
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Psychol., 01 April 2025
Sec. Emotion Science
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1527124
A few studies indicate that trait sexual prejudice is negatively related to trait empathy as measured by the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Whether this association persists at the state level and is modulated by gender remains unknown. Participants read vignettes describing gay/lesbian or straight male and female characters in emotional scenarios and rated their state empathy for each character. Women reported more empathy than men and gay/lesbian targets elicited less empathy than straight targets. In addition, state empathy positively correlated with trait empathy and both negatively correlated with trait sexual prejudice. Results demonstrate that the negative association between empathy and sexual prejudice persists at the state level. We discuss our findings through the lens of social identity theory and gender roles.
Empathy is an essential component of social cognition that is typically divided into affective and cognitive empathy (Decety and Lamm, 2006; Watt, 2007; Batson, 2009; Decety, 2011; Cuff et al., 2016). Affective empathy (also called affective sharing) is defined by spontaneously modulating one’s own emotional state to match the emotions of another person (Decety and Jackson, 2006; Decety and Lamm, 2006; Batson, 2009; Hatfield et al., 2009; van Baaren et al., 2009; Decety, 2011; Cuff et al., 2016). Empathic concern is a motivational construct defined by “feeling for” others, characterized by concerned, caring feelings for others’ wellbeing that prime one toward prosocial behavior to alleviate others’ distress (Davis, 1983; Decety and Lamm, 2006, 2009). Some researchers consider it to be affective (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2004, 2009; Dziobek et al., 2008; Calabria et al., 2009; Spreng et al., 2009; Harari et al., 2010; Hooker et al., 2010; Bock and Hosser, 2014) while others describe it as cognitive (Decety and Lamm, 2006, 2009; Fabi and Leuthold, 2016; Longobardi et al., 2019), with little consistency across studies on how this variably defined construct fits into the overall taxonomy of empathy. Perspective-taking describes the metacognitive process of consciously thinking about how others are feeling (Davis, 1983; Decety and Jackson, 2006; Decety and Lamm, 2006, 2009; Watt, 2007; Cuff et al., 2016). The present study focused on empathic concern and its association with sexual prejudice. For the purposes of this study, we consider empathic concern to be a cognitive empathy construct, as we used state-level self-report measures that require a degree of cognitive evaluation to complete. We note that this perspective is drawn from the social neuroscience literature on empathy, which is more congruent with the experimental paradigm used in the present study. For an in-depth discussion of empathic concern’s place in this literature, see Decety and Lamm (2009).
Empathy is partially a trait-level construct that is subject to individual differences (Davis, 1983; Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Jolliffe and Farrington, 2006; Spreng et al., 2009; Banissy et al., 2012; Cuff et al., 2016; Vachon and Lynam, 2016). Studies using trait measures such as the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1983) suggest the ability to empathize is stable across time, with reasonable test–retest reliability, and that women display more empathy than men (Toussaint and Webb, 2005; Sonnby-Borgström et al., 2008; Derntl et al., 2010; Kobach and Weaver, 2012; Groen et al., 2013; Longobardi et al., 2019; Di Tella et al., 2020; Stępień-Nycz et al., 2021). However, many state-level factors also influence empathic responding, including similarity between the empathizer and the person they empathize with (hereafter the target), cognitive load, and moral judgment of the target (Contreras-Huerta et al., 2014; Meiring et al., 2014; Sessa et al., 2014; Cuff et al., 2016; Fabi and Leuthold, 2016; Molenberghs et al., 2016; Fourie et al., 2017; Han, 2018; Suleiman et al., 2018), although the strength of these modulations is not fully understood. Another factor that has received much interest in recent empathy literature is prejudice, with most work focusing on how empathic concern differs based on the race of the target. In brief, individuals who score highly on the IRI and its empathic concern subscale tend to have low scores on trait measures of prejudice and vice versa (Bäckström and Björklund, 2007; Pettigrew and Tropp, 2008; Sidanius et al., 2013; Vanman, 2016; Hudson et al., 2019; Stathi et al., 2021). In research on empathy for physical pain, participants typically underestimate the pain of other-race targets and report less concern for their wellbeing relative to same-race targets (Forgiarini et al., 2011; Kaseweter et al., 2012; Trawalter and Hoffman, 2015; Suleiman et al., 2018). This research clearly indicates a negative association between racial prejudice and empathy at the trait and state level.
In contrast, the association between empathy and sexual prejudice has received little attention. Sexual prejudice is defined as prejudice against others based on their sexual orientation or behavior (Herek, 2000; Herek and McLemore, 2013). Research suggests that sexual prejudice is rooted in the feeling of disgust (Shields and Harriman, 1984; Herek, 2000; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Olatunji, 2008; Inbar et al., 2009; Terrizzi et al., 2010; Herek and McLemore, 2013; Crawford et al., 2014; Kiebel et al., 2017; O’Handley et al., 2017), a negative emotion that could influence empathy. Only a few studies have investigated the association between sexual prejudice and empathy. Castiglione et al. (2013) found a negative correlation between trait empathy and trait sexual prejudice, using measures of affective and cognitive empathy. Poteat et al. (2013) showed that empathic concern was a negative predictor of anti-LGBTQ+ bullying in adolescents. Burke et al. (2015) found that both the empathic concern and the perspective-taking subscales of the IRI were negative predictors of explicit bias against gays and lesbians in medical students. Similarly, in a large sample of college students, Marsden and Barnett (2020) found that lower scores on the IRI empathic concern and perspective-taking subscales predicted higher levels of sexual prejudice.
The studies on sexual prejudice exclusively used trait measures of empathy, with no measures of state empathy. We differentiate between the two not only based on their temporal aspects, but on the ways in which they are measured and the psychological processes involved. Trait empathy scales ask participants to report how often they feel, think, or act in ways that are considered empathic (see Davis, 1983; Spreng et al., 2009). Therefore, trait empathy measures necessarily promote self-reflection about one’s own empathic tendencies, recruiting explicit memory, metacognition, and other processes associated with self-evaluation. State empathy measures, even self-report rating scales, do not encourage this degree of self-reflection and therefore do not recruit additional processes to the same extent. Because of this, state empathy measures are seen by some as a purer measure of empathy that are less confounded by self-evaluative processes, as well as the biases that come with reporting on one’s own emotional and cognitive abilities (Murphy and Lilienfeld, 2019).
It is important to investigate the association between empathy and sexual prejudice at the state level because state-level empathy is closer to what people might feel during everyday experiences. When asked about their empathic tendencies on questionnaires, people’s answers do not always correspond well with how they respond in empathy-inducing scenarios (Murphy and Lilienfeld, 2019; Lima and Osório, 2021). Likewise, when people rate their feelings toward LGBTQ+ people, their responses do not always reflect how they feel and behave in everyday life (Herek and McLemore, 2013; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Morrison et al., 2009; Morrison and Morrison, 2003; Norris, 1992; Ratcliff et al., 2006; Rattazzi and Volpato, 2003). Additionally, trait measures of sexual prejudice lack the implicit nature of many real-world interactions, in which individuals must rely on contextual clues to discern the sexual orientation of others. Therefore, we should not assume that previous trait-level findings easily generalize to state-level contexts.
