Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Comput. Sci., 03 April 2023
Sec. Human-Media Interaction
This article is part of the Research Topic The Good Side of Technology: How We Can Harness the Positive Potential of Digital Technology to Maximize Well-being View all 14 articles

To use or be used? The role of agency in social media use and well-being

\nAngela Y. Lee
Angela Y. Lee1*Nicole B. EllisonNicole B. Ellison2Jeffrey T. HancockJeffrey T. Hancock1
  • 1Department of Communication, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 2School of Information, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States

In this paper, we develop the concept of agentic social media use: a way of engaging with social media that emphasizes having the beliefs, knowledge, and practices to use it intentionally. In comparison to instances of “mindless” social media use, people who use social media agentically do so with a purpose in mind: they leverage the affordances of social media to do things that are meaningful, useful, or satisfying for them. For example, people can use social media to intentionally build or manage their relationships, to seek out and learn new information about their interests, or to craft a positive image of themselves through the content they post. Crucially, however, there are many other valuable uses of social media that may not be considered conventionally productive but are nonetheless deliberate and useful, such as using social media intentionally to relax, unwind, and entertain themselves in an effort to modulate their emotions. To use social media agentically means to (1) hold an agentic mindset about one's relationship with social media, (2) have the knowledge and literacy to understand how to navigate social media effectively, and (3) enact practices that assert control over specific elements of social media use, such as curating content and refining algorithmic recommendation. Approaching social media use from the perspective of agency and intentionality allows us to better understand heterogeneous social media effects and to identify new ways of helping people benefit from these technologies.

Introduction

For many people, social media is integral to daily life–connecting them to a constant stream of information about their friends, their interests, and the world around them (Auxier and Anderson, 2021; Rideout et al., 2022). However, this constant connectivity (Wells et al., 2021; Abeele et al., 2022) raises important questions about the amount of control we have over our experiences with social media: Are we dependent on-or even “addicted” to-social media, or are we engaging with it on our own terms?

It is easy to find narratives that portray social media users as addicted to their devices and influenced by the pull of platforms like Instagram and TikTok. Public editorials (Petrillo, 2021; Haidt, 2022; Stabile, 2022) often emphasize the ways in which people spend more time on social media than they want to (for a review, see Sun and Zhang, 2021), or how specific features are designed to capture and ensnare user attention (Gray et al., 2018; Bail, 2021; Wells et al., 2021). Based on these dominant narratives of social media as a powerful force that exerts control over individuals, people are often urged to reduce their exposure to social media by disconnecting (e.g., taking a “digital detox,” Radtke et al., 2022) or deactivating their accounts altogether (Liao and Sundar, 2022).

However, reduction-based strategies are neither practical nor reflective of recent work on the effects of social media use on psychological well-being. Large-scale experiments show that digital detoxes do not often improve individuals' lives as theorized (Radtke et al., 2022), unless people disconnect when social media content is likely to be particularly volatile (e.g., during a contentious election cycle, Allcott et al., 2020). Instead, abstaining from social media use can even undermine well-being by removing individuals from established networks of social support, which can provide important social and emotional resources like social support and access to new opportunities (e.g., bridging social capital, Ellison et al., 2011). Indeed, new research from the pandemic era demonstrates how adolescents without social media were significantly more lonely than their digitally-connected peers (Minihan et al., 2021; Metherell et al., 2022).

Furthermore, it is increasingly evident that the amount of agency people feel that they have over social media can have powerful effects on their lives. Research on social media mindsets indicates that people who view social media as something they can control and use (i.e., an agentic mindset) report less depression, anxiety, and stress than those who view social media as inherently harmful and addictive (i.e., a low-agency mindset) (Lee et al., 2021). Having an agentic, positive mindset-such as viewing social media as a tool that can be used to enhance one's life-was a stronger predictor of well-being than the amount of time they spent online (Lee and Hancock, 2020). In light of these findings, deeper considerations of user agency may provide valuable insights into how to live with and benefit from social media. While it may be true that individuals hold less sway over the design of social media platforms than the corporations that run them, at the individual level there are many ways for everyday users to take control of their experiences with social media and optimize their engagement with it.

In this paper, we develop the concept of agentic social media use: a way of engaging with social media that emphasizes having the beliefs, knowledge, and practices to use it intentionally. In comparison to instances of “mindless” social media use, people who use social media agentically do so with a purpose in mind: they leverage the affordances of social media to do things that are meaningful, useful, or satisfying for them. For example, people can use social media to intentionally build or manage their relationships, to seek out and learn new information about their interests, or to craft a positive image of themselves through the content they post. Crucially, however, there are many other valuable uses of social media that may not be considered conventionally “productive” but are nonetheless deliberate and useful, such as using social media intentionally to relax, unwind, and entertain themselves (e.g., to modulate their emotions, Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015; Robinson and Knobloch-Westerwick, 2016). To use social media agentically means to (1) hold an agentic mindset about one's relationship with social media, (2) have the knowledge and literacy to understand how to navigate social media effectively, and (3) enact practices that assert control over specific elements of social media use, such as curating content and refining algorithmic recommendation. Approaching social media use from the perspective of agency and intentionality allows us to better understand heterogeneous social media effects and to identify new ways of helping people benefits from these technologies.

What is agency and why does it matter?

At a fundamental level, agency means “having the capacity to alter the course of events in some situation” (Cesafsky et al., 2019). When people feel they have agency, they believe they have some degree of control over the course of their own lives (Bandura, 2001) and that they can take action to get what they want or need (Moore and Fletcher, 2012).

These perceptions of agency are essential to well-being. Indeed, some of the most striking evidence of the importance of agency comes from studying its absence: people who feel they have little control over their lives tend to feel more stressed, anxious, and helpless. Indeed, one of the cognitive symptoms of depression is believing that the conditions of one's life are poor and there is little that can be done to improve it (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1986; Maddux and Meier, 1995; Vollmayr and Gass, 2013).

