Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 05 July 2018
Sec. Educational Psychology

The Mediating Role of Student Motivation in the Linking of Perceived School Climate and Achievement in Reading and Mathematics

  • Department of Psychological, Health, and Learning Sciences, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States

While the current literature provides valuable insight into how school climate and student motivation impact academic achievement, research examining the mediating effects of motivation in the linking of school climate perceptions and achievement is limited. The current study employed structural equation modeling to examine a model that illustrates the role of self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in the linking of student perceptions of school climate (teacher/student relationship, order/safety/discipline, fairness/clarity of school rules) to reading and mathematics achievement. Analysis used data from the Educational Longitudinal Study of 2002 (ELS: 2002, 2004) that were gathered in a survey conducted by the National Center for Education Statistics (2004) and are representative of a national sample of 14,639 10th grade students. The results showed that the hypothesized model fit the data well supporting the hypothesis that student self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation play a mediating role in the linking of perceptions of school climate with reading and mathematics achievement. Additionally, students' perceptions of school climate related significantly with both achievement outcomes, and perceptions of school climate concerning teacher/student relationship related significantly with all motivation variables both self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation.

Introduction

Within the realm of educational research, attention has been given to academic achievement due to its impact on students' school experiences. Given the significant impact of achievement, a body of research has been dedicated to identifying the factors, both environmental and personal, that influence student academic achievement. A variety of factors that fall into these two categories have been examined. Two significant lines of research include the study of (a) characteristics of schools' climates reflecting relationships as well as norms and values and (b) personal motivational processes, such as self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation. It has been indicated in existing studies that both school climate and student motivation consistently relate with academic achievement (e.g., Roeser and Eccles, 1998; Caraway et al., 2003; Hardre and Reeve, 2003; Gilman and Anderman, 2006; Stewart, 2008; Gustafsson and Nilsen, 2016; Scherer and Nilsen, 2016).

While the current body of research provides valuable insight into how school climate and student motivation impact academic achievement, less is known about the relations between school climate and motivation and about the mechanisms that link school climate and achievement. The current body of research that examines the relations between these variables presents with limitations. First, research examining the influence of school environment on student motivation is limited (Urdan and Schoenfelder, 2006). The research that has been conducted in this area tends to focus more on how teacher/student relationships promotes student motivation (Roeser and Eccles, 1998; Ferrer-Caja and Weiss, 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2000b; Montalvo et al., 2007). Less attention is given to how other dimensions of school climate relate to student motivation, especially those that represent schools' norms and values, such as disciplinary climate and sense of safety. Thus, further study of a wider range of school climate variables that influence student academic self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation is warranted.

Second, the roles of self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in the linking of school climate and academic achievement have received little attention (Mercer et al., 2011) even though it has been proposed that the linking of environmental factors with student outcomes can occur through students' motivation (Bandura, 1986). Those who have examined motivation in this role have found that self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation influence the linking of various school-related factors, such as leadership style (Charbonneau et al., 2001), victimization at school (Thijs and Verkuyten, 2008), homework (Zimmerman and Kitsantas, 2005), and challenging, caring, and mastery-oriented classrooms (Fast et al., 2010), with achievement. While these studies lend insight into the roles that these motivation constructs play in the linking of school climate and achievement, the current body of research as a whole includes few studies of this nature and would benefit from additional examination of the relations between these variables.

The Present Study

The current study addresses these limitations by examining the relations between school climate, student motivation, and achievement. More specifically, it tests a model that examines the manner in which multiple dimensions of academic motivation (self-efficacy in reading and math; intrinsic motivation in reading and math) link various dimensions of perceived school climate (order/safety/discipline; fairness/clarity of rules; teacher/student relationship) and achievement in reading and math (see Figure 1). Thus, the study will contribute to current research by measuring not only the direct influence of school climate on motivation but also the process through which school climate links with academic achievement through motivation. In sum, the following research questions will be addressed:

1. Do student perceptions of teacher/student relationship, order/safety/discipline, and fairness/clarity of school rules directly relate with student self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in reading and math?

2. Do student self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in reading and math play a mediating role in the linking of student perceptions of teacher/student relationship, order/safety/discipline, fairness/clarity of school rules, and academic achievement in reading and math?

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Model illustrating the hypothesized relations between perceptions of school climate, student motivation, and academic achievement. OSD, Order/Safety/Discipline; FC, Fairness/Clarity of Rules; TSR, Teacher/Student Relationship; IMM, Intrinsic Motivation Math; IMR, Intrinsic Motivation Reading; SEM, Self-Efficacy Math; SER, Self-Efficacy Reading.

More specifically, there are four distinct sets of relations the current study seeks to explore. The first set of relations are the direct links between students' perceived school climate (teacher/student relationship, order/safety/discipline, fairness/clarity of school rules) and motivational beliefs (self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in reading and math), while the second set focuses on the relation between student school motivation (self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in reading and math) and academic achievement in reading and math. The third set of relations assesses the links between students' perceived school climate (teacher/student relationship, order/safety/discipline, fairness/clarity of school rules) and math/reading achievement while taking into account of students' motivational beliefs known as the direct effect. The last set of relations assesses the mediating effects of student's motivational beliefs (self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in reading and math) in linking students' perceived school climate (teacher/student relationship, order/safety/discipline, fairness/clarity of school rules) and math/reading achievement, known as the indirect effect.

