
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Psychol., 14 April 2025
Sec. Educational Psychology
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1569400
This article is part of the Research TopicPhysical Activity Applied to Learning and Psycho-social Variables in Young PeopleView all 22 articles
This study analyzed the relationship between bullying and cyberbullying, both as victims and aggressors, and the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in adolescents aged 10 to 16. A total of 1,330 Spanish students participated (48.95% boys), with an average age of 13.22 years. The Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) was used to assess five key learning strategies: rehearsal, elaboration, organization, critical thinking, and metacognitive self-regulation. The European Bullying Intervention Project Questionnaire (EBIP-Q) and the European Cyberbullying Intervention Project Questionnaire (ECIP-Q) were applied to evaluate levels of bullying and cyberbullying. The association between variables was analyzed through analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) and binary logistic regression analysis. The findings revealed a statistically significant negative association between bullying (traditional and cyberbullying) and the use of learning strategies for both victims and aggressors. Girls were more affected, particularly in cases of cyberbullying, where they showed lower scores in rehearsal, elaboration, and metacognitive self-regulation. In contrast, boys who were bullying aggressors scored higher in critical thinking. The risk of less frequent use of learning strategies among victims increased by 1.3 times for bullying and 2 times for cyberbullying. Similarly, this risk for aggressors rose by 1.4 times for boys and 1.8 times for girls in cases of bullying, and by 2.5 times for both genders in cases of cyberbullying. The study suggests implementing specific and cooperative actions involving students, teachers, and families to strengthen the proper use of learning strategies among victims and aggressors, especially in girls involved in cyberbullying episodes.
Learning is influenced by multiple factors, including cognitive, emotional, and social variables (Hayat et al., 2020). Among these, peer interactions play a crucial role in shaping students’ academic experiences, with bullying and cyberbullying being particularly detrimental (Armitage, 2021; Cosma et al., 2022). Victimization and aggression in school settings can negatively impact students’ ability to process information, regulate their learning, and stay motivated (Cañas et al., 2020). Given these effects, it is essential to examine how bullying relates to the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies (Aparisi et al., 2021). Cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies refer to the active mental processes students use to acquire, understand, and retain knowledge, although being aware of their strengths and limitations (Ramírez et al., 2022). Cognitive strategies for learning include rehearsal, elaboration, organization and critical thinking. On the other hand, metacognitive strategies focus on planning, monitoring and skills to control one’s own thinking, all of them determining elements in self-regulation (Pintrich et al., 1991). Both cognitive and metacognitive strategies are involved in the acquisition and consolidation of knowledge and both are key mental processes for the academic success of schoolchildren (Donker et al., 2014; Yip, 2021; Zambrano Ortega et al., 2021). In addition to enhancing self-management of learning, these strategies have been found to foster a proactive attitude toward evaluation and adaptation of study methods (Vásquez, 2021). In particular, cognitive strategies structure thinking to facilitate knowledge acquisition and retention (Neroni et al., 2019), whereas metacognitive strategies involve self-reflection and regulation of learning, promoting a growth mindset and self-efficacy (Neroni et al., 2019; Xu et al., 2022).
In line with the above, rehearsal serves as an introductory technique, activating information in working memory and proving useful for low-complexity tasks, though its effectiveness for consolidating long-term knowledge is limited, as it does not encourage integration with prior knowledge (Vásquez, 2021). Elaboration, on the other hand, involves paraphrasing, summarizing, and constructing analogies, facilitating long-term memory storage by building internal connections between new information and existing knowledge (Vásquez, 2021). Organization through grouping and outlining helps students select relevant information and establish links between new material and prior knowledge (Guo et al., 2021; Vásquez, 2021). Finally, critical thinking enables students to apply previous knowledge to novel situations, supporting problem-solving, decision-making, and evaluation according to high standards (Guo et al., 2021). In addition to these cognitive strategies, metacognitive self-regulation plays a complementary role, allowing students to plan, monitor, and regulate their cognitive processes, thereby preparing them for adaptive, self-directed learning (Guo et al., 2021; Pintrich et al., 1991; Vásquez, 2021). Importantly, both cognitive and metacognitive strategies are influenced by personal factors, including the satisfaction of psychological needs, a sense of autonomy, and academic self-efficacy (Hayat et al., 2020; Rubén and Gómez 2019). These factors can be severely compromised in contexts of stress, low self-esteem, and anxiety, often resulting from persistent bullying or cyberbullying behaviors (Aparisi et al., 2021; Menestrel, 2020; McLoughlin et al., 2020). Consequently, school bullying and cyberbullying may serve as catalysts for complex challenges with long-term negative effects on young people’s learning (McLoughlin et al., 2020; Aparisi et al., 2021). According to the Self-Regulated Learning Theory, adverse social experiences such as bullying can disrupt students’ ability to plan, monitor, and regulate their learning processes, ultimately affecting cognitive and metacognitive strategies (Li et al., 2018).
Bullying is a specific form of peer aggression that differs from general aggression in three key aspects: (1) repetition, meaning the behavior occurs over time rather than as an isolated incident, (2) intentionality, where the aggressor deliberately inflicts harm, and (3) power imbalance, which prevents the victim from defending themselves effectively (Waseem and Nickerson, 2023; UNESCO, 2019). Unlike general aggressive behaviors, which can be reactive or situational, bullying is systematic and involves a persistent dynamic of dominance and victimization (Bork-Hüffer et al., 2020). Furthermore, while general aggression may occur in response to provocation or frustration, bullying often involves strategic behaviors aimed at asserting social power over a weaker individual (Cosma et al., 2022). These distinctions are crucial in understanding the long-term psychological and educational impact of bullying. Given its repetitive nature and power imbalance, bullying involves clearly defined roles: the bully or aggressor is the individual who initiates and perpetuates the harassment, while the victim is the target of repeated aggression, often experiencing social, emotional, and academic consequences. Among these forms, traditional bullying is characterized by aggressive behavior, sometimes recurring, which manifests as a complex and harmful form of violence, occurring through direct face-to-face interactions and impacting both the physical integrity and the self-esteem of the victim (Waseem and Nickerson, 2023). According to UNESCO (2019), its prevalence varies by region, ranging from 22.8 to 48.2%. Cyberbullying, on the other hand, uses digital platforms to harass, humiliate, or threaten others, and is more common in psychological contexts, affecting girls more frequently (Vismara et al., 2022). According to Zhu et al. (2021), the prevalence of cyberbullying among young people can reach up to 57.5% in some regions. Unlike traditional bullying, cyberbullying can invade all areas of a person’s private life and give aggressors a sense of anonymity and impunity (Bork-Hüffer et al., 2020; Jones et al., 2015). Both bullying and cyberbullying victims experience a reduction in their emotional well-being and self-regulation capacity, exacerbated by anxiety and low self-esteem (Li et al., 2022; Estévez et al., 2019; Obregón-Cuesta et al., 2022). This situation can lead to difficulties in social relationships (Laninga-Wijnen et al., 2023), also affecting their ability to self-regulate their studies, concentrate, and limiting their working memory (Aparisi et al., 2021; Menestrel, 2020). However, this emotional and cognitive deterioration is not limited to victims. Aggressors also face serious negative consequences, including behavioral problems, declining academic performance, and increased tendencies toward disruptive conduct. Additionally, they often experience a significant loss of self-esteem and school motivation, which can perpetuate cycles of aggression and academic failure, further exacerbating the long-term impact of bullying (Weinreich et al., 2023). Research findings indicate that both victims and aggressors exhibit poor self-regulation, which results in a superficial approach to learning and a lower predisposition to use complex metacognitive strategies (Leiner et al., 2014; Weinreich et al., 2023). The final consequence is a decline in students’ acquisition of competencies and an increased risk of school dropout (Obregón-Cuesta et al., 2022; Weinreich et al., 2023).