Although existing research describes empathy as a protective factor against prejudice (Poteat et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020), none of these correlational studies tested the directionality of the association between empathy and sexual prejudice. Even if we assumed that empathy is indeed a protective factor against prejudice at the trait level, this framing is difficult to translate into the state context where the experimenter would have to explicitly ask participants how much sexual prejudice they feel toward a particular target on each trial. Such an approach would produce strong desirability bias, as even very prejudiced individuals typically understand it is not considered acceptable to say one is prejudiced against a particular minority group (Herek and McLemore, 2013; Morrison and Morrison, 2003; Norris, 1992; Ratcliff et al., 2006; Rattazzi and Volpato, 2003).
Existing state-level research on empathy and racial prejudice typically uses measures of empathy as the dependent variable and manipulates characteristics of targets to make them representative of a specific minority group (Forgiarini et al., 2011; Kaseweter et al., 2012; Trawalter and Hoffman, 2015; Suleiman et al., 2018). Such an approach frames prejudice as interacting with others’ individual differences to dampen empathic responding. Although this view contrasts with the limited research on empathy and sexual prejudice, it is consistent with the broader trait prejudice literature (Sidanius et al., 2013; Vanman, 2016; Hudson et al., 2019). We adopted this theoretical framing for the present study because it is more congruent with state-level experimental paradigms. However, we did not explicitly test this causal assumption itself with this experiment; further research will be needed to empirically assess the causality of the association between empathy and sexual prejudice.
In existing work, trait empathy is most often measured by the IRI, despite recent psychometric evaluations showing issues with the IRI factor structure such that two of its subscales (Personal Distress and Fantasy) do not appear to map onto empathy-specific constructs, unlike its Empathic Concern and Perspective-Taking subscales (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Spreng et al., 2009; Vachon and Lynam, 2016; Murphy and Lilienfeld, 2019; Lima and Osório, 2021). The IRI also has relatively low convergent validity with other measures of empathy (Murphy and Lilienfeld, 2019; Lima and Osório, 2021). The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ) is a unidimensional scale of trait empathy most closely associated with empathic concern and shows greater convergent validity with state empathy than the IRI (Spreng et al., 2009; Lima and Osório, 2021). However, no study has used the TEQ in sexual prejudice research.
Likewise, no empathy study has investigated the gender effects associated with sexual prejudice. Sexual prejudice for same-gender targets is greater than for other-gender targets in heterosexual individuals (Kite and Whitley, 1996; Herek, 2000, 2002; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Herek and Gonzalez-Rivera, 2006; Ahrold and Meston, 2010; Herek and McLemore, 2013; Monto and Supinski, 2014), especially among male participants (Herek, 2000; Parrott and Gallagher, 2008; Herek and McLemore, 2013). For instance, a heterosexual male participant will likely express more prejudice and disgust toward a gay man than toward a lesbian, whereas the converse is true with heterosexual female participants. Despite this well documented gender bias, no study has yet investigated whether the effect of sexual prejudice on empathy depends on the gender of the participant and of their target at either the trait or state level.
The present study investigated whether both trait empathy, as measured by the TEQ, and state empathy, are negatively associated with sexual prejudice and whether these associations are modulated by target and participant gender. We designed empathy-inducing vignettes displaying negative or neutral scenarios and asked participants to rate their affect and level of empathic concern (state empathy) for the target character. We manipulated the sexual orientation and gender of the target character in each vignette. Since the TEQ’s stability over time is still poorly understood, we administered it twice to evaluate its test–retest reliability. We conceptualized the state-level process as follows: contextual clues (e.g., name, gendered pronouns, gendered pronouns of the target’s partner) indicate whether the target is straight or gay/lesbian; the latter would activate the participant’s latent sexual prejudice, especially so if the target is the same gender as the participant. The degree to which state-level sexual prejudice is activated would determine the degree to which the state-level empathic response to the target is dampened. Furthermore, state empathy should be dampened more if the participant is a man, compared to a woman, as men typically exhibit greater sexual prejudice than women.
Based on previous literature, we predicted that TEQ scores, our trait empathy measure, would be positively associated to empathy ratings of the vignette, our state empathy measure (Spreng et al., 2009; Lima and Osório, 2021). We expected both state and trait empathy scores to be higher for women, and for TEQ scores to be stable across time. In line with existing research (Poteat et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020), trait sexual prejudice as measured by the Modern Homonegativity Scale (MHS; Morrison and Morrison, 2003) was expected to be negatively related to trait empathy. We hypothesized that state-level empathy has a similar association to sexual prejudice, but that, in addition, participants would be even less empathetic for gay/lesbian characters relative to heterosexual characters. We also hypothesized that this association would be stronger when participants and targets were of the same gender, based on the gender effects observed in the literature on sexual prejudice (Kite and Whitley, 1996; Herek, 2000, 2002; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Herek and Gonzalez-Rivera, 2006; Ahrold and Meston, 2010; Herek and McLemore, 2013; Monto and Supinski, 2014).
We recruited a University of Waterloo (UW) undergraduate student group (n = 281) through the SONA system in exchange for course credit, and an Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (hereafter MTurk) group (n = 225) through the CloudResearch platform in exchange of monetary compensation ($CAN3.00 for completing the prerequisite study and $CAN12.00 for completing the main study, detailed below). Participants were required to be 18–30 years of age, be living in the United States or Canada, and be fluent English speakers. All participants provided informed written consent prior to participation. The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the UW Research Ethics Board. After exclusion (see Results), 87 SONA participants and 114 MTurk participants remained, combined into a single group (N = 201; 18 to 47 years old, M = 24.08, SD = 4.46; Table 1). Our final sample size greatly exceeded typical sample sizes in state empathy studies using neuroimaging (Range = 12–48, M = 30.57, SD = 12.57) (Contreras-Huerta et al., 2014; Meiring et al., 2014; Sessa et al., 2014; Fabi and Leuthold, 2016; Molenberghs et al., 2016; Fourie et al., 2017; Suleiman et al., 2018) and was near the average for studies using empathy-inducing vignettes (Range = 68–350, M = 213.00, SD = 108.61) (Arditte Hall et al., 2018; Hein et al., 2018; Vera Cruz and Mullet, 2019; McGrath and Haslam, 2020; Gehenne et al., 2021).
All participants completed a preliminary series of scales prior to the main study. SONA participants completed these scales as part of the Mass Testing process at UW at the beginning of every semester. MTurk participants completed a brief prerequisite study, created to mimic the Mass Testing process. All questionnaires were presented in semi-random (SONA) or random (MTurk) order using the Qualtrics survey platform (Figure 1A). Only the TEQ, MHS, and sexual orientation questionnaires were analyzed (see Table 2 for score range).
Figure 1. (A) Materials and procedure for the Mass Test/Prerequisite Study phase. (B) Materials and procedure for the main study phase, administered a few weeks later. (C) Sample trial in the Negative Male Gay/Lesbian condition; participants moved sliders to a discrete value between 1 and 9 before moving to the next screen with the memory-related questions.
The TEQ (Spreng et al., 2009) is a measure of trait empathy consisting of 16 statements such as “When someone else is feeling excited, I tend to get excited too” and “It upsets me to see someone being treated disrespectfully”; participants are asked to rate how frequently they feel or act in the manner described by the statement. Possible responses range from 0 (Never) to 4 (Always). Each participant’s responses were averaged across all items to mitigate missing items effects; scores ranged from 0 to 4, with high scores indicating high trait empathy. Psychometric studies suggest that the TEQ is a sound measure of empathy, with good construct validity and internal consistency (Totan et al., 2012; Kourmousi et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2020). Note that due to a clerical error, the TEQ was presented to participants without the final item, thus scores were computed based on 15 items1.
