Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 13 April 2022
Sec. STEM Education
This article is part of the Research Topic Future-Oriented Science Education for Agency and Sustainable Development View all 11 articles

The Connection of Finns’ Environmental Awareness to Their Anticipatory Competence

  • 1Faculty of Education, University of Lapland, Rovaniemi, Finland
  • 2Department of Sustainability Science, LUT University, Lahti, Finland

Knowledge of people’s abilities must be adapted to a new, sustainable society. Through sustainability competences, the necessary changes in people’s behavior in the pursuit of a sustainable society can be intensified. In this study, Finns (n = 2006) express their knowledge of climate change and biodiversity loss and evaluate their own anticipatory competence. The connection between their environmental awareness and the future’s orientation toward a society of sustainable actions will be studied by statistical analysis. The study discusses how learning sustainability competences can be promoted in science education and reveals the gap between females and males in their objectives for a sustainable future. Finns with higher education have greater environmental awareness than those with lower education. The connection between Finns’ environmental awareness and their structural skills for making a more sustainable future is quite clear.

Introduction

To make modern society more sustainable in the future, we need well-educated citizens to take the necessary actions to make changes. This does not merely require enhancing their knowledge; their awareness and commitment to solving these problems must be developed as well. The awareness concept is ultimately a stimulus, and the driving force to acquire knowledge is the need to solve problems. Environmental awareness is one of the prerequisites for an environmental attitude and pro-environmental behavior. Many studies have confirmed that behavioral change may be caused by activities with the objective of raising awareness (Halady and Rao, 2010; Swaim et al., 2014; Ojala and Bengtsson, 2019). Quite a long time ago, Hungerford and Volk (1990) pointed out that environmental education would create awareness and foster the necessary attitudes and behaviors for change. Here, we can assume, based on earlier research (e.g., Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Zsóka et al., 2013; Ratinen and Uusiautti, 2020), that changes in behavior are brought about by increasing scientific knowledge through raising awareness regarding climate change and biodiversity loss and by fostering an appropriate attitude toward the future. Moreover, we propose that individuals review their knowledge and anticipatory competence and then decide whether they have enough knowledge and awareness in the first place.

We need to prepare learners for their future and consider how we can support them in creating a more sustainable future instead of an uncertain future. For this purpose, Wiek et al. (2011) defined key competencies in sustainability and synthesized the substantive contributions in a coherent framework of sustainability research and problem-solving competence: system thinking competence, anticipatory competence, normative competence, strategic competence, and interpersonal competence.

In this study, we focus on anticipatory competence and define that competence as learners’ ability to collectively analyze, evaluate, and illustrate the future as it is related to sustainability issues and sustainability problem-solving frameworks. Anticipatory competence includes comparative skills related to estimating the “state of the art,” including concepts such as time, uncertainty, and scenarios. It is well known that without appropriate knowledge, learners are not capable of undertaking meaningful and effective environmental actions that increase their hope for the future (Ratinen, 2021). Therefore, we also focus on environmental awareness based on Finns’ knowledge of climate change and biodiversity issues.

Science education is one of the keys to helping humankind solve the environmental problems, such as climate change and loss of biodiversity, that we are facing today. Science education focuses primarily on teaching knowledge and skills. Although socio-scientific issues (SSI) (Mogensen and Schnack, 2010) and responsible research (RRI) (Heras and Ruiz-Mallén, 2017) have been developed to foster the learning of environmental issues, it is more common that environmental education stresses the incorporation of values and changing behaviors in education (Wals et al., 2014). Responsible science education needs to support learners’ meaning-making coping strategies and thus prevent their environmental anxiety (Ojala and Bengtsson, 2019). In science education, there is a need to consider the means to make science classes more meaningful for learners and bring examples of their everyday lives near—both now and in the future. Science education across the globe holds the responsibility for shaping Students’ environmental awareness and changing their attitudes toward the importance of preserving the environment.