Theoretical Framework

Theorists who examine human behavior from the perspectives of social cognitive theory, cognitive evaluation theory, and self-determination theory all recognize the influence of both environmental and personal factors (Ryan and Deci, 2000b; Bandura, 2001). These theoretical perspectives hold in common the view that external contexts not only relate directly to behavior but also influence behavior through individuals' cognitive processes of appraisal and evaluation (Bandura, 1978; Deci et al., 2001). These occur within the social environment in relation to both social relationships and social structures and contribute to the development of perceptions of both self and environment that lead individuals to demonstrate motivated behaviors such as effort, persistence, resilience (Pajares, 1996b), and the desire to seek and master challenges (Pintrich and Schunk, 2002).

Self-efficacy is described as individuals' perceptions and beliefs regarding their personal capabilities and what they expect that they can achieve with these capabilities (Linnenbrink and Pintrich, 2002). Intrinsic motivation describes individuals' desire to engage in activities simply for the sake of the engagement (Schunk and Zimmerman, 2006) because they find the activities interesting and enjoyable (Ryan and Deci, 2000b). Both the appraisal of relationships that occur within the social environment and appraisals of structures within the social environment which represent the broader systems that direct behavior such as rules, norms, and social practices (Bandura, 2001) are significant in the development and maintenance of personal motivational processes including both self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation. The recognition of the influence of social relationships and structures within educational settings is relevant, for it is within these social contexts that students receive valuable information regarding their behaviors (Bandura, 1997b; Ryan and Deci, 2000a,b).

School Motivation

Self-efficacy beliefs are domain-specific rather than global (Bandura, 1986; Linnenbrink and Pintrich, 2002). Thus measurement of this construct in academic studies varies according to the variable being examined (Pajares, 1996a). Education researchers often examine the domains of reading self-efficacy (Lau, 2009; Fan and Williams, 2010; Prat-Sala and Redford, 2010), writing self-efficacy (Prat-Sala and Redford, 2010), and math self-efficacy (Fan et al., 2009; Fan and Williams, 2010). Research is often conducted in educational contexts because students' competency beliefs are considered to be significant factors in their success or failure at school (Caraway et al., 2003; Pajares, 2003; Zimmerman and Kitsantas, 2005; Long et al., 2007; Turner et al., 2009) in that they affect student effort, persistence, resilience, and affective reactions (Pajares, 1996b).

Academic self-efficacy has been found to positively relate to general measures of academic achievement, such as grade point average, across grade levels for middle school (Long et al., 2007), high school (Caraway et al., 2003; Zimmerman and Kitsantas, 2005), and college students (Zajacova et al., 2005; Turner et al., 2009). Positive relations with achievement have also been found in specific academic content areas, including reading and math. For example, it has been found that this motivation construct is a significant predictor of grades (Wigfield and Eccles, 2002), test scores (Fan et al., 2009; Mercer et al., 2011), and various curriculum-based measures (Mercer et al., 2011) in these subject areas.

Intrinsic motivation drives individuals to pursue those topics of interest as well as their related activities (Unrau and Schlackman, 2006) and compels individuals to seek and master challenging tasks in order to validate competence (Pintrich and Schunk, 2002). In the academic setting, it is often measured as enjoyment (Taboada et al., 2009; Fan and Williams, 2010), level of involvement (Logan et al., 2011), and intrinsic interest in course work (Linnenbrink and Pintrich, 2002; Shores and Shannon, 2007). This motivation construct, like self-efficacy, has been examined in a domain-specific manner. Those who have examined intrinsic motivation in this way have measured this construct within the domains of both reading and math (Shores and Shannon, 2007; Fan and Williams, 2010).

The examination of intrinsic motivation is significant to educators in that increased levels of this construct have consistently been found to result in higher learning and creativity (Ryan and Deci, 2000a). For example, a positive relation between intrinsic motivation and grade point average has been found across grade levels (Gilman and Anderman, 2006; Turner et al., 2009) and in various regional settings (Hardre and Reeve, 2003). Performance in both reading and math is also positively affected by greater intrinsic motivation. This has been found in both assessment scores (Shores and Shannon, 2007; Taboada et al., 2009; Logan et al., 2011) and grades (Shores and Shannon, 2007) in these subject areas.

School Climate

School climate represents individuals' evaluation of the broad environment of their social context and structure in school, which reflects school norms, values, and relationships (Roeser et al., 1996; Ma and Klinger, 2000; Kuperminc et al., 2001; Benner et al., 2008). It is described as encompassing both the character and quality of a school which are influenced by its physical and organizational structure as well as its values and relationships (Koth et al., 2008; Cohen et al., 2009). Because of the multitude of factors that contribute to the overall construct of school climate, researchers have concluded that this construct is multidimensional in nature (Kuperminc et al., 1997; Verkuyten and Thijs, 2002) and have studied various aspects of this construct when studying it (Cohen et al., 2009; Zullig et al., 2010; Wang and Degol, 2015).

Three dimensions of school climate that reflect these values and relationships stand out in the literature as they have been often identified and examined. These dimensions are fairness of school rules (Haynes et al., 1993; Brand et al., 2003), order, safety, and discipline (Haynes et al., 1993; Kuperminc et al., 2001; Furlong et al., 2005), and student/teacher relationships (Haynes et al., 1993; Griffith, 2000). It has been recommended that these dimensions of school climate be examined individually (Kuperminc et al., 1997; Verkuyten and Thijs, 2002) because it is likely that the specific components of social environments are differentially salient to the individuals who function within these environments (Felner et al., 1985).

