Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol. , 03 April 2025

Sec. Pediatric Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1556416

This article is part of the Research Topic Risk and Protective Factors, Family Environment and (A)Typical Neurodevelopmental Outcomes - Volume II View all 4 articles

Early social communication and language development in moderate-to-late preterm infants: a longitudinal study

  • 1Institute for Community Inclusion (INICO), University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
  • 2Institute for Biomedical Research of Salamanca (IBSAL), University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
  • 3Department of Basic Psychology, Psychobiology, and Behavioral Science Methodology, University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
  • 4Department of Personality, Assessment, and Psychological Treatments, University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
  • 5University Hospital of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain

This study investigates early development and language acquisition in moderate-to-late preterm (MLPT) infants, focusing on social communication as a key factor. Using a longitudinal design, social communicative, cognitive and language outcomes were assessed at 12, 18, and 24 months in 106 infants, including 49 MLPT and 57 full-term (FT) infants. Standardized tools, including the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley-III), the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Vineland-3), and the Social Attention and Communication Surveillance-Revised (SACS-R), were used to assess early developmental performance. Group differences and the interaction between group and assessment time points were analyzed to examine developmental patterns over time. Additionally, predictive models identified early indicators of receptive and expressive language performance at 24 months. The results revealed significant developmental delays in the MLPT group compared to their FT peers, with receptive language showing the most pronounced deficits. Early social communication behaviors, such as pointing, following a point, and attending to sounds at 12 months, emerged as strong predictors of both receptive and expressive language performance. Cognitive abilities also played a significant role, particularly in receptive language development. These findings underscore the utility of tools like the SACS-R in identifying early communication challenges and guiding tailored support strategies. Sustained developmental monitoring and targeted interventions that foster communication skills may promote positive language outcomes in MLPT infants, supporting their long-term developmental potential within this population with increased developmental needs.

1 Introduction

Early social communication constitutes a prerequisite for both receptive and expressive language development, as it helps infants understand the purpose and function of language in social contexts, as well as in the acquisition of formal aspects of language (Bejarano-Martín et al., 2020; Määttä et al., 2012; Tomasello, 1992). Social communication refers to the ability to use verbal and nonverbal cues to interact with others in different situations. These early cues—such as gestures, facial expressions, vocalizations, word production, and joint attention—are socially oriented. They reflect a natural tendency to understand and share interests, experiences, and emotions, as well as to engage in interpersonal interactions (Hansen et al., 2018; Jethava et al., 2022). If these cues are absent or lack social intent, it may indicate developmental concerns and potential challenges in language acquisition. For example, the absence or delay of spoken language by age two is one of the earliest and most prominent warning signs of neurodevelopmental conditions, such as autism; however, these delays are often preceded by a paucity or lack of earlier social communication behaviors (Barbaro and Dissanayake, 2010; Delehanty et al., 2018; Nitzan et al., 2023). Additionally, early social communication is closely linked to cognitive development. The ability to share attention, recognize social cues, and respond appropriately shapes how a child processes social information and interacts with the environment (Jethava et al., 2022; Delehanty et al., 2018). Sharing attention with a social partner is considered a key milestone in infant neurodevelopment, as it promotes social learning opportunities and supports language acquisition (Olafsen et al., 2012; Tomasello, 2006). Consequently, early assessments of social and attentional communication are crucial for identifying the likelihood of diverse neurodevelopmental trajectories. The relationship between early social communication, language and cognitive development is bidirectional and complex, with each domain influencing the other rather than following a simple causal pathway.

Many external factors can influence neurodevelopment. For example, prematurity can significantly impact multiple domains, increasing the likelihood of neurodevelopmental disorders (Mateus et al., 2019; Pettinger et al., 2023). Significant developmental challenges are often attributed to infants classified as very preterm (Gestational age [GA] between week 28 and week 31 + 6 days) or extremely low preterm (less than 28 weeks of GA). However, there has been a recent increase in research focusing on moderate-to-late preterm infants (MLPT, GA between week 32 and week 36 + 6 days). As the largest group of preterm infants, MLPT infants have been increasingly suggested to be at a relatively higher risk of neurodevelopmental challenges compared to full-term (FT) infants (Pettinger et al., 2023). Likewise, these infants are also considered physiologically and metabolically immature (Mitha et al., 2024; Palumbi et al., 2018). The final weeks of gestation are crucial for neurobiological development, supporting the maturation of neural circuits and structural and functional connectivity involved in attention, sensory integration, emotional regulation, and higher-order cognitive functions (La Rosa et al., 2024; Tau and Peterson, 2010; Leisman et al., 2024). For example, when these processes are disrupted after week 32, MLPT infants may present, among other challenges, an underdeveloped sensory system (La Rosa et al., 2024). This can affect their ability to process tactile stimuli effectively, which is particularly significant given that early parent-infant interactions rely on touch—such as maternal skin-to-skin contact—to provide comfort, promote a sense of security, and facilitate subsequent socio communicative and emotional development (La Rosa et al., 2024). Considering these neurobiological mechanisms, a better understanding of their impact on MLPT infants is essential. Cognitive impairments have been identified as the most common adverse outcome in MLPT, followed by neuromotor and sensory challenges, as well as neurodevelopmental conditions (Palumbi et al., 2018; Johnson et al., 2015). Given the interdependence of developmental domains, it is crucial to identify and address social communication challenges as an early stage (Jethava et al., 2022; De Schuymer et al., 2011). Early interventions may foster not only cognitive and social development but also improvements in receptive and expressive language skills (Bejarano-Martín et al., 2020; Olafsen et al., 2012).