In contrast, people tend to feel more positively about themselves and more satisfied with their lives when they feel capable of taking care of themselves, whether this means feeling a sense of control over their health, relationships, or finances (Bandura, 2001; Adler, 2012; Moore and Fletcher, 2012). Indeed, perceptions of self-efficacy have long been thought to be essential to maintaining a positive self-image (Bandura, 2001), in part because these agentic beliefs are important precursors to actions that benefit the individual. For example, people with higher self-efficacy are more likely to pursue beneficial, but challenging, tasks (e.g., a new exercise routine, smoking cessation; Fletcher and Banasik, 2001; Gwaltney et al., 2009) because they believe they will succeed. Few people, after all, want to choose difficult pursuits they believe they cannot accomplish. The strength of the relationship between self-efficacy beliefs and behavioral change is such that many health interventions emphasize the need to build individuals' confidence that they will succeed as a pivotal first step toward change (Zimmerman, 2000). Indeed, a recent meta-analysis found that experimentally increasing individuals' self-efficacy was a strong predictor of individuals' health-related behaviors (Sheeran et al., 2016).

People often understand their agency relative to other forces in their lives. Even someone who generally feels in control of their life may feel less agentic in certain situations, like being in a regimented work environment that limit their choices, or in a social setting with strong norms about what is acceptable to share (e.g., the positivity bias on social media content, Schreurs and Vandenbosch, 2021). Classical research on loci of control emphasizes how beliefs about control over one's environment can influence well-being in powerful ways (Klonowicz, 2001; Mirowsky and Ross, 2007). People who view themselves as fundamentally in charge of their own lives and actions (i.e., having an internal locus of control) see themselves as capable of optimizing their experiences, such as being able to make decisions that will help them thrive by harnessing the tools around them, whether they be digital or analog. In contrast, people who have an external locus of control feel like their lives are dictated by outside forces.

Technologies like social media can be understood as one such force. People can feel controlled by their experiences with platforms like YouTube, Instagram, and TikTok if they feel they are spending more time online than they want to or if they find themselves influenced by what they see (Lanette et al., 2018). On the other hand, people can also feel in control of their social media use and capable of using it to get what they want or need (i.e., action initiation, Moore and Fletcher, 2012). These perceptions of agency and self-efficacy (Skurka et al., 2022) may be an important determinant of whether people obtain benefits from their use, or not. In the context of media use, self-efficacy is an important determinant of technology adoption (Waddell et al., 2014) as viewing the self as capable of learning to use new devices or platforms (e.g., smartphones) is an important first step because individuals generally want to do things they believe they can be successful at. In addition to influencing individuals' beliefs about their own potential, conceptualizations about agency can also influence behavior. Indeed, thousands of interventions to date have tried to help people protect their health (Sheeran et al., 2016), well-being (O'Sullivan, 2011), and livelihoods (Shoji et al., 2016) by helping them develop a sense of agency. Considerations of agency may thus similarly improve individuals' experiences with social media.

Conceptualizing agentic social media use

What does it mean for individuals to use social media agentically? An agentic perspective of social media focuses not on the time people spend on social media, but rather the extent to which they are intentionally or unintentionally using social media to fulfill valued needs and goals. As a first step, people should feel they are in control of the ways they engage with social media, whether this means using it extensively, or not at all. Aided by a basic understanding of how social media systems work, they can then change the ways they use social media in an effort to obtain its benefits while avoiding its harms.

By centering considerations of user agency, we can focus on identifying and analyzing the psychological forces that drive behavior: asking why people use social media in addition to documenting how they use social media. The same action, like watching a video, can be driven by diverse motivations which may in turn differentially affect people's lives. For example, enjoying a video as part of an intentional ritual of unwinding after a long day may help people relax, manage their stress, and improve their mood (Johnson and Knobloch-Westerwick, 2014; Robinson and Knobloch-Westerwick, 2021). In contrast, watching the same video to procrastinate unpleasant tasks or to tune out anxious thoughts may undermine well-being by preventing people from necessary and beneficial tasks, such as managing their work lives and processing their emotions adaptively (Robinson and Knobloch-Westerwick, 2016; Reinecke et al., 2017). As in prior work on teaching individuals how to best live with and benefit from everyday forces in their lives (e.g., stress optimization theory, Jamieson et al., 2018), we can help people optimize their experiences with social media by scaffolding the belief systems, literacy, and behaviors they need to make the most effective use of these digital tools.

Our conceptualization proposes that for people to take control of their experiences with social media, they should have a mindset that orients them toward its potential uses, the literacy to understand how social media systems work, and a repertoire of practices that will help them assert control the ways they use social media. We discuss each of the following components below.

Social media mindsets

For people to optimize their experiences with social media, they need to first believe it is possible for them to harness its affordances for their own benefit. This belief system can be understood through the lens of social media mindsets, which are the core beliefs people have about the fundamental nature of social media in their lives (Lee et al., 2021). Just as individuals can have growth or fixed mindsets about the nature of their intelligence (Dweck, 2008), people hold mindsets about the amount of agency they have over their social media use (i.e., high agency vs. low agency) and the expected valence of its effects (i.e., enhancing vs. harmful). These mindsets function like a mental shortcut, offering people simple answers to difficult questions like “How will using social media generally affect me?” and “How much control do I really have over this technology?” that can help guide them toward specific ways of thinking about, responding to, and using social media (Lee and Hancock, 2023).

Having an agentic and positive mindset toward social media use appears to be particularly adaptive. Research on the relationship between social media mindsets and mental health found that mindsets can have important effects on individual well-being. In fact, people's mindsets about the role of social media in their lives were a stronger predictor of their life satisfaction and psychological distress than the amount, frequency, or intensity of their use (Lee and Hancock, 2020). Those who believed that they were in control (agency) and that social media could be beneficial to their lives (valence) not only felt better about their lives, but also reported less depression, stress, and anxiety. In contrast, people with the mindset that their social media was inherently harmful and out of their control tended to feel more psychologically distressed, in line with prior work indicating that perceiving one's own use as “problematic” can undermine mental health (Andreassen et al., 2016; Cheng et al., 2019; Cunningham et al., 2021).