Students' perceptions of more positive school climate have been found to relate to both overall achievement and achievement in various academic content areas. For example, relationships with teachers, when measured as positive teacher regard and perceptions of teacher support, have been found to relate positively with student grades (Felner et al., 1985; Roeser and Eccles, 1998). At the same time, less favorable school climate variables, such as perception of school social problems and sense of not being safe, related inversely with grade point average (Stewart, 2008). Students' perceptions of a stable, safe, and fair disciplinary climate (Ma and Klinger, 2000) and caring and supportive relationships (Battistich et al., 1995) were found to positively relate to math, science, and writing performance for middle school and elementary students. Similarly, when school climate was measured from a negative perspective, it was found that perceptions of hostile relationships within the school showed inverse relations to both reading and math achievement (Ripski and Gregory, 2009).

Yet other studies have indicated conflicting results. First, contrary to the negative relation of hostility to student achievement, students' perceptions of unfair rules have been found to relate positively to reading and math achievement (Ripski and Gregory, 2009). Additionally, it has been indicated that there is a reciprocal relation between students' perceptions of school climate and achievement. For example, higher achievement was actually a predictor of more positive perceptions of fairness and interracial climate (Benner et al., 2008) in contrast to the more commonly found relation where school climate is the predictor.

School Climate and Motivation

While the relations between other aspects of school climate and motivation have not been extensively examined, the research that has been conducted in this area has shown that both the quality of the learning environments and the relationships with students that are promoted by teachers relate with students' academic motivation. For example, classrooms perceived as being positive, supportive, and warm were found to associate with more positive efficacy beliefs for both elementary (McMahon et al., 2009) and high school (Ferrer-Caja and Weiss, 2000; Montalvo et al., 2007) students. Similarly, when teachers conveyed that they believed in their students' abilities, these students experienced positive changes in their academic competence beliefs (Roeser and Eccles, 1998). Further, students with caring and supportive teachers were found to be more intrinsically motivated toward school than students with teachers who were reported as uncaring (Ryan and Deci, 2000b). Similarly, 6th and 7th grade students who reported teachers as demonstrating more warmth and support also reported higher levels of academic intrinsic motivation than did students who perceived their teachers as being colder and less supportive (Eccles et al., 1993). In addition, applying self-determination theory, research had found that perceived competence yielded a strong mediation effect on the relationship between perceived autonomy support from teachers and competitive performance (Halvari et al., 2009). Scherer and Nilsen (2016) studied the relations among different aspects of school climate, instructional quality, and students' achievement motivation for the TIMSS 2011 grade eight mathematics data sets comprising 50 countries and presented a partial mediation of instructional quality between school climate and achievement motivation in some countries.

Method

Data Set

Data from the Educational Longitudinal Study of 2002 (ELS: 2002, 2004) were used for this study. This is a public file provided by the National Center for Education Statistics (2004) and is a nationally representative sample of 10th grade students. Data were collected through a two-stage complex design in which schools were first selected using probability proportional to size (PPS), and then 26 students per school were selected using stratified systematic sampling procedures. In order to adjust for oversampling and nonresponse, weights were applied to prevent incorrect estimates. The final study sample consisted of 14,639 10th-graders. Of these participants, approximately 50% were female and 50% were male; 23% of students were from a low-income family; 57% were White, 13% were African American, 15% were Hispanic, 10% were Asian students, and the remaining students were Native American, Hawaiian, multiracial, or of other races.

Variables

Perceived School Climate

Three dimensions of school climate were examined: (a) students' perceptions of order/safety/discipline (OSD); (b) students' perceptions of fairness/clarity of school rules (FC); and (c) students' perceptions of teacher/student relationship (TSR). Items used for these scales (see Table 1) were adapted from the California School Climate and Safety Survey-Short Form (CSCSS-SF; Rosenblatt and Furlong, 1997; Furlong et al., 2005) and were rated by students according to the degree to which they agreed with the statement. Items were reverse coded where needed so that a higher value would indicate a more positive perception of each school climate variable. In past research, each scale has shown acceptable to good reliability and validity with internal consistency estimations indicating Cronbach's alphas ranging from 0.67 to 0.73 (Fan et al., 2011). Additionally, principal components analysis using varimax rotation showed moderate to high component loadings indicating that the school climate items measured the constructs well (National Center for Education Statistics, 2003; Fan et al., 2011).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. School climate items, means, standard deviations, and standardized factor loading.

Academic Motivation

Four areas of academic motivation were examined: (a) self-efficacy in math (SEM), (b) self-efficacy in reading (SER), (c) intrinsic motivation in math (IMM), and (d) intrinsic motivation in reading (IMR). These items (see Table 2) were adapted from the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA, National Center for Education Statistics, 2003) that is comprised of a variety of psychological scales including those that address intrinsic interest and self-efficacy. The items that comprised the intrinsic interest scale in the ELS: 2002 were adapted from the self-report Scale of Intrinsic Versus Extrinsic Orientation (Harter, 1981). The self-efficacy items were adapted from PISA and slightly modified to measure self-efficacy in reading and math rather than global self-efficacy (National Center for Education Statistics, 2003). Students responded to motivation items by rating how often they experienced the feelings/behaviors expressed in the item statements or the degree to which they agreed with the item statements. Items were reverse coded where needed so that a higher value would indicate a higher level of each motivation variable. Each motivation scale shows acceptable to good reliability (National Center for Education Statistics, 2003; Fan and Williams, 2010) and validity (Fan and Williams, 2010). Coefficient alphas for the math interest scale for the PISA countries ranged from 0.71 to 0.90 and the reading interest scale alpha coefficients ranged from 0.78 to 0.90 (National Center for Education Statistics, 2003).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Student motivation items, means, standard deviations, and standardized factor loading.