In the literature, most studies comparing preterm and FT infants focus on extremely and very preterm groups (De Schuymer et al., 2011; De Schuymer et al., 2012; Benassi et al., 2016; Evrard et al., 2011; Olafsen et al., 2006; Sansavini et al., 2015; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; De Groote et al., 2006), with fewer examining MLPT specifically or including all preterm categories (Mateus et al., 2019; Pettinger et al., 2023; Mitha et al., 2024; De Schuymer et al., 2012; Montirosso et al., 2010). However, these studies report that preterm infants, regardless of category, exhibit lower performance in early social communication behaviors compared to their FT peers. At 12 months, infants typically develop joint attention and basic communicative gestures, which are foundational for subsequent language acquisition. Preterm infants show reduced social response, fewer initiating behaviors, gestures, and vocalizations (Mateus et al., 2019; De Schuymer et al., 2011; Benassi et al., 2016; Evrard et al., 2011; Olafsen et al., 2006). They also demonstrate less engagement in interactions and a greater tendency for escape behaviors (De Schuymer et al., 2012; Evrard et al., 2011; Sansavini et al., 2015; Montirosso et al., 2010). By 18 months, rapid vocabulary growth and increased complexity in social interactions occur, making it a crucial period for identifying persistent delays and intervening accordingly. At this point, preterm infants continue to exhibit significantly reduced social responsiveness and initiating behaviors, often displaying greater passivity during interactions (Mateus et al., 2019; De Schuymer et al., 2011; Muller-Nix et al., 2004). At 24 months, language abilities are more pronounced, and infants are expected to have an increasing expressive vocabulary and more sophisticated receptive language skills. However, preterm infants demonstrate significantly less communicative behaviors and interest in interactions, including reduced social initiation (De Groote et al., 2006). Consequently, delays in language abilities become apparent at this age, with preterm infants showing difficulties in both language comprehension and production (Sansavini et al., 2015). Beyond infancy, language challenges in preterm children can persist into school age and adolescence (Palumbi et al., 2018), particularly in grammatical and morphosyntactic skills, narrative abilities, and vocabulary development. These children often struggle to construct complex sentences, organize coherent stories, and demonstrate reduced expressive and receptive vocabulary (Crosbie et al., 2010; Guarini et al., 2016; Van Noort-van Der Spek et al., 2012; Zimmerman, 2018). Conversational difficulties include reduced initiative and challenges in maintaining reciprocal interactions, thereby leading to comprehension delays that may impact their ability to follow complex instructions and engage in advanced linguistic tasks (Zimmerman, 2018; Martínez-Nadal and Bosch, 2020; Sanchez et al., 2020).

These early difficulties in social communication are observable as early as the first year of life in preterm infants (Pérez-Pereira et al., 2013). However, the extent and significance of such differences compared to FT peers can vary, influenced by factors such as GA, birth weight, perinatal or clinical characteristics, and variability in assessment methods (Pérez-Pereira, 2021; Sansavini et al., 2011). These behaviors are typically assessed through observations of mother-infant interactions, with researchers often developing coding schemes for these dyadic observations; nevertheless, standardized tools such as the Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS) (Mundy et al., 2003) are also frequently used. Previous research has examined how preverbal skills, assessed using the ESCS within the first 2 years of life, may mediate the relationship between preterm birth and later language acquisition (De Schuymer et al., 2011). To our knowledge, there is a scarcity of studies specifically addressing the social communication development of MLPT infants during the first 2 years of life. Addressing this gap is crucial, as these infants may experience subtle yet meaningful developmental challenges that can shape later language acquisition and broader socio-cognitive outcomes, highlighting the need for early identification and targeted interventions. The objectives of this study were therefore twofold: (i) to compare early developmental outcomes between MLPT and FT infants at 12, 18, and 24 months, examining the interaction between group and assessment time points; and (ii) to identify which social communication behaviors observed at 12 months, along with clinical, sociodemographic, and other relevant developmental measures, are the strongest predictors of language outcomes at 24 months, both within the MLPT preterm group and across a combined sample of preterm and FT infants. The hypotheses were, for objective one, MLPT preterm infants would exhibit significant differences in early developmental outcomes compared to FT infants at the assessment time points, and that a significant interaction between group and assessment time points would be observed, suggesting that the developmental trajectories of the two groups differ over time. For objective two, it was hypothesized that social communication behaviors observed at 12 months, along with clinical factors and sociodemographic variables, would be significant predictors of language outcomes at 24 months. Additionally, within the combined sample, prematurity would emerge as a significant predictor of language outcomes at 24 months.

2 Method

2.1 Participants

A total of 106 infants participated in this study (49 MLPT, 57 FT), with birth dates ranging from June 2020 to August 2023. The findings reported are part of a longitudinal study conducted by the University of Salamanca in collaboration with the University Clinical Hospital of Salamanca, Spain. The original cohort was assessed at 12, 18, 24, and 36 months. MLPT infants were recruited from the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU). The inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) GA less than 37 weeks; (ii) normal neuropediatric status, with no evidence of brain injury, syndromes, or congenital malformations; (iii) absence of hearing or visual impairments; and (iv) no siblings diagnosed with a neurodevelopmental condition. Infants requiring stabilization and NICU admission after birth were included, provided they met the other inclusion criteria. Healthy FT infants (GA > 37 weeks) were recruited from health centers in the provinces of Salamanca, Zamora, and Valladolid, Spain.

The composition of the sample is summarized in Table 1. The minimum sample size was determined based on prior studies using similar methodologies and tools (De Schuymer et al., 2011; Olafsen et al., 2006), and was exceeded in this study. Additionally, a post hoc power analysis indicated that our minimum final sample size per group (n = 41) provided 88% power to detect an effect size of d = 0.7 at a significance level of 5%, with reference studies reporting effect sizes ranging from medium to large. The two groups (MLPT/FT) showed significant differences in GA and birth weight. Both groups were equivalent in terms of gender distribution, parental age at conception, and corrected age at each assessment point. However, the MLPT group showed a significantly higher incidence of twin pregnancies, as well as pregnancy-related and neonatal complications. Significant differences were also found in family socioeconomic status (SES), with the MLPT group exhibiting a lower SES compared to the FT group. A considerable proportion of participants in both groups scored below the normative threshold (standard score < 85) on the Bayley-III cognitive and language scales, as well as on the Vineland-3 Adaptive Behavior Composite (ABC).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample composition and group comparisons on key sociodemographic and neonatal variables.

2.2 Procedure

The study was reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Salamanca (Registration Number: 562_211220), in full compliance with all ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all participating families. Families of the MLPT infants were contacted by phone through the University Clinical Hospital of Salamanca, while families of FT infants were invited to participate during their 9-month pediatric check-up at health centers. Interested families filled out a form that pediatricians forwarded to the research unit. Both groups were invited to participate in a neurodevelopmental evaluation, and appointments were scheduled for 12, 18, and 24 months, with a margin of 1 month. Corrected age was systematically applied to the MLPT cohort at each assessment point. All assessments were conducted in a single session, with duration varying based on the assessment time point (12, 18, or 24 months) but never exceeding 2 h. Sessions took place in a child-friendly environment designed to minimize distractions, with two trained examiners, the infant, and at least one parent present. Examiners were blinded to GA and clinical history, although complete blinding to group status was not always possible. Each session began with an informal conversation with the parent to explain the procedure and their role. The Bayley-III was administered first, starting with the cognitive scale, followed by the language scale, and concluding with the SACS-R. Meanwhile, one of the parents, seated nearby, completed the Vineland-3 reported form on a tablet. At 12 months. After these assessments, a pediatric neurologist conducted a physical examination and an interview to collect clinical and sociodemographic information.

