Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 26 April 2016
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Cognitive Reserve in Dementia: Implications for Cognitive Training

\r\nSara Mondini,,*Sara Mondini1,2,3*Ileana MadellaIleana Madella1Andrea Zangrossi,Andrea Zangrossi1,3Angela BigolinAngela Bigolin1Claudia Tomasi,Claudia Tomasi1,2Marta MichielettoMarta Michieletto1Daniele VillaniDaniele Villani2Giuseppina Di GiovanniGiuseppina Di Giovanni4Daniela Mapelli,Daniela Mapelli1,3
  • 1Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padova, Italy
  • 2Neuromotor Rehabilitation Unit, Casa di Cura Figlie di San Camillo, Cremona, Italy
  • 3Human Inspired Technology Research Centre, University of Padua, Padova, Italy
  • 4IRCCS Centro S. Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy

Cognitive reserve (CR) is a potential mechanism to cope with brain damage. The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of CR on a cognitive training (CT) in a group of patients with dementia. Eighty six participants with mild to moderate dementia were identified by their level of CR quantified by the CR Index questionnaire (CRIq) and underwent a cycle of CT. A global measure of cognition mini mental state examination (MMSE) was obtained before (T0) and after (T1) the training. Multiple linear regression analyses highlighted CR as a significant factor able to predict changes in cognitive performance after the CT. In particular, patients with lower CR benefited from a CT program more than those with high CR. These data show that CR can modulate the outcome of a CT program and that it should be considered as a predictive factor of neuropsychological rehabilitation training efficacy in people with dementia.

Introduction

The whole combination of variables including education, intelligence, cognitive learning and knowledge that one acquires throughout life is known as cognitive reserve (CR). The wider the spectrum of experiences and learning, the better the coping with brain damage (Stern, 2009, 2012). CR seems to have a relevant role in the context of degenerative diseases and it may explain the discrepancy between clinical manifestations and the underlying cerebral pathology.

Katzman et al. (1989) first reported findings on interesting incongruities between cognitive symptoms and the underlying neurological disease. Post-mortem examinations revealed that the brains of ten women showed clear histopathological signs of dementia, despite the absence of clinical cognitive signs in life. The authors attributed this incongruity to the bigger size of their brains—reflecting a greater number of neurons—later defined as brain reserve (BR; Stern, 2009, 2012). Mortimer et al. (2003) estimated that between 43% and 67% of individuals reach the threshold for a positive diagnosis of dementia at autopsy despite showing no signs of cognitive decline in life. BR is therefore a passive quantitative model of reserve. In addition, recent neuroimaging studies highlighted that more indirect indexes of neuropathology, such as anatomical changes (Liu et al., 2012), cerebral blood flow reduction (Kemppainen et al., 2008) and metabolic alterations (Hanyu et al., 2008) are found in people with higher level of CR suffering from dementia, despite their quite normal cognitive performance.

CR is an active mechanism which facilitates the flexible use of available BR through efficient information processing and strategies (Barulli and Stern, 2013; Xu et al., 2015). People with higher CR can cope with neuronal damage better than people with lower CR matched for level of pathology and BR (Steffener and Stern, 2012). CR may thus facilitate cognitive performance even in brain impairment and act as a dynamic, flexible and “plastic” mechanism in the brain.

This interpretation is consistent with data on the effects of an enriched environment in animal models in promoting neurogenesis and releasing brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which is fundamental for plasticity and brain changes (Brown et al., 2003; Nithianantharajah et al., 2009). Neuroplasticity is the ability of the brain to change according to environmental stimulations or after experiencing neurological damage (Wolf et al., 2006; Bosch et al., 2010). Indeed, living in an enriched environment seems to offset the cognitive decline due to Alzheimer’s disease (AD; Tucker and Stern, 2011).

CR is a theoretical construct that can be quantified through indirect proxies, such as education, occupational attainment and leisure time activities. Data from the literature provide evidence of a reduced risk of AD in people with high educational level (Scarmeas et al., 2004; Allegri et al., 2010). Some protective effects are found for occupations implying high responsibility (Qiu et al., 2003; Wajman and Bertolucci, 2010) and for leisure time activities involving learning, attention, memory and other intellectual skills (Fabrigoule et al., 1995; Wilson et al., 2013). Nevertheless, there is evidence that individuals with high CR have lower life expectancy and quicker progression of the disease after the onset of clinical symptoms of neurodegenerative disease (Hall et al., 2007). At first sight this appears to be counterintuitive, but it is consistent with neuroimaging studies showing a more extended cerebral pathology for high CR than low CR people with similar clinical severity (Hua et al., 2008; Vemuri et al., 2011). Thus, it is reasonable to suppose that, compared with low CR patients with AD, high CR patients’ brain capacity to re-organize networks and learn new information is reduced due to a more extended pathology.