Achievement in Mathematics and Reading

Student mathematics and reading Item Response Theory (IRT) scores served as the dependent criterions for our study. IRT scores accounted for test question difficulty, discriminating ability, omitted items, and guessing. IRT scoring has been reported to have several advantages over raw number-right scoring by reducing student guessing, estimating omitted items and placing the tests with different difficulty on the same scale (ELS: 2002, 2004). Generally, IRT scale scores represented the estimated number of items students would have answered correctly. The students were given 72 mathematics questions measuring skill knowledge, comprehension, and problem solving in various areas of mathematics (e.g., arithmetic, algebra, geometry, probability) and 51 reading questions based on passages followed by questions regarding reproduction of detail, comprehension, and inference. Both math IRT Score (M = 38.05, SD = 12.05) and reading IRT score (M = 29.92, SD = 9.85) were computed as the probabilities of correct answers, summed over all items in the pools.

Plan of Analysis

First, multiple imputation using SPSS 19 (IBM Corp., 2010) was conducted to handle the missing data. No pattern was observed for the missing data. Three imputed data sets were generated (Rubin, 1987). Next, analysis was conducted using the Mplus 7.3 statistical program (Muthen and Muthen, 2010) to implement structural equation modeling (SEM) using a structural regression (SR) model to assess a multiple predictors multiple mediation model. A two-step process was conducted to test hypotheses for both measurement and structural models as recommended (Anderson and Gerbing, 1988) by using robust weighted least squares (WLS) approach. In step one, confirmatory factor analysis was used to assess whether the observed variables measured the latent constructs well. While past research has indicated that these scales show good construct validity (Fan and Williams, 2010; Fan et al., 2011), this step was included because these scales have not been used in numerous studies and so verification of construct validity was warranted. In step two, the fit of the hypothesized structural model (see Figure 1) was assessed. Last, a multiple predictors multiple mediation model was assessed to evaluate the indirect effects of school climate on math and reading achievement through students' school motivation.

A four-index strategy was used to determine a good model fit and included the use of the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)along with its associated 90% confidence interval (CI) to assess model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). The A CFI of >0.95, a RMSEA < 0.06, and a TLI > 0.95 was used to indicate a strong model fit; a CFI that nears 0.90, a RMSEA that nears 0.08, and a TLI nears 0.90 was used to indicate an acceptable model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Due to the large sample used in this study and the tendency of large samples to affect the chi-square statistic resulting in the rejection of an acceptable model (Schumacker and Lomax, 2004), this statistic was not considered when determining the goodness of model fit.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 provides means and standard deviations for school climate items. The results indicated that 10th grade students tended to agree that their schools were orderly and safe and that students who misbehaved received discipline. An examination of the means of each indicator revealed that these students viewed safety more positively than they viewed order in their schools. Additionally, students tended to agree that relationships with teachers were positive. They most highly rated teachers' praising of students' efforts and getting along well with students. Finally, the results indicated that students tended to agree that school rules were clearly stated and fairly enforced with fairness of rules being rated the most positively.

Table 2 provides means and standard deviations for motivation items. The results indicated that 10th grade students tended to report sometimes or often perceiving themselves as able to do well in math and reading. An examination of individual indicators revealed that these students held the most positive perceptions of themselves as able to do an excellent job on assignments and tests in both math and reading as well as to master skills taught in these classes. Their self-efficacy for understanding difficult texts and classes in these subject areas was lower in comparison. In regard to intrinsic motivation, students tended to disagree or agree that they found interest and enjoyment in both math and reading. Students more often reported that math and reading were fun than that they became absorbed in these subject areas.

Structural Equation Analysis

Measurement Model

The measurement model featured 32 indicators (15 motivation items and 17 school climate items), seven latent variables and two observed achievement variables. We allowed saturated structural relations and no error covariances among indicators when assessing the measurement model. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated a good fit for the measurement model, x(508)2 = 19330.113, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.932, TLI = 0.925, RMSEA = 0.050 with CI of (0.050, 0.051), verifying that the indicators measured the latent constructs well. Standardized factor loadings for the school climate constructs were acceptable to good and ranged from.433 to.780; standardized factor loadings for the motivation variables were moderate to strong ranging from 0.598 to 0.923 (see Tables 1, 2). The factor loadings showed that each item loaded significantly and substantially on the factor it was designed to represent.

Structural Model

Given that the measurement model fit the data well, we next tested the overall structural model along with the indirect effects. Analysis of the structural model indicated that the fit of the data to the model was excellent, x(495)2 = 9752.355, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.967, TLI = 0.962, RMSEA = 0.036 with CI of (0.035, 0.036). No error covariances were added to the model. The results indicated that our priori structural model provides meaningful explanation of the data. All significant standardized parameter estimates are shown in Figure 2. All correlations among school climate constructs and the four correlations among student motivation constructs turned out to be significant.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Significant standardized estimates for path coefficients. OSD, Order/Safety/Discipline; FC, Fairness/Clarity of Rules; TSR, Teacher/Student Relationship; IMM, Intrinsic Motivation Math; IMR, Intrinsic Motivation Reading; SEM, Self-Efficacy Math; SER, Self-Efficacy Reading.

The association between school climate and motivation.

Results indicated that the relations between perceptions of teacher/student relationship and all four motivation constructs were significant and positive. Additionally, students' perceptions of fairness/clarity of school rules related positively to intrinsic motivation in math. However, negative relations between students' perceptions of order/safety/discipline and intrinsic motivation in math and reading were found. Negative relations were found between students' perceptions of fairness/clarity of school rules and self-efficacy in math and reading.

The association between motivation and achievement.

An examination of the relations between self-efficacy and achievement showed that self-efficacy in math related positively with both math and reading achievement, while self-efficacy in reading related positively with reading achievement. Intrinsic motivation in math related negatively with reading and math achievement, while intrinsic motivation in reading related positively with both achievement outcomes.