In terms of gender, boys are more likely to be both victims and aggressors, frequently resorting to physical violence or threats, while girls tend to be more vulnerable to psychological harassment and are generally less involved in bullying behaviors (Cosma et al., 2022). However, some studies suggest that these differences may not always be significant, as bullying dynamics can vary depending on contextual and cultural factors (Zhu et al., 2021). Beyond bullying involvement, gender differences also extend to cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies. Girls demonstrate greater internal control, effectively applying motivation, self-assessment, and time management techniques, whereas boys tend to show lower self-regulation but excel in concentration and information processing (Ogden et al., 2023). Additionally, other biological and social factors influence students’ cognitive development and use of learning strategies. Age plays a role in students’ ability to handle complex learning and social situations (Urruticoechea et al., 2021; El Zaatari and Maalouf, 2022). Higher levels of maternal education are associated with greater parental expectations and, consequently, better academic performance (Tantoh, 2023). Moreover, families with more resources can provide their children with additional educational tools, such as tutoring and access to diverse learning materials, creating a more supportive academic environment (Fateel et al., 2021). Finally, variables such as Body Mass Index (BMI) and the amount of weekly physical activity are closely linked to executive functions (De Greeff et al., 2018; Raine et al., 2020) and influence students’ self-esteem and motivation, key aspects for academic success (Wassenaar et al., 2021). Therefore, in studying the processing of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies, it is essential to control, as much as possible, for the potential effect of these covariates.
This study is grounded in the Social-Ecological Model of Cyberbullying (Patel and Quan-Haase, 2022), which expands on Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory by integrating the digital environment as a critical factor shaping bullying experiences. This model emphasizes the interaction between individual characteristics, peer relationships, school policies, and digital media in influencing cyberbullying behaviors. From a policy perspective, this framework highlights the need for multi-tiered interventions that address not only school environments but also the influence of digital media on student behavior, recognizing that cyberbullying occurs within complex and interconnected social systems (Patel and Quan-Haase, 2022). Additionally, we draw on Self-Regulated Learning Theory (Li et al., 2018), which posits that students’ ability to monitor and regulate their learning is directly impacted by social stressors, such as victimization or aggressive behaviors, that create emotional and cognitive disruptions. In line with this, prior research has shown that bullying and cyberbullying negatively correlate with learning motivation (Aparisi et al., 2021) and academic performance (Huang, 2020; Obregón-Cuesta et al., 2022). However, despite evidence linking these phenomena to educational outcomes, the specific extent of their association with cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies remains unclear. Given that self-regulation is a key factor in academic achievement, understanding and quantifying how bullying and cyberbullying interfere with the development and application of these strategies in detail is crucial for designing effective educational interventions (Li et al., 2018). Based on this, the aim of the present study was to analyze the association between bullying and cyberbullying victimization/aggression and the cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies used by boys and girls aged 10 to 16. Given that victims and aggressors are the primary agents directly involved in these behaviors, this study focused on examining their potential impact on learning strategies. This age range was selected as it encompasses key developmental transitions, during which bullying behaviors peak and cognitive strategies become more sophisticated in response to increasing academic demands (Zhu et al., 2021; El Zaatari and Maalouf, 2022). Additionally, the study aimed to assess the level of risk posed by bullying and cyberbullying victimization/aggression in relation to lower scores in the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies. Accordingly, the specific research question was: Do students who engage more frequently in bullying/cyberbullying behaviors, whether as victims or aggressors, use cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies less frequently than their peers who are not involved in such behaviors? Based on this, the hypothesis was that bullying and cyberbullying victimization/aggression would be negatively associated with the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies among boys and girls aged 10–16.
A total of 1,330 Spanish children and adolescents (651 boys, 48.95%) from 7 educational centers participated in this cross-sectional quantitative study. These institutions include both primary schools and secondary schools that provided access to students aged 10 to 16 years. The selection of educational centers was based on convenience, with four centers being urban (>10,000 inhabitants) and three rural (<10,000 inhabitants), located in different provinces of southern Spain. These centers were chosen based on their availability and accessibility. Within each center, participants were selected using a random system of complete groups (intact classrooms). Anthropometric and sociodemographic characteristics are presented in Table 1. The participants were students aged between 10 and 16 years (13.22 ± 1.75 years). Boys had a higher BMI and recorded a higher level of weekly physical activity compared to girls (p = 0.037 and p < 0.001, respectively). Additionally, boys showed greater involvement in aggressive behaviors compared to girls (p = 0.007). On the other hand, girls scored higher in maternal education level (p < 0.001), academic performance (p = 0.007), and cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies, excluding critical thinking (all p < 0.001).
Learning strategies were assessed using the “Motivational Strategies for Learning Questionnaire” (Pintrich et al., 1991). This self-report instrument consists of 81 items, grouped into 15 subscales, aimed at assessing both motivational orientations toward course content and the use of various learning strategies. For the present study, only the section of the questionnaire relating to cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies, which includes 31 items, was used. These items make up a total of 5 subscales: (1) Rehearsal (e.g., “When I study for this class, I practice saying the material to myself over and over.”), (2) Elaboration (e.g., “When reading for this class, I try to relate the material to what I already know.”), (3) Organization (e.g., “When I study the readings for this course, I outline the material to help me organize my thoughts.”), (4) Critical Thinking (e.g., “When a theory, interpretation, or conclusion is presented in class or in the readings, I try to decide if there is good supporting evidence.”), and (5) Metacognitive Self-Regulation (e.g., “When I become confused about something I’m reading for this class, I go back and try to figure it out.”). Responses are recorded using a Likert-type format with seven different alternatives (1 = Completely false for me - 7 = Completely true for me). Low scores indicate minimal use of the learning strategy, although high scores reflect strong engagement with that strategy. The reliability of all corresponding subscales of the MSLQ questionnaire used in this research was acceptable (Cronbach’s α between 0.77 and 0.89).
