Rev. Esp. Pedagog. 80:11. doi: 10.22550/REP80-3-2022-06

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Donaldson, S. (2017). Mechanics and metagame: exploring binary expertise in league of legends. Games Cult. 12, 426–444. doi: 10.1177/1555412015590063

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dörrenbächer-Ulrich, L., Dilhuit, S., and Perels, F. (2024). Investigating the relationship between self-regulated learning, metacognition, and executive functions by focusing on academic transition phases: a systematic review. Curr. Psychol. 43, 16045–16072. doi: 10.1007/s12144-023-05551-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Efklides, A. (2008). Metacognition: defining its facets and levels of functioning in relation to self-regulation and co-regulation. Eur. Psychol. 13, 277–287. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.13.4.277

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Efklides, A. (2009). “The new look in metacognition: from individual to social, from cognitive to affective” in Metacognition: New research developments. ed. C. Larson (New York, EE.UU: Nova Science Publishers), 137–151.

Google Scholar

Erstad, O., Kjällander, S., and Järvelä, S. (2021). Facing the challenges of ‘digital competence’ a Nordic agenda for curriculum development for the 21st century. Nordic J. Digit. Lit. 16, 77–87. doi: 10.18261/issn.1891-943x-2021-02-04

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Erstad, O., and Voogt, J. (2018). “The twenty-first century curriculum: issues and challenges” in Second handbook of information technology in primary and secondary education. eds. J. Voogt, G. Knezek, R. Christensen, and K. Lai (Berlin: Springer), 19–36.

Google Scholar

Fernández Velasco, P., and Loev, S. (2024). Metacognitive feelings: a predictive-processing perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1–23. doi: 10.1177/17456916231221976

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive—developmental inquiry. Am. Psychol. 34, 906–911. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, S. M. (2024). Metacognition and confidence: a review and synthesis. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 75, 241–268. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-022423-032425

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ford, J., Smith, E., Weissbein, D., Gully, S., and Salas, E. (1998). Relationships of goal orientation, metacognitive activity, and practice strategies with learning outcomes and transfer. J. Appl. Psychol. 83, 218–233. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.83.2.218

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frey, M. C. (2019). What we know, are still getting wrong, and have yet to learn about the relationships among the sat, intelligence and achievement. J. Intell. 7:26. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence7040026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandini, F. (2018). Metacognición y aprendizaje. In: A.M. Palacios, M.A. Pedragosa y M. Querejeta (Coords.). Encuentro en la encrucijada: Psicología, Cultura y Educación. La Plata: EDULP. (Libros de Cátedra. Sociales). En Memoria Académica. Available at: https://www.memoria.fahce.unlp.edu.ar/libros/pm.4860/pm.4860.pdf

Google Scholar

García Madruga, J. A., Gutierrez Martinez, F., and Carriedo López, N. (2002). “El desarrollo de los conocimientos y habilidades metacognitivas” in Psicología Evolutiva II. Desarrollo Cognitivo y lingüístico. Volumen 1 (Madrid: Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia).

Google Scholar

Garcia Martinez, S. (2014). Using commercial games to support teaching in higher education (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Quebec, Canada: Concordia University.

Google Scholar

Gauvain, M. (2020). Vigotsky on learning and development. In Developmental Psychology: Revisiting the classic studies, slater, M., y Quinn, P. Chapter 6, 89–101. Editorial sage. 2° Edición.

Google Scholar

Gaviria, C. (2019). Pensar la historia con el deseo: metacognición, motivación y comprensión histórica. Rev. Col. Psicol. 28, 147–164. doi: 10.15446/rcp.v28n1.70763

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gee, J. P. (2007). Good video games + good learning: collected essays on video games, learning and literacy: 27 (new literacies and digital epistemologies). Peter Lang Publishing Inc (Eds).

Google Scholar

Gee, J. P. (2011). “Reflections on empirical evidence on games and learning” in Computer games and instruction. eds. S. Tobias and J. Fletcher (Greenwich, GB: Information Age Publishing), 223–232.