The MHS (Morrison and Morrison, 2003) is a measure of trait sexual prejudice consisting of 12 statements such as “many homosexuals use their sexual orientation so that they can obtain special privileges” and “Homosexuals seem to focus on the way in which they differ from heterosexuals, and ignore the ways in which they are the same.” Participants are asked to rate their level of agreement with these statements on a scale from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree). Participants’ responses were averaged, resulting in possible MHS scores between 1 and 5, with high scores indicating high levels of sexual prejudice. The MHS has good construct validity and reliability, having been validated several times since its original publication (Grey et al., 2013; Morrison et al., 2009; Morrison et al., 2005; Peterson et al., 2017; Romero et al., 2015; Rye and Meaney, 2010).
A brief questionnaire was presented alongside the Mass Testing/Prerequisite Study scales, asking participants to identify their sexual orientation and gender identity.
Participants were invited to take part into a study on memory for social information. The order of materials in the main study phase is depicted in Figure 1B. The study was taken within 12 weeks of Mass Testing for SONA participants and within 1–2 weeks of the prerequisite study for MTurk participants.
All participants completed a simple demographics questionnaire on gender, age, and ethnicity which was compared to the information collected during Mass Testing/Prerequisite Study.
The main study included 125 brief written vignettes based on the empathy-inducing stimuli developed by McCrackin and Itier (2021a, 2021b). Five of these were practice trials, with the target gender, partner gender, and valence selected at random. The positive condition consisted of 24 trials [6 × 2 gender (male/female) × 2 sexual orientation (gay/lesbian, heterosexual)] and was included to prevent the study from feeling overwhelmingly negative. Positive trials were not analyzed. The remaining 96 vignettes were divided evenly into eight conditions according to a 2 valence (neutral, negative) × 2 target gender (male, female) × 2 target sexual orientation (gay/lesbian, heterosexual) design. The 12 vignettes making up each of the eight conditions were selected at random from 25 possible generic vignettes. For instance, one generic vignette was “(Character name) is a (age) year old living in (location) with [his/her] partner, (partner name). Outside of work, (character name) enjoys (hobby) and taking care of [his/her] pets. (Character name)’s pet dog was {fed/killed} yesterday afternoon.”
In each vignette, the character’s and partner’s names were randomly selected from a list of masculine or feminine names which implied the sexual orientation of the target. The vignette valence was determined by the final sentence, with a key word or phrase changing to make the scenario negative or neutral. The target’s age, location, and hobby were selected at random from lists of 30 options each, to create variety (see Figure 1C for an example). The vignettes making up each condition were sorted at random such that there were always 12 different vignettes from each condition for every participant, all intermixed at random along with the positive trials.
Below each vignette, participants were asked to rate how much empathy they felt for the character, and the valence of the emotion elicited by the vignette, on scales from 1/very little empathy/very negative to 9/extreme empathy/very positive. These rating scales were identical to those used by McCrackin and Itier (2021a, 2021b). Participants dragged the sliding scale to their response number using the mouse. Next, they were presented with a second screen asking them to recall two of the three following details: the target’s age, the target’s location, or the target’s hobby. Participants typed in their answers in the two text boxes provided. These questions ensured participants were paying attention to the vignettes and gave credence to the deception of studying memory for social information. Responses to these questions were used for data screening.
Within 12 weeks of the beginning of each term and prior to the main study, SONA participants completed the Mass Testing survey that included the demographics questionnaire, TEQ (hereafter TEQ1), MHS, sexual orientation questionnaire, and many other (not analyzed) questionnaires. The study spanned the winter 2021, spring 2021, fall 2021, and winter 2022 terms, i.e., during the COVID-19 pandemic in Ontario.
During the same time, MTurk Participants were recruited to the Prerequisite Study through an online notice on the MTurk platform, which informed them that completing this study would make them eligible for the better remunerated main study. A link directed them to the Prerequisite Study hosted on Qualtrics. Participants were first presented with an information and consent form, followed by the demographics questionnaire, TEQ1, MHS, sexual orientation questionnaire, and 5 other (not analyzed) questionnaires, all presented in random order. One week later, they received an email mentioning the main study was open. A final email invitation was sent another week later to those who had not yet taken the study. The maximum latency period between the Prerequisite Study and the main experiment was therefore shorter for the MTurk sample than the SONA sample.
The main study was hosted on Qualtrics and was identical for SONA and MTurk participants. Instructions on how to respond to the vignettes and their questions were provided, followed by five practice trials, then 120 experimental trials in random order (the 96 main vignettes mixed with the 24 positive vignettes). Empathy and valence ratings, as well as the memory questions, were recorded after each vignette. Every 25 vignettes, they were asked to solve a simple algebra problem as an attention check. At the same time, they received a reminder to take a brief break as needed to prevent fatigue. After the main task, participants were asked to complete the TEQ (hereafter TEQ2). The procedure is summarized in Figure 1.
Participants who did not consent to having their data used after debriefing (n = 8) and those who did not complete the study (n = 141) were rejected. All non-heterosexual participants were removed (n = 47).
We calculated the longest sequence of identical responses for each participant and defined a cutoff point as the third quartile plus the interquartile range of this variable (16 for SONA, 12 for MTurk). Those who repeated the same response to the empathy rating question for more than that number of trials in a row were rejected (n = 41). We also calculated the number of trials with incorrect responses for each participant and defined a cutoff point as the third quartile plus the interquartile range of this variable (14 trials for SONA, 6 trials for MTurk). We rejected those who failed to answer at least one memory question correctly on that calculated number of trials or more (n = 29), as such participants were consistently not paying attention to the vignettes. We rejected participants who took longer than the median time plus half the median time to complete the study (n = 8) as we did not deem it reasonable for a single session to take longer than this (1 h 50 min for SONA, 1 h 25 min for MTurk). Similarly, any participant who completed the study in less than half the median time (36 min and 42 s for SONA, 28 min and 33 s for MTurk) was rejected (n = 36) as they were likely not reading all the vignettes and question prompts.
For the remaining participants, trials without at least one correctly answered memory question were removed. We reasoned that if participants could not recall the answer for either question, they were not paying attention during that trial. The number of trials rejected per participant ranged from 0 to 13 in the SONA sample (M = 4.48, SD = 3.28) and from 0 to 6 (M = 2.61, SD = 1.54) in the MTurk sample. Thus, our final sample consisted of 87 SONA participants and 114 MTurk participants, for a total of 201 participants. Given their similarity (see Data Analysis section below), the two samples were combined. Data from this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.
For all dependent variables, univariate outliers (>3 standard deviations above or below the mean of a particular variable) were winsorized (26 total) using scores corresponding to 3 SDs above or below the mean. Descriptive statistics for all variables of interest revealed no skew values greater than ±3 and no kurtosis values greater than ±10, indicating approximately normal distributions for all variables (Kline, 1998). Visual inspection of Q-Q plots for each variable was consistent with this assessment; the assumption of normality was not violated for any of the present analyses. Since each factor in the ANOVA had no more than two levels, the sphericity of the data was not a cause for concern. Tests of homogeneity of variance were conducted as part of the primary ANOVA analysis and are described below. All results were calculated using SPSS version 28.0.1.0 with α = 0.05; Bayes factors (denoted BF) were obtained for each correlation using JASP version 0.16.3 and interpreted as described by Lee and Wagenmakers (2013).2 Observed power was calculated with G*Power 3.1.9.7 (Faul et al., 2007).