Why is it important for people to have such a particular mindset toward their social media use? At a high level, mindsets guide people toward certain pathways of understanding and engaging with social media–mechanisms that have been explored in the context of appraisal effects and behavioral change (Claro et al., 2016; Crum et al., 2017; Clark et al., 2018; Yeager et al., 2019; Paakkari et al., 2021; Przybylski et al., 2021). For instance, people who see themselves as in control of their social media use tend to view instances of their social media use as meaningful and useful. As one participant described in an in-depth interview about their mindset (Lee et al., 2021), social media “is for doing things” that “[make] life easier and more colorful,” whether that involves “serious” informational or social tasks like engaging with news and maintaining friendships overseas, or simply using it to take a moment to relax in the interstitial downtime between events (Squire and Dikkers, 2012; Chess, 2018).

In contrast, people who see themselves as dependent on their social media–or subject to its influence—feel worse after they spend time on social media, whereas those with more agentic mindsets do not (Lee and Hancock, 2020). Similar results were observed in Ernala et al. (2022), where Facebook use was associated with reduced subjective well-being when people perceived their Facebook use as bad, but not when they perceived it as good. If people already hold the mindset that their social media use is not under their control, they may appraise further use as a failure to resist temptation (e.g., self-control failures, Lanette and Mazmanian, 2018; Lanette et al., 2018). This may be particularly harmful to individuals' self-esteem given the highly fragmented nature of social media use throughout the day (e.g., through push notifications and repeated phone pick-ups, Reeves et al., 2021; Brinberg et al., 2022), which may serve as a constant reminder of their perceived lack of self-control. Therefore, holding an agentic mindset that allows individuals to consider the potential ways in which they can use it for their own benefit may be an important precursor to positive experiences with social media.

Social media literacy

People can also use social media more intentionally when they understand the technical and social workings of social media platforms. Knowing more about how platforms function may be particularly important as “social media” comes to encompass an increasingly broad set of technologies, ranging from conventional feed-based platforms like Twitter to video- and game-based platforms like TikTok and Roblox (Bayer et al., 2020)–each of which presents unique affordances and challenges.

Social media literacy can improve people's ability to make informed choices about the ways they engage with social media. While everyday users may not need to understand the details of how platforms run under the hood, they can better optimize their experiences with social media if they have a working knowledge of core features (e.g., algorithmic recommendation systems) and common social dynamics on social media (e.g., self-presentation biases when presenting content) (Schreurs and Vandenbosch, 2021; Schreurs et al., 2022). Indeed, research on self-efficacy emphasizes that domain-specific knowledge is important to helping people translate a general sense of agency in their lives into action in specific facets of their life (Bandura, 2001; Mirowsky and Ross, 2007; Skurka et al., 2022). Knowing more about a particular concept–such as how social media is curated both by platforms and by people–can complement an agentic mindset in helping people assert control over their experiences with social media content.

Consider, for example, the ways in which increased literacy about content curation can support people in obtaining greater benefits from social media. It is now the norm for most platforms to use personalized algorithms to recommend content to their users (Bhandari and Bimo, 2022). Algorithm audits (Metaxa et al., 2021) and internal documents (Wells et al., 2021) indicate that feeds like the TikTok For You page and the Instagram Explore page curate content by identifying users' interests through an analysis of digital trace data, like the posts they look at, like, share, or skip. While the specifics of these processes are rarely made known to the general public—much less everyday users–knowing how these algorithm-based systems work can help people have more agency over what they see and share. For instance, research on the algorithmic crystal finds that people who understand how the algorithms reflect elements of their own identity can better shape and refine its recommendations for them by changing their own behaviors (Lee et al., 2022). Literacy can also help people manage the ways they are seen by others, through the algorithm. Research on adaptive folk theorization reveals that people with a stronger working knowledge of algorithms were better equipped to maintain their desired self-presentations (e.g., sharing certain facets with certain communities, DeVito, 2021), even in light of updates to the algorithm over time.

Literacy can also help people make intentional decisions about the ways they think about and engage with content produced by others, whether they are creators, peers, or strangers. A notable example can be seen with regards to social comparison on social media (Nesi and Prinstein, 2015; Keles et al., 2020). We know from media psychological research that self-presentations on social media are often biased toward the positive (Walther et al., 2015; Yau and Reich, 2019) as individuals strive to put their “best foot forward” and to share the highlights of their personal, professional, and romantic lives (Schreurs and Vandenbosch, 2021). Everyday users may not realize this, however, and may instead interpret these idealized self-presentations as realistic glimpses into the lives of others (Fan et al., 2019)–potentially triggering harmful processes of downwards social comparison (Chou and Edge, 2012; Frison and Eggermont, 2016; Lee, 2020). Research on the Social Media Literacy model (Schreurs and Vandenbosch, 2021) indicates that literacy may buffer against such effects. People who understand the ways in which the positivity bias of social media content distorts what they see online are protected against the adverse effects of seeing idealized content on their self-esteem (Schreurs et al., 2022). Interventions have demonstrated that teaching adolescents about these biases can improve well-being by changing how people responded to idealized social media content (Weber et al., 2022).

Practices

Using social media agentically also requires individuals to enact specific strategies to assert control over their experiences with social media. Whereas holding an agentic mindset is an important precursor to obtaining benefits from social media, and literacy can enhance individuals' ability to translate self-efficacy into action, people should also put agency into practice by changing their behavior. We define agentic practices as those where individuals modulate the ways in which they engage with social media by intentionally considering the ways in which its affordances may serve them in fulfilling valued needs and goals.

