
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Psychol. , 07 March 2025
Sec. Consciousness Research
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1557796
This article is part of the Research Topic Spirituality and Religion: Implications for Mental Health View all 41 articles
Background: Multiple neuroimaging studies have been published to report brain processing of religious behavior related to Christianity, such as prayer and recitation of the Bible. This meta-analysis aimed to pool data across studies to identify brain regions consistently activated in response to such religious tasks.
Methods: Web of Science, Scopus, and PubMed were queried to identify relevant studies. Brain coordinates and sample size were manually extracted from the identified studies, and entered into a dedicated software called GingerALE to conduct meta-analysis.
Results: Meta-analytic results based on 11 studies showed that brain processing of Christian behavior was associated with the right middle frontal gyrus and superior frontal gyrus, with a peak location (at 44, 38, 26; cluster size = 760 mm3) preferentially associated with working memory, cognitive task, and executive function according to Neurosynth data. Sub-analyses on Christian subject data revealed no significant results at the pre-defined threshold. With a more liberal threshold, Christian tasks > non-Christian tasks showed activation in the anterior cingulate and medial frontal gyrus (peak at 4, 48, −4; cluster size = 256 mm3) that were frequently associated with reward, self-referential, and reinforcement learning, whereas non-Christian tasks > Christian tasks showed activation in the right middle frontal gyrus (peak at 48, 36, 24; cluster size = 472 mm3) that frequently associated with working memory, executive function, arithmetic, and calculation.
Conclusion: This study has revealed the relevance of frontal and limbic regions to Christian behavior.
The intersection of religion and neuroscience has emerged as a popular domain of research study, offering insights into how religious experiences are represented in the brain. Religious behavior can be both external and internal, voluntary (proactive) or unconscious (reactive). Christian practices such as reciting the Bible and engaging in active prayer constitute complex experiences that encompass cognitive, emotional, and spiritual dimensions. In particular, prayer can be divided into learned prayer (pre-formulated, e.g., Lord’s Prayer) and spontaneous prayer (i.e., personalized). Understanding the neural mechanisms underlying these practices can reveal the cerebral processing of religious beliefs and spiritual activities. For example, Newberg et al. (2006) investigated the neural correlates of glossolalia (speaking in tongues) among Christians. Compared to singing with gospel music, glossolalia caused decreased activity in the prefrontal cortices, left caudate and left temporal pole, and increased activity in the left superior parietal lobe and right amygdala. The authors interpreted the findings as a sign of reduced intentional control and altered emotional activity. Meanwhile, Schjødt et al. (2008) has reported that Christian subjects showed a heightened activation at the caudate, an important part of the dopaminergic reward system, when they performed the Lord’s Prayer and personal prayer. In contrast, the caudate was deactivated when they performed a rhyme or made a wish to Santa Claus.
The effect of long-term religious exercise was also investigated. For instance, a week of spiritual retreat based on the Spiritual Exercises of St. Ignatius of Loyola was found to significantly reduce the transporter binding of dopamine in basal ganglia and of serotonin in the midbrain, altering resting-state functional connectivity between the posterior cingulate, frontal and parietal regions that form a network in the brain associated with religious or spiritual processing, as well as reducing the self-perceived levels of tension and fatigue (Newberg et al., 2018; Wintering et al., 2020). Results from the abovementioned examples were reasonable, as Christian experiences often involve personal reflection, emotional engagement, and a sense of connection with the divine. These findings suggest that engagement in religious activities activates brain regions associated with self-referential cognition and reward processing.
Neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and, to a lesser extent, positron emission tomography (PET), have been pivotal in mapping brain activities associated with various religious tasks. Studies could utilize these techniques to explore the neural correlates of religious practices such as scripture recitation, prayer, and meditation. While individual studies have provided valuable insights, results were often inconsistent due to heterogeneity in study design and subject characteristics. For example, a study on Carmelite nuns who lived in secluded areas and separated from the society requested them to recall most intense mystical experience as a Carmelite (Beauregard and Paquette, 2006). The task was very personal as the recall from each nun would be unique, so that the results might be less generalizable than those synthesized from more impersonal tasks such as reciting scripture repeatedly.
Given the paradigm constraints unfolded above, embracing a meta-analytic approach allows for a more robust understanding by pooling data across studies, therefore increasing the statistical power and generalizability of the findings (Müller et al., 2018; Yeung, 2023). In the current meta-analysis, we attempted to synthesize existing neuroimaging research in order to identify the neural correlates of Christian experiences. As religious behaviors often provide emotional satisfaction and involve deep personal reflection as elaborated from the abovementioned examples, our central hypothesis was that the pooled data from neuroimaging studies on Christian behaviors (tasks) would show significant brain activation in regions responsible for reward processing and self-referential cognition.