The association between school climate and achievement (direct effect).

The results indicated that students' perceptions of teacher/student relationship related positively with both math and reading achievement. In addition, the relations between the remaining school climate dimensions, perceptions of fairness/clarity of rules and order/safety/discipline, and achievement were significant. Students' perceptions of fairness/clarity of rules related negatively with both math and reading achievement, while their perceptions of order/safety/discipline related positively with both achievement outcomes.

Motivation in linking school climate and achievement (indirect effect)

. The results of the multiple predictors multiple mediation model detected various significant indirect effects. In sum, the student motivation variables seemed to have yielded significant overall mediation effects in linking students' perceived fairness and clarity of rules as well as teacher-student relationship to both math and reading achievements (see Table 3). More specifically, students' perceived teacher-student relationship indirectly impacted their math and reading achievement through its relations with students' self-efficacy in math and intrinsic motivation in math and reading respectively. Students' perceived school fairness and clarification indirectly influenced their math and reading achievement through its relations with students' self-efficacy in math as well as intrinsic motivation in math. Moreover, the overall indirect effects of students' intrinsic motivation in math and reading in linking students' perceived school order and safety to their math and reading achievement were significant. In addition, students' perceived fairness and clarification as well as teacher-student relationship indirectly influenced their reading achievement respectively through students' perceived confidence in reading.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Indirect effects for the multiple mediator models.

In other words, students who perceived more positively about their teacher-student relationship tended to have higher confidence in their academic abilities in both math and reading as well as greater interest in reading, and in turn were likely to have higher math and reading achievement. On the contrary, students who perceived more positively about their teacher-student relationship tended to have higher confidence in their academic abilities in both math and reading as well as greater interest in reading and were more likely to obtain higher math and reading achievement. Also, students who perceived more positively about the school fairness and clarification tended to have greater interest in both math and reading and were less likely to perform well in math but more likely to perform well in reading. Students who perceived more positively about school order and safety tended to lower levels of interest in both math and reading and were more likely to obtain higher math achievement but lower reading achievement.

Discussion

The current study provides valuable insight into one of the underlying mechanisms that explains the associations between students' perceived school climate and their achievement through the role of students' school motivation. Findings suggest that strategies seeking to promote students' achievement that rely solely on their perceived school climate without considering students' motivational beliefs and interest are incomplete. The study expands the extant research by showing possible pathways that promote student achievement. Among all the pathways, several are of particular interests. In one pathway, students tend to have greater math and reading achievement when they perceive more positively about their teacher-student relationship and thus are likely to have greater confidence in their academic abilities in both subjects as well as greater interest in reading. In another pathway, when students perceive greater levels of fairness and clarification of rules in school, they tend to be less confident in their abilities in math and reading and thus become less likely to receive higher achievement scores in both subjects. Moreover, when students perceive higher levels of order and safety in school, they are less likely to feel genuinely interested in reading and thus less likely to have lower reading achievement. In general, these findings support our theoretical hypotheses that school climate as external context relates to students' academic outcomes through individual's cognitive perceptions.

School Climate and Achievement

The results of the study indicated that when their teachers were interested in them, praised their effort, and engaged in positive interactions with them, 10th grade students were more likely to achieve at higher levels in reading and math. In addition, when they perceived that their schools maintained orderly and safe environments, they achieved at higher levels in these academic content areas. However, when 10th grade students perceived that school rules were clearly stated and fairly enforced, they demonstrated lower levels of achievement in reading and math. The negative relation between fairness/clarity of school rules and both achievement variables was unexpected and contradictory to the finding of Ma and Klinger (2000), but consistent with the finding from Ripski and Gregory (2009). Ripski and Gregory (2009) proposed that students' perceptions of fairness/clarity of school rules might be more closely connected with personal experiences in the classroom, while standardized assessments are more impersonal and less connected with students' classroom experiences. That is, even though classrooms are perceived positively in terms of fairness and clarity of rules, the positive relations might not be evidenced in achievement measures that are not directly related to these classroom experiences. If achievement in reading and math were measured as grades for the class, the direction of the relations might change. Due to still limited research in this area, additional studies are warrantied in order to build the body of research that examines students' perceptions of fairness/clarity of rules and its relation to achievement and to reconcile these inconsistent findings.

Motivation and Achievement

The present results revealed that 10th grade students who perceived greater ability to understand math materials and perform well on math tasks were found to perform better on standardized math and reading assessments. Those who reported greater ability in reading were found to do better on these assessments in reading but not in math. In addition, those who reported greater interest in reading demonstrated higher levels of achievement in both subject areas. However, 10th grade students who reported greater interest in math demonstrated lower levels of achievement in reading and math. The negative relation between intrinsic motivation in math and achievement in reading might be explained by students' preference and perceived ability in different academic content areas. In other words, students who are more intrinsically motivated in math might prefer this academic area over reading and feel more competent in math than in reading.

The negative relation found between intrinsic motivation in math and math achievement was unexpected. One possible explanation is that students' reports of interest and enjoyment of math might be reflective of more personal classroom experiences that are influenced by educators' efforts to create situational interest. How students understood the nature of the items assessing intrinsic motivation in math might be very different. For example, some students might consider math in general fun and interesting, but find current math topics being taught or math class activities boring and uninteresting due to environmental contexts such as poor instructional strategies or poor teacher-student relationship. Thus, it is recommended that future examination include intrinsic motivation measures that are adjusted to more closely reflect students' classroom experiences. In addition, its relation with achievement might be better measured with grades that are based upon performance in the classroom.