In a recent review of media use efficacy, Skurka et al. (2022) highlight how feeling capable of using media for one's own benefit is a powerful predictor of positive behavioral change. Indeed, decades of interventions on behavioral change have helped people be healthier, spend more wisely, and live more meaningfully (Bandura, 1992; Baldwin et al., 2006; Schwarzer et al., 2008) by boosting their self-efficacy and teaching them useful practices (e.g., how to create an exercise routine, how to budget, how to maintain meaningful relationships).

In a similar vein, people can apply agentic social media use practices to learn how to obtain more benefits from their social media use. Take, for example, the ways in which people can better control their engagement with their feeds. People may feel that their feed is shaped by what is most popular overall, and therefore showing them content from interests that are not relevant to them (Eslami et al., 2016; Verduyn et al., 2022). Alternately, they may also feel that the algorithm is not dynamically keeping up with changes in their self-concept, and is instead showing them content from facets of their identity that are no longer important to them (e.g., pictures from out-of-touch friends, past partners, or old hobbies, Lee et al., 2022). A simple, yet powerful, way that people can increase their control over social media content is by making judicious use of features like the block, unfollow, and “show me less” functions (e.g., information repertoire filtration, Zhang et al., 2022). Just as people spring-clean their houses, it may be valuable for individuals to iteratively or routinely sort through their social media content to prune what is no longer useful and appreciate what brings them joy. For instance, people can remove themselves from communities they are no longer interested in or unfollow accounts that do not enhance their lives. On the other hand, people should also use existing features to positively curate their feeds by deliberately following new creators and joining new groups. Furthermore, people can manage their audience preferences by setting “close friends” lists or creating group-chats to take control of who sees their posts, and thus better manage social boundaries (Litt, 2012).

In a similar vein, people can assert control over the recommendations provided to them by personalized algorithms by using the practice of strategic refinement. Theory and research on the algorithmic crystal (Lee et al., 2022) emphasizes that individuals can shape the algorithms' model of their preferences by changing how they engage with content on the platform. Guided by the notion that their behaviors inform personalization processes (e.g., a personal engagement folk theory, DeVito et al., 2017), they can strategically increase or decrease their interaction with specific forms of content to change the algorithm's recommendations. This practice increases individuals' agency over their feeds by providing individuals a pathway to refine what they see via the algorithm, so that its recommendations either better align with their self-concept (e.g., learning that they enjoy specific music, fashion, humor, or views) or who they might like to be (e.g., new hobbies they want to try, new perspectives they want to consider). In one noteworthy example, a white adolescent who realized her feed was mostly other white adolescents was able to strategically refine her TikTok algorithm to show her videos from more creators of color to support more diverse artists, musicians, and activists (Lee et al., 2022). To do so, she began to watch, like, and comment messages of support on posts from BIPOC individuals. The same practice of strategically modulating engagement behaviors can similarly be applied to help individuals take a more active role in the process of curating content received through personalized algorithms.

Implications for theory and interventions

Considering agency in experiences with social media can advance theory on the differential effects of technology use on well-being. Furthermore, it can illuminate new pathways for interventions to help everyday people make the most out of these ever-present digital tools.

Understanding the extent to which individuals are using social media intentionally or unintentionally may enrich how we describe, study, and assess social media use. As increasing work confirms that social media affects people differently (Beyens et al., 2021; Pouwels et al., 2021), it is clear that using social media can both help and harm people's lives (Orben, 2020; Meier and Reinecke, 2021; Hancock et al., 2022), and that these effects cannot be explained by differences in time spent with social media alone (Przybylski et al., 2020; Parry et al., 2021). Examining the extent to which people use social media intentionally or unintentionally may help explain when social media is most enhancing. Already, research on social media mindsets indicates that people who hold more agentic mindsets experience better well-being, whereas low-agency mindsets are associated with worse well-being. Furthermore, Cunningham et al. (2021) found that the component of social media use that was most detrimental to adolescent depression was teenagers' perception of their own use as “out of their control” (i.e., problematic social media use).

Thinking about social media use in terms of its intentionality can also complement and extend existing approaches to parsing enhancing and harmful social media effects. For example, it has been commonly theorized that active social media use (e.g., posting, sharing, commenting) can improve well-being whereas passive use (e.g., browsing, watching videos) undermines it (Verduyn et al., 2015). However, new research indicates that we should go beyond examinations of type of use alone to understand heterogeneous social media effects (Valkenburg et al., 2022). For instance, eye-tracking studies like Ellison et al. (2020) find that users spend equivalent time gazing at social media content that they do and do not click on, suggesting that actions that appear “passive”–like watching a video or scrolling through a feed–may involve active thought and consideration. In fact, intentional actions that are not captured by clicks, such as calling someone after seeing a social media post, may be more powerful for relationship development and well-being than one-click “likes” or “shares.”

Indeed, there are many ways for active use to be harmful (e.g., cyberbullying, Giumetti and Kowalski, 2022) and for passive use to be restorative (e.g., watching videos to relax, Cauberghe et al., 2021). Considering these two activities with an orientation around user agency highlights the fact that social media use can be a means of pursuing a goal–which can be to hurt another person (Mishna et al., 2016), or to regulate one's own emotions (et al., 2020).

Adopting an agency-centered approach can also support the development of interventions to improve individuals' experiences with social media, without necessarily requiring them to reduce their use. Indeed, research from the person-specific framework indicates that reducing social media use may be enhancing for some individuals and harmful for others (Beyens et al., 2021), and therefore may not be a useful recommendation for all (Radtke et al., 2022). Instead, when we think about social media use from the perspective of individual agency, we can encourage individuals to develop the belief systems and behaviors to identify how best to use social media in their own lives.