Published studies that reported neuroimaging results on Christian behaviors would be initially eligible in this work. Adhering to the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) guideline, papers were searched from multiple literature databases, namely Web of Science, Scopus, and PubMed, on 28 August 2024. No review protocol was preregistered. The following search string was used to search for the titles, abstracts and keywords of papers: (fMRI OR “functional magnetic resonance imaging” OR “functional MRI” OR sMRI OR “structural magnetic resonance imaging” OR “structural MRI” OR “functional neuroimaging” OR “structural neuroimaging”) AND (christian* OR jesus OR bible OR biblical OR pray*). Since PubMed did not record keywords of papers, only the title and abstract fields were searched in this database. No filter was placed on publication year, meaning that existing articles on the topic published up to the search date could be included. Reference lists of relevant publications were manually screened to identify potentially missed papers. This study did not involve human or animal subjects and hence ethical approval was not applicable.
The search initially yielded 127 papers. After excluding duplicates, 64 remained. Forty-eight of them were excluded after further screening because of irrelevance, while another five were excluded because they did not provide brain coordinates for meta-analysis. The list of excluded studies is provided in Supplementary File 1. The corresponding authors of these papers were contacted to request for the brain coordinates, but no response was received. Finally, 11 studies remained (Figure 1).
The following items were manually extracted from the analyzed papers, including the number of subjects, their age and gender, the religious status of the subjects, the contrasts selected for brain coordinate extraction, the number of brain coordinates extracted, as well as the statistical threshold used to report fMRI results (the most stringent one was recorded if there were multiple items) (Table 1).
Using the ALE method, the coordinate-based meta-analysis converged brain coordinates and sample size data across the included papers in order to identify the brain regions consistently activated by means of probability distribution modeling (Eickhoff et al., 2009). From each included paper, the brain coordinates of regions with significant activation were extracted. Within a paper, if there were multiple similar contrasts tested and reported, the most representative ones would be selected. For example, a study made comparisons among three conditions: active prayer, speaking to the loved one (imagining a close personal friend), and baseline (imaging a variety of animals while naming them consecutively) (Neubauer, 2014). In this case, the brain coordinates from the contrasts of prayer > loved one and loved one > prayer were extracted, whereas those from the contrasts involving baseline were discarded.
A minority of the included papers reported brain coordinates in Talairach system. To unify the format, these coordinates were converted into the MNI system by Lancaster transform (Lancaster et al., 2007). The conversion was performed with the ALE meta-analytic software GingerALE 3.0.2 (freely available from http://brainmap.org/ale/) (Eickhoff et al., 2012; Eickhoff et al., 2009; Turkeltaub et al., 2012). Subject-based full-width half-maximum values were applied with default settings (Eickhoff et al., 2009). The more conservative whole-brain mask, but not the dilated one, was selected. Following the latest recommendations of using GingerALE for ALE meta-analysis (Eickhoff et al., 2016; Müller et al., 2018; Yeung et al., 2023; Yeung et al., 2019), a cluster of brain voxels was considered significantly activated if it had a cluster p < 0.05 (corrected by familywise error rate, PFWE) with a primary cluster-defining threshold of p < 0.001. If no significant result was found for some of the meta-analyses, exploratory analyses were conducted with a more liberal threshold of voxel p < 0.001 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons, Punc) and cluster size > 200 voxels. Thresholded ALE maps were overlaid onto the Colin brain template in MNI space (Holmes et al., 1998) for results visualization with Mango 4.0 (freely available from http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/mango.html).
Three ALE meta-analyses were conducted: (1) an overall meta-analysis that covered all data extracted from all included studies (19 contrasts, 101 brain coordinates), (2) a meta-analysis that covered data from Christian tasks > non-Christian tasks performed by Christian subjects (5 contrasts, 17 brain coordinates), and (3) a meta-analysis that covered data from non-Christian tasks > Christian tasks performed by Christian subjects (7 contrasts, 41 brain coordinates). Due to the small number of studies adopted, similar meta-analyses based on data from non-Christian subjects, or, a meta-analysis directly comparing Christian and non-Christian subjects could not be performed.