The level of bullying was assessed using the “European Bullying Intervention Project Questionnaire,” Spanish version by Ortega-Ruiz et al. (2016), which includes a total of 14 items. This questionnaire evaluates both victimization (e.g., “Other students have pushed or hit me on purpose”) and aggression (e.g., “I have insulted a classmate to make them feel bad”). Reliability results are acceptable (Cronbach’s α for victimization = 0.83, Cronbach’s α for aggression = 0.79). On the other hand, to assess cyberbullying, the Spanish version of the “European Cyberbullying Intervention Project Questionnaire” (ECIPQ; Del Rey et al., 2015) was used, which includes a total of 22 items. This instrument also differentiates between cybervictimization (e.g., “Someone has posted offensive comments about me online”) and cyberaggression (e.g., “I have spread rumors about a classmate on social media”). Reliability results are acceptable (α for cybervictimization = 0.87, α for cyberaggression = 0.82). Both questionnaires distinguish between two dimensions (victimization and aggression) and use a Likert-type scale with scores ranging from 1 = never to 5 = more than once a week. Low scores indicate minimal experiences or involvement in bullying or cyberbullying, although high scores reflect frequent experiences of victimization or aggression. To ensure that the measured behaviors reflect bullying rather than general aggression or conflict, both instruments assess its defining characteristics: repetition over time and power imbalance. The questionnaires explicitly frame behaviors within the last 2 months, emphasizing their recurrent nature and helping to distinguish bullying from isolated aggression. Additionally, items assess situations where the victim is at a disadvantage (Ortega-Ruiz et al., 2016), while the cyberbullying questionnaire incorporates persistent online harassment and the perceived power asymmetry due to anonymity or lack of control (Del Rey et al., 2015). Both questionnaires were administered individually and took approximately 15 min to complete. The items explore the frequency of the behaviors described over the past 2 months. To minimize response bias, the questionnaire described behaviors without explicitly labeling them as “bullying” or “cyberbullying.” While participants were fully aware of the behaviors being assessed, avoiding an oral presentation of the questions to the entire class was intended to help students feel more at ease and reduce concerns about potential consequences or retaliation, both for victims and aggressors. This approach aimed to mitigate the influence of socially sensitive or invasive questions, which can lead respondents to modify their answers due to social desirability or discomfort (Choi and Pak, 2005; Runze and van IJzendoorn, 2024).
The age and mother’s education level of each participant were recorded through a sociodemographic data questionnaire. Age was considered a confounding variable due to its relevance in previous studies, which have shown that cognitive and emotional maturity significantly influence how individuals learn and interact with their environment (El Zaatari and Maalouf, 2022; Urruticoechea et al., 2021). It has also been established that the mother’s education level is significantly associated with academic performance and cognitive variables that strongly influence learning, such as self-regulation, attention, and working memory (Baharvand et al., 2021; Fateel et al., 2021).
The level of physical activity was included as a covariate, as recent research shows how physical activity influences cognitive development and academic performance in students (Li et al., 2023; Petrigna et al., 2022). It is also considered that both BMI and physical activity are related to physical and mental well-being, as well as students’ learning and self-esteem, and therefore may mediate the effectiveness of learning strategies (Bacon and Lord, 2021; Seum et al., 2022). BMI was calculated using the Quetelet formula: weight (kg) / height2 (m). A digital ASIMED® scale, type B, class III, and a portable SECA® 214 stadiometer (SECA Ltd., Hamburg, Germany) were used to measure weight and height. Both measurements were taken with light clothing and without shoes. Weekly physical activity level was assessed using the PACE+ Adolescent Physical Activity Measure (Prochaska et al., 2001). This questionnaire consists of two items that ask how many days participants accumulated 60 min of moderate or vigorous physical activity over the last 7 days and during a typical week. The final score was obtained by averaging both responses: (P1 + P2) / 2. Its reliability index was α = 0.79.
Before data collection began, parents, teachers, and the school administration were informed about the purpose of the study. Informed consent was obtained from the parents or legal guardians. Each participant’s name was coded to ensure anonymity and confidentiality. The measurements were conducted during school hours, as arranged by the schools. The questionnaires were completed in the usual classroom environment and were supervised by researchers and classroom tutors. This study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Jaén (Spain), reference NOV.22/2.PRY. The design complies with Spanish regulations on clinical research in humans (Law 14/2007, of July 3, on Biomedical Research), the regulations on data protection of private information (Organic Law 15/1999), and the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (2013, Brazil). It should be noted that this study was not preregistered.
The comparison of continuous and categorical variables between boys and girls was conducted using Student’s t tests and χ2 tests, respectively. The normality and homoscedasticity of the data were verified using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Levene tests, respectively. To examine whether adolescents who had never experienced bullying or cyberbullying victimization/aggression reported a higher use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies compared to those who had been victims or aggressors, an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed. Each cognitive and metacognitive learning strategy (rehearsal, elaboration, organization, critical thinking, and metacognitive self-regulation) was used as a dependent variable, and bullying victimization, bullying aggression, cyberbullying victimization, and cyberbullying aggression were introduced as fixed factors. Bullying and cyberbullying scores were dichotomized as follows: participants who reported never having been victims/aggressors of bullying and/or cyberbullying (questionnaire score = 1) were labeled as “Never,” whereas those who had been victims/aggressors at some point (questionnaire score > 1) were labeled as “Sometimes.” Given that many comparison groups had different sample sizes, effect size was calculated using Hedges’ ğ, where 0.2 = small effect, 0.5 = medium effect, and 0.8 = large effect (Martínez-López et al., 2018). The percentage difference between groups was calculated as: [(large measurement – small measurement) / small measurement] x 100. To assess the level of risk posed by bullying and cyberbullying victimization/aggression for lower values in the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies, binary logistic regression was conducted. For this, the dependent variables were dichotomized using the median as a reference (Kwon et al., 2020; Lepinet et al., 2023). In each strategy, participants were classified as high ≥ median (reference group) vs. low < median (risk group). Age, BMI, mother’s education level, and weekly physical activity were used as covariates in all analyses. Missing data were handled using listwise deletion, as the proportion of cases with missing data was low (≤5%). The missing values resulted from random individual errors, such as unanswered items due to oversight or illegible responses, rather than systematic patterns. Given this minimal percentage and its randomness, its impact on the results was negligible, and alternative methods such as multiple imputation or full information maximum likelihood (FIML) were not necessary. Listwise deletion was chosen to maintain data consistency while avoiding potential biases associated with imputation techniques (Işıkoğlu and Atar, 2020). All analyses were conducted separately for boys and girls. A 95% confidence level was used for all results (p < 0.05). All calculations were performed using SPSS statistical software, version 25.0 for WINDOWS (SPSS Inc., Chicago).
Girls who were victims of bullying reported a 7.3% lower use of critical thinking strategies compared to those who had never experienced victimization (4.98 ± 1.06 vs. 4.64 ± 1.22 u.a.) F(1,643) = 7.325, p = 0.007, ğ = 0.283, 1-β = 0.771 (Figure 1d). No statistically significant differences were found in any other learning variables for either girls or boys in relation to bullying victimization (all p > 0.05; Figure 1).
Figure 1. Differences between non-victims and victims of bullying in cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in boys and girls.