This hypothesis has clear implications in the context of cognitive rehabilitation. Nowadays, findings on cognitive interventions in dementia are of great interest because of the documented existence of a certain degree of neuroplasticity even in the elderly and because of poor evidence of pharmacological treatment efficacy (Cotelli et al., 2012). Researchers are thus encouraged to focus their studies on non-pharmacological interventions. In the last decade many protocols of cognitive training (CT) intervention for AD patients have been validated and their efficacy proved (Yu et al., 2009; Bergamaschi et al., 2013; Mapelli et al., 2013). A set of tasks designed to involve cognitive functions, such as attention, memory, language and problem solving is typically included in any CT and combined with a reality orientation session. The training can be administered either individually or in group sessions with increased levels of difficulty. The belief is that practice with specific cognitive function tasks may improve, or at least maintain, functioning in a given domain and that any effects of practice can be generalized and can produce some improvement of cognitive and social functioning.

Not all the patients involved in these studies obtained the same benefits, probably because of the great inter-individual variability in the elderly (Gorus et al., 2008). To explain this variability many factors have been proposed, such as insight level, severity of behavioral disturbances and comorbidity with other pathologies (Zanetti et al., 2002; Binetti et al., 2013).

Other authors have signaled CR as a factor to take into account when evaluating the efficacy of CT (Olazarán et al., 2004; Berlucchi, 2011; Dorbath et al., 2013; Robertson, 2013). Olazarán et al. (2004) described an interesting effect of CR in old people with cognitive decline. They tried to validate the efficacy of a cognitive-motor intervention in people with minor cognitive impairment (MCI) and early AD and found that less educated benefited more from the rehabilitation program than the more educated. The authors explained their findings by referring to the CR paradigm: given a similar level of clinical symptoms, AD could be more advanced in more educated people and thus could limit their learning ability.

The present work endeavors to investigate the influence that CR might have in modulating the efficacy of a rehabilitation program in AD patients. Assuming that in high CR patients the underlying pathology could be more advanced, the learning process could be prevented or weakened. We hypothesized a cognitive treatment to have more positive outcomes in lower CR patients than in higher CR patients.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

A total of 86 participants aged 61–91 years, all community-dwelling people with mild to moderate dementia, were recruited from four different geriatric clinics in Northern Italy. Participants underwent a deep neurological examination, which included either magnetic resonance or computed tomography scans and a complete neuropsychological evaluation. All of them met the diagnostic criteria of the 5th revision of the diagnostic and statistical manual (DSM-5) for probable major NCD due to AD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria for probable AD (Dubois et al., 2007). Of all our subjects, 63 of them received a diagnosis of probable AD and the other 23 were diagnosed with mixed dementia. All patients were able to withstand a formal evaluation, and did not have behavioral disorders that could compromise the CT intervention. Participants and/or caregivers gave their written informed consent for participation in the study, which was approved by the Ethical Committee for the Psychological Research of the University of Padua. In order to evaluate the efficacy of CT we used a global measure of cognition, the Mini—mental state examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975). The use of this single quantitative score was done on purpose because it met the object of the study, having a score on the global cognitive status.

Participants’ level of CR was quantified by the CR Index questionnaire (CRIq; Nucci et al., 2012). The CRIq was administered to their caregivers (a family member), considering that memory and awareness deficits are typical of dementia. Table 1 shows demographic characteristics of all participants and Table 2 reports their clinical data.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of 86 participants (75% female) at baseline.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Psychometric evaluation of patients at baseline.

The MMSE was administered to all participants before the treatment (T0) to assess their level of cognitive impairment, and after the treatment (T1) in order to evaluate the change in their performance after a cycle of CT. Participants underwent a thorough neuropsychological assessment, but for the reasons explained above we will here report only the MMSE score, which is the most widely used and reliable instrument to assess global cognition in dementia. All patients had at baseline a cognitive status considered as mild to moderate dementia (MMSE; Mean = 20, 26; SD = 2, 79; range 14–25).