The peak coordinates resulted from the ALE meta-analyses were entered into the Neurosynth database (freely available from: https://neurosynth.org/) (Yarkoni et al., 2011) to conduct its in-built location-based analysis. This analysis provides associations between activation at the pre-defined voxel and terms indexed by Neurosynth. The location-based analysis has four output metrics, namely z-score, posterior probability, functional connectivity (r), and meta-analytic coactivation (r), all of which are automatically computed based on data hosted by Neurosynth. The first metric, z-score, is obtained from a two-way ANOVA testing for the presence of a non-zero association between term use and voxel activation. In other words, the magnitude of the z-score informs researchers of whether activation in a region occurs more consistently for studies that mention a particular term than for studies that do not mention it. For the definitions of the other metrics, readers may refer to the FAQs section of Neurosynth database.
There were 11 papers included in the current meta-analysis, published in the period between 2001 and 2017 in journals with an impact factor issued by Journal Citation Reports 2023 (IF). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (IF 3.9) and Frontiers in Human Neuroscience (IF 2.4) each published two papers adopted in the meta-analysis. The number of subjects per study ranged from 12 to 40, with the exception of a single-subject study (Table 1). The mean age of the subjects reported from the papers fell within the range of 21.8–72 years. Most of them recruited subjects with a balanced gender ratio. Six studies recruited Christian subjects only, while the Christian subjects came from a variety of Christian denominations. The studies mainly reported significant results for religious task > secular task and vice versa, from Christian subjects. The studies mainly used fMRI as their imaging modality, except for one PET study (Azari et al., 2001) and one structural MRI study (Kober et al., 2017). The extracted brain coordinates are provided in Supplementary File 2.
There was one significant cluster that mainly covered the right middle frontal gyrus as well as the superior frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 9) (Figure 2 and Table 2). Location-based analysis suggested that this location is frequently associated with working memory, cognitive task, and executive function (Table 3).
Figure 2. Significant activation from the overall analysis (cluster PFWE < 0.05 and cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001).
Table 3. Representative associations of the peak coordinates with meta-analysis maps hosted by Neurosynth.
Under the planned threshold, no significant cluster was found. Exploratory analysis revealed one significant cluster in the anterior cingulate that extended to the medial frontal gyrus (mainly Brodmann area 32, also Brodmann area 10) (Figure 3A and Table 2). Location-based analysis suggested that this location is frequently associated with reward, self-referential, and reinforcement learning (Table 3).
Figure 3. Significant activation from the exploratory analyses (voxel Punc < 0.001 and cluster size >200 voxels) of Christian subjects on (A) Christian task > non-Christian task, and (B) non-Christian task > Christian task.
Under the planned threshold, no significant cluster was found. Exploratory analysis showed one significant cluster that covered the right middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 9) (Figure 3B and Table 2). Location-based analysis suggested that this location is frequently associated with working memory, executive function, arithmetic, and calculation (Table 3).
This neuroimaging meta-analysis has revealed that the processing of Christian belief could be associated with the right middle frontal gyrus and the superior frontal gyrus, with a peak location preferentially associated with working memory, cognitive task, and executive function according to existing literature. Sub-analyses on Christian subject data revealed no significant results at the pre-defined threshold. With a more liberal threshold, Christian tasks > non-Christian tasks showed activation in the anterior cingulate and medial frontal gyrus, which are frequently associated with reward, self-referential, and reinforcement learning, whereas non-Christian tasks > Christian tasks showed activation in the right middle frontal gyrus, which is frequently associated with working memory, executive function, arithmetic, and calculation. Readers can refer to comprehensive review papers for more complete understanding or a framework regarding neural processing of religion and spirituality (Grafman et al., 2020; Van Elk and Aleman, 2017). For example, it was discussed that the anterior cingulate is involved in acquiring and maintaining intuitive supernatural beliefs, as well as detecting conflicts between religious beliefs and task stimuli or demands (Grafman et al., 2020; Van Elk and Aleman, 2017).