For example, Lee and Hancock (2023) helped individuals develop a more agentic, positive social media mindset through a series of self-guided reflective writing exercises. The intervention scaffolded a new way of seeing social media by asking individuals to reflect on questions like “What are some ways in which you already take advantage of social media to do things that are important or useful to you?” and “What can you do differently to make the most of social media in your life?” In addition, participants also developed a personalized plan for supporting their own agentic social media use going forward by listing several ways in which they could take control of their experiences. Results showed that the intervention was successful in not only cultivating more adaptive mindsets, but also more agentic social media use. One participant in the treatment group wrote, “I have specific goals when I'm using social media… It's not controlling, it's just a tool I can use” and described how they would leverage social media in the future: “I can use social media as a support system I can go to even during difficult times like COVID-19.”

An advantage of this interventional approach is that it allows individuals to develop their own plans for using social media agentically at an individual level. As the field comes to recognize the ways in which both social media use and mental health vary between, and within, individuals (Ram and Gerstorf, 2009; Valkenburg et al., 2021), interventions to improve peoples' experiences with social media should be flexible enough to account for such differences. Consider the challenge of supporting individuals in optimizing their experiences with diverse forms of social media content. While some forms of content, like extreme violence, will hurt all individuals, other kinds of content may enhance well-being for some and hurt it for others. Photos and videos of travel destinations may inspire some, but evoke envy or fear of missing out for others (van der Wal et al., 2022). Furthermore, the same person can respond to the same content in different ways at different points in time. For example, looking at photos of a romantic partner may elicit substantially different affective responses depending on the current well-being of the relationship. Providing a normative recommendation for how individuals should engage with most forms of content may thus be challenging. An agency-oriented intervention could instead support individuals in developing the beliefs, literacies, and practices they need to manage their own content streams to optimize their own well-being (e.g., by guiding individuals with reflective questions like “What kinds of social media content tend to make you feel better or worse, and why?”). By teaching individuals how to take control of their own exposure to social media content and to enact agentic practices for curating their feed, with knowledge of its underlying functionalities, we may be better able to support individuals in making the most out of their experiences with social media.

Conclusion

If social media is to be a part of our lives, we should find a way to harness its benefits and minimize its harms. Identifying the ways in which we can use social media agentically can advance theory on social media effects by introducing the intentionality of one's social media use as an important potential determinant of positive outcomes. We build on prior research to argue that people may be better able to obtain benefits from their social media use when they have an agentic mindset that empowers them to use it for valued goals, understand enough about the workings of social media to be informed users, and enact practices that allow them to exert control over their engagement with social media.

Data availability statement

This commentary does not include any original data. Inquiries regarding the article can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

AL was supported by the Stanford Interdisciplinary Graduate Fellowship.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abeele, M. M. V., Halfmann, A., and Lee, E. W. (2022). Drug, demon, or donut? theorizing the relationship between social media use, digital well-being and digital disconnection. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 45, 101295. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adler, J. M. (2012). Living into the story: agency and coherence in a longitudinal study of narrative identity development and mental health over the course of psychotherapy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 367. doi: 10.1037/a0025289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S., and Gentzkow, M. (2020). The welfare effects of social media. Am. Econ. Rev. 110, 629–676. doi: 10.1257/aer.20190658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreassen, C. S., Billieux, J., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., Demetrovics, Z., Mazzoni, E., et al. (2016). The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: a large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 30, 252. doi: 10.1037/adb0000160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auxier, B., and Anderson, M. (2021). Social Media Use in 2021. Pew Research Center 1, 1–4.

Google Scholar

Bail, C. (2021). Breaking the Social Media Prism. In Breaking the Social Media Prism. Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9780691216508-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, A. S., Rothman, A. J., Hertel, A. W., Linde, J. A., Jeffery, R. W., Finch, E. A., et al. (2006). Specifying the determinants of the initiation and maintenance of behavior change: an examination of self-efficacy, satisfaction, and smoking cessation. Health Psychol. 25, 626. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1992). Exercise of personal agency through the self-efficacy mechanism. Self Efficacy Thought Control Action 1, 3–37.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayer, J. B., Trieu, P., and Ellison, N. B. (2020). Social media elements, ecologies, and effects. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 71, 471–497. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., Keijsers, L., and Valkenburg, P. M. (2021). Social media use and adolescents' well-being: developing a typology of person-specific effect patterns. Communic. Res. doi: 10.1177/00936502211038196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhandari, A., and Bimo, S. (2022). Why's everyone on tiktok now? the algorithmized self and the future of self-making on social media. Soc. Media Soc. 8. doi: 10.1177/20563051221086241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brinberg, M., Ram, N., Wang, J., Sundar, S. S., Cummings, J. J., Yeykelis, L., et al. (2022). Screenertia: understanding “stickiness” of media through temporal changes in screen use. Communic. Res. doi: 10.1177/00936502211062778

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cauberghe, V., Van Wesenbeeck, I., De Jans, S., Hudders, L., and Ponnet, K. (2021). How adolescents use social media to cope with feelings of loneliness and anxiety during COVID-19 lockdown. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 24, 250–257. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2020.0478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cesafsky, L., Stayton, E., and Cefkin, M. (2019). “Calibrating agency: human-autonomy teaming and the future of work amid highly automated systems,” in Ethnographic Praxis in Industry Conference Proceedings. 65–82.