Meanwhile, girls who were victims of cyberbullying reported a lower use of Rehearsal: −3.8% (5.61 ± 1.01 vs. 5.40 ± 1.12 u.a.) F(1,643) = 6.945, p = 0.009, ğ = 0.184, 1-β = 0.749; Elaboration: −5.4% (5.25 ± 1.12 vs. 4.98 ± 1.12 u.a.) F(1,643) = 12.680, p < 0.001, ğ = 0.245, 1-β = 0.945; Organization: −2.9% (5.62 ± 1.24 vs. 5.46 ± 1.25 u.a.) F(1,643) = 4.462, p = 0.035, ğ = 0.132, 1-β = 0.559; Critical Thinking: −3.5% (4.78 ± 1.18 vs. 4.62 ± 1.22 u.a.) F(1,643) = 4.407, p = 0.036, ğ = 0.131, 1-β = 0.554; and Metacognitive Self-Regulation: −5.5% (5.37 ± 0.81 vs. 5.09 ± 0.92 u.a.), F(1,643) = 20.830, p < 0.001, 1-β = 0.995, ğ = 0.328 (Figures 2a–e, respectively). Among boys who were victims of cyberbullying, the results showed similar values to non-victims across the five learning strategies (all p > 0.05). An additional analysis of the average across the five learning strategies revealed that girls who were victims of bullying and cyberbullying scored 4.1 and 4.0% lower, respectively, than non-victims (5.38 ± 0.84 vs. 5.18 ± 0.93 u.a.) F(1,643) = 4.161, p = 0.042, ğ = 0.221, 1-β = 0.531 for bullying (Figure 1f), and (4.96 ± 0.92 vs. 4.77 ± 0.89 u.a.) F(1,643) = 6.761, p = 0.010, ğ = 0.304, 1-β = 0.738 for cyberbullying (Figure 2f).
Figure 2. Differences between non-victims and victims of cyberbullying in cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in boys and girls.
Girls who were bullying aggressors scored significantly lower across all cognitive and metacognitive learning variables: Rehearsal: −3.2% (5.67 ± 1.09 vs. 5.41 ± 1.14 u.a.) F(1,643) = 7.999, p = 0.005, ğ = 0.231, 1-β = 0.806; Elaboration: −6.8% (5.33 ± 1.05 vs. 4.99 ± 1.15 u.a.) F(1,643) = 12.380, p < 0.001, ğ = 0.301, 1-β = 0.940; Organization: −5.5% (5.74 ± 1.11 vs. 5.44 ± 1.30 u.a.) F(1,643) = 7.362, p = 0.007, ğ = 0.238, 1-β = 0.773; Critical Thinking: −5.4% (4.86 ± 1.17 vs. 4.61 ± 1.22 u.a.) F(1,643) = 7.812, p = 0.005, ğ = 0.207, 1-β = 0.797; and Metacognitive Self-Regulation: −4.9% (5.38 ± 0.98 vs. 5.13 ± 0.90 u.a.), F(1,643) = 11.155, ğ = 0.276, p = 0.001, 1-β = 0.915 (Figures 3a–e, respectively). On the other hand, boys who were aggressors scored 5.5% higher in Critical Thinking compared to non-aggressors (4.63 ± 1.12 vs. 4.39 ± 1.27 u.a.) F(1,596) = 4.297, p = 0.039, ğ = 0.206, 1-β = 0.544 (Figure 3d).
Figure 3. Differences between non-aggressors and aggressors in bullying in cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in boys and girls.
The results also showed that girls who were cyberbullying aggressors had lower values in four of the analyzed variables: Rehearsal: −2.6% (5.09 ± 1.18 vs. 4.96 ± 1.15 u.a.) F(1,643) = 5.403, p = 0.020, ğ = 0.108, 1-β = 0.641; Elaboration: −5.5% (5.21 ± 1.13 vs. 4.94 ± 1.12 u.a.) F(1,643) = 10.782, p = 0.001, ğ = 0.243, 1-β = 0.906; Critical Thinking: −4.8% (4.78 ± 1.20 vs. 4.56 ± 1.21 u.a.) F(1,643) = 5.090, p = 0.024, ğ = 0.179, 1-β = 0.615; and Metacognitive Self-Regulation: −4.7% (5.31 ± 0.89 vs. 5.07 ± 0.87 u.a.), F(1,643) = 11.897, p = 0.001, ğ = 0.276, 1-β = 0.931 (Figures 4a,b,d,e, respectively). Meanwhile, boys who were cyberbullying aggressors scored 4.5% higher in Critical Thinking (4.68 ± 0.97 vs. 4.48 ± 1.27 u.a.) F(1,596) = 6.629, p = 0.010, ğ = 0.176, 1-β = 0.729 (Figure 4d) compared to non-aggressors. No significant differences were found in the Organization strategy (p > 0.05; Figure 4c). An additional analysis, performed on the average of the five learning strategies, showed that girls who were bullying and cyberbullying aggressors scored 4.3 and 3.9% lower, respectively, than non-aggressors (5.33 ± 0.89 vs. 5.11 ± 0.94 u.a.) F(1,643) = 12.500, p < 0.001, ğ = 0.239, 1-β = 0.942 (Figure 3f) for bullying, and (5.29 ± 0.94 vs. 5.09 ± 0.89 u.a.) F(1,643) = 7.035, p = 0.003, ğ = 0.221, 1-β = 0.833 (Figure 4f) for cyberbullying.
Figure 4. Differences between non-aggressors and aggressors in cyberbullying in cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in boys and girls.
Data showing the risk of exposure to bullying and cyberbullying victimization in relation to cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies are presented in Table 2. Girls who were victims of bullying were 1.26 and 1.28 times more likely, and thus at higher risk than non-victims, of having low use of Elaboration (Odds Ratio [OR] = 1.265; p = 0.034) and Organization (OR = 1.281; p = 0.027), respectively. However, no differential risk was found in boys for any of the five factors analyzed (all p > 0.05). On the other hand, girls who were victims of cyberbullying were more likely than non-victims to have lower scores in Rehearsal (OR = 1.724; p = 0.005), Elaboration (OR = 2.098; p < 0.001), and Metacognitive Self-Regulation (OR = 2.794; p < 0.001). The analysis in boys only showed a statistically significant risk for Metacognitive Self-Regulation (OR = 1.606; p = 0.024). An additional analysis, conducted on the average of the five learning strategies, showed that both girls and boys who were victims of bullying were more likely than non-victims to have low use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies (OR = 1.287; p = 0.025; OR = 1.266; p = 0.037, respectively). The data also revealed that girls and boys who were victims of cyberbullying were at higher risk of having lower scores in the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies (OR = 1.905; p = 0.001; OR = 1.866; p = 0.003).
Table 2. Binary logistic regression for bullying and cyberbullying victimization (1 = never - 5 = more than once/week) according to categorized indicators (high vs. low) of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in adolescent boys and girls.