It is reported (Tombaugh, 2005) that the mean difference in MMSE test-retest (i.e., the practice effect) in an average time interval of 3 months is negligible in healthy subjects. Thus we administered the same version of the test both before and after the training without expecting any practice effects in patients. Furthermore, as reported by Rush (2000), the test-retest reliability of the MMSE in patients with dementia (most of them with Alzheimer’s type) ranged from 0.75 to 0.94 (Pearson’s r) in a number of studies that employed test-retest intervals from 1 day to 9 weeks.

Cognitive Reserve Index Questionnaire (CRIq)

CR is a complex construct that can be measured by an array of indirect indexes. The literature reports three main proxies of CR: education, occupational attainment, and leisure time activities. However, there is lack of agreement about the importance given to these variables and their quantification. With few exceptions (e.g., Pillai et al., 2012), most studies consider education as the fundamental proxy of CR and most studies converge to this proxy, excluding others, as shown in Meng and D’Arcy’s (2012) systematic meta-analysis.

Nucci et al. (2012) proposed a new tool to measure CR: the CRIq (free download at www.cri.psy.unipd.it). This instrument is a semi-structured interview that gathers and quantifies all the experiences that a person has acquired throughout their life. The CRIq includes demographic data and 20 items grouped into three sections: cognitive reserve index education score (CRI-Education), CRI-Working Activities and CRI-Leisure Time Activities. CRI-Education refers to a person’s school years plus any other educational course (e.g., learning to play a musical instrument or to speak a foreign language). The second section (CRI-Working Activities) considers five categories of working activities with different degrees of responsibility and demands. The number of years spent doing each job is calculated and considered. In the section on “Leisure Time Activities”, information regarding activities done weekly, monthly and yearly are recorded. Low correlation has been reported between these three areas (Nucci et al., 2012), so the information collected by the three sections of the questionnaire does not seem to be redundant and the three proxies can be considered independent of one another. The CR Index (CRI) is the total score resulted from the combination of the sub-scores of each single section, and it is adjusted for age to allow comparisons between groups of different age. The questionnaire requires about 10–15 min to be administered and since it is not a performance test, information can be provided by the patient’s caregiver thus avoiding vagueness or lack of information due to memory loss and poor insight. The questionnaire is a very useful and complete tool to quantify CR in AD patients.

Cognitive Training

All patients participated in a cycle of CT for a period of 2–4 months. They attended 1 or 2 h sessions once or twice a week, with an expert neuropsychologist, in small groups (5–6 patients in each group). As the CT was administered in different centers and at different times, the length of intervention varied: some patients were trained for 4 months (weekly 1 h sessions), others were trained for 2 months (weekly 2 h sessions). However, all patients received the same amount of training. The types of activities carried out were very similar to the CT described in the literature which has been reported in detail elsewhere (Bergamaschi et al., 2013; Mapelli et al., 2013). All paper-and-pencil exercises were taken from Bergamaschi et al. (2008) and Iannizzi et al. (2015). The specific exercises were designed to stimulate spatial and temporal orientation, memory, attention, language, perception, and visual analysis. This type of CT is the same described in Mapelli et al. (2013) and in Bergamaschi et al. (2013), and has proven its efficacy in decreasing severity of dementia and behavioral symptoms as well as in improving cognitive performance.

Each CT session started with personal, spatial and orientation tasks and continued with some exercises for the structured stimulation of cognitive domains. The specific exercises were designed to stimulate cognition and recognize emotional expressions. For example, in the time orientation exercises the patients had to perform recognition and naming tasks of pictures of clothing and tasks in which they had to associate clothing with pictures of the four seasons. In spatial orientation tasks the patients had to imagine going into different rooms of a house drawn on a plan and indicate the direction (left, right, etc.) of their movements. In the logical reasoning exercises the patients were asked either to complete simple puzzles or rearrange sentences in a logical sequence (e.g., I went to the supermarket, I bought vegetables, I cooked a soup, I ate it, I washed the dishes). Throughout the training, the difficulty of the CT exercises was gradually increased.

The aim of the present study is to ascertain the possible influence of CR on the outcomes of a cognitive rehabilitation program for patients with dementia.