2.3 Measures

2.3.1 Clinical and sociodemographic variables

Several variables related to pregnancy and childbirth were considered, including GA calculated in complete weeks from the mother’s last menstrual period, birth weight, twin pregnancy, method of conception (e.g., in vitro fertilization [IVF]), pregnancy-related complications (e.g., gestational diabetes, hypertension, hypothyroidism), mode of delivery, and length of stay in the NICU. Neonatal complications were also recorded, such as fetal distress; hypoglycemia; neonatal respiratory disorders (including respiratory distress syndrome and apnea); intraventricular hemorrhage (grade I); and retinopathy of prematurity (grade I). Additionally, parental age at conception was recorded, and SES was determined using the Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975), which evaluates SES based on the highest educational attainment and employment status of both parents.

2.3.2 Bayley scales of infant and toddler development—third edition (Bayley-III)

The Bayley-III is a widely used instrument for assessing cognitive, language, and motor development in infants and toddlers up to 42 months, and has demonstrated validity and strong reliability in preterm populations (Aylward, 2017). In this study, its standardized Spanish version (Bayley, 2015) was used by the examiners to assess cognitive and language development. Raw scores were converted into scaled scores (range 1–19), and then into composite scores (mean = 100, SD = 15). Composite scores were used descriptively for the cognitive and the language scale, which combines the receptive and expressive subscales. For the primary analyses, scaled scores were used, resulting in three outcome measures: Cognitive, Receptive, and Expressive.

2.3.3 Vineland adaptive behavior scales—third edition (Vineland-3)

Social-adaptive development was assessed using the Spanish version of the Vineland-3 comprehensive parent-caregiver form (Sparrow et al., 2016), based on parent-reported information. Scaled scores (range 1–24) were derived for each subdomain, along with ABC, a composite score that reflects overall adaptive behavior (M = 100, SD = 15). Seven outcomes were derived: Receptive language, Expressive language, Personal, Interpersonal Relationships, Play and Leisure, and ABC.

2.3.4 Social attention and communication surveillance (SACS-R)

Social attention and communication were evaluated using the SACS-R, a valid and reliable screening tool designed for identifying behaviors related to social attention and communication difficulties in children aged 12–36 months (Barbaro and Dissanayake, 2010). It was originally developed to detect early signs of autism and is widely recognized as one of the most widely used tools. However, it has also been shown to be effective in identifying developmental and language delays in children (Barbaro et al., 2022). The tool includes age-specific critical behavioral items, which are positively scored when observed by the examiner (e.g., pointing, waving “bye-bye,” imitation or response to name). The absence of these behaviors may indicate potential challenges in social attention and communication difficulties. No modifications were made to the scale. Eleven outcomes from the 12-month assessment were analyzed, reflecting key early social communication behaviors (see Table 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for MLPT and FT groups across assessment points.

2.4 Analysis

Prior to conducting the analyses, variability related to GA within the preterm group was partially controlled by restricting the analysis to MLPT infants, thereby reducing the range of GA. However, four infants outside the MLPT category were included, as their presence did not significantly impact the results. Descriptive statistics were computed for all demographic and clinical variables, and group differences were examined using t-tests for continuous variables and Chi-square tests for categorical variables.

To address the first aim —comparing early developmental outcomes between MLPT and FT infants across assessment points— t-tests were performed, with Cohen’s d reported as the effect size for significant findings. For categorical variables, Chi-square tests were conducted. Variables showing significant group differences were further analyzed using a 2 × 3 repeated-measures factorial ANOVA to examine main effects and interaction effects. Partial eta squared (η2p) was used to quantify effect sizes for overall model, while Cohen’s d was calculated for post hoc contrasts. When the assumption of sphericity was violated, the Huynh-Feldt correction was used. Bonferroni post hoc tests were applied to adjust for multiple comparisons.

To address the second aim—identifying predictors of language outcomes at 24 months— multiple linear regression models were employed. Key predictors were initially identified through bivariate correlations of clinical data and assessment outcomes collected at 12 months, with language outcomes at 24 months. Variables with significant correlations were included as independent variables. Predictive models of receptive and expressive language were developed using a stepwise approach to retain the most relevant predictors. Four predictive models were constructed: two global models combining MLPT and FT infants (with prematurity as a variable) and two specific models for the MLPT group (with prematurity as a constant). For each model reported the adjusted R-squared (R2) (indicating the variance explained, as well as the regression coefficients).

Descriptive analyses, marginal means plots (interaction plots) and multiple linear regression analyses were performed using SPSS v. 28 (IBM Corp, 2021), while factorial ANOVAs were conducted using jamovi (The jamovi project, 2024).

2.5 Missing data

Sample sizes varied across measures and visits. Despite attrition at certain assessment points, post hoc power analyses indicated that our study maintained sufficient power to detect meaningful effects. For example, pregnancy and childbirth data were incomplete for four FT participants due to unfinished pediatric examinations. Additionally, 16 FT participants were excluded from the SES index analysis due to a lack of parental employment information. By the 18-month visit, seven participants had withdrawn from the study, and three had missed the visit due to scheduling conflicts. By the 24-month visit, five additional participants had withdrawn. Additional data losses occurred primarily due to infant fatigue during assessments or inaccuracies in parental reporting. As a result, final sample sizes for developmental assessments were as follows: at 12 months, the sample sizes were MLPT = 48 (SACS-R, n = 49) and FT = 55 (Vineland-3, n = 54); at 18 months, MLPT = 44 (Vineland-3, n = 43) and FT = 50; and at 24 months, MLPT = 41 (Vineland-3, n = 39) and FT = 48.

3 Results

3.1 Aim 1

Table 2 summarizes the means and standard deviations for the MLPT and FT groups on the Bayley-III, Vineland-3, and SACS-R measures. At 12 months, significant differences were observed only in Cognitive. By 18 months, significant differences emerged in Receptive. By 24 months, MLPT infants scored significantly lower than their FT peers across all three Bayley-III scales, with moderate to large effect sizes. No significant differences were found in any of the Vineland-3 subdomains at any age or in the SACS-R items at 12 months.

In the 2 × 3 factorial ANOVA, significant main effects were observed for Cognitive scores, for group (MLPT < FT) (F = 5.85, p = 0.018, η2p = 0.067) and assessment time point (F = 4.36, p = 0.014, η2p = 0.051). Post hoc analysis revealed a significant difference between 18 and 24 months (t = −2.822, p = 0.018, d = 0.31). Although descriptive data suggested a larger group difference at 24 months (Figure 1), no significant interaction effect was observed (F = 1.87, p = 0.157, η2p = 0.022).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cognitive scores as a function of group and assessment time point. This figure illustrates differences in Cognitive scores between MLPT and FT groups across assessment points (12, 18, and 24 months).