Google Scholar

Gisbert-Pérez, M., López-Fernández, O., and Griffiths, M. D. (2024). Gender differences in internet gaming among university students: a discriminant analysis. Front. Psychol. 15, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1412739

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., and Schneider, W. (2006). Perceptual knowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions. J. Neurosci. 26, 4917–4921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5389-05.2006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, D. A., and Berg, E. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 404–411. doi: 10.1037/h0059831

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1967). Creativity: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. J. Creative Behav. 1, 3–14. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1967.tb00002.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadwin, A., and Järvellä, S. (2011). Social aspects of self-regulated learning: where social and self-meet in the strategic – regulation of learning. Teach. Coll. Rec. 113, 235–239. doi: 10.1177/016146811111300201

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J., Stickler, U., Herodotou, C., and Iacovides, I. (2020). Player conceptualizations of creativity in digital entertainment games. Convergence 26, 1226–1247. doi: 10.1177/1354856519880791

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, J. L., Drescher, D., Sarkar, K., Kellett, G., and Scanlon, M. (2011). Enhancing visual attention and working memory with a web-based cognitive training program. Mensa Res. J. 42, 13–20.

Google Scholar

Hardy, J. L., Nelson, R. A., Thomason, M. E., Stern, E., and Sable, J. J. (2015). Enhancing cognitive abilities with comprehensive training: A large, online, randomized, active-controlled trial. PLOS One 13:e0204667. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134467

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, A., Yuill, N., and Luckin, R. (2008). The influence of context-specific and dispositional achievement goals on children’s paired collaborative interaction. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 78, 355–374. doi: 10.1348/000709907X267067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, L. (2005). Video games: a significant cognitive artifact of contemporary youth culture. DiGRA Proceedings, 2, 1–14.

Google Scholar

Hu, W., and Luo, Y. (2024). Chinese English language learners’ vocabulary retention: investigating the effectiveness of neuro/metacognitive and socio-cultural strategies. BMC Psychol. 12:113. doi: 10.1186/s40359-024-01612-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huertas, P., Vesga, G., and Galindo, M. (2014). Validación del intrumento “inventario de habilidades metacognitivas (MAI) con estudiantes colombianos. Praxis Saber 5, 55–74. doi: 10.19053/22160159.3022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, M., Day, E., Wang, X., Schuelke, M., Arsenault, M., Harkrider, L., et al. (2013). Learner-controlled practice difficulty in the training of a complex task cognitive and motivational mechanisms. J. Appl. Psychol. 98, 80–98. doi: 10.1037/a0029821

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutton, B., Salanti, G., Caldwell, D. M., Chaimani, A., Schmid, C. H., Cameron, C., et al. (2015). The PRISMA extension statement for reporting of systematic reviews incorporating network meta-analyses of health care interventions: checklist and explanations. Ann. Intern. Med. 162, 777–784. doi: 10.7326/M14-2385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Imanian, M., Khatibi, A., Heydarinejad, S., Saemi, E., and Veisia, E. (2024). Effects of electronic sports on the cognitive skills of attention, working memory, and cognitive flexibility : Research Square.

Google Scholar

Kahila, J., Tedre, M., Kahila, S., Vartianen, H., Valtonen, T., and Mäkitalo, K. (2020). Children’s gaming involves much more than the gaming itself: a study of the metagame among 12 to 15 year old children. Convergence, 20, 1–19. doi: 10.1177/1354856520979482

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahila, J., Valtonen, T., López-Pernas, S., Saqr, M., Vartiainen, H., Kahila, S., et al. (2023). A typology of metagamers: identifying player types based on beyond the game activities. Games Cult. 20, 38–58. doi: 10.1177/15554120231187758