Statistical Analysis

The considered dependent measure was the difference in MMSE scores between T1 and T0.

Thus three types of results were possible: (a) a positive value indicating improvement; (b) a negative value indicating worsening; and (c) a value equal to zero indicating unchanged performance. Fifty-seven participants (66.3%) improved the MMSE score between T0 and T1 (Mean = 2.67; SD = 1.44), fourteen (16.3%) worsened (Mean = −1.36; SD = 0.49) and fifteen (17.4%) showed no differences. The differences ranged from −2 to +7. High differences in MMSE score (ranging from −4 to +10) in a 3 months’ interval test-retest have been assessed on healthy individuals, as well (e.g., Tombaugh, 2005). We then performed multiple regression analyses to investigate the relation between the dependent variable, age and CR measures. The years of education were not considered since this information was taken into account by the CRI-Education. A total of seventeen models were built. The null model—Model 0—contained the intercept only. From Model 1 to 3, age and cognitive reserve index total score (CRI-Total) were considered as predictors: Model 1 included age only, Model 2 included CRI-Total score only and Model 3 included both age and CRI-Total score.

Then, from Model 4 to 17 the CRI sub-section scores were considered (CRI-Education, CRI-Working Activity and CRI-Leisure Time) and all the combinations of age and CRI sub-sections as predictors were evaluated (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Linear regression models compared through BIC, AIC, BF and R2.

Models considering CRI-Total score (Models 1–3) were compared via a set of indexes providing both absolute and relative goodness-of-fit measures: R2, Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Burnham and Anderson, 2002), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978) and Bayes Factor (BF; Kass and Raftery, 1995; Lavine and Schervish, 1999). The same procedure was followed comparing models considering CRI sub-section scores (Models 4–17). According with these indexes, the lower BIC and AIC and the higher BF and R2, the better model fit of the data. All statistical analyses were performed using R Software (R Core Team, 2013)1.

Results

All the previously described models, tested through regression analyses, were significant (p < 0.05) apart from Model 1 (with age as predictor) and Model 5 (with age and CRI-Education).

Based on the analysis of AIC, BIC, BF and R2, the best models (considering CRI-Total score and CRI sub-sections respectively) were identified as those that minimized AIC and BIC and maximized BF and R2 (Table 3).

For the CRI-Total score, the best model was Model 2 with CRI-Total score only as predictor (BIC = 362.27; AIC = 354.9; BF = 148.73; R2 = 0.16; p < 0.001). CRI-Total score was a significant influencing factor (t = −3.958, p < 0.001), with lower CRI-Total participants obtaining higher differences in MMSE scores between T1 and T0, if compared with higher CRI-Total participants (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effect of cognitive reserve index total (CRI-Total) score. Graphical representation of the effect of CRI-Total score on the difference in Mini—mental state examination (MMSE) score between T1 and T0, according to Model 2. The line indicates the regression line and the gray area indicates its 95% C.I.

For CRI sub-sections, Model 14 was the best model (BIC = 366.41; AIC = 356.59; BF = 41.49; R2 = 0.16; p < 0.001) with CRI-Working Activity (t = −0.028; p = 0.017) and CRI-Leisure Time (t = −0.022; p = 0.016) as significant predictors. This result indicates that participants with lower CRI-Working Activity and lower CRI-Leisure Time showed higher differences in MMSE scores between T1 and T0, when compared with participants with higher CRI-Working Activity and higher CRI-Leisure Time (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. CRI-Working Activity and CRI-Leisure Time effects. Graphical representation of the effects of CRI-Working Activity and CRI-Leisure Time on the difference in MMSE score between T1 and T0, according to Model 14. The line indicates the regression line and the gray area indicates its 95% C.I.

Discussion

The present study investigated the influence that CR may have in modulating the efficacy of a CT rehabilitation program in people with degenerative disease. Our results show that taking into account age and level of CR, only CR can predict a significant change in patients’ cognitive performance after rehabilitation training. In fact, in patients with similar clinical symptomatology this change takes the form of an improvement only in subjects with lower level of CR, while patients with higher CR do not seem to similarly benefit from the rehabilitation program. We suggest that in high CR patients the underlying pathology may be more advanced, thus their potential learning ability is reduced and diminishes with the progression of the disease.