For Receptive scores, significant main effects were found for group (F = 15.9, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.163) and assessment time point (F = 4.20, p = 0.017, η2p = 0.049). Post hoc analysis showed a significant difference between 12 and 24 months (t = −2.50, p = 0.043, d = 0.27). Significant interaction effects were found between group and assessment time points (F = 5.09, p = 0.007, η2p = 0.058). As shown in Figure 2, group effects varied by time point, with the interaction ocurring between 12 months (non-significant) and 18 months (significant). Post hoc tests revealed that while no significant differences were observed at 12 months (t = 1.96, p = 0.806), differences appeared at 18 months (t = 4.31, p < 0.001, d = 0.94) and remained at 24 months (t = 0.74, p = 0.005, d = 0.16).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Receptive scores as a function of group and assessment time point. This figure illustrates differences in Receptive scores between MLPT and FT groups across assessment points (12, 18, and 24 months).

Finally, main effects for Expressive scores showed significant differences for group (F = 4.379, p = 0.039, η2p = 0.051) and assessment time point (F = 6.37, p = 0.003, η2p = 0.072). Post hoc analysis showed significant differences between 12 and 18 months (t = 3.91, p < 0.001, d = 0.43). Significant interaction effects between group and assessment time points approached significance (F = 3.28, p = 0.046, η2p = 0.038). Figure 3 shows that group differences varied across time points, with the interaction occurring between 18 and 24 months.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Expressive scores as a function of group and assessment time point. This figure illustrates differences in Expressive scores between MLPT and FT groups across assessment points (12, 18, and 24 months).

3.2 Aim 2

Both global models (including the entire sample of MLPT and FT infants) and specific models (focusing on the MLPT group) were developed to predict language outcomes on the Bayley-III at 24 months. The bivariate correlation matrix, identifying key 12-month predictive variables for the models, is presented in Supplementary Table 1. Table 3 shows the R2 gain contributed by each variable entered, total adjusted R2, along with the regression coefficients for each model.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression coefficients and R2 gains for models predicting 24-month language outcomes.

For the global model of receptive language (Model 1), the initial set of predictors included: group (MLPT/ FT), GA, birth weight, pregnancy-related complications, neonatal complications, SES; Cognitive and Expressive from the Bayley-III; Expressive and Receptive language, Personal, and ABC from the Vineland-3; Pointing, Eye contact, Waving ‘bye-bye’, Imitation, Response to name, Follows point, Says 1–3 clear words, Understands simple instructions, and Attending to sounds from the SACS-R. To avoid redundancy, the Receptive variable from the Bayley-III at 12 months was excluded. After stepwise estimation, the significant predictors in Model 1 were: Pointing, Cognitive, Attending to sounds, Follows point, and group (MLPT/FT). Together, these predictors explained 50% of the variability in receptive language at 24 months (adjusted R2 of 0.50).

In the specific model of receptive language (Model 2), the following predictors were considered: SES; Cognitive and Expressive from the Bayley-III; Expressive and Receptive language, Interpersonal, and ABC from the Vineland-3; Pointing, Eye contact, Response to name, Follows point, Understands simple instructions, and Attending to sounds from the SACS-R. The significant predictors in Model 2 were: Attending to sounds, Follows point and Cognitive, which together explain 60.5% of the variability in MLPT infants’ receptive language at 24 months (adjusted R2 of 0.605).

Regarding the global model of expressive language (Model 3), the following independent variables were entered: GA, pregnancy-related complications, neonatal complications, SES; Cognitive and Receptive from the Bayley-III; Receptive and Expressive language, Interpersonal, and ABC from the Vineland-3; Pointing, Eye contact, Waving ‘bye-bye’, Response to name, Follows point, Says 1–3 clear words, Understands simple instructions, and Attending to sounds from the SACS-R. The Expressive variable from the Bayley-III at 12 months was not included to avoid redundancy. The significant predictors in Model 3 were: Cognitive, Pointing and Interpersonal, which together explain 38% of the variability in expressive language at 24 months (adjusted R2 of 0.380).

Finally, for the specific model of expressive language (Model 4), the following predictors were considered: SES; Cognitive and Receptive from the Bayley-III; Receptive and Expressive language, Interpersonal, and ABC from the Vineland-3; Pointing, Response to name, and Attending to sounds from the SACS-R. The significant predictors in Model 4 were: Interpersonal, Attending to sounds and Receptive, which together explain 37.2% of the variability in MLPT infants’ expressive language at 24 months (adjusted R2 = 0.372).

4 Discussion

4.1 Longitudinal assessment of early developmental differences between MLPT and FT

For early developmental assessment, the cognitive and language subscales of the Bayley-III were used, along with the subdomains of the Vineland-3 to evaluate adaptive behavior, and the SACS-R to screen early attentional and social communication skills. Regarding the Bayley-III, infants consistently demonstrated better performance on the cognitive scale than on both language subscales across the three assessment time points. At 12 months, significant differences were observed in Cognitive, with MLPT infants scoring lower than their FT peers, aligning with previous findings (Oliveira et al., 2024; Sansavini et al., 2014). At this age, no significant differences were observed in Receptive or Expressive, consistent with Benassi et al. (2016) findings on extremely preterm infants. The relatively low linguistic demands at this age, combined with potential limitations of the Bayley-III in detecting subtle language variations, may obscure or underestimate early delays, as suggested by studies on preterm populations (Spencer-Smith et al., 2015; Garfinkle et al., 2024). At 18 months, difficulties in Receptive became apparent. Similar results were reported by Greene et al. (2013) in very low birth weight infants, highlighting that receptive language exhibited the most pronounced differences among subscales, with preterm infants demonstrating significantly lower scores over time. Finally, by 24 months, significant group differences were observed across all Bayley-III scales. These findings align with those of Olsen et al. (2022), who reported similar differences in extremely preterm infants, suggesting that developmental disparities are also evident in MLPT infants. Conversely, parental ratings on the Vineland-3 showed no significant group differences across the three assessment time points, in contrast to the objectively measured differences identified through the Bayley-III. In some subdomains, such as Expressive language and Personal, MLPT infants even scored higher. These results may reflect parental overestimation of their infants’ abilities or an adjustment of their expectations to accommodate early developmental challenges (Sansavini et al., 2011). The lack of significant group differences in adaptive measures underscores the limitations of parent-reported tools, which may fail to accurately capture developmental delays, particularly in MLPT infants. Finally, although a higher proportion of MLPT infants were at risk for delays compared to FT, the SACS-R did not reveal significant group differences. T This finding contradicts our expectations based on ESCS results at 12 months in extremely and very preterm infants (Mateus et al., 2019; De Schuymer et al., 2011; Olafsen et al., 2006). The developmental surveillance approach and categorical nature of the SACS-R, primarily designed to identify autism-related behaviors, may limit its sensitivity in detecting group differences in research contexts. Nonetheless, the SACS-R’s capacity to flag early social communication challenges, which may also indicate broader developmental risks such as language delays (Barbaro and Dissanayake, 2010), underscores its value as a tool for prevention and early intervention.