Science education has recently focused more on Students’ future thinking (Branchetti et al., 2018). Levrini et al. (2021) found that environmentally oriented science education can support learners’ future−scaffolding skills. These skills consist of structural skills and dynamical skills. Structural skills refer to learners’ abilities to recognize temporal, logical, and causal relationships and build systemic views. Dynamical skills are, in turn, learners’ abilities to navigate scenarios, relating local details to global views, past to present and future, and individual to collective actions. Science education helps people to perceive future global multiple scenarios that they can influence and shape in the present while using that knowledge for the future (Rickards et al., 2014). This anticipatory competence leans on interdisciplinary future studies, which typically involves disciplines but also scholars and practitioners from the arts, social sciences, natural sciences, technology, and engineering to orient actions in the present that can influence and create preferable or desirable futures. Levrini et al. (2021) incorporated future thinking skills into school science, including scenario thinking, systems thinking, thinking beyond the realm of possibilities, action competence, and skills to manage uncertainty and complexity, and thus brought future thinking close to the principles of sustainability thinking (Wiek et al., 2011; Sterling, 2021).

Environmental Awareness and Anticipatory Competence Among Finns

Learning sustainable development competencies requires a systematic focus on the concepts, methods, and skills of each competence (Brundiers et al., 2021). In addition, competence learning should focus on teaching staff so that awareness and understanding of competencies can be brought into general education for sustainable development. In Finland, sustainability competencies are not well known from the point of view of science education, although environmental crises have been known for a long time. Vuorio et al. (2021) found that university teachers in chemistry evaluated the learning of critical thinking skills as important in teaching. Moreover, promoting the competencies of sustainable development was evaluated as important. Tolppanen and Kärkkäinen (2021a) found that few pre-service teachers seem to examine climate change mitigation through a systems-thinking approach. Pre-service teachers did not internalize that individuals, governments, and businesses all play a role in climate change mitigation.

According to Lehtonen et al. (2020), Finns’ average knowledge about climate change is rather good, and they can make a realistic assessment of their own level of knowledge. Most Finnish people believe estimate that climate change is mainly due to increased carbon dioxide in the atmosphere. Finns are also able to link the increased amount of carbon dioxide to the use of fossil fuels. However, there is also misunderstanding of the causes of sustainability issues among Finnish people (Lehtonen et al., 2020). Namely, only about half know that deforestation is not the main cause of climate change and that climate change is not caused by ozone depletion. A closer look at the results of Lehtonen et al. (2020) reveals that 57% of men and 42% of women have good or very good knowledge of climate issues based on knowledge-based questions. The differences between age groups are not large. The knowledge of climate change issues of those under the age of 50 is better than that of older age groups. However, the knowledge of those under 30 is no different from that of others under 50.

Finns’ environmental awareness is relatively high, but knowledge moves slowly from words to deeds (Hyry, 2017). More recently, Finns expressed that recycling is the most effective way to reduce one’s own emissions, even though its impact is actually quite small (Lehtonen et al., 2020). Therefore, there are shortcomings in Finns’ basic information. In Finland, women’s attitudes toward the environment are more positive than men’s. The general environmental attitudes of young people appear to be more negative than those of older respondents (Hyry, 2017). The general environmental attitudes of respondents who are dissatisfied with their lives or who do not perceive their lives as valuable are clearly more negative than those respondents who say they are happy with their current lives or feel their lives are valuable.

Finns’ anticipatory competence related to sustainability issues is not well known. Heikkilä et al. (2017) found that some young people criticize the Western consumption-oriented lifestyle and express a desire for fairness in income and lifestyle in the future. Working together and communality come to the fore as important issues for these individuals. On the technology side, young people being connected in advocating for and promoting environmentally friendly and sustainable energy solutions is expected to become more common. Adults’ anticipatory competence is also unknown. Eurobarometer (2021) revealed that climate change and environmental issues are one of the EU’s main challenges for the future among European people, but our opinions varied significantly across EU member states.