Google Scholar

Cheng, J., Burke, M., and Davis, E. G. (2019). “Understanding perceptions of problematic facebook use: when people experience negative life impact and a lack of control,” in Proceedings of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 1–13. doi: 10.1145/3290605.3300429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chess, S. (2018). A time for play: interstitial time, invest/express games, and feminine leisure style. New Media Soc. 20, 105–121. doi: 10.1177/1461444816660729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, H. T. G., and Edge, N. (2012). “They are happier and having better lives than I am”: the impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others' lives. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 15, 117–121. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2011.0324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, J. L., Algoe, S. B., and Green, M. C. (2018). Social network sites and well-being: the role of social connection. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 32–37. doi: 10.1177/0963721417730833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claro, S., Paunesku, D., and Dweck, C. S. (2016). Growth mindset tempers the effects of poverty on academic achievement. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 113, 8664–8668. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608207113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, A. J., Akinola, M., Martin, A., and Fath, S. (2017). The role of stress mindset in shaping cognitive, emotional, and physiological responses to challenging and threatening stress. Anxiety Stress Coping 30, 379–395. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2016.12755856g

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, A. J., Jamieson, J. P., and Akinola, M. (2020). Optimizing stress: An integrated intervention for regulating stress responses. Emotion. 20, 120–125. doi: 10.1037/emo0000670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, S., Hudson, C. C., and Harkness, K. (2021). Social media and depression symptoms: a meta-analysis. Res. Child Adolesc. Psychopathol. 49, 241–253. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00715-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVito, M. A. (2021). Adaptive folk theorization as a path to algorithmic literacy on changing platforms. Proc. ACM Hum. Comput. Interact., 5, 1–38. doi: 10.1145/3476080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVito, M. A., Birnholtz, J., and Hancock, J. T. (2017). “Platforms, people, and perception: Using affordances to understand self-presentation on social media,” in Proceedings of the 2017 ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work and Social Computing (New York, NY: ACM), 740–754).

Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S. (2008). Can personality be changed? the role of beliefs in personality and change. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 391–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00612.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., and Lampe, C. (2011). Connection strategies: Social capital implications of Facebook-enabled communication practices. New Media Soc. 13, 873–892. doi: 10.1177/1461444810385389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellison, N. B., Trieu, P., Schoenebeck, S., Brewer, R., and Israni, A. (2020). Why we don't click: interrogating the relationship between viewing and clicking in social media contexts by exploring the “non-click”. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 25, 402–426. doi: 10.1093/jcmc/zmaa013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernala, S. K., Burke, M., Leavitt, A., and Ellison, N. B. (2022). “Mindsets matter: how beliefs about facebook moderate the association between time spent and well-being,” in Proceedings of the 2022 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. p. 1–13.

Google Scholar

Eslami, M., Karahalios, K., Sandvig, C., Vaccaro, K., Rickman, A., Hamilton, K., et al. (2016). “First I“ like” it, then i hide it: folk theories of social feeds,” In Proceedings of the 2016 cHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 2371–2382. doi: 10.1145/2858036.2858494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., Deng, N., Dong, X., Lin, Y., and Wang, J. (2019). Do others' self-presentation on social media influence individual's subjective well-being? a moderated mediation model. Telemat. Inform. 41, 86–102. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2019.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fletcher, J. S., and Banasik, J. L. (2001). Exercise self-efficacy. Clin. Excell. Nurse Pract. 5, 134–143. doi: 10.1054/xc.2001.24203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frison, E., and Eggermont, S. (2016). Exploring the relationships between different types of Facebook use, perceived online social support, and adolescents' depressed mood. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 34, 153–171. doi: 10.1177/0894439314567449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giumetti, G. W., and Kowalski, R. M. (2022). Cyberbullying via social media and well-being. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 45:101314. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. M., Kou, Y., Battles, B., Hoggatt, J., and Toombs, A. L. (2018). “The dark (patterns) side of UX design,” in Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 1–14. doi: 10.1145/3173574.3174108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gwaltney, C. J., Metrik, J., Kahler, C. W., and Shiffman, S. (2009). Self-efficacy and smoking cessation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 23, 56. doi: 10.1037/a0013529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haidt, J. (2022). “Why the Past 10 Years of American Life Have Been Uniquely Stupid.” The Atlantic. Available online at: https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2022/05/social-media-democracy-trust-babel/629369/

Google Scholar

Hancock, J., Liu, S. X., Luo, M., and Mieczkowski, H. (2022). Psychological Well-Being and Social Media Use: A Meta-Analysis of Associations between Social Media Use and Depression, Anxiety, Loneliness, Eudaimonic, Hedonic and Social Well-Being. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.4053961

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, J. P., Crum, A. J., Goyer, J. P., Marotta, M. E., and Akinola, M. (2018). Optimizing stress responses with reappraisal and mindset interventions: an integrated model. Anxiety Stress Coping 31, 245–261. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2018.1442615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, B. K., and Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2014). Glancing up or down: mood management and selective social comparisons on social networking sites. Comput. Human Behav. 41, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keles, B., McCrae, N., and Grealish, A. (2020). A systematic review: the influence of social media on depression, anxiety and psychological distress in adolescents. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 25, 79–93. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2019.1590851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klonowicz, T. (2001). Discontented people: reactivity and locus of control as determinants of subjective well-being. Eur. J. Pers. 15, 29–47. doi: 10.1002/per.387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2015). The selective exposure self-and affect-management (SESAM) model: applications in the realms of race, politics, and health. Communic. Res. 42, 959–985. doi: 10.1177/0093650214539173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanette, S., Chua, P. K., Hayes, G., and Mazmanian, M. (2018). How much is' too much'? the role of a smartphone addiction narrative in individuals' experience of use. Proc. ACM Hum. Comput. Interact. 2, 1–22. doi: 10.1145/3274370

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanette, S., and Mazmanian, M. (2018). “The smartphone“ addiction” narrative is compelling, but largely unfounded,” In Extended Abstracts of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 1–6. doi: 10.1145/3170427.3188584

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. Y., and Hancock, J. (2020). “The impact of social media mindsets on psychological well-being,” In Paper presented at the Conference for the International Communication Association.