4.2 Interactions between group and assessment time points with Bayley-III

Main effects of group and assessment time point were significant across all Bayley-III scales, indicating that MLPT infants consistently underperformed compared to term peers, and that overall performance varied significantly across time points. Interaction effects between group and assessment time points were not significant for Cognitive. However, a marginally significant effect was found for Expressive, indicating that the gap may start to widen between 18 and 24 months. Finally, a significant interaction effect was observed for Receptive, emerging at 18 months and persisting at 24 months. This suggest a widening developmental gap in language domains over time, that aligns with prior evidence showing that cognitive delays in preterm infants tend to remain stable but may contribute to language-specific deficits (Zimmerman, 2018). Receptive difficulties may precede and intensify expressive language delays, underscoring the foundational role of comprehension in supporting language production. As environmental demands become more linguistically and cognitively complex, deficits in comprehension can further exacerbate challenges in cognitive development, given that language serves as a critical tool for navigating and processing increasingly intricate social and cognitive tasks (Guarini et al., 2016; Sansavini et al., 2014). This brings into consideration when catch-up occurs, as corrected age is commonly used until 2 years of age (Parekh et al., 2016) and often discontinued thereafter, assuming that preterm infants have closed the gap with their FT peers. However, there is increasing uncertainty about when to discontinue age correction. In line with this, our findings suggest that MLPT infants continue to experience cumulative difficulties as developmental demands increase, indicating that the trajectory of their development remains distinct beyond this point. Prior research supports this pattern, showing that differences across all Bayley-III scales persist beyond 36 months in preterm infants, even when corrected age is applied (Sansavini et al., 2014; López-Hernández et al., 2021). Greene et al. (2013) further reported that delays in cognitive and receptive language skills among very low birth weight preterm infants become more pronounced between the first and second years of life. Similarly, Zimmerman (2018) highlights persistent deficits in both expressive and receptive language that extend into the school years among very preterm children. These findings emphasize the complex and non-linear nature of early social communication, cognitive development, and language acquisition in preterm population, reinforcing the need for sustained and targeted intervention strategies strategies that extend beyond infancy to effectively support their developmental progress.

4.3 Predictive models of 24-month language outcomes on the Bayley-III

Predictors of language outcomes at 24 months were identified through early social communication behaviors assessed at 12 months using the SACS-R, alongside developmental measures from the Bayley-III, parent-reported subdomains of the Vineland-3, and clinical and sociodemographic factors. The SACS-R effectively identified early social communication behaviors that predicted Receptive and Expressive performance at 24 months.

Previous research has consistently demonstrated the link between early social communication skills and later language performance. For example, Suttora and Salerni (2012) reported that communicative gestures, such as pointing at 12 months, are positively associated with linguistic skills at 18 and 24 months in very preterm infants. Gestures, particularly pointing, facilitate interaction and shared meaning, which are fundamental for language learning (Olafsen et al., 2006). Similarly, Wong et al. (2014) found that higher scores on autism-related checklists, such as the Q-CHAT, are associated with lower Bayley-III language scores in extremely and very preterm infants. While these behaviors are often assessed in autism screening, their importance for joint attention and social interaction makes them central to early language development. In our study, key predictors of language outcomes at 24 months included Attending to sounds, Follows point, and Pointing, with better performance on these social communication behaviors at 12 months being positively associated with higher Receptive and Expressive scores at 24 months. These items represent the core attentional processes and joint attention skills assessed by the SACS-R, highlighting their central role in early language development. In line with this, Di Rosa et al. (2016) found that extremely preterm infants at 24 months showed persistent developmental delays, with attention and language issues being prominent outcomes. In the receptive language models, attentional response behaviors (e.g., Attending to sounds and Follows point) were significant predictors in both the global and preterm-specific models. This reinforces the role of early attentional regulation in supporting receptive language acquisition, as these behaviors require the infant to focus and respond to meaningful social stimuli. Furthermore, the initiating joint attention behavior of Pointing emerged as a key predictor of both Receptive and Expressive in the global models. This emphasizes the universal importance of pointing for language performance, regardless of GA. Suttora and Salerni (2012) identified pointing as a significant skill in very preterm infants, whereas De Schuymer et al. (2011) highlighted initiating behavioral requests as the strongest predictor of expressive language development in the same population. Early social communication behaviors that rely on attentional processes are closely linked to broader cognitive functions. The ability to sustain and direct attention toward relevant stimuli facilitates early engagement in social interactions, which forms the foundation for receptive and expressive language growth.

Additionally, Cognitive at 12 months emerged as significant predictors of Receptive performance at 24 months, whereas Receptive at 12 months predicted Expressive performance at 24 months, in both the global and preterm-specific models. Once again, cognitive skills may provide the foundation for early comprehension, and comprehension, in turn, facilitates expressive language growth. Finally, in both expressive language models, Interpersonal skills was the only subdomain of the Vineland-3 to emerge as a significant predictor. Higher performance in Interpersonal skills at 12 months was associated with better Expressive performance at 24 months, reflecting the broader role of early social abilities in supporting expressive language development.

Regarding other potential predictors described in the literature, birth status (MLPT/FT) was a significant predictor of Receptive performance, consistent with De Schuymer et al. (2011), with MLPT infants showing a higher risk for delays. In contrast, sociodemographic and neonatal factors, such as SES and clinical complications, were not significant predictors in our models. This aligns with other studies on preterm populations, which reported minimal influence of SES, birth weight or GA on language outcomes (Van Noort-van Der Spek et al., 2012; Di Rosa et al., 2016). However, other investigations found strong associations between language outcomes and factors such as socioeconomic disadvantage (e.g., SES, ethnicity) and neonatal risks (Greene et al., 2013; Wong et al., 2014; Palomo-Osuna et al., 2022). These discrepancies underscore the need for further research using adequate methodologies to better understand the role of SES, clinical factors, and environmental influences.

Overall, this study highlights the utility of tools like the SACS-R in identifying early social communication deficits that predict later language delays, particularly in Receptive, which consistently emerged as the most affected domain across all analyses. While primarily designed for autism screening, our findings demonstrate its broader value in detecting risks for language development. Given the foundational role of receptive in supporting expressive language and overall developmental outcomes, integrating tools like the SACS-R into early screening protocols could facilitate timely interventions that target both social communication skills and language outcomes. This, in turn, could support better developmental trajectories for at-risk populations, such as MLPT.