Aims

Wiek et al. (2011) and colleagues believe that defining the key competencies required for sustainable development is important to profile and assess the right kind of competence. Although we know about Finns’ environmental expertise, we still have gaps in our knowledge of how Finns’ knowledge is combined with their anticipatory competence and how that knowledge could be used in the development of science education. This study is based on the following research questions:

• How does Finns’ knowledge of climate change and biodiversity loss depict their environmental awareness?

• How is Finns’ environmental awareness associated with their anticipatory competence?

The purpose of the present article is to increase our understanding of the relationship between Finns’ environmental awareness and their anticipatory competence. Based on the results, novel and more effective ways to respond to sustainability challenges in science education are presented.

Materials and Methods

Procedure and Participation

The target group consisted of 2,006 Finnish people living in Finland, Åland excluded. Åland is a Swedish-speaking autonomous region belonging to Finland. The survey was only in Finnish, so Åland was excluded from the survey. The average age was 47.8 years, and the sample was composed of 52.1% females and 47.5% males. Nine respondents (0.4%) did not want to express their gender (Table 1). There was no missing data because answering the questionnaire required an expression of opinion on each question. However, two participants did not inform their age and 24 participants their education as well. Nine participants did not identify their gender. All those missing participants were excluded in the analysis because the small number does not allow for comparisons with the rest of the participants. The data collection was carried out as a web survey tool developed by Feedback Group. Web consumer research panels of the CPX (Cint Panel Exchange) network were used for the target group definition. Respondents were selected from several different research panels, thus preventing a possible panel-specific structural skew. Respondents are recruited to various web panels using a registration form that asks the panelist their background information. Based on these backgrounds, respondents can be queried and quota-selected. Upon registration, the panelist also agrees that research invitations may be sent to his or her email. Thus, at the beginning of an individual study, consent to the study is no longer specifically requested, as the panelist has already given his or her consent once. Respondents were determined at the sampling stage based on the demographic structure of Finland. E-mail invitations to the survey were sent to all panelists who participated in the target group selection. During the data collection, additional invitations and reminders were sent to those who did not respond.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Respondents’ backgrounds.

Measures and Statistical Tests

To measure Finns’ environmental awareness, the participants were asked to evaluate climate change and biodiversity issues, and 10 possible responses were provided (Table 2). Each response could be rated on a five−point Likert scale: strongly disagree = 1, disagree = 2, no disagreement or agreement = 3, agree = 4, or strongly agree = 5. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to look for differences between groups by age and education level and t-test for gender. Principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted for the calculation of the principal scores using a regression method. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) value of 0.861 showed that the sample was suitable for performing PCA, and a varimax rotation method was chosen. The principal component solution accounted for 56.8% of the total variance, and the factor loadings were satisfactory (0.50 or greater) (Table 3). Finally, two scales were created: understanding (α = 0.79) and misunderstanding (α = 0.76). The principal component scores were calculated using regression methods. These scores were used for the calculation of Pearson correlation coefficients.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Finnish people’s environmental awareness (n = 2006).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Finnish people’s awareness of environmental issues (n = 2006).

In this study, we define competence as a combination of skills, knowledge, and attitudes that enable a particular task to be performed or a problem to be solved (Baartman et al., 2007; Wiek et al., 2011; Voogt and Roblin, 2012). Brundiers et al. (2021) updated Wiek et al. (2011) model. In our questionnaire, the competencies to be added to the original model were an integrated problem-solving competency that included the utilization of combinations of the competencies in the model. Our questionnaire involved identifying and leveraging the necessary problem-solving skills. Another competence to be added was intrapersonal competence. This is described as the ability to be aware of one’s own feelings, desires, thoughts, behaviors, and personality, as well as the ability to regulate, motivate, and develop oneself. The third modified competence in our questionnaire was solution competence, which refers to the collective ability to put plans and visions into practice and to understand the long-term and iterative nature of sustainable development projects.