These findings are consistent with the study by Olazarán et al. (2004) that highlighted an analogous role of CR in the context of rehabilitation. However, while they considered education as the only proxy for the reserve concept, in the present investigation other variables were also taken into account. An important role of working experiences and leisure time activities was found, that is, the other two proxies of CR measured with the CRIq. Our data show that improvement after treatment is inversely related to total CR (the lower CRI-Total index, the better the improvement) and to both occupational attainment (the lower CRI-Working Activities, the better the improvement) and free-time activities (the lower CRI-Leisure Time, the better the improvement). Thus, a CT program seems to be more beneficial for people with lower CR than for people with higher CR.

These results are of great interest because they highlight that CR cannot be equated to education (Meng and D’Arcy, 2012), nor quantified without considering the wider range of experiences and knowledge one acquires over the lifespan (i.e., working activities and leisure time activities). This does not assume that education by itself is irrelevant in the context of CR. Rather, in our study the lack of a significant effect of education (measured with the CRIq, CRI-Education) and cognitive performance after CT could be due to the demographic characteristics of our sample (about 80% attended only 5 years of formal education).

Altogether, our findings show that a CT program for patients with dementia is more beneficial for low CR than high CR patients. This can be explained by a more extended underlying neuropathology in high CR patients which prevents them from learning and improving their performance after a cycle of cognitive stimulation. A high CR initially allows people to cope better with neuropathology and to delay its clinical manifestation, but according to Stern’s model of CR, the development of pathology will eventually lead to the point after which reserve can no longer withstand the pathology. At this inflection point the progression of the disease is dramatically faster in people with high CR than in people with low CR (Stern, 2009).

All these data suggest that the building of reserve takes place along the lifespan, including adulthood, and thus support the presence of brain plasticity across the entire existence (Burgener et al., 2008; Mondini et al., 2014). Furthermore, this highlights that the CR measure must consider all lifespan activities that can stimulate cognition and the amount of CR.

To our knowledge the present work is the first that has investigated the influence of CR in modulating the efficacy of a rehabilitation program in people with degenerative disease. The results are promising. However, notwithstanding the large sample of patients involved in this research, they need to be replicated with the addition of other measures of global cognitive functioning and considering or comparing different types of rehabilitation programs.

Author Contributions

SM: contribution in collecting data, cohordinating the group and writing the manuscript. IM: contribution in collecting data and writing the manuscript. AZ: contribution in analyzing data and writing the manuscript. AB, CT, MM: contribution in collecting data and writing the manuscript. DV: contribution in selecting patients and writing the manuscript. GDiG: contribution in selecting patients and writing the manuscript. DM, DV: contribution in collecting data and writing the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by University of Padova, Department of General Psychology (ex 60% to SM).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Our thanks go to Dr. O. Zanetti for reading our manuscript and giving us his suggestions.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.R-project.org/

References

Allegri, R. F., Taragano, F. E., Krupitzki, H., Serrano, C. M., Dillon, C., Sarasola, D., et al. (2010). Role of cognitive reserve in progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia. Dement. Neuropsychol. 4, 28–34.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-V. Whashington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Barulli, D., and Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 502–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergamaschi, S., Arcara, G., Calza, A., Villani, D., Orgeta, V., and Mondini, S. (2013). One-year repeated cycles of cognitive training (CT) for Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 25, 421–426. doi: 10.1007/s40520-013-0065-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergamaschi, S., Iannizzi, P., Mondini, S., and Mapelli, D. (2008). Demenza: 100 Esercizi di Stimolazione Cognitiva. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore.

Berlucchi, G. (2011). Brain plasticity and cognitive neurorehabilitation. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 21, 560–578. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2011.573255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binetti, G., Moretti, D. V., Scalvini, C., di Giovanni, G., Verzelletti, C., Mazzini, F., et al. (2013). Predictors of comprehensive stimulation program efficacy in patients with cognitive impairment. Clinical practice recommendations. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 26–33. doi: 10.1002/gps.3785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosch, B., Bartrés-Faz, D., Rami, L., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Fernández-Espejo, D., Junqué, C., et al. (2010). Cognitive reserve modulate task-induced activations and deactivations in healthy elders, amnestic mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 46, 451–461. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J., Cooper-Kuhn, C. M., Kemperman, G., van Praag, H., Winkler, J., and Gage, F. H. (2003). Enriched environment and physical activity stimulate hippocampal but not olfactory bulb neurogenesis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 2042–2046. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02647.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgener, S. C., Yang, Y., and Marsh-Yant, S. (2008). The effects of multimodal intervention on outcomes of persons with early-stage dementia. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 23, 382–394. doi: 10.1177/1533317508317527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnham, K., and Anderson, D. (2002). Model Selection and Multimodal Inference. 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