From a theological standpoint, particularly within Protestant traditions, the neural correlates from the middle frontal gyrus and superior frontal gyrus may underscore the integral role of cognition in the relationship between the self and the Transcendent. The activation of brain areas linked to working memory, cognitive tasks, and executive function aligns with the Reformation principle of “Sola Scriptura” (Engelder, 1916), emphasizing the importance of the Word in divine revelation. This cognitive engagement reflects the belief that God’s re-connection with humanity is mediated through intellectual recognition and understanding of the Scriptures. Dialectical theology, as postulated by Barth (1933), posits that divine revelation is external and revelatory, implying that cognitive faculties are essential for receiving and interpreting this revelation. In the same vein, Pannenberg’s narrative and evolutionary theology (Pannenberg, 1968) posits that divine revelation is an ongoing, historical process that requires active cognitive participation to understand the unfolding narrative of God’s Spirit throughout history. These theological perspectives collectively highlight that cognitive appraisal and therefore neurological engagement are central to the experience and practice of Christian faith, as evidenced by the neural correlates identified in this study. The theological thoughts mentioned here are merely examples. In this research context, exploring how theological insights align with current and future neurological findings could promote a productive line of study.
A recent neuroimaging meta-analysis of 22 fMRI studies on Buddhist meditation found no significant results at false-discovery rate-corrected p < 0.05, or PFDR < 0.05 (Chaudhary et al., 2023). At Punc < 0.0001, activation was found at the medial frontal gyrus, precuneus, insula, and inferior parietal lobule, for which the authors considered to be associated with social cognition, performance monitoring, self-referential processing and self-awareness. Here, it could be observed that the current results and the results from Buddhist meditation shared one similarity: the activation of medial frontal gyrus, a part of the default mode network responsible for self-referential processing (Raichle et al., 2001).
In the current meta-analysis, all included studies recruited generally healthy subjects. In fact, the neural correlates of prayer have been elucidated in patients as well. For example, a 6-week intervention with a Christian form of prayer that “that focused on forgiveness and psycho-spiritual healing” was found to increase the activation in the medial prefrontal cortex when focusing on traumatic memory among patients with depression (Baldwin et al., 2016). Meanwhile, craving following prayer (unclear if it was Christian prayer or not) negatively correlated with activation in the default mode network and brain regions associated with self-referential processing, among people with alcohol-use disorders who joined Alcoholics Anonymous (Galanter et al., 2017).
One possible common confounding factor shared by the neuroimaging studies on prayer (praying or listening to prayer) is the environmental factor. It is very different (and uncomfortable) to pray inside the narrow space of an MRI machine with loud noise in supine position compared to sitting in a chair, lowering the head and praying in a silent environment (Ladd et al., 2015). In a previous study (Ladd et al., 2015), subjects with diverse religious backgrounds (including agnostic and atheist) were asked to pray in a silent room and inside an fMRI machine with the operating noise (without a real scan). The subjects indicated that prayer in the fMRI setting was significantly less similar to their normal prayer experience, less comfortable, and they were less able to focus on praying compared to prayer in the silent room.
In fact, the experimental limitations of inducing religiosity during fMRI scans have been addressed by some of the included studies in the current meta-analysis. For example, some Christian subjects commented that to induce repeated, transient religious states within a single scanning session was antithetical and disrespectful to authentic religious experience; instead, they agreed to induce and sustain the religious state for the entire duration of a scanning session by reciting Psalm 23:1 (Azari et al., 2001). In another study, some Carmelite nuns commented that “God cannot be summoned at will” so that they agreed to recall a mystical experience rather than actually attempting to achieve one, during fMRI scan (Beauregard and Paquette, 2006).
Another issue that could be identified from the meta-analyzed studies is the lack of data from inter-group comparison between Christian and non-Christian subjects. Many studies either reported insignificance, or simply did not report such results. Other studies recruited only Christian subjects, so that such comparison was not possible. While it is insightful to reveal brain regions with significant activations during religious state experienced by Christian subjects, a more comprehensive understanding of the neurobiology could be obtained by comparing and contrasting the similarities and differences between Christian and non-Christian subjects. However, dividing people into Christian vs. non-Christian may raise methodological/theological concerns in this research context. With more data accumulated in the future, a more interesting analysis would be to examine which brain regions show activation levels highly correlated to certain measures of religiosity or spirituality.
It could be argued that some of the earlier fMRI studies on Christianity were underpowered. Since the analysis of fMRI data involves comparisons between thousands of brain voxels, it is critical to report results that are controlled for the false positive rate through numerous means, such as FWE or FDR (Han et al., 2019; Poldrack et al., 2017). It is reassuring to see that the meta-analyzed studies published since 2009 have adopted statistical thresholds corrected for multiple comparisons. Future studies are encouraged to continue with the practice of reporting corrected results, with a possible addition of providing unthresholded brain maps so that readers may gain access to a more complete dataset (Poldrack et al., 2017).