To measure Finns’ anticipatory competence, the participants were asked to evaluate their anticipatory competence, and nine possible responses were provided (Table 4). Each response could be rated on a five−point Likert scale: strongly disagree = 1, disagree = 2, no disagreement or agreement = 3, agree = 4, or strongly agree = 5. Principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted for the calculation of the principal scores using a regression method. The KMO value was 0.847, and a varimax rotation method was chosen. The total explanation of variance was 61.4%, and the factor loadings were satisfactory (0.50 or greater) (Table 4). Finally, two scales were created: structural skills (α = 0.81) and dynamic skills (α = 0.83). The principal component scores were calculated using regression methods. These scores were used for the calculation of Pearson correlation coefficients.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Finnish people’s anticipatory competence (n = 2006).

Results

In Finland, citizens’ environmental awareness seems to be relatively high (Table 2). Both the understanding of the main cause of climate change as greenhouse gases (73.9% agree or totally agree with the statement) and the main roots of biodiversity loss, namely our large-scale exploitation of nature (80.2%), are scientifically correct. The Finns’ ideas that CO2 emissions are released from burning fossil fuels (70.2%) and that nature loss is caused by the environmental impact of production chains (62.4%) indicate high environmental awareness. Finnish people are also confident that scientists are sure that people are the reason for climate change (68.8%). However, this study, like earlier studies (Ratinen, 2016; Lehtonen et al., 2020), shows that 71.1% of respondents confused climate change with the depletion of the ozone layer. Scientifically, the connection between climate change and ozone depletion is not strong (IPCC, 2007). It is also interesting that many Finns (24.3%) think that even if we, as individuals, were to significantly reduce material consumption, it would have no effect on biodiversity.

According to ANOVA, only one variable— even if we, as individuals, were to significantly reduce material consumption, it would have no effect on biodiversity—differed statistically significantly by age [F(5,1,956) = 3.940, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.01]. The effect size for this analysis (η2 = 0.01) was found to approach Cohen’s (1988) convention for a small effect. A post-hoc test (Bonferroni) revealed that the groups from 55 to 65 (p < 0.003) and over 65 (p < 0.05) years old believed more than 16–24-year-olds that individuals can minimize biodiversity loss by de-creasing consumption.

Gender was a clear distinguishing factor in environmental awareness (Table 2). A t-test revealed that females had significantly different ideas about how climate change is caused by greenhouse gases [t(1,915) = 3.848, p < 0.000, d = 0.18] and palm oil consumption has reduced biodiversity [t(1,995) = 2.410, p < 0.02, d = 0.11], and they also confused climate change and ozone depletion more often [t(1,811) = 7.554, p < 0.000, d = 0.36]. Moreover, females more often believe that the loss of biodiversity is a result of the current exploitation of nature [t(1,995) = 4.139, p < 0.000, d = 0.19] and believe in scientists’ evidence for rapid climate change [t(1,878) = 4.116, p < 0.000, d = 0.19]. Males more often believe that climate change is natural [t(1,995) = −5.531, p < 0.000, d = 0.25], individuals’ consumption reduction does not affect biodiversity [t(1,940) = −5.578, p < 0.000, d = 0.25], and people are unable to significantly affect the number of species [t(1,954) = −4.710, p < 0.000, d = 0.21]. The effect size for this analysis (Cohen’s d) was found to approach Cohen’s (1988) convention for a small effect < 0.50.

The level of education affected respondents’ environmental awareness, but the effect size for this analysis (η2 = 0.01–0.04) was found to approach Cohen’s (1988) convention for a small effect: “climate change is caused by greenhouse gases” [F(6,1,975) = 12.746, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.04], “the increase of palm oil consumption” [F(6,1,975) = 4.531, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.01], “climate change is natural” [F(6,1,975) = 6.945, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.02], “the loss of biodiversity” [F(6,1,975) = 10.515, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.03], “scientists are sure that people” [F(6,1,569) = 2.563 p < 0.02, η2 = 0.01], “the decrease in the number of species” [F(6,1,975) = 6.512, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.02], “combustion of fossil fuels releases carbon dioxide” [F-B(6,1,611) = 12.492, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.04],” the main reason for biodiversity loss” [F-B(6,1,508) = 3.864, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.01], and “even if we as individuals…” [F(6,1,975) = 5.851, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.02].