Cotelli, M., Manenti, R., Zanetti, O., and Miniussi, C. (2012). Non-pharmacological intervention for memory decline. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:46. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorbath, L., Hasselhorn, M., and Titz, C. (2013). Effects of education on executive functioning and its trainability. Educ. Gerontol. 39, 314–325. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2012.700820

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., Dekosky, S. T., Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J., et al. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 6, 734–746. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(07)70178-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabrigoule, C., Letenneur, L., Dartigues, J. F., Zarrouk, M., Commenges, D., and Barbeger-Gateau, P. (1995). Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: a prospective longitudinal study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 43, 485–490. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1995.tb06093.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Gorus, E., De Raedt, R., Lambert, M., Lemper, J. C., and Mets, T. (2008). Reaction times and performance variability in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 21, 204–218. doi: 10.1177/0891988708320973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. B., Derby, C., LeValley, A., Katz, M. J., Verghese, J., and Lipton, R. B. (2007). Education delays accelerated decline on a memory test in persons who develop dementia. Neurology 69, 1657–1664. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000278163.82636.30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanyu, H., Sato, T., Shimizu, S., Kanetaka, H., Iwamoto, T., and Koizumi, K. (2008). The effect of education on rCBF changes in Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal SPECT study. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 35, 2182–2190. doi: 10.1007/s00259-008-0848-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hua, X., Leow, A. D., Parikshak, N., Lee, S., Chiang, M. C., Toga, A. W., et al. (2008). Tensor-based morphometry as a neuroimaging biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease: an MRI study of 676 AD, MCI and normal subjects. Neuroimage 43, 458–469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannizzi, P., Bergamaschi, S., Mondini, S., and Mapelli, D. (2015). Il Training Cognitivo per le Demenze e le Cerebrolesioni Acquisite. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore.

Google Scholar

Kass, R. R., and Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 90, 773–795. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzman, R., Aronson, M., Fuld, P., Kawas, C., Brown, T., Morgenster, H., et al. (1989). Development of dementing illnesses in an 80-year-old volunteer cohort. Ann. Neurol. 25, 317–324. doi: 10.1002/ana.410250402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemppainen, N. M., Aalto, S., Karrasch, M., Nagren, K., Savisto, N., Oikonen, V., et al. (2008). Cognitive reserve hypothesis: Pittsburgh compound B and fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography in relation to education in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 63, 112–118. doi: 10.1002/ana.21212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavine, M., and Schervish, M. J. (1999). Bayes factors: what they are and what they are not. The Am. Stat. 53, 119–122. doi: 10.2307/2685729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Julkunen, V., Paajanen, T., Westman, E., Wahlund, L. O., Aitken, A., et al. (2012). Education increases reserve against Alzheimer’s disease—evidence from structural MRI analysis. Neuroradiology 54, 929–938. doi: 10.1007/s00234-012-1005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mapelli, D., Di Rosa, E., Nocita, R., and Sava, D. (2013). Cognitive stimulation in patients with dementia: randomized controlled trial. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra 3, 263–271. doi: 10.1159/000353457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meng, X., and D’Arcy, C. (2012). Education and dementia in the context of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: a systematic review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses. PLoS One 7:e38268. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mondini, S., Guarino, R., Jarema, G., Kehayia, E., Nair, V., Nucci, M., et al. (2014). Cognitive reserve in a cross-cultural population: the case of Italian emigrants in montreal. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 26, 655–659. doi: 10.1007/s40520-014-0224-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mortimer, J. A., Snowdon, D. A., and Markesbery, W. R. (2003). Head circumference, education and risk of dementia: findings from the Nun study. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25, 671–679. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.671.14584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nithianantharajah, J., Barkus, C., Vijiaratman, N., Clement, O., and Hannan, A. J. (2009). Modeling brain reserve: experience-dependent neuronal plasticity in healthy and huntington’s disease transgenic mice. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 17, 196–209. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318196a632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nucci, M., Mapelli, D., and Mondini, S. (2012). Cognitive reserve index questionnaire [CRIq]: a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 24, 218–226. doi: 10.3275/7800