Data showing the risk of exposure to bullying and cyberbullying aggression in relation to cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies are presented in Table 3. Girls who were bullying aggressors were 1.77, 1.78, 1.32, and 1.63 times more likely than non-aggressors to have low scores in Rehearsal (OR = 1.777; p < 0.001), Elaboration (OR = 1.787; p < 0.001), Critical Thinking (OR = 1.329; p = 0.049), and Metacognitive Self-Regulation (OR = 1.635; p = 0.001), respectively. However, in boys who were aggressors, only a statistically significant risk of low scores in metacognitive self-regulation was found (OR = 1.408; p = 0.024). On the other hand, girls who were cyberbullying aggressors were more likely to have low scores in Rehearsal (OR = 2.325; p < 0.001), Elaboration (OR = 2.571; p < 0.001), Critical Thinking (OR = 1.616; p = 0.029), and Metacognitive Self-Regulation (OR = 3.851; p < 0.001). Among boys who were cyberbullying aggressors, a statistically significant risk of low scores was found only for Rehearsal (OR = 1.801; p = 0.020) and Metacognitive Self-Regulation (OR = 2.497; p = 0.001). An additional analysis, conducted on the average of the five learning strategies, showed that both girls and boys who were bullying aggressors were more likely than non-aggressors to have lower use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies (OR = 1.768; p < 0.001 and OR = 1.388; p = 0.028, respectively). The data also revealed that, in general, girls and boys who were cyberbullying aggressors were 2.54 and 2.48 times more likely, respectively, to have lower scores than non-aggressors in the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies (OR = 2.549; p = 0.001 and OR = 2.489; p < 0.001, respectively).
Table 3. Binary logistic regression for bullying and cyberbullying aggression (1 = never - 5 = more than once/week) according to categorized indicators (high vs. low) of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in adolescent boys and girls.
The main objective of this study was to analyze the association between bullying and cyberbullying victimization/aggression and the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in children and adolescents aged 10–16. The results revealed that, regardless of being a victim or aggressor of bullying and/or cyberbullying, almost all bullying behaviors are negatively associated with the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies, especially in girls. Table 4 summarizes the data for victims and aggressors in bullying and cyberbullying scenarios, differentiating the cognitive and metacognitive variables along with the percentage decreases/increases and associated risk increments.
Our data show that girls who are victims of bullying exhibit 7.3% lower use of critical thinking compared to non-victims. This confirms that persistent harassment not only compromises the mental health and emotional well-being of adolescents but also hinders the use of learning strategies and negatively impacts academic performance (Armitage, 2021; Deol and Lashai, 2022; Menestrel, 2020). We also found that when the harassment occurs through cyberbullying, victimized girls present particularly low scores in rehearsal, elaboration, organization, critical thinking, and metacognitive self-regulation. It seems that cyberbullying victims suffer a more severe impact than those of traditional bullying due to the pervasive nature of digital harassment, which generates constant stress and profoundly affects learning motivation and self-regulation (Bork-Hüffer et al., 2020; Aparisi et al., 2021). More specifically, in the analyzed girls who suffer from cyberbullying, we found that the risk of having lower engagement in elaboration and rehearsal increases by 2.1 and 1.7 times, respectively. These results support previous studies that concluded cyberbullying is associated with negative metacognitive beliefs, such as perceptions of uncontrollability and excessive responsibility, which affect cognitive confidence, particularly in memory, and lead to school avoidance behaviors (McLoughlin et al., 2022).
Among all the strategies mentioned, we found that metacognitive self-regulation is the most affected, with cyberbullying victims being twice as likely to have lower scores than non-victims. In the context of an adolescent who is being bullied, having low cognitive self-regulation can impact their learning in various ways. For example, in class, instead of paying attention to the lesson or processing information, the adolescent may be distracted by thoughts about the bullying, such as fear of future incidents or reliving past experiences (Vacca et al., 2023). This emotional distress can make it difficult to organize thoughts, keep track of tasks, or manage time effectively. Additionally, the victim may experience feelings of doubt and low self-esteem, further weakening their ability to regulate cognitive processes and fully engage in learning (Deol and Lashai, 2022; Laninga-Wijnen et al., 2023).
On the other hand, the data presented here highlight the need to study the effects of bullying on learning separately by gender. Although girls who are victims of bullying and cyberbullying experience a more severe impact on the use of cognitive strategies, boys who are victims of bullying or cyberbullying do not seem to be as negatively affected in the use of these strategies. Previous studies report greater emotional vulnerability to bullying in females, which affects their ability to organize and control the information necessary for effective learning (Menestrel, 2020). Additionally, the stress, anxiety, and low self-esteem that girls often experience in these situations interfere with their ability to manage cognitive processes such as planning and organizing learning (Hayat et al., 2020; Leiner et al., 2014). However, in boys, this weaker association could be explained by their greater exposure to physical bullying or direct aggression, leading them to activate immediate defense mechanisms, such as verbal or physical confrontations (Gomes et al., 2022). These defensive mechanisms help mitigate the impact of bullying on cognitive and metacognitive processes, partially preserving their short-term performance (Ogden et al., 2023; Cosma et al., 2022).
Our results revealed that girls who are bullying and cyberbullying aggressors scored significantly lower in rehearsal, elaboration, critical thinking, and metacognitive self-regulation. This implies that bullying behaviors not only affect the victims but also negatively impact the aggressors, causing difficulties in organizing and planning their studies, as well as in emotional self-regulation and social interaction (Aparisi et al., 2021; Cañas et al., 2020). We also observed that girls who are cyberbullying aggressors are at a higher risk of obtaining low scores in skills such as rehearsal, elaboration, critical thinking, and especially metacognitive self-regulation, where the risk can increase up to four times. When this is combined with the difficulty in managing negative emotions, such as anger and frustration, it likely results in a limitation in their ability to focus on tasks that require reflection and deep analysis (Yousefi et al., 2021; Estévez et al., 2019). Furthermore, whereas the anonymity of cyberbullying allows young aggressors to avoid immediate consequences, it does not mitigate the long-term emotional impact, increasing the likelihood of using learning strategies ineffectively (Vismara et al., 2022). It seems that this emotional dysregulation could be strongly linked to cognitive instability, which affects their ability to organize thoughts and plan effectively, ultimately harming their academic performance (Cosma et al., 2022; Hawley, 2015).
On the other hand, boys who are aggressors show higher use of critical thinking (5.5% for bullying and 4.5% for cyberbullying), along with high-risk values indicating a decrease in the use of cognitive strategies, particularly in metacognitive self-regulation. Although metacognitive self-regulation and critical thinking are related, they do not always develop simultaneously. Previous studies suggest that metacognitive self-regulation facilitates critical thinking, but its absence does not necessarily inhibit it (Efklides and Metallidou, 2020; Gurcay and Ferah, 2018). Among aggressors, the social context and power dynamics in bullying may encourage the use of critical thinking to justify or plan their actions (Cañas et al., 2020; Pan et al., 2023). These boys may, therefore, use critical thinking strategically to assess vulnerabilities and manipulate situations, protecting their social status, even though they do so destructively (Cosma et al., 2022; Hawley, 2015).