For more detailed knowledge about how education affects environmental awareness, the PCA was generated (Table 3). The principal component of understanding represents higher environmental awareness, i.e., the scientific view of climate change and biodiversity loss, but misunderstanding does not do so. The post-hoc test (Bonferroni) indicates that those with bachelor’s (p < 0.008) and master’s degrees who graduated from university (p < 0.000) ex-pressed a greater understanding of climate change and biodiversity than people in or who graduated from only basic school education. Similarly, bachelor’s (p < 0.001) and master’s (p < 0.000) recipients who graduated from university or college (p < 0.01) expressed greater environmental awareness than respondents who graduated from vocational school. Moreover, master’s recipients from the university (p < 0.003) exemplified greater awareness than bachelor’s recipients who graduated from the university of applied sciences. People graduating with a master’s at university had less misunderstanding related to climate change and bio-diversity loss than people in or who graduated from only basic school education (p < 0.002) or vocational school (p < 0.000), people who graduated from college (p < 0.002), or people who graduated from a university of applied sciences.

The connection between Finns’ environmental awareness and their anticipatory competence was studied using Pearson’s correlation analysis. Before the analysis, two principal components were generated. Structural skills represent the respondent’s confidence that the future will be better if actions are implemented (see Levrini et al., 2021). Dynamic skills describe respondents’ personal opinions toward the means or skills to make the better future.

The present study indicates a fairly clear connection between environmental awareness and structural skills for making a more sustainable future. The result of Pearson’s correlation explains the connection between Finns’ environmental awareness and their anticipatory competence. Finns’ scientifically accurate knowledge of the reasons for climate change and nature loss (awareness) correlated rather strongly with their structural skill that climate change and biodiversity loss can be tackled through active measures, such as legislation (r = 0.446, p < 0.000, R2 = 0.20). Instead, Finns’ misunderstanding of environmental issues negatively correlated with their structural skills for solving environmental crises in the future (r = −0.375, p < 0.000, R2 = 0.14).

However, the result becomes unclear when compared to Finns’ personal anticipatory skills in interpreting or assessing climate change and nature loss in the future. Finns’ awareness of climate change and biodiversity weakly correlated with their dynamic skills (r = 0.104, p < 0.000, R2 = 0.01). Surprisingly, Finns’ lower environmental awareness also correlates weakly with their dynamic skills (r = 0.100, p < 0.000, R2 = 0.01). The result suggests that Finns’ environmental awareness is not obviously associated with their dynamic skills for building a more sustainable future. However, the R2-values indicate that only 1% of the variance in dynamic skills is shared with the variations of environmental awareness. The small effect sizes reveal that the correlation is unimportant.

Conclusion

According to the present study, environmental awareness among Finns seems to be quite high. Compared to previous studies, climate change awareness is similar (Lehtonen et al., 2020). Finns understand quite well the increase in the concentration of greenhouse gases in the atmosphere caused by climate change. The right information will help mitigate climate change mitigation measures. The most significant misunderstanding is that ozone depletion is causally linked to climate change. This result is very similar to previous studies (Tobler et al., 2012; Ratinen, 2016; Besel et al., 2017; Lehtonen et al., 2020). The understanding of biodiversity is also at a relatively high level. However, the results show that diversity is somewhat more unfamiliar to Finns than climate change, as the percentages of responses in the right direction were slightly lower. The result is similar to that of Lindemann-Matthies and Bose (2008), and they pointed out that limited knowledge of the public about biodiversity might explain why, in surveys, the loss of species is considered only a minor environmental problem.