School Climate and Motivation

Perceptions of teacher/student relationship were found to relate positively with self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation in both reading and math as hypothesized. This finding suggests that educators can influence students' perceptions of self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation at school through their thoughtful interactions with students. It is consistent with an extant body of research indicating the manner in which teachers interact with students and the type of classroom environment they maintain are critical in supporting student motivation (e.g., Schunk et al., 2008). Research has also shown that it is not only the direct interactions between teachers and students but also the classroom environments maintained by teachers that influence student motivation. It is thus important that teachers provide the verbal feedback and classroom environments that influence student motivation because they enhance current experiences in school and affect important future educational decisions.

Fairness/Clarity of Rules, Order/Safety/Discipline, and Motivation

These school climate constructs are considered together in the examination of their relations with motivation because they in common reflect the climate of the overall school as well as that of the classroom. They are also both related negatively to motivation. Negative relations between fairness/clarity of rules and self-efficacy in reading and between order/safety/discipline and intrinsic motivation were found. Limited research examining these variable relations is currently available. However, the findings of the current study conflict with those reported in a previous study that examined similar variables. McMahon et al. (2009) found that lower levels of perceived friction predicted greater self-efficacy. However, this was measured according to students' perceptions of the classroom climate rather than their perceptions of the school climate. The current study differs in that the items that comprised these school climate factors extend beyond the classroom and measure students' perceptions of their schools. Yet, the motivation measures are more reflective of perceptions of classroom experiences. Future research examining these dimensions of school climate might define this variable in a way that more closely reflects students' perceptions of their classroom experiences.

Limitations of the Current Study

While contributing to the existing body of literature that examines the relations among school climate, motivation, and achievement, the study has several limitations. First, the study is a cross-sectional study which does not generate causal inferences. Because both motivation, school climate measures as well as achievement used in the study were collected at the same time wave, we are not able to conduct a longitudinal investigation and make any causal conclusions about the relations. Future studies investigating such relations using data from multiple time points will provide valuable information to the literature. Second, the sample used in the current study includes only 10th grade students. While the study of adolescents is considered important due to the changes that occur in motivation at this stage (Gottfried et al., 2001; Lepper et al., 2005; Otis et al., 2005; Unrau and Schlackman, 2006; Lau, 2009), the inclusion of this age group does not allow for generalization of the findings to students of other ages. For example, to the relations among students' perceived school climate, motivation and academic achievement might differ for younger students in elementary schools. Future research further testing these relations for different student populations will provide valuable information to the literature. Moreover, future research addressing the size of the effects on the relationships among students' academic achievement, school motivation and perception of school climate will be a valuable addition to the literature. Notwithstanding, the findings of the present study complements and extends earlier studies by examining the underlying mechanism among school climate, student motivation and achievement, demonstrating a unique and useful perspective for understanding how we might be better promoting student achievement.

Ethics Statement

The IRB form of this study was approved by the committee of University of Houston.

Author Contributions

WF made substantial contributions to the conception and design, and the acquisition of data, and the analysis and interpretation of data, participated in drafting the article and revising it critically for important intellectual content, gave final approval of the version to be submitted and any revised version. CW participated in data analysis, results interpreting, drafting the article, and revising it critically for important intellectual content.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1978). The self-system in reciprocal determinism. Am. Psychol. 33, 344–358. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura, A. (1997b). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: Freeman.

Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Kim, D., Watson, M., and Schapps, E. (1995). Schools as communities, poverty levels of student populations, and students' attitudes, motives, and performance: a multilevel analysis. Am. Educ. Res. J. 32, 627–658.

Google Scholar

Benner, A. D., Graham, S., and Mistry, R. S. (2008). Discerning direct and mediated effects of ecological structures and processes on adolescents' educational outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 44, 840–854. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.840.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, S., Felner, R., Shim, M., Seitsinger, A., and Dumas, T. (2003). Middle school improvement and reform: development and validation of a school-level assessment of climate, cultural pluralism, and school safety. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 570–588. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.570

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caraway, K., Tucker, C. M., Reinke, W. M., and Hall, C. (2003). Self-efficacy, goal orientation, and fear of failure as predictors of school engagement in high school students. Psychol. Sch. 40, 417–427. doi: 10.1002/pits.10092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charbonneau, D., Barling, J., and Kelloway, E. K. (2001). Transformational leadership and sports performance: the mediating role of intrinsic motivation. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 31, 1521–1534. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02686.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J., McCabe, E., Michelli, N. M., and Pickeral, T. (2009). School climate: research, policy, practice, and teacher education. Teach. Coll. Rec. 111, 180–213.

Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., and Ryan, R. M. (2001). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation in education: reconsidered once again. Rev. Educ. Res. 71, 1–27. doi: 10.3102/00346543071001001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Midgley, C., Reuman, D., Mac Iver, D., and Feldlaufer, H. (1993). Negative effects of traditional middle schools on students' motivation. Elem. Sch. J. 93, 553–574.

Google Scholar

ELS: 2002 (2004). Base Year Data File User's Manual. Available online at: http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2004405

Fan, W., and Williams, C. M. (2010). The effects of parental involvement on students' academic self-efficacy, engagement, and intrinsic motivation. Educ. Psychol. 30, 53–74. doi: 10.1080/01443410903353302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, W., Lindt, S. F., Arroyo-Giner, C. A., and Wolters, C. A. (2009). The role of social relationships in promoting student academic self-efficacy and MIMIC approaches to assess factorial mean invariance. Int. J. Appl. Educ. Stud. 5, 34–53.