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapp, F., Spangenberg, P., Kruse, L., and Narciss, S. (2019). Investigatin changes in self-evaluation of technical competences in the serious games Serena Supergreen: findings, challenges, and lesson learned. Metacogn. Learn. 14, 387–411. doi: 10.1007/s11409-019-09209-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kickmeier-Rust, M. D., and Albert, D. (2012). Educationally adaptive: balancing serious games. Int. J. Comput. Sci. Sport 11, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Kim, B., Park, H., and Baek, Y. (2009). Not just fun, but serious strategies: using meta-cognitive strategies in game-based learning. Comput. Educ. 52, 800–810. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2008.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleitman, S., and Gibson, J. (2011). Metacognitive beliefs, self-confidence and primary learning environment of sixth grade students. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 728–735. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.08.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimenko, O., and Alvares, J. (2009). Aprender cómo aprendo: la enseñanza de estrategias metacognitivas. Educ. Educ. 12, 11–28.

Google Scholar

Levis, D. (2013). Los videojuegos, un fenómeno de masas. Sivel.

Google Scholar

Li, W., Liu, C. Y., and Tseng, J. C. (2024). Development of a metacognitive regulation-based collaborative programming system and its effects on students' learning achievements, computational thinking tendency and group metacognition. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 55, 318–339. doi: 10.1111/bjet.13358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Zhang, B., Du, H., Zhu, Z., and Ming-Li, Y. (2015). Metacognitive planning: development and validation of an online measure. Psychol. Assess. 27, 260–271. doi: 10.1037/pas0000019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberati, A., Altman, D., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P., Ioannidis, J., et al. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and Meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med. 6:e1000100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lickteig, S. J. (2020). Metagaming: Cognition in gaming environments and systems. [Tesis de doctorado, Kansas State University]. Repositorio Institucional Universidad Kansas State University.

Google Scholar

Luang-Ng, L., Sharieful-Azizie, R., and Yi-Chew, S. (2022). Factors influencing ESL players’ use of vocabulary learning strategies in massively multiplayer online role-playing games (MMORPG). Asia Pac. Educ. Res. 31, 369–381. doi: 10.1007/s40299-021-00578-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahayanti, N. W. S., Kusuma, I. P. I., and Wibawa, S. (2020). Digital game-based learning in EFL: its effect on young. Asian ESP J. 16, 5–30.

Google Scholar

Mariano, L., and González, F. (2020). Metacognición aplicada: Estrategias para aprender a aprender. Barcelona: Editorial Octaedro.

Google Scholar

Marín-Díaz, V. (2019). ¿El poder de la gamificación educativa? Rev. Educ. Mediá. TIC 7, I–IV. doi: 10.21071/edmetic.v7i2.11146

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín del Pozo, M., Basilotta Gómez-Pablos, V., and García-Valcárcel Muñoz-Repiso, A. (2017). A quantitative approach to pre-service primary school teachers’ attitudes towards collaborative learning with video games: previous experience with video games can make the difference. Int. J. Educ. Technol. High. Educ. 14:11. doi: 10.1186/s41239-017-0050-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayor, J., Suengas, A., and González-Marqués, J. (1993). Estrategias Metacognitivas. Aprender a aprender y aprender a pensar. Síntesis Psicología.

Google Scholar

McCaslin, M., and Hickey, D. T. (2001). “Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: a Vygotskian view” in Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives. eds. B. Zimmerman and D. Schunk. 2nd ed. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 227–252.

Google Scholar

Medina, I., Palacios, R., and Dejo, L. (2020). La revisión sistemática de literatura como evidencia del desempeño de competencias profesionales. Rev. Epistemia 5.

Google Scholar

Michael, J., Ford, P., and Kozlowski, S. W. (2012). Error framing and learner-controlled practice in training. Hum. Factors 54, 56–67.

Google Scholar

Misak, J. (2018). A virtual bridge not too far: teaching narrative sense of place with virtual reality. Comput. Compos. 50, 39–52. doi: 10.1016/j.compcom.2018.07.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitsea, E., Drigas, A., and Skianis, C. (2024). Well-being technologies and positive psychology strategies for training metacognition, emotional intelligence and motivation Meta-skills in clinical populations: a systematic review. Psych 6, 305–344. doi: 10.3390/psych6010019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moncarz, H. (2011). The relationship between playing games and metacognitive awareness [Tesis de doctorado, George Mason University]. Repositorio Educational Resources Information Center.