From the point of view of developing the teaching of science, it is interesting to look at Finns’ environmental awareness. Based on the results, it seems obvious that higher education increases environmental awareness. Those who attend primary and vocational school have a lower level of environmental education than those with higher education. However, the effect sizes between the groups remained small. Moreover, Dimante et al. (2016) found that teaching changed some undergraduate Students’ household chemical consumption patterns, indicating the ambiguous impact of education on environmental awareness. Based on this study, it can be stated that age and education do not have a very significant effect on Finns’ environmental awareness. The environmental awareness of women was somewhat higher than that of men, but for all variables, the effect size remained small.

The results suggest that Finnish primary school teaches students quite well about climate change and biodiversity, or that those who have attended primary school are quite environmentally conscious with information obtained elsewhere. Based on the results, it is worth paying attention to the causes of climate change and emphasizing the human impact on current climate change. In the context of biodiversity, it is worth highlighting the impact of production systems and consumption on biodiversity loss. Because climate change and biodiversity are complex, interrelated issues, it would be worthwhile to look at them simultaneously in teaching. However, as Barelli et al. (2018) indicated, the task is not easy because adults are not very comfortable dealing with scientific and epistemological concepts related to complex systems.

There is a clear link between Finns’ environmental awareness and anticipatory structural skills. Conversely, the present research suggests that there is a partly contradictory connection between Finns’ more personal dynamic skills and their environmental awareness. It would seem obvious that with an emphasis on future skills in science teaching, attention should be paid to ways to improve individuals’ abilities to assess environmental issues from a more sustainable future perspective. As Tolppanen and Kärkkäinen (2021b) pointed out, the task of making education more sustainable is not simple: student teachers seem to have reluctance to make lifestyle changes that could significantly reduce their carbon emissions. This study suggests that Finns think broadly in the same way as Finnish student teachers. Teacher education is needed to foster student teachers’ action competence, and thereby their competence to support their Students’ and future citizens’ action competence (Tolppanen and Kärkkäinen, 2021b). If teachers’ own skills and will to make the future more sustainable are uncertain, it is unlikely that they will be able to guide their students toward a sustainable lifestyle.

Based on the results of the present study, it would be worth considering how sustainability education could be extended beyond education to the world of work. Finland is well-known for its education and this study shows the positive impact of Finnish higher education on citizens’ environmental awareness. We still need more knowledge how awareness at the work places will lead sustainable environmental measures. The gap between females and males in their objectives for a sustainable future is also revealed, pointing out areas that deserve attention by science education. The better understanding for the results of the present study outside of Finland would be significant if the study was implemented in other countries.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent from the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

IR contributed to conception and design of the study, performed the statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. IR and LL organized the database. Both authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Baartman, L. K. J., Bastiaens, T. J., Kirschner, P. A., and van der Vleuten, C. P. M. (2007). Evaluat-ing assessment quality in competence-based education: a qualitative comparison of two frameworks. Educ. Res. Rev. 2, 114–129. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2007.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barelli, E., Branchetti, L., Tasquier, G., Albertazzi, L., and Levrini, O. (2018). Science of complex systems and citizenship skills: a pilot study with adult citizens. Eurasia J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 14, 1533–1545.

Google Scholar

Besel, R. D., Burke, K., and Christos, V. (2017). A life history approach to perceptions of global cli-mate change risk: young adults’ experiences about impacts, causes, and solutions. J. Risk Res. 20, 61–75. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2015.1017830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branchetti, L., Cutler, M., Laherto, A., Levrini, O., Palmgren, E. K., Tasquier, G., et al. (2018). The I SEE project: an approach to futurize STEM education. Vis. Sustain. 9, 10–26.

Google Scholar

Brundiers, K., Barth, M., Cebrián, G., Cohen, M., Diaz, L., Doucette-Remington, S., et al. (2021). Key competencies in sustainability in higher education—toward an agreed-upon reference framework. Sustain. Sci. 16, 13–29. doi: 10.1007/s11625-020-00838-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Dimante, D., Tambovceva, T., and Atstaja, D. (2016). Raising environmental awareness through ed-ucation. Int. J. Contin. Eng. Educ. Life Long Learn. 26, 259–272.