4.4 Factors influencing development and future research directions

Finally, several considerations should be taken into account regarding this study. One key limitation is the use of convenience sampling, combined with differences in recruitment methods between MLPT and FT groups, may limit the generalizability of the findings. However, this approach is common in studies of this nature due to the challenges of accessing this specific population.

One important consideration is that cognitive performance was not statistically controlled as a covariate, as it was treated as a variable of interest in this study. Consequently, it remains unclear to what extent the observed effects and predictions would hold if cognitive performance had been accounted for (Van Noort-van Der Spek et al., 2012; Zimmerman, 2018). This limitation may be particularly relevant in Model 1, where cognitive performance plays a significant role, and other explanatory variables could potentially better account for receptive performance. However, at the descriptive level, although a portion of MLPT infants scored below the standard cutoff of 85 on the Bayley-III cognitive scale across all assessment points, a consistently higher proportion fell below this threshold on the language subscales. This pattern suggests that some language delays may extend beyond general cognitive challenges. Future studies should control for cognitive scores for the generalizability of conclusions.

Another relevant aspect is the potential influence of interventions received during the first 2 years on language development was not examined. While much of the literature on early development focuses on how parental emotional states (Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Treyvaud et al., 2010; Gueron-Sela et al., 2015; Feldman and Eidelman, 2007) and interaction styles (Delonis et al., 2017; Korja et al., 2010; Hall et al., 2015; Sansavini et al., 2015; Loi et al., 2017) influence the development of their preterm infants, interventions initiated in the NICU (i.e., effective touch through skin-to-skin contact or kangaroo care) (La Rosa et al., 2024; Lejeune et al., 2019; Beltrán et al., 2022), or studies directly implementing early interventions (Bejarano-Martín et al., 2020; Meijssen et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2016), less attention has been given to research that considers early interventions as a study variable, particularly those targeting the development of specific domains. This highlights the need for further research in this area, as it is directly related to the developmental outcomes of preterm infants, the catch-up process of their FT peers, and how they close the gap.

Lastly, the cohort examined in this study was born during the COVID-19 pandemic, a factor that has been linked to reduced social interaction variability—due to restrictions and mask usage—which in turn has been associated with negative effects on language development (Feijoo et al., 2023). However, specific pandemic-related factors were not included in the analyses.

These considerations provide context for interpreting the findings and highlight areas for potential future investigation into the developmental trajectories and challenges faced by MLPT infants.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Bioethics Committee of the University of Salamanca. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

BP-S: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. VS-G: Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing, Visualization. MM-M: Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ÁB-M: Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. IR-A: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. VdV-D: Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing. GM: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. ED-V: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. RC-B: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by the R + D + i projects through MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033/ (ref.: PID2019-107177RB-I00 and ref.: TED2021-129301B-I00); by the ‘Programa de Apoyo a Proyectos de Investigación de la Junta de Castilla y León’ (Orden EDU/1192/2020, ref.: SA119P20); and by the Fundación Mutua Madrileña (ref.: FMM23/002). ED-V was partially supported by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 (ref.: PID2020-115861GB-I00); and BP-S has been funded by a predoctoral grant from the University of Salamanca co-funded by Banco Santander (ref.: 463A.B.01). The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript. During the preparation of this works the authors used ChatGPT in order to check English grammar and language. After using this tool, the authors reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1556416/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

MLPT, moderate-to-late preterm; FT, full-term; SACS-R, social attention and communication surveillance-revised; GA, gestational age; SES, family socioeconomic status; NICU, neonatal intensive care unit; IVF, in vitro fertilization; ABC, Adaptive Behavior Composite; ESCS, early social communication scales.

References

Aylward, G. P. (2017) in Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-Third edition [Internet]. eds. J. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, and B. Caplan (Cham: Springer International Publishing).

Google Scholar

Barbaro, J., and Dissanayake, C. (2010). Prospective identification of autism spectrum disorders in infancy and toddlerhood using developmental surveillance: the social attention and communication study. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. JDBP. 31, 376–385. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181df7f3c

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbaro, J., Sadka, N., Gilbert, M., Beattie, E., Li, X., Ridgway, L., et al. (2022). Diagnostic accuracy of the Social Attention and Communication Surveillance–Revised with preschool tool for early autism detection in very young children. JAMA Netw. Open 5:e2146415. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.46415

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayley, N. (2015). Spanish adaptation of the Bayley scales of infant and toddler development. 3rd Edn. Madrid: Pearson Educación SA.

Google Scholar

Bejarano-Martín, Á., Canal-Bedia, R., Magán-Maganto, M., Fernández-Álvarez, C., Lóa-Jónsdóttir, S., Saemundsen, E., et al. (2020). Efficacy of focused social and communication intervention practices for young children with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Early Child Res. Q. 51, 430–445. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.01.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beltrán, M. I., Dudink, J., de Jong, T. M., Benders, M. J. N. L., and van den Hoogen, A. (2022). Sensory-based interventions in the NICU: Systematic review of effects on preterm brain development. Pediatr. Res. 92, 47–60. doi: 10.1038/s41390-021-01718-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benassi, E., Savini, S., Iverson, J. M., Guarini, A., Caselli, M. C., Alessandroni, R., et al. (2016). Early communicative behaviors and their relationship to motor skills in extremely preterm infants. Res. Dev. Disabil. 48, 132–144. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crosbie, S., Holm, A., Wandschneider, S., and Hemsley, G. (2010). Narrative skills of children born preterm. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 100824014249025:100824014249025. doi: 10.3109/13682821003624998

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Groote, I., Roeyers, H., and Warreyn, P. (2006). Social-communicative abilities in young high-risk preterm children. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 18, 183–200. doi: 10.1007/s10882-006-9011-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Schuymer, L., De Groote, I., Beyers, W., Striano, T., and Roeyers, H. (2011). Preverbal skills as mediators for language outcome in preterm and full term children. Early Hum. Dev. 87, 265–272. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Schuymer, L., De Groote, I., Desoete, A., and Roeyers, H. (2012). Gaze aversion during social interaction in preterm infants: A function of attention skills? Infant Behav. Amp. Dev. 35, 129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delehanty, A. D., Stronach, S., Guthrie, W., Slate, E., and Wetherby, A. M. (2018). Verbal and nonverbal outcomes of toddlers with and without autism spectrum disorder, language delay, and global developmental delay. Autism Dev. Lang. Impair. 3, 1–19. doi: 10.1177/2396941518764764