Google Scholar

Monem, R. (2015). Metacognition and self-scaffolding in MMORPGs: a case study of an adolescent male gamer. Qual. Rep. 20, 454–465. doi: 10.46743/2160-3715/2015.2121

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero, B. (2017). Aplicación de juegos didácticos como metodología de enseñanza: Una revisión de la literatura. Experiencias docentes 1, 75–92.

Google Scholar

Naeimi, S., and Rafezi, Z. (2023). The role of mindfulness, metacognitive beliefs and self-control in predicting job satisfaction of Tehran airlines employees. Psychol. Res. Manage. 9, 99–118. doi: 10.22034/jom.2023.703855

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osses Bustingorry, S., and Jaramillo Mora, S. (2008). Metacognición: un camino para aprender a aprender. Estud. Pedagóg. 34, 187–197. doi: 10.4067/S0718-07052008000100011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouellette, M. (2019). I’m controlling and composing: the role of metacognition in the incredible machine. Invisible Culture 30, 1–22. doi: 10.47761/494a02f6.a04844c6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M., McKenzie, J., Bossuyt, P., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2020). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. bmj. 372. doi: 10.1186/s13643-021-01626-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Páramo, P. (2020). Cómo elaborar una revisión sistemática. Universidad Pedagógica Nacional. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.31465.85608

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pegalajar Palomino, M. C. (2021). Implicaciones de la gamificación en Educación Superior: una revisión sistemática sobre la percepción del estudiante. Rev. Investig. Educ. 39, 169–188. doi: 10.6018/rie.419481

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez, G., and González, L. (2020). Una posible definición de metacognición para la enseñanza de las ciencias. Investig. Ensino Ciênc. 25, 384–404. doi: 10.22600/1518-8795.ienci2020v25n1p384

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pertegal-Vega, M. Á., Oliva-Delgado, A., and Rodriguez-Meirinhos, A. (2019). Systematic review of the current state of research on online social networks: taxonomy on experience of use. Comunicar 27, 81–90. doi: 10.3916/C60-2019-08

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piédalue, C. (2022). Assessment of executive functioning difficulties among ADHD children through task performance in Minecraft (Master's thesis, Queen's University (Canada).

Google Scholar

Pillay, H., Brownlee, J., and Wilss, L. (1999). Cognition and recreational computer games: implications for educational technology. J. Res. Comput. Educ. 32, 203–216. doi: 10.1080/08886504.1999.10782624

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P., Wolters, C., and Baxter, G. (2000). “Assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning” in Issues in the measurement of metacognition. eds. G. Schraw and J. Impara (Taylor & Francis, Ltd, Buros Institute of Mental Measurements), 43–97.

Google Scholar

Prendes Espinosa, M. P., Martínez Sánchez, F., and Gutiérrez Porlán, I. (2017). Competencia digital: una necesidad del profesorado universitario en el siglo XXI. Rev. Educ. Distancia. 2:22. doi: 10.6018/red/56/7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quesada, A., and Tejedor, S. (2016). Aplicaciones educativas de los videojuegos: el Caso de world of Warcraft. Rev. Medios Educ. 48, 187–196. doi: 10.12795/pixelbit.2015.i48.12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, B. H., Schwarzwald, J., and Koslowsky, M. (1998). Conceptualizing and measuring a power/interaction model of interpersonal influence. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 307–332. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01708.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricker, A., and Richert, A. (2021). Digital gaming and metacognition in middle childhood. Comput. Human Behav. 115:106593. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106593

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roque-Herrera, Y., Valdivia, P., Alonso, S., and Zagalaz, M. (2018). Metacognición y aprendizaje autónomo en la educación superior. Educ. Méd. Sup., 32, 293–302.