Google Scholar

Eurobarometer (2021). Future of Europe. Available online at: https://aliautonomie.it/wp-content/uploads/2021/03/Special-Eurobarometer-Future-of-Europe_Key-Findings_EN.pdf (accessed Octember 11, 2021).

Google Scholar

Halady, I. R., and Rao, P. H. (2010). Does awareness to climate change lead to behavioral change? Int. J. Clim. Chang. Strateg. Manag. 1, 6–22. doi: 10.1108/17568691011020229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heikkilä, K., Nevala, T., Ahokas, I., Hyttinen, L., and Ollila, J. (2017). Nuorten Tulevaisuuskuvat 2067: Näkökulma Suomalaisen Yhteiskunnan Kehittämiseksi. [Future images of young people 2067: A Perspective for the Development of Finnish Society] Turun Yliopisto. TUTU eJulkaisuja 6/2017. Available online at: https://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi-fe2019052116250 (accessed November 17, 2021).

Google Scholar

Heras, M., and Ruiz-Mallén, I. (2017). Responsible research and innovation indicators for science education assessment: how to measure the impact? Int. J. Sci. Educ. 39, 2482–2507. doi: 10.1186/s12913-016-1423-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hungerford, H. R., and Volk, T. L. (1990). Changing learner behaviour through environmental education. J. Environ. Educ. 21, 8–21. doi: 10.1080/00958964.1990.10753743

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyry, J. (2017). Resurssiviisas Kansalainen. [Resource wise citizen]. Available online at: https://media.sitra.fi/2017/06/28164035/Sitra-Resurssiviisas-kansalainen-2017_Raportti.pdf (accessed November 11, 2021).

Google Scholar

IPCC (2007). Climate Change 2007: Synthesis Report. Contribution of Working Groups I, II and III to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, eds Core Writing Team, R. K. Pachauri, and A. Reisinger (Geneva: IPCC), 104.

Google Scholar

Kollmuss, A., and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 8, 239–260. doi: 10.1080/13504620220145401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehtonen, T., Niemi, M. K., Perälä, A., Pitkänen, V., and Westinen, J. (2020). Ilmassa Ristivetoa: Löytyykö Yhteinen Ymmärrys? Tutkimus Kansalaisten, Kuntapäättäjien ja Suuryritysten Johtajien Ilmastoasenteista. [Making Sense of Climate Change: Is There a Common Understanding? A Study of the Climate Attitudes of Citizens, Municipal Decision-Makers and Managers of Large Companies] e2 Tutkimus; Vaasan Yliopisto. Available online at: https://www.uwasa.fi/sites/default/files/2020-11/Ilmassa_ristivetoa%20loppuraportti_30_11_2020.pdf (accessed November 11, 2021)

Google Scholar

Levrini, O., Tasquier, G., Barelli, E., Laherto, A., Palmgren, E., Branchetti, L., et al. (2021). Recognition and operationalization of Future-Scaffolding Skills: results from an empirical study of a teaching–learning module on climate change and futures thinking. Sci. Educ. 105, 281–308. doi: 10.1002/sce.21612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindemann-Matthies, P., and Bose, E. (2008). How many species are there? Public under-standing and awareness of biodiversity in Switzerland. Hum. Ecol. 36, 731–742. doi: 10.1007/s10745-008-9194-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogensen, F., and Schnack, K. (2010). The action competence approach and the ‘new’ discourses of education for sustainable development, competence and quality criteria. Environ. Educ. Res. 16, 59–74. doi: 10.1080/13504620903504032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojala, M., and Bengtsson, H. (2019). Young people’s coping strategies concerning climate change: relations to perceived communication with parents and friends and pro-environmental behavior. Environ. Behav. 51, 907–935. doi: 10.1177/0013916518763894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratinen, I. (2016). Primary student teachers’ climate change conceptualization and implementation on inquiry-based and communicative science teaching: a design research. Jyväskylä studies in education. Psychol. Soc. Res. 555:155.