To ensure that group status did not influence our results, we first ran an omnibus 2 × 2 × (2 × 2 × 2) mixed model ANOVA with vignette valence (negative, neutral), target gender (male, female) and target sexual orientation (gay/lesbian, heterosexual) as within-subject factors and group (SONA, MTurk) and participant gender (men, women) as between-subject factors. There was no main effect of group, F(1, 197) = 2.72, MSE = 20.80, p = 0.101, η2 = 0.01, and no key interaction with group3, indicating that empathy ratings did not vary meaningfully between SONA or MTurk samples. We thus re-ran the omnibus ANOVA without the group factor to increase power and report these results below. We predicted:
P1: There would be a main effect of valence such that empathy ratings would be higher for negative than neutral vignettes (manipulation check).
P2: We predicted a vignette valence by participant gender interaction such that women would display more empathy in the negative condition than men.
P3: We predicted a significant interaction between vignette valence and target sexual orientation, such that participants would express less empathy for gay/lesbian targets relative to heterosexual targets within negative vignettes.
P4: Following the sexual prejudice literature, this decreased empathy for gay/lesbian targets would be more pronounced for same-gender compared to other-gender gay/lesbian targets, such that a four-way interaction was expected.
Pearson’s correlations were calculated to determine the associations between participants’ mean state empathy ratings, average scores on the MHS, and TEQ1. For each participant, we computed the mean state empathy score across (i) all 8 negative conditions, (ii) the 4 negative conditions with gay/lesbian targets and (iii) the 4 negative conditions with heterosexual targets. According to Kline’s (1998) normality criteria, all aggregate scores were normally distributed. We predicted:
P5: Trait sexual prejudice would be negatively correlated with participants’ empathy ratings and this association would be stronger for gay/lesbian characters than for heterosexual characters.
P6: TEQ scores would be positively correlated with average empathy ratings across all negative conditions.
P7: TEQ scores would be negatively correlated with MHS scores.
To evaluate the stability of the TEQ, we first conducted a 2 (Time: TEQ1, TEQ2) × 2 (Participant Gender: male, female) mixed-model ANOVA. We predicted:
P8: Trait empathy would be stable over time and thus there would be no effect of time.
P9: Women would score higher than men on the TEQ.
P10: If TEQ was stable as expected, then we predicted a strong correlation between TEQ1 and TEQ2 scores.
Homogeneity of variance was not violated in any condition (all Levene’s tests ps > 0.05). The omnibus ANOVA revealed the expected significant effect of vignette valence (prediction P1), F(1, 199) = 619.24, MSE = 2020.80, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.76, driven by higher empathy scores for negative (M = 7.11, SD = 0.07) relative to neutral vignettes (M = 4.87, SD = 0.09), indicating that the manipulation check was successful (BF = 1.29 × 1013; Power = 1)4. Higher empathy scores were found for women (M = 6.20, SD = 0.10) than men (M = 5.77, SD = 0.10), F(1, 199) = 11.03, MSE = 84.76, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.05 (BF = 10.66, Power = 0.91). These main effects were qualified by the expected significant interaction between vignette valence and participant gender (P2), F(1, 199) = 4.79, MSE = 15.62, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.02 (BF = 1.83, Power = 0.59; see Figure 2). Simple effects testing revealed higher empathy ratings for women (M = 7.44, SD = 0.11) than men (M = 6.78, SD = 0.10) within negative vignettes, F(1, 199) = 19.81, MSE = 21.65, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.09. By contrast, in neutral vignettes men (M = 4.73, SD = 0.13) had very similar empathy ratings compared to women (M = 5.00, SD = 0.10), F(1,199) = 2.10, MSE = 3.45, p = 0.149, η2 = 0.01. There was also an unexpected, small main effect of target gender such that female targets generally exhibited greater empathy (M = 6.02, SD = 0.07) relative to male targets (M = 5.96, SD = 0.07), F(1,199) = 3.94, MSE = 1.48, p = 0.049, η2 = 0.02 (BF = 0.11, Power = 0.51).
Figure 2. Effects of vignette valence, participant gender, target gender, and target orientation. Neg, negatively valenced vignettes. Neu, neutrally valenced vignettes. G/L, gay/lesbian target characters. St, straight target characters. Significant differences between negative conditions are denoted with an asterisk (*).
The predicted interaction between vignette valence and target sexual orientation (P3) was also significant, F(1, 199) = 4.10, MSE = 0.58, p = 0.044, η2 = 0.02 (BF = 0.12, Power = 0.52). This effect was driven by an effect of target sexual orientation in the negative condition only (F(1, 199) = 6.01, MSE = 1.07, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.03, Power = 0.68), with slightly more empathy for straight targets (M = 7.14, SD = 0.07) relative to gay/lesbian targets (M = 7.08, SD = 0.08) (see Figure 2). The predicted four-way interaction (P4) was not significant, F(1, 199) = 0.03, MSE = 0.01, p = 0.856, η2 = 0.00 (BF = 2.69 × 10−7, Power = 0.05, suggesting the test was greatly underpowered). No other significant effects or interactions were observed.
The correlation between MHS scores and state empathy scores was significant in both the average gay/lesbian negative condition (Figure 3), r = −0.33, p < 0.001, BF = 10053.86, Power = 1.00, and in the average heterosexual condition, r = −0.27, p < 0.001, BF = 249.39, Power = 0.99, providing partial support for prediction P5. In line with prediction P6, the correlation between TEQ1 scores and average state empathy scores across all negative conditions was positive and significant, r = 0.37, p < 0.001 (Figure 4); BF = 357154.03, Power = 1.00. The correlation between TEQ1 scores and MHS scores was negative and significant, r = −33, p < 0.001, in line with prediction P7 (Figure 5); BF = 15639.56, Power = 1.00.
Figure 3. Significant correlations between MHS and mean state empathy scores. Note MHS scores reflect trait sexual prejudice. Mean state empathy scores averaged across gay/lesbian negative conditions are denoted with black dots, state empathy scores averaged across straight negative conditions are denoted with grey dots.
Figure 4. Significant correlation between TEQ1 and mean state empathy scores. Note TEQ1 refers to TEQ scores obtained during Mass Testing/prerequisite study. Mean state empathy scores were averaged across all negative conditions during the experimental study.
Figure 5. Significant correlation between TEQ1 and MHS scores. Note Scores on both scales were obtained during Mass Testing/prerequisite study. Note TEQ scores were lower in the experiment (TEQ2) than during the Mass Testing/Prerequisite study (TEQ1).
A significant main effect of time was found, F(1, 198) = 111.661, MSE = 29.22, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.36, such that participants’ TEQ scores were higher during Mass Testing/prerequisite study (TEQ1, M = 2.85, SD = 0.04) compared to after the experiment (TEQ2, M = 2.31, SD = 0.03) contrary to our expectation (P8) that their scores would not change significantly over time (BF = ~∞; Power = 1.00). We also observed the expected significant main effect of participant gender (P9) such that women had greater TEQ scores (M = 2.70, SD = 0.04) than men (M = 2.46, SD = 0.04), F(1, 198) = 18.05, MSE = 5.69, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.08 (BF = 366.30; Power = 0.99). The interaction between time and participant gender was not significant, F(1, 198) = 2.38, MSE = 0.62, p = 0.125, η2 = 0.01 (BF = 1.49, Power = 0.34) (see Figure 6) for depiction of results. In contrast to prediction P10, the correlation between the two TEQ scores was not significant, r = 0.13, p = 0.060 (BF = 0.99, Power = 0.60) (Figure 7).