Google Scholar

Sánchez-Meca, J. (2010). Cómo realizar una revisión sistemática y un meta-análisis. Aula Abierta 38, 53–64. Available at: https://www.um.es/metaanalysis/pdf/5030.pdf

Google Scholar

Sánchez-Rodríguez, P. A., Alfageme-González, M. B., and Serrano-Pastor, F. J. (2010). Aspectos sociales de los videojuegos. Rev. Latinoam. Tecnol. Educ. 9:4352.

Google Scholar

Schrader, P. G., Carroll, M. C., McCreery, M. P., and Head, D. L. (2020). Mixed methods for human–computer interactions research: An iterative study using Reddit and social media. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 58, 818–841. doi: 10.1177/0735633119878066

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schraw, G., Crippen, K., and Hartley, K. (2006). Promoting self-regulation in science education: metacognition as part of a broader perspective on learning. Res. Sci. Educ. 36, 111–139. doi: 10.1007/s11165-005-3917-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schraw, G., and Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 19, 460–475. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1994.1033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schunk, D. (2001). “Social cognitive theory and self-regulation” in Selfregulated learning and academic achievement. eds. B. Zimmerman and D. Schunk (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).

Google Scholar

Shaw, J. (2022). Cognitive predictors of creativity: exploring relationships in minecraft. Creat. Res. J. 34, 15–29. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2022.2061172

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, C. A., and Muis, K. R. (2024). An integrated model of socially shared regulation of learning: the role of metacognition, affect, and motivation. Educ. Psychol. 1-18. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2023.2294881

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobocinski, M., Dever, D., Wiedbusch, M., Mubarak, F., Azevedo, R., and Järvelä, S. (2024). Capturing self-regulated learning processes in virtual reality: causal sequencing of multimodal data. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 55, 1486–1506. doi: 10.1111/bjet.13393

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sourmelis, T., Ioannou, A., and Zaphiris, P. (2017). Massively multiplayer online role playing games (MMORPGs) and the 21st century skills: a comprehensive research review from 2010 to 2016. Comput. Hum. Behav. 67, 41–48. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.10.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Howard, L. H., Miller, L. A., and Murphy, C. (2002). Measures of children’s knowledge and regulation of cognition. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 27, 51–79. doi: 10.1006/ceps.2001.1091

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, K. (2011). Video games and learning: Teaching and participatory culture in the digital age. Cambridge, MA: Teachers College Press.

Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18:643. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, M., Wang, M., Wegerif, R., and Peng, J. (2022). How do students generate ideas together in scientific creativity tasks through computer-based mind mapping? Comput. Educ. 176:104359. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2021.104359

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sungur, S. (2007). Modeling the relationships among students’ motivational beliefs, metacognitive strategy use, and effort regulation. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 51, 315–326. doi: 10.1080/00313830701356166

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, W., Supian, N., and Cheah, K. S. (2022) Game-based learning in improving English vocabulary and detecting metacognitive awareness among English for specific purposes undergraduates. In 2nd international conference on social science, humanities, education and society development (ICONS 2021), Atlantis Press, pp. 135–140.

Google Scholar

Tangkui, R. B., and Keong, T. C. (2023). The effect of Minecraft on Learners' higher-order thinking skills in fractional problem-solving. Anatol. J. Educ. 8, 1–20. doi: 10.29333/aje.2023.821a

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taub, M., Zevedo, R., Bradbury, A., Millar, G., and Lester, J. (2018). Using sequence mining to reveal the efficiency in scientific reasoning during STEM learning with a game-based learning environment. Learn. Instr. 54, 93–103. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.08.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Teng, M. F. (2024). Metacognition and autonomy in building a community for language learning through VR digital gaming. Comput. Educ. X Reality 4, 100060–106780. doi: 10.1016/j.cexr.2024.100060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tokarieva, A. V., Volkova, N. P., Harkusha, I. V., and Soloviev, V. N. (2019). Educational digital games: models and implementation. Educ. Dimension 1, 5–26. doi: 10.31812/educdim.v53i1.3872