At this point, it is worth emphasizing some behavioral differences based on gender, which are likely influenced by the type of aggression. Girls who are aggressors tend to engage in more subtle, relational forms of aggression, such as social manipulation, exclusion, or spreading rumors, which carry a significant emotional burden (Vismara et al., 2022). This type of aggression (manipulation, exclusion, or rumor-spreading) demands a high level of emotional management, requiring girls to be constantly attuned to the social effects of their actions and the reactions of their victims. This can be cognitively exhausting and deplete their resources for using more complex learning strategies (Gomes et al., 2022; Obregón-Cuesta et al., 2022). In contrast, boys who are aggressors tend to use more direct and physical forms of aggression, which, although confrontational, do not require the same level of constant emotional monitoring. They often make impulsive decisions under pressure, limiting their ability to plan long-term, reflect on their actions, and evaluate consequences, negatively affecting their self-regulation and effective learning (Obregón-Cuesta et al., 2022). Additionally, the constant stress from physical aggression can raise cortisol levels, interfering with key cognitive processes like decision-making, planning, and concentration (James et al., 2023). Although this type of direct aggression may foster pragmatic and critical thinking skills in the short term, in the long run, it negatively impacts their ability to engage in deeper learning, which requires reflection and self-regulation (Gomes et al., 2022).
The findings of this study have important implications for educational policies and interventions. The Social-Ecological Model of Cyberbullying (Patel and Quan-Haase, 2022) highlights the need for multi-tiered strategies that address both school environments and digital interactions, as both influence students’ learning processes. Likewise, Self-Regulated Learning Theory (Li et al., 2018) underscores how bullying-related stress disrupts cognitive and metacognitive strategies, impairing students’ ability to plan, monitor, and regulate their learning. Addressing these challenges requires targeted interventions that strengthen self-regulation, resilience, and emotional coping skills.
Based on these considerations, prior research (Cañas et al., 2020; Efklides and Metallidou, 2020; Patel and Quan-Haase, 2022) and our data, Table 5 presents a number of study-based recommendations aimed at strengthening the use of learning strategies for both victims and aggressors of bullying and cyberbullying. These guidelines are tailored for students, teachers, and families. Gender differences and the type of aggression have been considered in their classification. It is important to note that while these recommendations are grounded in prior literature and our findings, they require further empirical validation before being considered evidence-based. These specifications do not detract from the primary focus on prevention, which seeks to avoid such consequences in the first place. In fact, to ensure the physical and mental well-being of victims, it is essential to implement preventive strategies, as well as provide awareness programs for aggressors to dissuade them from engaging in these bullying behaviors.
Table 5. Study-based recommendations, differentiated by gender, type of aggression (bullying or cyberbullying), and role (victim or aggressor) to maximize the impact of interventions.
This study has several methodological limitations that should be noted. Among them is its cross-sectional design, which does not allow for the establishment of causal relationships. Additionally, the sample was one of convenience, which limits its representativeness. However, the strength of the study lies in several key practices: coding techniques were applied to ensure participant anonymity and confidentiality, highly reliable measurement instruments with proven internal validity were used, and a wide range of important covariates such as age, body mass index, mother’s educational level, and weekly physical activity were considered. All of this allowed for the identification of specific results and risk levels, previously unknown, which could contribute to significant advancements in the field of education.
The present study concludes that the use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies among young people is negatively associated with both victimization and perpetration in bullying and cyberbullying. There is a more pronounced negative association for girls, with particularly severe effects observed in cases of cyberbullying. Girls who were victims of bullying were 7.3% less likely to use critical thinking than non-victims. On the other hand, girls who were victims of cyberbullying were also less likely to use the strategies of rehearsal (3.8%), elaboration (5.4%), critical thinking (3.5%) and metacognitive self-regulation (5.5%). The latter are the most affected, as girls and boys who are victims of cyberbullying are 2.8 and 1.6 times more likely, respectively, to use less them. Girls are 2.1 and 1.7 times more likely to have low use of elaboration and rehearsal strategies. In addition, aggressor girls mainly show a lower use of elaboration (6.8% for bullying and 5.5% for cyberbullying), critical thinking (5.4 and 4.8%) and metacognitive self-regulation (4.9 and 4.7%). In contrast, aggressor boys show a higher use of critical thinking (5.5% for bullying and 4.5% for cyberbullying). In terms of probability, aggressor girls’ risk of low use of learning strategies is multiplied in the factors of rehearsal (x1.8 for bullying and x2.3 for cyberbullying), elaboration (x1.8 and x2.6), critical thinking (x1.3 and x1.6) and metacognitive self-regulation (x1.6 and x3.9), whereas for boys it is only observed in the factor of metacognitive self-regulation (x1.4 and x2.5).
It is suggested to carry out specific and collaborative actions between students, teachers and families to strengthen the appropriate use of learning strategies in victims and aggressors, especially girls who are immersed in cyberbullying episodes. It is also suggested to implement clear prevention policies and to define strong consequences for aggressors in order to ensure the well-being of students and to promote an environment that facilitates effective learning.
The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the data from this study cannot be shared as it is part of a larger research project involving multiple researchers, and maintaining the confidentiality of participants is a top priority. One of the conditions for participation was that the data would be used exclusively by our research team for the specific purposes of this project. Sharing the data externally, even if anonymized, would violate the ethical commitment made with participants and could compromise the integrity of the larger research project. We adhere strictly to ethical and legal regulations, such as the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR), to ensure the privacy and trust of all participants. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to requests for further information or specific queries regarding the study can be directed to Jose Luis Solas Martínez (anNvbGFzQHVqYWVuLmVz). Please note that, in adherence to ethical commitments and confidentiality agreements with participants, the dataset itself cannot be provided. However, we are happy to address questions or provide aggregated information where possible, ensuring the privacy and trust of all participants remain protected.
The studies involving humans were approved by University of Jaen (code reference: NOV.22/2.PRY). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.
JS-M: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft. MT-C: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AR-M: Data curation, Writing – original draft. EM-L: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by funding from the Ministry of Science and Innovation of Spain (grant number PID2022-137432OB-I00).
The authors would like to thank all participants and centers involved in this study.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1569400/full#supplementary-material
Aparisi, D., Delgado, B., Bo, R. M., and Martínez-Monteagudo, M. C. (2021). Relationship between cyberbullying, motivation and learning strategies, academic performance, and the ability to adapt to university. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:10646. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182010646
Armitage, R. (2021). Bullying in children: impact on child health. BMJ Paediatrics Open 5, 1–8. doi: 10.1136/bmjpo-2020-000939
Bacon, P., and Lord, R. N. (2021). The impact of physically active learning during the school day on Children’s physical activity levels, time on task and learning Behaviours and academic outcomes. Health Educ. Res. 36, 362–373. doi: 10.1093/her/cyab020
Baharvand, P., Nejad, E. B., Karami, K., and Amraei, M. (2021). A review study of the role of socioeconomic status and its components in Children’s health. Global J. Med. Pharmaceut. Biomed. 16:9. doi: 10.25259/gjmpbu_10_2021
Bork-Hüffer, T., Mahlknecht, B., and Kaufmann, K. (2020). Cyberbullying in schools–when bullying stretches across CON/FFlating spaces. Child. Geograph 19, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/14733285.2020.1784850
Cañas, E., Estefanía Estévez, M., Martínez-Monteagudo, C., and Delgado, B. (2020). Emotional adjustment in victims and perpetrators of cyberbullying and traditional bullying. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 23, 917–942. doi: 10.1007/s11218-020-09565-z
Choi, B. C., and Pak, A. W. (2005). A catalog of biases in questionnaires. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2:A13.