Figure 6. TEQ scores during mass testing/prerequisite study and during main study. Note TEQ scores were lower in the experiment (TEQ2) than during the Mass Testing/Prerequisite study (TEQ1). Significant differences are denoted with an asterisk (*).
Figure 7. Non-significant correlation between TEQ1 and TEQ2 scores. Note TEQ1 refers to TEQ scores obtained during Mass Testing/Prerequisite Study and TEQ2 refers to TEQ scores taken after the experiment.
A handful of studies report that high empathy is associated with low sexual prejudice (Castiglione et al., 2013; Poteat et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020). Furthermore, the sexual prejudice literature suggests that individuals are more prejudiced against LGBTQ+ people if they are of the same gender (Kite and Whitley, 1996; Herek, 2000, 2002; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Herek and Gonzalez-Rivera, 2006; Parrott and Gallagher, 2008; Ahrold and Meston, 2010; Herek and McLemore, 2013; Monto and Supinski, 2014). The present study tested whether these under-studied associations between empathy and sexual prejudice persist when measuring state empathy ratings in response to vignettes. We also investigated whether existing trait empathy findings using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) would replicate with the Toronto Empathy Quotient (TEQ) which has stronger convergent validity than the IRI (Spreng et al., 2009; Lima and Osório, 2021). We discuss our findings below.
Heterosexual participants’ empathy ratings were significantly lower for negative vignettes with gay/lesbian target characters relative to straight targets. This confirmed our predictions (P3) and shows for the first time that the association between empathy and sexual prejudice generalizes to the state level (P2), consistent with the emergent trait-level literature (Castiglione et al., 2013; Poteat et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020). However, contrary to our expectations (P4), we found no evidence that participants displayed less state empathy toward gay/lesbian targets of the same gender as the participant compared to gay/lesbian targets of another gender (Kite and Whitley, 1996; Herek, 2000, 2002; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Herek and Gonzalez-Rivera, 2006; Ahrold and Meston, 2010; Herek and McLemore, 2013; Monto and Supinski, 2014). State empathy was negatively related to trait sexual prejudice, as predicted (P2) based on existing trait-level research (Bäckström and Björklund, 2007; Pettigrew and Tropp, 2008; Castiglione et al., 2013; Poteat et al., 2013; Sidanius et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020; Stathi et al., 2021), and so was trait empathy (P5). However, this association was not unique to gay/lesbian conditions.
Our state empathy analyses may have been underpowered, as suggested by Bayes factors and observed power for the interactions. Furthermore, it is possible that the differences between straight and gay/lesbian conditions were too small for any interactions with target gender to be detected. Social identity theory states that prejudice occurs when members of a social outgroup are perceived as a threat to one’s ingroup identity (Turner and Reynolds, 2010). Research suggests that the degree to which LGBTQ+ people threaten heterosexual identity depends on the degree to which they conform to gender roles (Hamner, 1992; Basow and Johnson, 2000; Herek, 2002; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Glick et al., 2007; Vandello et al., 2008; Bosson et al., 2009; Ahrold and Meston, 2010; Gerhardstein and Anderson, 2010; Herek and McLemore, 2013; Monto and Supinski, 2014; Glotfelter and Anderson, 2017; Brassel and Anderson, 2020). Moreover, conformity to gender roles can modulate sexual prejudice responses (Monto and Supinski, 2014; Caswell and Sackett-Fox, 2018; Grigoryan et al., 2022). The vignettes used in the present study did not include characters that challenged traditional gender roles beyond having a same-sex partner, and may not have sufficiently threatened participants’ social identities to elicit large differences in empathy between straight and gay/lesbian conditions.
Relatedly, our state-level results may have been impacted by uncontrolled stereotype congruence through the randomized hobbies ascribed to each target. Although these were included simply to provide additional social context, some of these hobbies were stereotypically masculine (e.g., woodworking, hunting) while others were stereotypically feminine (e.g., knitting and baking) and others were not stereotypical of any gender (e.g., photography and swimming). The degree to which a gay/lesbian target violated traditional gender norms may have been modulated by their randomly assigned hobby. For instance, a gay man target may have elicited less social identity threat if their hobby was weightlifting compared to knitting. Since hobbies were not controlled for stereotypical masculinity/femininity across targets, this may have suppressed the expected interaction between target and participant gender through a lack of consistent gender norm violation from gay/lesbian targets. However, since hobby assignment was random we cannot definitively conclude that this contributed to our results without conducting a similar experiment in which stereotypical masculinity/femininity of hobbies is a controlled independent variable.
Our study is based on the framework used in studies of empathy and racial prejudice at the state level, which describe prejudice as being activated by certain diagnostic features of the target (e.g., skin color and facial features), leading to a corresponding down-regulation of empathic processing (Forgiarini et al., 2011; Kaseweter et al., 2012; Trawalter and Hoffman, 2015; Suleiman et al., 2018). Existing research on trait empathy and sexual prejudice reverses this view and conceptualizes empathy as a protective factor that inhibits prejudiced attitudes, leading to a reduction in prejudiced behaviors (Poteat et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020). Although the present study is not a formal test of either model, our state-level results are more consistent with the former conceptualization. Testing the trait-level model of empathy and sexual prejudice at the state level poses significant practical challenges due to social desirability bias. Collecting state-level sexual prejudice ratings, for instance, may prime participants to report low levels of prejudice because they are explicitly asked whether they express attitudes that are socially undesirable. One option for circumventing this bias is implicit measures such as Implicit Association Tests, although these measures have been criticized in recent years (see Gawronski, 2019 for a review of the debate). Another option is to use measures of prejudiced behaviors, operationalized through choices in a simulated interaction or through coding naturalistic interactions. Future state-level research that seeks to directly test different conceptual models of empathy may require a shift in methods.
Even though we used simple, brief text vignettes, empathy ratings in the negative and neutral conditions were similar to those reported in the studies from which the vignettes were adapted, which coupled the vignettes with face images (McCrackin and Itier, 2021a, 2021b). Most studies of state empathy use faces expressing pain or other negative emotions (Derntl et al., 2010; Forgiarini et al., 2011; Montalan et al., 2012; Groen et al., 2013; Choi et al., 2014; Contreras-Huerta et al., 2014; Balconi and Canavesio, 2016; Fabi and Leuthold, 2016; Wang et al., 2016; Suleiman et al., 2018), or brief videos of painful or emotionally negative situations (Coll et al., 2012; Kaseweter et al., 2012; Meiring et al., 2014; Fourie et al., 2017; Martínez-Velázquez et al., 2020; Gehenne et al., 2021). When empathy-related vignettes are presented as text alone, the negative scenarios are typically long and elaborate (Arditte Hall et al., 2018; Hein et al., 2018; Vera Cruz and Mullet, 2019; McGrath and Haslam, 2020). Our results suggest that even simple vignettes can produce the expected main effect of empathy and a small but significant interaction between valence and target orientation.
However, our valence ratings were only slightly lower in negative conditions relative to neutral conditions, a result that deviates from previous studies (McCrackin and Itier, 2021a, 2021b). Without the added immersion of a visual stimulus to represent the characters or a greater degree of context, participants may not have been emotionally engaged by our stimuli. Participants may have engaged in cognitive empathy without having a strong affective sharing response to our vignettes. Such a distinction may reflect the broader differences between affective and cognitive empathy (Batson, 2009; Cuff et al., 2016). Future studies will need to carefully disentangle these constructs and better interpret participants’ state-level empathy.