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olazarán, J., Muñiz, R., Reisberg, B., Peña-Casanova, J., del Ser, T., Cruz-Jentoft, A. J., et al. (2004). Benefits of cognitive-motor intervention in MCI and mild to moderate Alzheimer disease. Neurology 63, 2348–2353. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000147478.03911.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pillai, J. A., McEvoy, L. K., Hagler, D. J., Holland, D., Dale, A. M., Salmon, D. P., et al. (2012). Higher education is not associated with greater cortical thickness in brain areas related to literacy or intelligence in normal aging or mild cognitive impairment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 34, 925–935. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.702733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, C., Karp, A., von Strauss, E., Winblad, B., Fratiglioni, L., and Bellander, T. (2003). Lifetime principal occupation and risk of Alzheimer’s disease in the Kungsholmen project. Am. J. Ind. Med. 43, 204–211. doi: 10.1002/ajim.10159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team. (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. (ISBN 3-900051-07-0).

Google Scholar

Robertson, I. H. (2013). A noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve: implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rush, A. J. (2000). Handbook of Psychiatric Measures. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Scarmeas, N., Zarahn, E., Anderson, K. E., Honig, L. S., Park, A., Hilton, J., et al. (2004). Cognitive reserve-mediated modulation of positron emission tomographic activations during memory tasks in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 61, 73–78. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.1.73

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, G. E. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffener, J., and Stern, Y. (2012). Exploring the neural basis of cognitive reserve in aging. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1822, 467–473. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombaugh, T. N. (2005). Test-retest reliable coefficients and 5-year change scores for the MMSE and 3MS. Arch Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 485–503. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, A. M., and Stern, Y. (2011). Cognitive reserve in aging. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 8, 354–360. doi: 10.2174/156720511795745320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vemuri, P., Weigand, S. D., Przybelski, S. A., Knopman, D. S., Smith, G. E., Trojanowski, J. Q., et al. (2011). Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers are independent determinants of cognition. Brain 134, 1479–1492. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wajman, J. R., and Bertolucci, P. H. F. F. (2010). Intellectual demand and formal education ad cognitive protection factors in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Neuropsychol. 4, 320–324.

Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A., and Bennett, D. A. (2013). Life-span cognitive activity, neuropathologic burden and cognitive aging. Neurology 81, 314–321. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31829c5e8a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, S. A., Kronenberg, G., Lehmann, K., Blankenship, A., Overall, R., Staufenbiel, M., et al. (2006). Cognitive and physical activity differently modulate disease progression in the amyloid precursor protein [APP]-23 model of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1314–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, W., Yu, J. T., Tan, M. S., and Tan, L. (2015). Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Neurobiol. 51, 187–208. doi: 10.1007/s12035-014-8720-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, F., Rose, K. M., Burgener, S. C., Cunnigham, C., Buettner, L. L., Beattie, E., et al. (2009). Cognitive training for early-stage Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. J. Gerontol. Nurs. 35, 23–29. doi: 10.3928/00989134-20090301-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanetti, O., Oriani, M., Geroldi, C., Binetti, G., Frisoni, G. B., Di Giovanni, G., et al. (2002). Predictors of cognitive improvement after reality orientation in Alzheimer’s disease. Age Ageing 31, 193–196. doi: 10.1093/ageing/31.3.193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive reserve, dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, cognitive training, rehabilitation

Citation: Mondini S, Madella I, Zangrossi A, Bigolin A, Tomasi C, Michieletto M, Villani D, Di Giovanni G and Mapelli D (2016) Cognitive Reserve in Dementia: Implications for Cognitive Training. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:84. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00084

Received: 26 January 2016; Accepted: 04 April 2016;
Published: 26 April 2016.

Edited by:

Michael Hornberger, University of East Anglia, UK

Reviewed by:

Kyrana Tsapkini, Johns Hopkins Medicine, USA
Ramesh Kandimalla, Emory University, USA
Vanessa Taler, University of Ottawa, Canada

Copyright © 2016 Mondini, Madella, Zangrossi, Bigolin, Tomasi, Michieletto, Villani, Di Giovanni and Mapelli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sara Mondini, c2FyYS5tb25kaW5pQHVuaXBkLml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.