Cosma, A., Bjereld, Y., Elgar, F. J., Richardson, C., Bilz, L., Craig, W., et al. (2022). Gender differences in bullying reflect societal gender inequality: a multilevel study with adolescents in 46 countries. J. Adolesc. Health 71, 601–608. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2022.05.015
De Greeff, J. W., Bosker, R. J., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., and Hartman, E. (2018). Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595
Del Rey, R., Casas, J. A., Ortega-Ruiz, R., Schultze-Krumbholz, A., Scheithauer, H., Smith, P., et al. (2015). Structural validation and cross-cultural robustness of the European cyberbullying intervention project questionnaire. Comput. Hum. Behav. 50, 141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.065
Deol, Y., and Lashai, M. (2022). Impact of cyberbullying on adolescent mental health in the midst of pandemic – hidden crisis. Eur. Psychiatry 65:S432. doi: 10.1192/j.eurpsy.2022.1096
Donker, A. S., de Boer, H., Kostons, D., Dignath van Ewijk, C. C., and van der Werf, M. P. C. (2014). Effectiveness of learning strategy instruction on academic performance: a Meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 11, 1–26. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2013.11.002
Efklides, A., and Metallidou, P. (2020). Metacognition, motivation, and learning. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education. Oxford University Press.
El Zaatari, W., and Maalouf, I. (2022). How the Bronfenbrenner bio-ecological system theory explains the development of students’ sense of belonging to school? SAGE Open 12, 1–18. doi: 10.1177/21582440221134089
Estévez, E., Jiménez, T. I., and Segura, L. (2019). Emotional intelligence and empathy in aggressors and victims of school violence. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 488–496. doi: 10.1037/edu0000292
Fateel, M. J., Mukallid, S., and Arora, B. (2021). The interaction between socioeconomic status and preschool education on academic achievement of elementary school students. Int. Educ. Stud. 14:60. doi: 10.5539/ies.v14n8p60
Gomes, A., Martins, M. C., Silva, B., Ferreira, E., Nunes, O., and Caldas, A. C. (2022). How different are girls and boys as bullies and victims? Comparative perspectives on gender and age in the bullying dynamics. Int. J. Educ. Psychol. 11, 237–260. doi: 10.17583/ijep.9310
Guo, H., Tong, F., Wang, Z., Tang, S., Yoon, M., Ying, M., et al. (2021). Examining self-regulated learning strategy model: a measurement invariance analysis of Mslq-Cal among college students in China. Sustainability 13:10133. doi: 10.3390/su131810133
Gurcay, D., and Ferah, H. O. (2018). High school students’ critical thinking related to their metacognitive self-regulation and physics self-efficacy beliefs. J. Educ. Train. Stud. 6:125. doi: 10.11114/jets.v6i4.2980
Hawley, P. H. (2015). Social dominance in childhood and its evolutionary underpinnings: why it matters and what we can do. Pediatrics 135, S31–S38. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3549D
Hayat, A. A., Shateri, K., Amini, M., and Shokrpour, N. (2020). Relationships between academic self-efficacy, learning-related emotions, and metacognitive learning strategies with academic performance in medical students: a structural equation model. BMC Med. Educ. 20:76. doi: 10.1186/s12909-020-01995-9
Huang, L. (2020). Exploring the relationship between school bullying and academic performance: the mediating role of students’ sense of belonging at school. Educ. Stud. 48, 216–232. doi: 10.1080/03055698.2020.1749032
Işıkoğlu, M. A., and Atar, B. (2020). Investigation of the effect of missing data handling methods on measurement invariance of multi-dimensional structures. J. Measure. Eval. Educ. Psychol. 11, 311–323. doi: 10.21031/epod.749370
James, K. A., Stromin, J. I., Steenkamp, N., and Combrinck, M. I. (2023). Understanding the relationships between physiological and psychosocial stress, cortisol and cognition. Front. Endocrinol. 14:1085950. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2023.1085950
Jones, L. M., Mitchell, K. J., and Turner, H. A. (2015). Victim reports of bystander reactions to in-person and online peer harassment: a National Survey of adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 2308–2320. doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0342-9
Kwon, S., Welch, S., and Mason, M. (2020). Physical education environment and student physical activity levels in low-income communities. BMC Public Health 20, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-8278-8
Laninga-Wijnen, L., van den Berg, Y. H. M., Garandeau, C. F., Mulder, S., and de Castro, B. O. (2023). Supplemental material for does being defended relate to decreases in victimization and improved psychosocial adjustment among victims? J. Educ. Psychol. 363–377. doi: 10.1037/edu0000712.supp
Leiner, M., Dwivedi, A. K., Villanos, M. T., Singh, N., Blunk, D., and Peinado, J. (2014). Psychosocial profile of bullies, victims, and bully-victims: a cross-sectional study. Front. Pediatr. 2, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fped.2014.00001
Lepinet, U., Tanniou, J., Communier, E. C., Créach, V., and Beaumont, M. (2023). Impact of anthropometric data and physical activity level on the closed kinetic chain upper extremity stability test (CKCUEST) score: a cross-sectional study. Phys. Ther. Sport 64, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.ptsp.2023.09.004
Li, C., Wang, P., Martin-Moratinos, M., Bella-Fernández, M., and Blasco-Fontecilla, H. (2022). Traditional bullying and cyberbullying in the digital age and its associated mental health problems in children and adolescents: a Meta-analysis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 33, 2895–2909. doi: 10.1007/s00787-022-02128-x
Li, D., Wang, D., Zou, J., Li, C., Qian, H., Yan, J., et al. (2023). Effect of physical activity interventions on Children’s academic performance: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Eur. J. Pediatr. 182, 3587–3601. doi: 10.1007/s00431-023-05009-w
Li, J., Ye, H., Tang, Y., Zhou, Z., and Hu, X. (2018). What are the effects of self-regulation phases and strategies for Chinese students? A Meta-analysis of two decades research of the association between self-regulation and academic performance. Front. Psychol. 9:2434. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02434
Martínez-López, E. J., De La Torre-Cruz, M., Suarez-Manzano, S., and Ruiz-Ariza, A. (2018). Analysis of the effect size of overweight in muscular strength tests among adolescents: reference values according to sex, age, and body mass index. J. Strength Cond. Res. 32, 1404–1414. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000001967
McLoughlin, L. T., Lagopoulos, J., and Hermens, D. F. (2020). Cyberbullying and adolescent neurobiology. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01511
McLoughlin, L. T., Simcock, G., Schwenn, P., Beaudequin, D., Driver, C., Lee, K.-D., et al. (2022). Cyberbullying, metacognition, and quality of life: preliminary findings from the longitudinal adolescent brain study (LABS). Discov. Psychol. 2:5. doi: 10.1007/s44202-021-00013-3
Menestrel, S. L. (2020). Preventing bullying: consequences, prevention, and intervention. J. Youth Develop. 15, 8–26. doi: 10.5195/JYD.2020.945
Neroni, J., Meijs, C., Gijselaers, H. J. M., Kirschner, P. A., and de Groot, R. H. M. (2019). Learning strategies and academic performance in distance education. Learn. Individ. Differ. 73, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2019.04.007
Obregón-Cuesta, A. I., Mínguez-Mínguez, L. A., León-del-Barco, B., Mendo-Lázaro, S., Fernández-Solana, J., González-Bernal, J. J., et al. (2022). Bullying in adolescents: differences between gender and school year and relationship with academic performance. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:9301. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19159301
Ogden, T., Olseth, A., Sørlie, M. A., and Hukkelberg, S. (2023). Teacher’s assessment of gender differences in school performance, social skills, and externalizing behavior from fourth through seventh grade. J. Educ. 203, 211–221. doi: 10.1177/00220574211025071
Ortega-Ruiz, R., Del Rey, R., and Casas, J. A. (2016). Evaluar El Bullying y El Cyberbullying Validación Española Del EBIP-Q y Del ECIP-Q. Psicologia Educativa 22, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pse.2016.01.004
Pan, B., Garandeau, C. F., Li, T., Ji, L., Salmivalli, C., and Zhang, W. (2023). Supplemental material for the dynamic associations between social dominance goals and bullying from middle to late childhood: the moderating role of classroom bystander behaviors. J. Educ. Psychol. 349–362. doi: 10.1037/edu0000776.supp
Patel, M., and Quan-Haase, A. (2022). The social-ecological model of cyberbullying: digital media as a predominant ecology in the everyday lives of youth. New Media Soc. 26, 5507–5528. doi: 10.1177/14614448221136508
Petrigna, L., Thomas, E., Brusa, J., Rizzo, F., Scardina, A., Galassi, C., et al. (2022). Does learning through movement improve academic performance in primary schoolchildren? A systematic review. Front. Pediatr. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fped.2022.841582
Pintrich, P., Smith, D., Garcia, T, and McKeachie, W (1991). A manual for the use of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ) motivated strategies for learning questionnaire manual. Available online at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/271429287.