Google Scholar

Lee, A. Y., and Hancock, J. (2023). Social media mindsets: A new approach to understanding social media use & psychological well-being. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/f8wny

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. Y., Katz, R., and Hancock, J. (2021). The role of subjective construals on reporting and reasoning about social media use. Soc. Media Soc. 7. doi: 10.1177/20563051211035350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. Y., Mieczkowski, H., Ellison, N. B., and Hancock, J. T. (2022). The algorithmic crystal: conceptualizing the self through algorithmic personalization on tiktok. Proc. ACM Hum. Comput. Interact. 6, 1–22. doi: 10.1145/3555601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. K. (2020). The effects of social comparison orientation on psychological well-being in social networking sites: serial mediation of perceived social support and self-esteem. Curr. Psychol. 41, 6247–6259. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01114-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, M., and Sundar, S. S. (2022). Sound of silence: does muting notifications reduce phone use?. Comput. Human Behav. 134, 107338. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litt, E. (2012). Knock, knock. who's there? the imagined audience. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media 56, 330–345. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2012.705195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maddux, J. E., and Meier, L. J. (1995). Self-efficacy and depression. In Self-Efficacy, adaptation, and adjustment. (Boston, MA: Springer). p. 143–169. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-6868-5_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, A., and Reinecke, L. (2021). Computer-mediated communication, social media, and mental health: a conceptual and empirical meta-review. Communic. Res. 48, 1182–1209. doi: 10.1177/0093650220958224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metaxa, D., Park, J. S., Robertson, R. E., Karahalios, K., Wilson, C., Hancock, J., et al. (2021). Auditing algorithms: understanding algorithmic systems from the outside in. Found. Trends Hum. Comput. Interact. 14, 272–344. doi: 10.1561/1100000083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metherell, T. E., Ghai, S., McCormick, E. M., Ford, T. J., and Orben, A. (2022). Digital access constraints predict worse mental health among adolescents during COVID-19. Sci. Rep. 12, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-23899-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minihan, S., Orben, A., Songco, A., Fox, E., Ladouceur, C. D., Mewton, L., et al. (2021). Social Determinants of Mental Health During a Year of the COVID-19 Pandemic. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/64v7x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirowsky, J., and Ross, C. E. (2007). Life course trajectories of perceived control and their relationship to education. Am. J. Sociol. 112, 1339–1382. doi: 10.1086/511800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishna, F., McInroy, L. B., Lacombe-Duncan, A., Bhole, P., Van Wert, M., Schwan, K., et al. (2016). Prevalence, motivations, and social, mental health and health consequences of cyberbullying among school-aged children and youth: protocol of a longitudinal and multi-perspective mixed method study. JMIR Res. Protoc. 5, e5292. doi: 10.2196/resprot.5292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. W., and Fletcher, P. C. (2012). Sense of agency in health and disease: a review of cue integration approaches. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesi, J., and Prinstein, M. J. (2015). Using social media for social comparison and feedback-seeking: gender and popularity moderate associations with depressive symptoms. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 1427–1438. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0020-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., and Seligman, M. E. (1986). Learned helplessness in children: a longitudinal study of depression, achievement, and explanatory style. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 435. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.2.435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orben, A. (2020). Teenagers, screens and social media: a narrative review of reviews and key studies. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 55, 407–414. doi: 10.1007/s00127-019-01825-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Sullivan, G. (2011). The relationship between hope, eustress, self-efficacy, and life satisfaction among undergraduates. Soc. Indic. Res. 101, 155–172. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9662-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paakkari, L., Tynjäl,ä, J., Lahti, H., Ojala, K., and Lyyra, N. (2021). Problematic social media use and health among adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 1885. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parry, D. A., Fisher, J. T., Mieczkowski, H., Sewall, C. J., and Davidson, B. I. (2021). Social media and well-being: a methodological perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/exhru

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrillo, S. (2021). What Makes TikTok so Addictive?: An Analysis of the Mechanisms Underlying the World's Latest Social Media Craze.

Google Scholar

Pouwels, J. L., Valkenburg, P. M., Beyens, I., van Driel, I. I., and Keijsers, L. (2021). Social media use and friendship closeness in adolescents' daily lives: an experience sampling study. Dev. Psychol. 57, 309. doi: 10.1037/dev0001148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybylski, A. K., Nguyen, T. V. T., Law, W., and Weinstein, N. (2021). Does taking a short break from social media have a positive effect on well-being? evidence from three preregistered field experiments. J. Technol. Behav. Sci. 6, 507–514. doi: 10.1007/s41347-020-00189-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybylski, A. K., Orben, A., and Weinstein, N. (2020). How much is too much? Examining the relationship between digital screen engagement and psychosocial functioning in a confirmatory cohort study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. 59, 1080–1088. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radtke, T., Apel, T., Schenkel, K., Keller, J., and von Lindern, E. (2022). Digital detox: an effective solution in the smartphone era? a systematic literature review. Mob. Media Commun. 10, 190–215. doi: 10.1177/20501579211028647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ram, N., and Gerstorf, D. (2009). Time-structured and net intraindividual variability: tools for examining the development of dynamic characteristics and processes. Psychol. Aging 24, 778. doi: 10.1037/a0017915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeves, B., Ram, N., Robinson, T. N., Cummings, J. J., Giles, C. L., Pan, J., et al. (2021). Screenomics: a framework to capture and analyze personal life experiences and the ways that technology shapes them. Hum. Comput. Interact. 36, 150–201. doi: 10.1080/07370024.2019.1578652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinecke, L., Aufenanger, S., Beutel, M. E., Dreier, M., Quiring, O., Stark, B., et al. (2017). Digital stress over the life span: the effects of communication load and internet multitasking on perceived stress and psychological health impairments in a German probability sample. Media Psychol. 20, 90–115. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2015.1121832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rideout, V., Peebles, A., Mann, S., and Robb, M. B. (2022). Common Sense Census: Media Use by Tweens and Teens, 2021. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense.

Google Scholar

Robinson, M. J., and Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2016). “Mood management through selective media use for health and well-being,” in the Routledge Handbook of Media Use and Well-Being. Routledge, p. 65-79.