Finally, this meta-analysis has some limitations. First, the subjects were mainly young and middle-aged adults, so that the results may not be generalized to children and adolescents. The analyzed studies usually have a small sample size. Moreover, very few original studies were identified, rendering it impossible to acquire enough data to directly draw comparison between Christian and non-Christian subjects. Again, even though the current work has reported sub-analyses on the differences between Christian and non-Christian tasks, readers should be reminded of their exploratory nature. It is because the small number of experiments per group did not meet the recommended number of 17–20 (Eickhoff et al., 2016) and no significant results were found when the recommended statistical threshold with FWE correction was adopted (Müller et al., 2018). Besides, the experimental designs of the included studies were heterogeneous, and therefore it was not possible to compare the effect between formal written prayer and informal personal prayer. With more studies published in the future, perhaps it would be interesting to investigate whether there exists different neural correlates of recitation versus meditation, and whether belief and practice (behavior) can enhance each other through a positive feedback loop, as beliefs and believing are different concepts (Seitz, 2022).
Within the limitations of this meta-analysis, it was concluded that the analyzed Christian behavior could be associated with the right middle frontal gyrus and superior frontal gyrus, with a peak location preferentially associated with working memory, cognitive task, and executive function. With a more liberal threshold, Christian tasks > non-Christian tasks among Christian subjects showed activation in the anterior cingulate and medial frontal gyrus, areas that are frequently associated with reward, self-referential, and reinforcement learning.
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
AY: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. NW: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. IT: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. TL: Data curation, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1557796/full#supplementary-material
SUPPLEMENTARY FILE 1 | List of excluded studies.
SUPPLEMENTARY FILE 2 | List of brain coordinates extracted from the included studies.
Azari, N. P., Nickel, J., Wunderlich, G., Niedeggen, M., Hefter, H., Tellmann, L., et al. (2001). Neural correlates of religious experience. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 1649–1652. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01527.x
Baldwin, P. R., Velasquez, K., Koenig, H. G., Salas, R., and Boelens, P. A. (2016). Neural correlates of healing prayers, depression and traumatic memories: a preliminary study. Complement. Ther. Med. 27, 123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2016.07.002
Beauregard, M., and Paquette, V. (2006). Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neurosci. Lett. 405, 186–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.060
Chaudhary, I. S., Shyi, G. C.-W., and Huang, S.-T. T. (2023). A systematic review and activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of fMRI studies on arousing or wake-promoting effects in Buddhist meditation. Front. Psychol. 14:1136983. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1136983
Eickhoff, S. B., Bzdok, D., Laird, A. R., Kurth, F., and Fox, P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. NeuroImage 59, 2349–2361. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017
Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., and Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate-based ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2907–2926. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20718
Eickhoff, S. B., Nichols, T. E., Laird, A. R., Hoffstaedter, F., Amunts, K., Fox, P. T., et al. (2016). Behavior, sensitivity, and power of activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empirical simulation. NeuroImage 137, 70–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.072
Elmholdt, E.-M., Skewes, J., Dietz, M., Møller, A., Jensen, M. S., Roepstorff, A., et al. (2017). Reduced pain sensation and reduced BOLD signal in parietofrontal networks during religious prayer. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:337. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00337
Engelder, T. (1916). “The three principles of the reformation: sola scriptura, sola Gratia, sola fide” in Four hundred years: Commemorative essays on the reformation of Dr. Martin Luther and its blessed results, in the year of the four-hundredth anniversary of the reformation. ed. W. H. T. Dau (St. Louis: Concordia), 97–109.