Google Scholar

Ratinen, I. (2021). Students’ knowledge of climate change, mitigation and adaptation in the context of constructive hope. Educ. Sci. 11:103. doi: 10.3390/educsci11030103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratinen, I., and Uusiautti, S. (2020). Finnish students’ knowledge of climate change mitigation and its connection to hope. Sustainability 12:2181. doi: 10.3390/su12062181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rickards, L., Ison, R., Fünfgeld, H., and Wiseman, J. (2014). Opening and closing the future: climate change, adaptation, and scenario planning. Environ. Plann. C Gov. Policy 32, 587–602. doi: 10.1068/c3204ed

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterling, S. (2021). Educating for the Future We Want. Available online at: https://mailchi.mp/greattransition/the-pedagogy-of-transition-educating-for-the-future-we-want?e=a4721327ad (accessed Octember 22, 2021).

Google Scholar

Swaim, J., Maloni, M., Napshin, S., and Henley, A. (2014). Influences on student intention and behaviour toward environmental sustainability. J. Bus. Ethics 124, 465–484. doi: 10.1007/s10551-013-1883-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobler, C., Visschers, V. H., and Siegrist, M. (2012). Consumers’ knowledge about climate change. Clim. Chang. 114, 189–209. doi: 10.1007/s10584-011-0393-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolppanen, S., and Kärkkäinen, S. (2021a). The blame-game: pre-service teachers views on who is responsible and what needs to be done to mitigate climate change. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 43, 2402–2425. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2021.1965239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolppanen, S., and Kärkkäinen, S. (2021b). Limits of caring: pre-service teachers’ reasons for not taking high-impact actions to mitigate climate change. Environ. Educ. Res. 1–17.

Google Scholar

Voogt, J., and Roblin, N. P. (2012). A comparative analysis of international frameworks for 21st century competences: implications for national curriculum policies. J. Curric. Stud. 44, 299–321. doi: 10.1080/00220272.2012.668938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuorio, E., Pernaa, J., and Aksela, M. (2021). Kestävän kehityksen kompetenssien ja opetuksen edistämineen kemian yliopistokoulutuksessa. [Promoting sustainable development competencies and teaching in chemistry university education.]. FMSERA J. 4, 34–55.

Google Scholar

Wals, A. E., Brody, M., Dillon, J., and Stevenson, R. B. (2014). Convergence between science and environmental education. Science 344, 583–584. doi: 10.1126/science.1250515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiek, A., Withycombe, L., and Redman, C. L. (2011). Key competencies in sustainability: a reference framework for academic program development. Sustain. Sci. 6, 203–218. doi: 10.1007/s11625-011-0132-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zsóka, Á, Szerényi, Z. M., Széchy, A., and Kocsis, T. (2013). Greening due to environmental education? Environmental knowledge, attitudes, consumer behavior and everyday pro-environmental activ-ities of Hungarian high school and university students. J. Clean. Prod. 48, 126–138. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2012.11.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: environmental awareness, anticipatory competence, science education, Finns, climate change, biodiversity

Citation: Ratinen I and Linnanen L (2022) The Connection of Finns’ Environmental Awareness to Their Anticipatory Competence. Front. Educ. 7:838005. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.838005

Received: 17 December 2021; Accepted: 24 February 2022;
Published: 13 April 2022.

Edited by:

Olivia Levrini, Alma Mater Studiorum - University of Bologna, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jinjin Lu, Xi’an Jiaotong-Liverpool University, China
Rachel Mamlok-Naaman, Weizmann Institute of Science, Israel
Pedro Reis, University of Lisbon, Portugal

Copyright © 2022 Ratinen and Linnanen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ilkka Ratinen, Ilkka.ratinen@ulapland.fi

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.