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, A., Ramírez, M., and Romero, L. (2019). Valoración y evaluación de los Aprendizajes Basados en Juegos (GBL) en contextos e-learning. Educ. Knowl. Soc. 19, 109–128. doi: 10.14201/eks2018194109128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Urrútia, G., and Bonfill, X. (2010). Declaración PRISMA: una propuesta para mejorar la publicación de revisiones sistemáticas y metaanálisis. Med. Clin. 135, 507–511. doi: 10.1016/j.medcli.2010.01.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valencia-Molina, T., Serna-Collazos, A., Ochoa-Angrino, S., Caicedo-Tamayo, A., Montes-González, J., and Chávez-Vescance, J. (2017). Competencias y estándares TIC desde la dimensión pedagógica: una perspectiva desde los niveles de apropiación de las TIC en la práctica educativa docente. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana.

Google Scholar

Valentine, K. D., and Jensen, L. J. (2021). Mobile entanglements and communitas: the embodied nature of play in Pokémon Go. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 69, 1955–1985. doi: 10.1007/s11423-020-09930-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlachopoulos, D., and Makri, A. (2017). The effect of games and simulations on higher education: a systematic literature review. Int. J. Educ. Technol. High. Educ. 14:22. doi: 10.1186/s41239-017-0062-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Gillern, S., and Stufft, C. (2023). Multimodality, learning and decision-making: children's metacognitive reflections on their engagement with video games as interactive texts. Literacy 57, 3–16. doi: 10.1111/lit.12304

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, A. M., and Greiff, S. (2023). ICT skills in the deployment of 21st century skills: a (cognitive) developmental perspective through early childhood. Appl. Sci. 13:4615. doi: 10.3390/app13074615

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A. (2009). Metacognitive therapy for anxiety and depression. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Wilson, R. J., Hannon, D., and Martens, R. (2022). Enhancing metacognitive awareness in ESP learners through gameplay. J. Lang. Teach. Res. 10, 285–300.

Google Scholar

Wilson, T., Supian, N., and Sung, C. K. (2023). Using game-based learning in developing metacognition among ESP students: a case study. Asian J. Univ. Educ. 19, 506–518. doi: 10.24191/ajue.v19i3.23495

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, M., and Baek, J. (2018). Development and application of the steam education based on the soccer robot for elementary students. Int. J. Mob. Blended Learn. 10, 11–22. doi: 10.4018/IJMBL.2018070102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X. (2024) Exploring cognitive and language acquisition processes in second language learning of English. In 3rd international conference on education, language and art (ICELA 2023), Atlantis Press, pp. 248–254. doi: 10.2991/978-2-38476-214-9_32

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, G., Zheng, J., Ratner, K., Li, Q., Estevez, M., and Burrow, A. L. (2024). How trait and state positive emotions, negative emotions, and self-regulation relate to adolescents' perceived daily learning progress. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 77:102275. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102275

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. (2000). “Attaining self-regulation: a social cognitive perspective” in Handbook of self-regulation. eds. M. Boekaerts, M. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 13–39.

Google Scholar

Zimmerman, E. (2008). “Gaming literacy: Game design as a model for literacy in the twenty-first century” in The video game theory reader (vol. 2). eds. B. Perron and Y. M. Wolf, 23–33.

Google Scholar

Zumbach, J., Rammerstorfer, L., and Deibl, I. (2020). Cognitive and metacognitive support in learning with a serious game about demographic change. Comput. Human Behav. 103, 120–129. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: commercial video games, 21st century skills, metacognition, systematic review, education

Citation: Checa-Romero M and Gimenez-Lozano JM (2025) Video games and metacognition in the classroom for the development of 21st century skills: a systematic review. Front. Educ. 9:1485098. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1485098

Received: 23 August 2024; Accepted: 20 December 2024;
Published: 24 January 2025.

Edited by:

Pedro Román-Graván, University of Seville, Spain

Reviewed by:

Elizabeth A. Boyle, University of the West of Scotland, United Kingdom
Jesús Valverde-Berrocoso, University of Extremadura, Spain

Copyright © 2025 Checa-Romero and Gimenez-Lozano. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mirian Checa-Romero, bS5jaGVjYUB1YWguZXM=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.