Another potential explanation for our low valence ratings may lie in habituation to repeated emotional stimuli over the course of the experiment. Individuals who are repeatedly exposed to empathy-inducing stimuli and situations gradually deplete their ability to empathize, resulting in compassion fatigue (Hunt et al., 2017; Wilkinson et al., 2017; Coetzee and Laschinger, 2018; Cavanagh et al., 2020). To test whether this affected our results, we conducted a follow-up analysis as described in Supplementary material. We arranged participants’ state empathy ratings in temporal order and calculated the average rating in each quarter of the experiment, independent of condition. A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no significant effect of time on state-level empathy ratings and a series of Pearson correlations demonstrated positive and significant correlations between empathy ratings in all quarters of the study. Therefore, we are confident that our state-level results are relatively stable across time and not substantially driven by compassion fatigue.
Despite previous reports of associations between the TEQ and state measures of empathy such as mentalizing and social perception tasks (Spreng et al., 2009; Lima and Osório, 2021), no studies have yet reported TEQ association with self-reported empathy ratings obtained from a vignette design. Our results provide direct support to the notion that the TEQ is related to state measures of empathy (Spreng et al., 2009; Lima and Osório, 2021), using a novel state measure based on vignettes. Since the TEQ is believed to primarily reflect empathic concern (Spreng et al., 2009; Lima and Osório, 2021), this trait–state correlation suggests that individuals’ perception of their own empathy is strongly tied to their caring, concerned feelings for others. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that other constructs such as perspective-taking may also significantly inform self-perceptions of empathy, as the TEQ captures at least some variance related to this construct (Spreng et al., 2009; Lima and Osório, 2021). Further research is needed to parse out the contributions that different empathy constructs make toward state-level empathy.
Most studies that demonstrate a negative association between sexual prejudice and trait empathy have used the IRI (Castiglione et al., 2013; Poteat et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020). The present study is the first replication of this association using the TEQ, with a much smaller sample size than prior studies which ranged from 618 to 2044 participants (Poteat et al., 2013; Burke et al., 2015; Marsden and Barnett, 2020). This suggests that individuals who are highly prejudiced against LGBTQ+ people may be specifically lacking in empathic concern, which the TEQ mainly reflects (Spreng et al., 2009). Since sexual prejudice is fuelled by feelings of disgust (Shields and Harriman, 1984; Herek, 2000; Mahaffey et al., 2005; Olatunji, 2008; Inbar et al., 2009; Terrizzi et al., 2010; Herek and McLemore, 2013; Crawford et al., 2014; Kiebel et al., 2017; O’Handley et al., 2017), it seems fitting that individuals who score highly on measures of sexual prejudice are unlikely to be concerned for those they find disgusting. However, additional research is needed to determine whether it is sexual prejudice that fuels a deficit in empathy or vice versa, and whether disgust is at the core of this association.
Surprisingly, TEQ scores were significantly lower following the experiment relative to TEQ scores obtained during Mass Testing/prerequisite study and were not significantly correlated with each other. As TEQ2 was administered after completion of the experiment, participants’ ability to empathize may have been diminished. This would suggest that state-level influences can have an impact on supposedly trait-level measures. Indeed, it would suggest that a state-level empathy task would draw from the well of trait-level empathy, reducing the overall amount of trait empathy following the task. Of course, we cannot assume such causality from the present data and the same result could also simply be the product of low test–retest reliability. The studies reporting adequate test–retest reliability of the TEQ have used low sample sizes such as 65 or 77 participants (Spreng et al., 2009; Totan et al., 2012). Furthermore, it appears that scores on certain items of the TEQ are particularly variable across time (Xu et al., 2020). More stability analyses of the TEQ using very large samples are needed.
The present study investigated only one facet of sexual prejudice and our results cannot directly speak to the association between empathy and prejudice against other groups who are often included under the umbrella of sexual prejudice, including other sexual minorities such as bisexual people, those who are marginalized due to their gender identity such as transgender and non-binary people, or individuals who face prejudice based on their association styles, such as non-monogamous people. Given that our samples were limited to young adults, our findings may not generalize to older adults; who are in particular more likely to exhibit high sexual prejudice (Herek and McLemore, 2013). Furthermore, we did not control for the personal memories and emotional associations our participants may have had to specific names, which may have biased their responses on some trials. Finally, our vignette and self-report empathy rating paradigm should not be assumed to generalize to real-world social situations or different state empathy behavioral tasks. Determining whether our findings replicate with different empathy tasks such as the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), the Interpersonal Perception Task (Costanzo and Archer, 1989) and the Pictorial Empathy Test (Lindeman et al., 2018), is a potential avenue for future research.
Our state-level results suggest that the primary negative association between empathy and sexual prejudice persists at the state level, providing evidence beyond self-report trait scales. However, we did not observe modulation of this association by participant or target gender, suggesting our materials may not have produced a sufficiently large effect on which the influence of gender could be detected. Therefore, researchers interested in this topic should ensure that stimuli depicting LGBTQ+ people sufficiently challenge traditional gender roles and test the degree to which this influences results. Further research should also consider the different conceptual models used to describe the association between empathy and sexual prejudice and how these models can best be tested against one another at both the state and trait levels. We observed a promising novel correlation between state empathy and trait sexual prejudice, but further research is needed to determine which aspect of empathy drives this association. We also replicated and extended prior findings at the trait level using the TEQ, raising the question of which empathic constructs are most relevant in the context of sexual prejudice. The associations between empathy and sexual prejudice at both the trait and state level should be continued with diverse research designs, methodologies, and analytic tools to shed more light on the underlying mechanisms and develop strategies to address its real-world consequences.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving humans were approved by University of Waterloo Research Ethics Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
SW: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RI: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Council of Canada (Discovery grant #418431; PGS-D scholarship # PGSD3-569559-2022), the Canada Foundation for Innovation (grant # 213322), and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (CGS-M scholarship).
We thank Bill Eickmeier, Vid Sharda, and Zuhayr Shaikh for their technical expertise in developing our online experiment platform.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1527124/full#supplementary-material
1. ^The text of the final item was “When I see someone being taken advantage of, I feel kind of protective toward him\her.”
2. ^According to this interpretation, a BF between 1 and 3 indicates anecdotal evidence for the alternative hypothesis, a value between 3 and 10 indicates moderate evidence, a value between 10 and 30 indicates strong evidence, a value between 30 and 100 indicates very strong evidence, and any value greater than 100 indicates extreme evidence for the alternative hypothesis. Conversely, a BF between 1 and 0.333 indicates anecdotal evidence for the null hypothesis, a value between 0.333 and 0.100 indicates moderate evidence, a value between 0.100 and 0.033 indicates strong evidence, a value between 0.033 and 0.010 indicates very strong evidence, and a value below 0.010 indicates extremely strong evidence for the null hypothesis.
3. ^The interaction between target gender, participant gender, and group was the only significant interaction, F(1, 197) = 5.652, MSE = 2.084, p = 0.018, η2 = 0.028. However, based on simple effects testing, this small interaction was driven by female MTurk participants (M = 6.415, SD = 0.133) rating their empathy slightly higher than female SONA participants (M = 6.029, SD = 0.153) during vignettes with a female target, F(1, 197) = 4.094, MSE = 3.921, p = 0.049, η2 = 0.020, which did not compromise our grouping of the two samples.