Google Scholar

Robinson, M. J., and Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2021). The Role of Affect and Mood Management in Selective Exposure to Media Messages. Routledge international handbook of emotions and media, p. 69–84. doi: 10.4324/9780429465758-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreurs, L., Meier, A., and Vandenbosch, L. (2022). Exposure to the positivity bias and adolescents' differential longitudinal links with social comparison, inspiration and envy depending on social media literacy. Curr. Psychol. 1–21. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03893-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreurs, L., and Vandenbosch, L. (2021). Introducing the Social Media Literacy (SMILE) model with the case of the positivity bias on social media. J. Child. Media 15, 320–337. doi: 10.1080/17482798.2020.1809481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzer, R., Luszczynska, A., Ziegelmann, J. P., Scholz, U., and Lippke, S. (2008). Social-cognitive predictors of physical exercise adherence: three longitudinal studies in rehabilitation. Health Psychol. 27, S54–63. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.1(Suppl.).S54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheeran, P., Maki, A., Montanaro, E., Avishai-Yitshak, A., Bryan, A., Klein, W. M., et al. (2016). The impact of changing attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy on health-related intentions and behavior: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 35, 1178. doi: 10.1037/hea0000387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shoji, K., Cieslak, R., Smoktunowicz, E., Rogala, A., Benight, C. C., and Luszczynska, A. (2016). Associations between job burnout and self-efficacy: a meta-analysis. Anxiety Stress Coping 29, 367–386. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2015.1058369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skurka, C., Troy, C., Cui, Z., and Gil de Zúñiga, H. (2022). Efficacy constructs in media use and effects: organizing and appraising the literature. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 1–36. doi: 10.1080/23808985.2022.2142150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, K., and Dikkers, S. (2012). Amplifications of learning: Use of mobile media devices among youth. Convergence 18, 445–464. doi: 10.1177/1354856511429646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stabile, A. (2022). “TikTok addiction: Experts weigh in on the social media craze and what's behind the app's massive influence” Fox News. Available online at: https://www.foxnews.com/lifestyle/tiktok-addiction-experts-social-media-craze-app-massive-influence

Google Scholar

Sun, Y., and Zhang, Y. (2021). A review of theories and models applied in studies of social media addiction and implications for future research. Addict. Behav. 114, 106699. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valkenburg, P., Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., and Keijsers, L. (2021). Social media use and adolescents' self-esteem: heading for a person-specific media effects paradigm. J. Commun. 71, 56–78. doi: 10.1093/joc/jqaa039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valkenburg, P. M., Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., and Keijsers, L. (2022). Social media browsing and adolescent well-being: challenging the “passive social media use hypothesis”. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 27. doi: 10.1093/jcmc/zmab015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Wal, A., Valkenburg, P. M., and van Driel, I. I. (2022). In Their Own Words: How Adolescents Differ in Their Social Media Use and How it Affects Them. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/mvrpn

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verduyn, P., Gugushvili, N., and Kross, E. (2022). Do social networking sites influence well-being? the extended active-passive model. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 31, 62–68. doi: 10.1177/09637214211053637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., et al. (2015). Passive facebook usage undermines affective well-being: experimental and longitudinal evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 144, 480. doi: 10.1037/xge0000057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollmayr, B., and Gass, P. (2013). Learned helplessness: unique features and translational value of a cognitive depression model. Cell Tissue Res. 354, 171–178. doi: 10.1007/s00441-013-1654-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waddell, J. C., McLaughlin, C., LaRose, R., Rifon, N., and Wirth-Hawkins, C. (2014). Promoting Online Safety among Adolescents: Enhancing Coping Self-Efficacy And Protective Behaviors Through Enactive Mastery. In Communication and Information Technologies Annual. Emerald Group Publishing Limited. doi: 10.1108/S2050-206020140000008021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walther, J. B., Van Der Heide, B., Ramirez Jr, A., Burgoon, J. K., and Peña, J. (2015). Interpersonal and Hyperpersonal Dimensions of Computer-Mediated Communication. The Handbook of the Psychology of Communication Technology. P. 1−22. doi: 10.1002/9781118426456.ch1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, S., Messingschlager, T., and Stein, J. P. (2022). This is an insta-vention! exploring cognitive countermeasures to reduce negative consequences of social comparisons on Instagram. Media Psychol. 25, 411–440. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2021.1968440

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, G., Horwitz, J., and Seetharaman, D. (2021). “Facebook knows Instagram is toxic for teen girls, company documents show.” The Wall Street Journal. Available online at: https://www.wsj.com/articles/facebook-knows-instagram-is-toxic-for-teen-girls-company-documents-show-11631620739?mod=hp_lead_pos7andmod=article_inline

Google Scholar

Yau, J. C., and Reich, S. M. (2019). “It's just a lot of work”: Adolescents' self-presentation norms and practices on Facebook and Instagram. J. Res. Adolesc. 29, 196–209. doi: 10.1111/jora.12376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeager, D. S., Hanselman, P., Walton, G. M., Murray, J. S., Crosnoe, R., Muller, C., et al. (2019). A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature 573, 364–369. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1466-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Lin, W. Y., and Dutton, W. H. (2022). The political consequences of online disagreement: the filtering of communication networks in a polarized political context. Soc. Media Soc. 8. doi: 10.1177/20563051221114391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining Self-regulation: A Social Cognitive Perspective. In Handbook of self-regulation. Academic press, p. 13–39. doi: 10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50031-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: well-being, social media, mindsets, psychological well-being, agency, control, social cognitive theory

Citation: Lee AY, Ellison NB and Hancock JT (2023) To use or be used? The role of agency in social media use and well-being. Front. Comput. Sci. 5:1123323. doi: 10.3389/fcomp.2023.1123323

Received: 18 December 2022; Accepted: 06 March 2023;
Published: 03 April 2023.

Edited by:

Kostas Karpouzis, Panteion University, Greece

Reviewed by:

Barbara Caci, University of Palermo, Italy
Yannis Skarpelos, Panteion University, Greece
Nikolaos Tselios, University of Patras, Greece

Copyright © 2023 Lee, Ellison and Hancock. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Angela Y. Lee, YW5nZWxhOCYjeDAwMDQwO3N0YW5mb3JkLmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.