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delonis, M. S., Beeghly, M., and Irwin, J. L. (2017). Mother–toddler interaction quality as a predictor of developmental and behavioral outcomes in a very preterm sample. Merrill-Palmer Q. 63, 15–53. doi: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.63.1.0015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Rosa, G., Cavallaro, T., Alibrandi, A., Marseglia, L., Lamberti, M., Giaimo, E., et al. (2016). Predictive role of early milestones-related psychomotor profiles and long-term neurodevelopmental pitfalls in preterm infants. Early Hum. Dev. 101, 49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.04.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evrard, D., Charollais, A., Marret, S., Radi, S., Rezrazi, A., and Mellier, D. (2011). Cognitive and emotional regulation developmental issues in preterm infants 12 and 24 months after birth. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 8, 171–184. doi: 10.1080/17405620903504538

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feijoo, S., Amadó, A., Sidera, F., Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., and Serrat, E. (2023). Language acquisition in a post-pandemic context: The impact of measures against COVID-19 on early language development. Front. Psychol. 14:14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1205294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., and Eidelman, A. I. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant-mother and infant-father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone: Developmental Psychobiology. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 290–302. doi: 10.1002/dev.20220

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkle, J., Khairy, M., Simard, M. N., Wong, J., Shah, P. S., Luu, T. M., et al. (2024). Corrected Age at Bayley Assessment and Developmental Delay in Extreme Preterms. Pediatrics 153:e2023063654. doi: 10.1542/peds.2023-063654

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, M. M., Patra, K., Silvestri, J. M., and Nelson, M. N. (2013). Re-evaluating preterm infants with the Bayley-III: Patterns and predictors of change. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 2107–2117. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guarini, A., Marini, A., Savini, S., Alessandroni, R., Faldella, G., and Sansavini, A. (2016). Linguistic features in children born very preterm at preschool age. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 58, 949–956. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gueron-Sela, N., Atzaba-Poria, N., Meiri, G., and Marks, K. (2015). The caregiving environment and developmental outcomes of preterm infants: Diathesis stress or differential susceptibility effects?: Child Development. Child Dev. 86, 1014–1030. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12359

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, R. A. S., Hoffenkamp, H. N., Tooten, A., Braeken, J., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., and Van Bakel, H. J. A. (2015). The quality of parent–infant interaction in the first 2 years after full-term and preterm birth. Parenting 15, 247–268. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2015.1053333

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, S. G., Carnett, A., and Tullis, C. A. (2018). Defining early social communication skills: A systematic review and analysis. Adv. Neurodev. Disord. 2, 116–128. doi: 10.1007/s41252-018-0057-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four-factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

Google Scholar

IBM Corp. (2021). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 28.0) [Computer Software]. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Jethava, V., Kadish, J., Kakonge, L., and Wiseman-Hakes, C. (2022). Early attachment and the development of social communication: a neuropsychological approach. Front. Psych. 13:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.838950

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S., Evans, T. A., Draper, E. S., Field, D. J., Manktelow, B. N., Marlow, N., et al. (2015). Neurodevelopmental outcomes following late and moderate prematurity: a population-based cohort study. Arch. Dis. Child-Fetal Neonatal. Ed. 100, F301–F308. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-307684

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Korja, R., Ahlqvist-Björkroth, S., Savonlahti, E., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., et al. (2010). Relations between maternal attachment representations and the quality of mother–infant interaction in preterm and full-term infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 33, 330–336. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

La Rosa, V. L., Geraci, A., Iacono, A., and Commodari, E. (2024). Affective touch in preterm infant development: Neurobiological mechanisms and implications for child–caregiver attachment and neonatal care. Children 11:1407. doi: 10.3390/children11111407

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leisman, G., Alfasi, R., and D’Angiulli, A. (2024). From sensory motor and perceptual development to primary consciousness in the fetus: converging neural, behavioral, and imaging correlates of cognition-mediated emergent transitions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 60:101455. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2024.101455

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lejeune, F., Lordier, L., Pittet, M. P., Schoenhals, L., Grandjean, D., Hüppi, P. S., et al. (2019). Effects of an early postnatal music intervention on cognitive and emotional development in preterm children at 12 and 24 months: Preliminary findings: Frontiers in Psychology. Front. Psychol. 10:10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loi, E. C., Vaca, K. E. C., Ashland, M. D., Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A., and Feldman, H. M. (2017). Quality of caregiver-child play interactions with toddlers born preterm and full term: Antecedents and language outcome. Early Hum. Dev. 115, 110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Hernández, Á. M., Padilla-Muñoz, E. M., Duque-Sánchez, C., and Lanzarote-Fernández, M. D. (2021). Influence of perinatal complications on the development of a sample of 36-month-old premature infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 62:101507. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101507

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Määttä, S., Laakso, M. L., Tolvanen, A., Ahonen, T., and Aro, T. (2012). Developmental trajectories of early communication skills. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 55, 1083–1096. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0305)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Nadal, S., and Bosch, L. (2020). Cognitive and learning outcomes in late preterm infants at school age: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:74. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18010074

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mateus, V., Osório, A., and Martins, C. (2019). Effects of prematurity on infants’ joint attention abilities: A meta-analytic study. Infant Child Dev. 28:e 2145. doi: 10.1002/icd.2145

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meijssen, D., Wolf, M., Koldewijn, K., Houtzager, B. A., Van Wassenaer, A., Tronick, E., et al. (2010). The effect of the infant behavioral assessment and intervention program on mother–infant interaction after very preterm birth. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51, 1287–1295. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02237.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitha, A., Chen, R., Razaz, N., Johansson, S., Stephansson, O., Altman, M., et al. (2024). Neurological development in children born moderately or late preterm: national cohort study. BMJ 384:e075630. doi: 10.1136/bmj-2023-075630

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Borgatti, R., Trojan, S., Zanini, R., and Tronick, E. (2010). A comparison of dyadic interactions and coping with still-face in healthy pre-term and full-term infants. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 28, 347–368. doi: 10.1348/026151009X416429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A., and Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother–child interactions. Early Hum. Dev. 79, 145–158. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mundy, P, Delgado, C, Goldstein, J, Venexzia Parlade, M, Hogan, A, and Seibert, J. Early social communication scales (ESCS) [Internet]. University of Miami (2003). Available online at: https: www.researchgate.net/publication/228984460_Early_social_communication_scales_ESCS (Accessed December 04, 2024).