4. ^A supplementary ANOVA with the same factors performed on valence ratings also found a main effect of valence, F(1, 199) = 11.128, MSE = 143.165, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.053. This effect was driven by slightly more negative valence in the negative conditions (M = 4.304, SE = 0.148) than the neutral conditions (M = 4.901, SE = 0.075), as expected. No other effects or interactions were significant.
Ahrold, T. K., and Meston, C. M. (2010). Ethnic differences in sexual attitudes of U.S. college students: gender, acculturation, and religiosity factors. Arch. Sex. Behav. 39, 190–202. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9406-1
Arditte Hall, K. A., Joormann, J., Siemer, M., and Timpano, K. R. (2018). The impact bias in self and others: affective and empathic forecasting in individuals with social anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 106, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.05.001
Bäckström, M., and Björklund, F. (2007). Structural modeling of generalized prejudice: the role of social dominance, authoritarianism, and empathy. J. Individ. Differ. 28, 10–17. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.28.1.10
Balconi, M., and Canavesio, Y. (2016). Is empathy necessary to comprehend the emotional faces? The empathic effect on attentional mechanisms (eye movements), cortical correlates (N200 event-related potentials) and facial behaviour (electromyography) in face processing. Cogn. Emot. 30, 210–224. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.993306
Banissy, M. J., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., and Rees, G. (2012). Inter-individual differences in empathy are reflected in human brain structure. NeuroImage 62, 2034–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.081
Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., and Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with Normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715
Basow, S. A., and Johnson, K. (2000). Predictors of homophobia in female college students. Sex Roles 42, 391–404. doi: 10.1023/A:1007098221316
Batson, D. (2009). “These things called empathy: eight related but distinct phenomena” in The social neuroscience of empathy (Cambridge, Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology), 3–16.
Bock, E. M., and Hosser, D. (2014). Empathy as a predictor of recidivism among young adult offenders. Psychol. Crime Law 20, 101–115. doi: 10.1080/1068316X.2012.749472
Bosson, J. K., Vandello, J. A., Burnaford, R. M., Weaver, J. R., and Arzu Wasti, S. (2009). Precarious manhood and displays of physical aggression. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 623–634. doi: 10.1177/0146167208331161
Brassel, S. T., and Anderson, V. N. (2020). Who thinks outside the gender box? Feminism, gender self-esteem, and attitudes toward trans people. Sex Roles 82, 447–462. doi: 10.1007/s11199-019-01066-4
Burke, S. E., Dovidio, J. F., Przedworski, J. M., Hardeman, R. R., Perry, S. P., Phelan, S. M., et al. (2015). Do contact and empathy mitigate bias against gay and lesbian people among heterosexual first-year medical students? A report from the medical student CHANGE study. Acad. Med. 90, 645–651. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000000661
Calabria, M., Cotelli, M., Adenzato, M., Zanetti, O., and Miniussi, C. (2009). Empathy and emotion recognition in semantic dementia: a case report. Brain Cogn. 70, 247–252. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.009
Castiglione, C., Licciardello, O., Rampullo, A., and Campione, C. (2013). “Intergroup anxiety, empathy and cross-group friendship: Effects on attitudes towards gay men” in Procedia: social and behavioral sciences. eds. F. Odabasi and D. Islek (Brussels, Belgium: Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences), 969–973.
Caswell, T. A., and Sackett-Fox, K. (2018). Gender-atypical personality or sexual behavior: what is disgusting about male homosexuality? J. Soc. Psychol. 158, 591–602. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2018.1424111
Cavanagh, N., Cockett, G., Heinrich, C., Doig, L., Fiest, K., Guichon, J. R., et al. (2020). Compassion fatigue in healthcare providers: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nurs. Ethics 27, 639–665. doi: 10.1177/0969733019889400
Choi, D., Nishimura, T., Motoi, M., Egashira, Y., Matsumoto, R., and Watanuki, S. (2014). Effect of empathy trait on attention to various facial expressions: evidence from N170 and late positive potential (LPP). J. Physiol. Anthropol. 33:18. doi: 10.1186/1880-6805-33-18
Coetzee, S. K., and Laschinger, H. K. S. (2018). Toward a comprehensive, theoretical model of compassion fatigue: an integrative literature review: theoretical model of compassion fatigue. Nurs. Health Sci. 20, 4–15. doi: 10.1111/nhs.12387
Coll, M.-P., Budell, L., Rainville, P., Decety, J., and Jackson, P. L. (2012). The role of gender in the interaction between self-pain and the perception of pain in others. J. Pain 13, 695–703. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.04.009
Contreras-Huerta, L. S., Hielscher, E., Sherwell, C. S., Rens, N., and Cunnington, R. (2014). Intergroup relationships do not reduce racial bias in empathic neural responses to pain. Neuropsychologia 64, 263–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.045
Costanzo, M., and Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behavior of others: the interpersonal perception task. J. Nonverbal Behav. 13, 225–245. doi: 10.1007/BF00990295
Crawford, J. T., Inbar, Y., and Maloney, V. (2014). Disgust sensitivity selectively predicts attitudes toward groups that threaten (or uphold) traditional sexual morality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 70, 218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.001
Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., and Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: a review of the concept. Emot. Rev. 8, 144–153. doi: 10.1177/1754073914558466
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
Decety, J. (2011). Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Emot. Rev. 3, 92–108. doi: 10.1177/1754073910374662
Decety, J., and Jackson, P. L. (2006). A social-neuroscience perspective on empathy. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 54–58. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00406.x
Decety, J., and Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. Sci. World J. 6, 1146–1163. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.221
Decety, J., and Lamm, C. (2009). “Empathy versus personal distress: recent evidence from social neuroscience” in The social neuroscience of empathy. eds. J. Decety and W. J. Ickes (Cambridge, Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology), 199–214.
Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Eickhoff, S., Kellermann, T., Falkenberg, D. I., Schneider, F., et al. (2010). Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities: neural correlates and gender differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.10.006
Di Tella, M., Miti, F., Ardito, R. B., and Adenzato, M. (2020). Social cognition and sex: are men and women really different? Personal. Individ. Differ. 162:110045. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110045
Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., et al. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 464–473. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x
Fabi, S., and Leuthold, H. (2016). Empathy for pain influences perceptual and motor processing: evidence from response force, ERPs, and EEG oscillations. Soc. Neurosci. 12, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1238009
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146
Forgiarini, M., Gallucci, M., and Maravita, A. (2011). Racism and the empathy for pain on our skin. Front. Psychol. 2:108. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00108
Fourie, M. M., Stein, D. J., Solms, M., Gobodo-Madikizela, P., and Decety, J. (2017). Empathy and moral emotions in post-apartheid South Africa: an fMRI investigation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 881–892. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx019
Gawronski, B. (2019). Six lessons for a cogent science of implicit bias and its criticism. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 574–595. doi: 10.1177/1745691619826015
Gehenne, L., Christophe, V., Eveno, C., Carnot, A., Turpin, A., Pannier, D., et al. (2021). Creating scripted video-vignettes in an experimental study on two empathic processes in oncology: reflections on our experience. Patient Educ. Couns. 104, 654–662. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2020.08.041
Gerhardstein, K. R., and Anderson, V. N. (2010). There’s more than meets the eye: facial appearance and evaluations of transsexual people. Sex Roles 62, 361–373. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9746-x
Glick, P., Gangl, C., Gibb, S., Klumpner, S., and Weinberg, E. (2007). Defensive reactions to masculinity threat: more negative affect toward effeminate (but not masculine) gay men. Sex Roles 57, 55–59. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9195-3
Glotfelter, M. A., and Anderson, V. N. (2017). Relationships between gender self-esteem, sexual prejudice, and trans prejudice in cisgender heterosexual college students. Int. J. Transgenderism 18, 182–198. doi: 10.1080/15532739.2016.1274932
Grey, J. A., Robinson, B., Bean, E., Coleman, E., and Bockting, W. O. (2013). A systematic review of instruments that measure attitudes toward homosexual men. J. Sex Res. 50, 329–352. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2012.746279
Grigoryan, L., Cohrs, J. C., Boehnke, K., Van De Vijver, F. A. J. R., and Easterbrook, M. J. (2022). Multiple categorization and intergroup bias: examining the generalizability of three theories of intergroup relations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 122, 34–52. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000342
Groen, Y., Wijers, A. A., Tucha, O., and Althaus, M. (2013). Are there sex differences in ERPs related to processing empathy-evoking pictures? Neuropsychologia 51, 142–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.012
Hamner, K. M. (1992). “Gay-bashing: a social identity analysis of violence against lesbians and gay men” in Hate crimes: Confronting violence against lesbians and gay men (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications), 179–190.