Prochaska, J. J., Sallis, J. F., and Long, B. (2001). A physical activity screening measure for use with adolescents in primary care. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 155, 554–559. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.155.5.554
Raine, L. B., Kao, S. C., Drollette, E. S., Pontifex, M. B., Pindus, D., Hunt, J., et al. (2020). The role of BMI on cognition following acute physical activity in preadolescent children. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 21:100143. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2020.100143
Ramírez, O. C., Larruzea-urkixo, N., and Bully, P. (2022). Adaptación al Contexto Universitario Español y Propiedades Psicométricas Del MSLQ: Contribución a La Medida y Análisis de Las Diferencias de Género Del Aprendizaje Autorregulado. Anales Psicol. 38, 295–306. doi: 10.6018/analesps.444851
Rubén, T. R., and Gómez, N. N. (2019). The influence of the teacher on the motivation, learning strategies, critical thinking and academic performance of high school students in physical education. Psychol. Soc. Educ. 11, 137–150. doi: 10.25115/psye.v10i1.2230
Runze, J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2024). Response bias is genetically biased: another argument for Kagan's philippic against questionnaires in developmental psychology. Dev. Psychol. 60, 2008–2015. doi: 10.1037/dev0001614
Seum, T., Meyrose, A. K., Rabel, M., Schienkiewitz, A., and Ravens-Sieberer, U. (2022). Pathways of parental education on Children’s and Adolescent’s body mass index: the mediating roles of behavioral and psychological factors. Front. Public Health 10, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.763789
Tantoh, M. C. (2023). Parental level of education and its implications of their expectations towards their children academic performance. Int. J. Psychol. Cognit. Educ. 2, 1–20. doi: 10.58425/ijpce.v2i1.109
UNESCO (2019). Behind the numbers: ending school violence and bullying. Behind the numbers: ending school violence and bullying. doi: 10.54675/trvr4270
Urruticoechea, A., Oliveri, A., Vernazza, E., Giménez-Dasí, M., Martínez-Arias, R., and Martín-Babarro, J. (2021). The relative age effects in educational development: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:8966. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18178966
Vacca, M., Cerolini, S., Zegretti, A., Zagaria, A., and Lombardo, C. (2023). Bullying victimization and adolescent depression, anxiety and stress: the mediation of cognitive emotion regulation. Children 10:1897. doi: 10.3390/children10121897
Vásquez, A. S. (2021). Estrategias de Aprendizaje de Estudiantes Universitarios Como Predictores de Su Rendimiento Académico. Revista Complutense Educ. 32, 159–170. doi: 10.5209/rced.68203
Vismara, M., Girone, N., Conti, D., Nicolini, G., and Dell’Osso, B. (2022). The current status of cyberbullying research: a short review of the literature. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 46:101152. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101152
Waseem, M., and Nickerson, A. (2023). Identifying and addressing bullying continuing education activity. Available online at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/books/NBK441930/?report=printable.
Wassenaar, T. M., Wheatley, C. M., Beale, N., Nichols, T., Salvan, P., Meaney, A., et al. (2021). The effect of a one-year vigorous physical activity intervention on fitness, cognitive performance and mental health in young adolescents: the fit to study cluster randomised controlled trial. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 18, 1–15. doi: 10.1186/s12966-021-01113-y
Weinreich, L., Haberstroh, S., Schulte-Körne, G., and Moll, K. (2023). The relationship between bullying, learning disorders and psychiatric comorbidity. BMC Psychiatry 23, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/s12888-023-04603-4
Xu, L., Duan, P., Padua, S. A., and Li, C. (2022). The impact of self-regulated learning strategies on academic performance for online learning during COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 13:1047680. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1047680
Yip, M. C. W. (2021). The linkage among academic performance, learning strategies and self-efficacy of Japanese university students: a mixed-method approach. Stud. High. Educ. 46, 1565–1577. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2019.1695111
Yousefi, M., Barzegar, M., Kouroshnia, M., and Khayyer, M. (2021). Investigating the mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies in the relationship between Meta-cognitive beliefs and learning anxiety. Iran. Evol. Educ. Psychol. 3, 256–267. doi: 10.52547/ieepj.3.3.256
Zambrano Ortega, T., Sabando García, Á. R., Ugando Peñate, M., Celi Pinza, D.M., and Miranda Rojas, J.E. (2021). Revista de La Universidad Del Zulia Ciencias de La Educación Año 12 N° 35 Noviembre-2021 Tercera Época Maracaibo-Venezuela. Available online at: http://Issuu.com/revistadelauniversidaddelzulia.
Keywords: academic performance, adolescents, aggressors, self-regulated learning, victims
Citation: Solas-Martínez JL, De la Torre-Cruz MJ, Rusillo-Magdaleno A and Martínez-López EJ (2025) Bullying and cyberbullying is associated with low levels of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies in young people. Front. Psychol. 16:1569400. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1569400
Received: 31 January 2025; Accepted: 28 March 2025;
Published: 14 April 2025.
Edited by:
Shruti Soudi, PES University, IndiaReviewed by:
Jessica Wilke, University of Oldenburg, GermanyCopyright © 2025 Solas-Martínez, De la Torre-Cruz, Rusillo-Magdaleno and Martínez-López. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Manuel J. De la Torre-Cruz, bWFqZWNydXpAdWphZW4uZXM=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.