Google Scholar

Nitzan, T., Koller, J., Ilan, M., Faroy, M., Michaelovski, A., Menashe, I., et al. (2023). The Importance of language delays as an early indicator of subsequent ASD diagnosis in public healthcare settings. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 53, 4535–4544. doi: 10.1007/s10803-022-05757-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olafsen, K. S., Rønning, J. A., Handegård, B. H., Ulvund, S. E., Dahl, L. B., and Kaaresen, P. I. (2012). Regulatory competence and social communication in term and preterm infants at 12 months corrected age. Results from a randomized controlled trial. Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 140–149. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olafsen, K. S., Rønning, J. A., Kaaresen, P. I., Ulvund, S. E., Handegård, B. H., and Dahl, L. B. (2006). Joint attention in term and preterm infants at 12 months corrected age: the significance of gender and intervention based on a randomized controlled trial. Infant Behav. Dev. 29, 554–563. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.07.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, S. R. D., Machado, A. C. C. D. P., Magalhães, L. D. C., Miranda, D. M. D., Paula, J. J. D., and Bouzada, M. C. F. (2024). Cognitive assessment in preterms by Bayley-III: development in the first year and associated factors. Rev. Paul. Pediatr. 42:e2022164. doi: 10.1590/1984-0462/2024/42/2022164

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsen, J. E., Lee, K. J., Spittle, A. J., Anderson, P. J., Doyle, L. W., and Cheong, J. L. Y. (2022). The causal effect of being born extremely preterm or extremely low birthweight on neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at 2 years. Acta Paediatr. 111, 107–114. doi: 10.1111/apa.16098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palomo-Osuna, J., Lanzarote-Fernández, M. D., Salazar, A., and Padilla-Muñoz, E. M. (2022). Sociodemographic impact of variables on cognitive, language and motor development in very preterm infants. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 62, e125–e130. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2021.08.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palumbi, R., Peschechera, A., Margari, M., Craig, F., Cristella, A., Petruzzelli, M. G., et al. (2018). Neurodevelopmental and emotional-behavioral outcomes in late-preterm infants: an observational descriptive case study. BMC Pediatr. 18:318. doi: 10.1186/s12887-018-1293-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parekh, S. A., Boyle, E. M., Guy, A., Blaggan, S., Manktelow, B. N., Wolke, D., et al. (2016). Correcting for prematurity affects developmental test scores in infants born late and moderately preterm. Early Hum. Dev. 94, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.01.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez-Pereira, M. (2021). Prevalence of language delay among healthy preterm children, language outcomes and predictive factors. Children 8:282. doi: 10.3390/children8040282

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez-Pereira, M., Fernández, P., Resches, M., and Gómez-Taibo, M. L. (2013). Determinants of early language and communication in preterm and full term infants: A comparative study. Enfance 65, 59–76. doi: 10.4074/S0013754513001079

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettinger, K. J., Copper, C., Boyle, E., Blower, S., Hewitt, C., and Fraser, L. (2023). Risk of developmental disorders in children born at 32 to 38 weeks’ gestation: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics 152:e2023061878. doi: 10.1542/peds.2023-061878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchez, K., Spittle, A. J., Boyce, J. O., Leembruggen, L., Mantelos, A., Mills, S., et al. (2020). Conversational language in 3-year-old children born very preterm and at term. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 63, 206–215. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansavini, A., Bello, A., Guarini, A., Savini, S., Alessandroni, R., Faldella, G., et al. (2015). Noun and predicate comprehension/production and gestures in extremely preterm children at two years of age: Are they delayed? J. Commun. Disord. 58, 126–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.06.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., and Savini, S. (2011). Retrasos lingüísticos y cognitivos en niños prematuros extremos a los 2 años: ¿retrasos generales o específicos? Rev Logop Foniatría Audiol. 31, 133–147. doi: 10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70182-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansavini, A., Pentimonti, J., Justice, L., Guarini, A., Savini, S., Alessandroni, R., et al. (2014). Language, motor and cognitive development of extremely preterm children: modeling individual growth trajectories over the first three years of life. J. Commun. Disord. 49, 55–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.02.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansavini, A., Sansavini, A., Zavagli, V., Zavagli, V., Zavagli, V., Guarini, A., et al. (2015). Dyadic co-regulation, affective intensity and infant’s development at 12 months: A comparison among extremely preterm and full-term dyads. Infant Behav. Dev. 40, 29–40. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.03.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., and Saulnier, C. A. (2016). Vineland adaptive behavior scales–Third edition. Minnesota: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Spencer-Smith, M. M., Spittle, A. J., Lee, K. J., Doyle, L. W., and Anderson, P. J. (2015). Bayley-III Cognitive and Language Scales in preterm children. Pediatrics 135, e1258–e1265. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suttora, C., and Salerni, N. (2012). Gestural development and its relation to language acquisition in very preterm children. Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tau, G. Z., and Peterson, B. S. (2010). Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 147–168. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

The jamovi project. (2024). jamovi. (Version 2.6) [Computer Software]. https://www.jamovi.org.

Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (1992). The social bases of language acquisition. Soc. Dev. 1, 67–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00135.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2006). “Social-cognitive basis of language development” in Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (Second Edition). ed. K. Brown (Elsevier), 459–462. doi: 10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/00839-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Treyvaud, K., Anderson, V. A., Lee, K. J., Woodward, L. J., Newnham, C., Inder, T. E., et al. (2010). Parental mental health and early social-emotional development of children born very preterm. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 35, 768–777. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Noort-van Der Spek, I. L., MCJP, F., and Weisglas-Kuperus, N. (2012). Language functions in preterm-born children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 129, 745–754. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1728

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, H. S., Huertas-Ceballos, A., Cowan, F. M., and Modi, N. (2014). Evaluation of early childhood social-communication difficulties in children born preterm using the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers. J. Pediatr. 164, 26–33.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.07.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y. C., Hsieh, W. S., Hsu, C. H., Chang, J. H., Chou, H. C., Hsu, H. C., et al. (2016). Intervention effects on emotion regulation in preterm infants with very low birth weight: A randomize controlled trial. Res. Dev. Disabil. 48, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, E. (2018). Do infants born very premature and who have very low birth weight catch up with their full term peers in their language abilities by early school age? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 61, 53–65. doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: moderate-to-late preterm infants, prematurity, social communication, language development, cognitive development, neurodevelopment, early predictors, longitudinal study

Citation: Palomero-Sierra B, Sánchez-Gómez V, Magán-Maganto M, Bejarano-Martín Á, Ruiz-Ayúcar I, de Vena-Díez VB, Mannarino GV, Díez-Villoria E and Canal-Bedia R (2025) Early social communication and language development in moderate-to-late preterm infants: a longitudinal study. Front. Psychol. 16:1556416. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1556416

Received: 06 January 2025; Accepted: 10 March 2025;
Published: 03 April 2025.

Edited by:

Anna Gui, University of Essex, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Magda Rivero, University of Barcelona, Spain
Elena Commodari, University of Catania, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Palomero-Sierra, Sánchez-Gómez, Magán-Maganto, Bejarano-Martín, Ruiz-Ayúcar, de Vena-Díez, Mannarino, Díez-Villoria and Canal-Bedia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Blanca Palomero-Sierra, YnBhbG9tZXJvc0B1c2FsLmVz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more