Galanter, M., Josipovic, Z., Dermatis, H., Weber, J., and Millard, M. A. (2017). An initial fMRI study on neural correlates of prayer in members of alcoholics anonymous. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 43, 44–54. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2016.1141912
Ge, J., Gu, X., Ji, M., and Han, S. (2009). Neurocognitive processes of the religious leader in Christians. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 4012–4024. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20825
Grafman, J., Cristofori, I., Zhong, W., and Bulbulia, J. (2020). The neural basis of religious cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 29, 126–133. doi: 10.1177/0963721419898183
Han, H., Glenn, A. L., and Dawson, K. J. (2019). Evaluating alternative correction methods for multiple comparison in functional neuroimaging research. Brain Sci. 9:198. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9080198
Han, S., Mao, L., Gu, X., Zhu, Y., Ge, J., and Ma, Y. (2008). Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/17470910701469681
Harris, S., Kaplan, J. T., Curiel, A., Bookheimer, S. Y., Iacoboni, M., and Cohen, M. S. (2009). The neural correlates of religious and nonreligious belief. PLoS One 4:e7272. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007272
Holmes, C. J., Hoge, R., Collins, L., Woods, R., Toga, A. W., and Evans, A. C. (1998). Enhancement of MR images using registration for signal averaging. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 22, 324–333. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199803000-00032
Kober, S. E., Witte, M., Ninaus, M., Koschutnig, K., Wiesen, D., Zaiser, G., et al. (2017). Ability to gain control over one’s own brain activity and its relation to spiritual practice: a multimodal imaging study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:271. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00271
Ladd, K. L., Cook, C. A., Foreman, K. M., and Ritter, E. A. (2015). Neuroimaging of prayer: questions of validity. Psychol. Relig. Spiritual. 7, 100–108. doi: 10.1037/a0039124
Lancaster, J. L., Tordesillas-Gutiérrez, D., Martinez, M., Salinas, F., Evans, A., Zilles, K., et al. (2007). Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1194–1205. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20345
Müller, V. I., Cieslik, E. C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., et al. (2018). Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012
Neubauer, R. L. (2014). Prayer as an interpersonal relationship: a neuroimaging study. Relig., Brain Behav. 4, 92–103. doi: 10.1080/2153599X.2013.768288
Newberg, A. B., Wintering, N. A., Morgan, D., and Waldman, M. R. (2006). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during glossolalia: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 148, 67–71. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.07.001
Newberg, A. B., Wintering, N., Yaden, D. B., Zhong, L., Bowen, B., Averick, N., et al. (2018). Effect of a one-week spiritual retreat on dopamine and serotonin transporter binding: a preliminary study. Relig., Brain Behav. 8, 265–278. doi: 10.1080/2153599X.2016.1267035
Poldrack, R. A., Baker, C. I., Durnez, J., Gorgolewski, K. J., Matthews, P. M., Munafò, M. R., et al. (2017). Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 115–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.167
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676
Schjødt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., Lund, T. E., and Roepstorff, A. (2011). The power of charisma—perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive network of believers in intercessory prayer. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 119–127. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq023
Schjødt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., and Roepstorff, A. (2008). Rewarding prayers. Neurosci. Lett. 443, 165–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.068
Schjødt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., and Roepstorff, A. (2009). Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 199–207. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn050
Seitz, R. J. (2022). Believing and beliefs—neurophysiological underpinnings. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:880504. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.880504
Silveira, S., Bao, Y., Wang, L., Pöppel, E., Avram, M., Simmank, F., et al. (2015). Does a bishop pray when he prays? And does his brain distinguish between different religions? PsyCh J. 4, 199–207. doi: 10.1002/pchj.116
Turkeltaub, P. E., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Fox, M., Wiener, M., and Fox, P. (2012). Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1–13. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21186
Van Elk, M., and Aleman, A. (2017). Brain mechanisms in religion and spirituality: an integrative predictive processing framework. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 73, 359–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.031
Wintering, N. A., Yaden, D. B., Conklin, C., Alizadeh, M., Mohamed, F. B., Zhong, L., et al. (2020). Effect of a one-week spiritual retreat on brain functional connectivity: a preliminary study. Religions 12:23. doi: 10.3390/rel12010023
Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R. A., Nichols, T. E., Van Essen, D. C., and Wager, T. D. (2011). Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods 8, 665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635
Yeung, A. W. K. (2023). Do “ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis” receive equal attention and accurate quotation? An examination on the quotations to an influential neuroimaging meta-analysis guideline. NeuroImage 39:103496. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2023.103496
Yeung, A. W. K., Robertson, M., Uecker, A., Fox, P. T., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2023). Trends in the sample size, statistics, and contributions to the BrainMap database of activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses: an empirical study of 10-year data. Hum. Brain Mapp. 44, 1876–1887. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26177
Keywords: fMRI, CBMA, activation likelihood estimation, meta-analysis, Christianity, religiosity
Citation: Yeung AWK, Wong NSM, Tsui ISY and Lee TCP (2025) Neural correlates of religious behavior related to Christianity: an ALE meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 16:1557796. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1557796
Received: 09 January 2025; Accepted: 20 February 2025;
Published: 07 March 2025.
Edited by:
M. Walid Qoronfleh, Q3 Research Institute, United StatesReviewed by:
Sasa Horvat, University of Rijeka, CroatiaCopyright © 2025 Yeung, Wong, Tsui and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Andy Wai Kan Yeung, bmR5ZXVuZ0Boa3UuaGs=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.