Google Scholar

Fan, W., Williams, C. M., and Corkin, D. M. (2011). A multilevel analysis of student perceptions of school climate: the effect of social and academic risk factors. Psychol. Sch. 48, 632–647. doi: 10.1002/pits.20579

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fast, L. A., Lewis, J. L., Bryant, M. J., Bocian, K. A., Cardullo, R. A., Rettig, M., et al. (2010). Does math self-efficacy mediate the effect of the perceived classroom environment on standardized math test performance? J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 729–740. doi: 10.1037/a0018863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felner, R. D., Aber, M. S., Primavera, J., and Cauce, A. M. (1985). Adaptation and vulnerability in high-risk adolescents: an examination of environmental mediators. Am. J. Community Psychol. 13, 365–379. doi: 10.1007/BF00911214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrer-Caja, E., and Weiss, M. R. (2000). Predictors of intrinsic motivation among adolescent students in physical education. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 71, 267–279. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2000.10608907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furlong, M. J., Greif, J. L., Bates, M. P., Whipple, A. D., Jiminez, T. C., and Morrison, R. (2005). Development of the California School Climate and Safety Survey – short form. Psychol. Sch. 42, 137–149. doi: 10.1002/pits.20053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilman, R., and Anderman, E. M. (2006). The relationship between relative levels of motivation and intrapersonal, interpersonal, and academic functioning among older adolescents. J. Sch. Psychol. 44, 375–391. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., and Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation from childhood through late adolescence: a longitudinal study. J. Educ. Psychol. 93, 3–13. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffith, J. (2000). School climate as group evaluation and group consensus: student and parent perceptions of the elementary school environment. Elem. Sch. J. 101, 35 – 61. doi: 10.1086/499658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafsson, J. E., and Nilsen, T. (2016). “The impact of school climate and teacher quality on mathematics achievement: a difference-in-differences approach,” in Teacher Quality, Instructional Quality and Student Outcomes, eds T. Nilsen and J. E. Gustafsson (Cham: Springer), 81–95.

Google Scholar

Halvari, H., Ulstad, S. O., Bagøien, T. E., and Skjesol, K. (2009). Autonomy support and its links to physical activity and competitive performance: mediations through motivation, competence, action orientation and harmonious passion, and the moderator role of autonomy support by perceived competence. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 53, 533–555. doi: 10.1080/00313830903302059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardre, P. L., and Reeve, J. (2003). A motivational model of rural students' intentions to persist in, versus drop out of, high school. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 347–356. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation: Motivation and informational components. Dev. Psychol. 17, 300–312. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haynes, N. M., Emmons, C., and Comer, J. P. (1993). Elementary and Middle School Climate Survey. New Haven, CT: Yale University Child Study Center.

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp. (2010). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 19.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Koth, C. W., Bradshaw, C. P., and Leaf, P. J. (2008). A multilevel study of predictors of student perceptions of school climate: the effect of classroom-level factors. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 96–104. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.96

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuperminc, G. P., Leadbeater, B. J., and Blatt, S. J. (2001). School social climate and individual differences in vulnerability to psychopathology among middle school students. J. Sch. Psychol. 39, 141–159. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(01)00059-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuperminc, G. P., Leadbeater, B. J., Emmons, C., and Blatt, S. J. (1997). Perceived school climate and difficulties in the social adjustment of middle school students. Appl. Dev. Sci. 1, 76–88. doi: 10.1207/s1532480xads0102_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, K. (2009). Reading motivation, perceptions of reading instruction and reading amount: a comparison of junior and senior secondary students in Hong Kong. J. Res. Read. 32, 366–382. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01400.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., and Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: age differences and academic correlates. J. Educ. Psychol. 97, 184–196. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linnenbrink, E. A., and Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Motivation as an enabler for academic success. School Psych. Rev. 31, 313–327. Available online at: http://naspjournals.org

Google Scholar

Logan, S., Medford, E., and Hughes, N. (2011). The importance of intrinsic motivation for high and low ability readers' reading comprehension performance. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 124–128. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, J. F., Monoi, S., Harper, B., Knoblauch, D., and Murphy, P. K. (2007). Academic motivation and achievement among urban adolescents. Urban Educ. 42, 196–222. doi: 10.1177/0042085907300447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X., and Klinger, D. A. (2000). Hierarchical linear modeling of student and school effects on academic achievement. Can. J. Educ. 25, 41–55. doi: 10.2307/1585867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMahon, S. D., Wernsman, J., and Rose, D. S. (2009). The relation of classroom environment and school belonging to academic self-efficacy among urban fourth- and fifth-grade students. Elem. Sch. J. 109, 267–281. doi: 10.1086/592307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercer, S. H., Nellis, L. M., Martínez, R. S., and Kirk, M. (2011). Supporting the students most in need: academic self-efficacy and perceived teacher support in relation to within-year academic growth. J. Sch. Psychol. 49, 323–338. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2011.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montalvo, G. P., Mansfield, E. A., and Miller, R. B. (2007). Liking or disliking the teacher: student motivation, engagement and achievement. Eval. Res. Educ. 20, 144–158. doi: 10.2167/eri406.0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthen, L. K., and Muthen, B. O. (2010). Mplus: User's Guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen and Muthen.

National Center for Education Statistics (2003). Educational Longitudinal Study of 2002 Base Year Field Test Report. Washington, DC: Author.

National Center for Education Statistics (2004). Educational Longitudinal Study of 2002: Base Year Data User's Manual. Washington, DC: Author.

Otis, N., Grouzet, F. M. E., and Pelletier, L. G. (2005). Latent motivational change in an academic setting: a 3-year longitudinal study. J. Educ. Psychol. 97, 170–183. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pajares, F. (1996a). “Assessing self-efficacy beliefs and academic outcomes: the case of specificity and correspondence,” in Paper Presented at the Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (New York, NY).

Google Scholar

Pajares, F. (1996b). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Rev. Educ. Res. 66, 543–578. doi: 10.3102/00346543066004543

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pajares, F. (2003). Self-efficacy beliefs, motivation, and achievement in writing: a review of the literature. Read. Writ. Q. 19, 139–158. doi: 10.1080/10573560308222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R., and Schunk, D. H. (2002). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research, and Applications, 2nd Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Prat-Sala, M., and Redford, P. (2010). The interplay between motivation, self-efficacy, and approaches to studying. British J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 283–305. doi: 10.1348/000709909X480563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ripski, M. B., and Gregory, A. (2009). Unfair, unsafe, and unwelcome: do high school students' perceptions of unfairness, hostility, and victimization in school predict engagement and achievement? J. Sch. Violence 8, 355–375. doi: 10.1080/15388220903132755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeser, R. W., and Eccles, J. S. (1998). Adolescents' perceptions of middle school: relation to longitudinal changes in academic and psychological adjustment. J. Res. Adolesc. 8, 123–158.

Google Scholar

Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., and Urdan, T. C. (1996). Perceptions of the school psychological environment and early adolescents' psychological and behavioral functioning in school: the mediating role of goals and belonging. J. Educ. Psychol. 88, 408–422.

Google Scholar

Rosenblatt, J. A., and Furlong, M. J. (1997). Assessing the reliability and validity of student self-reports of campus violence. J. Youth Adolesc. 26, 187–202. doi: 10.1023/A:1024552531672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York, NY: Wiley

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000a). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 54–67. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000b). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, R., and Nilsen, T. (2016). “The relations among school climate, instructional quality, and achievement motivation in mathematics,” in Teacher Quality, Instructional Quality and Student Outcomes, eds T. Nilsen and J. E. Gustafsson (Cham: Springer), 51–80.

Google Scholar

Schumacker, R. E., and Lomax, R. G. (2004).A Beginner's Guide to Structural Equation Modeling, 2nd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H., and Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). “Competence and control beliefs: distinguishing the means and ends,” in Handbook of Educational Psychology, 2nd Edn, eds P. A. Alexander and P. H. Winne (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 349–367.

Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., and Meece, J. L. (2008). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research, & Applications, 3rd Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.

Google Scholar

Shores, M. L., and Shannon, D. M. (2007). The effects of self-regulation, motivation, anxiety, and attributions on mathematics achievement for fifth and sixth grade students. Sch. Sci. Math. 107, 225–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1949-8594.2007.tb18284.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, E. B. (2008). School structural characteristics, student effort, peer associations, and parental involvement: the influence of school- and individual-level factors on academic achievement. Educ. Urban Soc. 40, 179–204. doi: 10.1177/0013124507304167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taboada, A., Tonks, S. M., Wigfield, A., and Guthrie, J. T. (2009). Effects of motivational and cognitive variables on reading comprehension. Read. Writ. 22, 85–106. doi: 10.1007/s11145-008-9133-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thijs, J., and Verkuyten, M. (2008). Peer victimization and academic achievement in a multiethnic sample: the role of perceived academic self-efficacy. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 754–764. doi: 10.1037/a0013155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, E. A., Chandler, M., and Heffer, R. W. (2009). The influence of parenting styles, achievement motivation, and self-efficacy on academic performance in college students. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 50, 337–346. doi: 10.1353/csd.0.0073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unrau, N., and Schlackman, J. (2006). Motivation and its relationship with reading achievement in an urban middle school. J. Educ. Res. 100, 81–101. doi: 10.3200/JOER.100.2.81-101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urdan, T., and Schoenfelder, E. (2006). Classroom effects on student motivation: goal structures, social relationships, and competence beliefs. J. Sch. Psychol. 44, 331–349. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verkuyten, M., and Thijs, J. (2002). School satisfaction of elementary school children: the role of performance, peer relations, ethnicity, and gender. Soc. Indic. Res. 59, 203–228. doi: 10.1023/A:1016279602893

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-T., and Degol, J. L. (2015). School Climate: a review of the construct, measurement, and impact on student outcomes. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 28, 315–352.

Google Scholar

Wigfield, A., and Eccles, J. S. (2002). “The development of competence beliefs and values from childhood through adolescence,” in Development of Achievement Motivation, eds A. Wigfield and J. S. Eccles (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 92–120.

Google Scholar

Zajacova, A., Lynch, S. M., and Espenshade, T. J. (2005). Self-efficacy, stress, and academic success in college. Res. High. Educ. 46, 677–706. doi: 10.1007/s11162-004-4139-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J., and Kitsantas, A. (2005). Homework practices and academic achievement: the mediating role of self-efficacy and perceived responsibility beliefs. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 30, 397–417. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zullig, K. J., Koopman, T. M., Patton, J. M., and Ubbes, V. A. (2010). School climate: historical review, instrument development, and school assessment. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 28, 139–152. doi: 10.1177/0734282909344205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-efficacy, school climate, intrinsic motivation, academic achievement, mediation effect

Citation: Fan W and Williams C (2018) The Mediating Role of Student Motivation in the Linking of Perceived School Climate and Achievement in Reading and Mathematics. Front. Educ. 3:50. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2018.00050

Received: 12 December 2016; Accepted: 12 June 2018;
Published: 05 July 2018.

Edited by:

Jesus de la Fuente, University of Almería, Spain

Reviewed by:

Trude Nilsen, University of Oslo, Norway
Calista E. Kelly, Claremont Graduate University, United States

Copyright © 2018 Fan and Williams. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Weihua Fan, d2ZhbkBjZW50cmFsLnVoLmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.