Han, S. (2018). Neurocognitive basis of racial ingroup bias in empathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 400–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.013
Harari, H., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Ravid, M., and Levkovitz, Y. (2010). Double dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 175, 277–279. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.03.002
Hatfield, E., Rapson, R. L., and Yen-Chi, L. (2009). “Emotional contagion and empathy” in The social neuroscience of empathy. eds. J. Decety and W. J. Ickes (Cambridge: Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology), 19–30.
Hein, S., Röder, M., and Fingerle, M. (2018). The role of emotion regulation in situational empathy-related responding and prosocial behaviour in the presence of negative affect: empathy, prosocial behaviour and emotion regulation. Int. J. Psychol. 53, 477–485. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12405
Herek, G. M. (2000). SexualPrejudice and gender: do heterosexuals’ attitudes toward lesbians and gay men differ? J. Soc. Issues 56, 251–266. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00164
Herek, G. M. (2002). Gender gaps in public opinion about lesbians and gay men. Public Opin. Q. 66, 40–66. doi: 10.1086/338409
Herek, G. M., and Gonzalez-Rivera, M. (2006). Attitudes toward homosexuality among U.S. residents of Mexican descent. J. Sex Res. 43, 122–135. doi: 10.1080/00224490609552307
Herek, G. M., and McLemore, K. A. (2013). Sexual prejudice. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 309–333. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143826
Hooker, C. I., Verosky, S. C., Germine, L. T., Knight, R. T., and D’Esposito, M. (2010). Neural activity during social signal perception correlates with self-reported empathy. Brain Res. 1308, 100–113. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.10.006
Hudson, S. T. J., Cikara, M., and Sidanius, J. (2019). Preference for hierarchy is associated with reduced empathy and increased counter-empathy towards others, especially out-group targets. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 85:103871. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103871
Hunt, P. A., Denieffe, S., and Gooney, M. (2017). Burnout and its relationship to empathy in nursing: a review of the literature. J. Res. Nurs. 22, 7–22. doi: 10.1177/1744987116678902
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Knobe, J., and Bloom, P. (2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion 9, 435–439. doi: 10.1037/a0015960
Jolliffe, D., and Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of the basic empathy scale. J. Adolesc. 29, 589–611. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010
Kaseweter, K. A., Drwecki, B. B., and Prkachin, K. M. (2012). Racial differences in pain treatment and empathy in a Canadian sample. Pain Res. Manag. 17, 381–384. doi: 10.1155/2012/803474
Kiebel, E. M., McFadden, S. L., and Herbstrith, J. C. (2017). Disgusted but not afraid: feelings toward same-sex kissing reveal subtle homonegativity. J. Soc. Psychol. 157, 263–278. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2016.1184127
Kite, M. E., and Whitley, B. E. (1996). Sex differences in attitudes toward homosexual persons, behaviors, and civil rights: a meta-analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22, 336–353. doi: 10.1177/0146167296224002
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford.
Kobach, M. J., and Weaver, A. J. (2012). Gender and empathy differences in negative reactions to fictionalized and real violent images. Commun. Rep. 25, 51–61. doi: 10.1080/08934215.2012.721087
Kourmousi, N., Amanaki, E., Tzavara, C., Merakou, K., Barbouni, A., and Koutras, V. (2017). The Toronto empathy questionnaire: reliability and validity in a nationwide sample of Greek teachers. Soc. Sci. 6:62. doi: 10.3390/socsci6020062
Lee, M. D., and Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2013). Bayesian cognitive modeling: a practical course. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lima, F. F. D., and Osório, F. D. L. (2021). Empathy: assessment instruments and psychometric quality – a systematic literature review with a meta-analysis of the past ten years. Front. Psychol. 12:781346. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.781346
Lindeman, M., Koirikivi, I., and Lipsanen, J. (2018). Pictorial empathy test (PET): an easy-to-use method for assessing affective empathic reactions. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 34, 421–431. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000353
Longobardi, E., Spataro, P., and Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2019). Direct and indirect associations of empathy, theory of mind, and language with prosocial behavior: gender differences in primary school children. J. Genet. Psychol. 180, 266–279. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2019.1653817
Mahaffey, A. L., Bryan, A., and Hutchison, K. E. (2005). Sex differences in affective responses to homoerotic stimuli: evidence for an unconscious bias among heterosexual men, but not heterosexual women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 34, 537–545. doi: 10.1007/s10508-005-6279-4
Marsden, A. D., and Barnett, M. D. (2020). The role of empathy in the relationship between social political ideology and sexual prejudice in heterosexual college students in the U.S. Arch. Sex. Behav. 49, 1853–1861. doi: 10.1007/s10508-019-01545-5
Martínez-Velázquez, E. S., Ahuatzin González, A. L., Chamorro, Y., and Sequeira, H. (2020). The influence of empathy trait and gender on empathic responses: a study with dynamic emotional stimulus and eye movement recordings. Front. Psychol. 11:23. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00023
McCrackin, S. D., and Itier, R. J. (2021a). Feeling through another’s eyes: perceived gaze direction impacts ERP and behavioural measures of positive and negative affective empathy. NeuroImage 226:117605. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117605
McCrackin, S. D., and Itier, R. J. (2021b). I can see it in your eyes: perceived gaze direction impacts ERP and behavioural measures of affective theory of mind. Cortex 143, 205–222. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.05.024
McGrath, M. J., and Haslam, N. (2020). Development and validation of the harm concept breadth scale: assessing individual differences in harm inflation. PLoS One 15:e0237732. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237732
Meiring, L., Subramoney, S., Thomas, K. G., Decety, J., and Fourie, M. M. (2014). Empathy and helping: effects of racial group membership and cognitive load. South Afr. J. Psychol. 44, 426–438. doi: 10.1177/0081246314530280
Molenberghs, P., Gapp, J., Wang, B., Louis, W. R., and Decety, J. (2016). Increased moral sensitivity for outgroup perpetrators harming ingroup members. Cereb. Cortex 26, 225–233. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu195
Montalan, B., Lelard, T., Godefroy, O., and Mouras, H. (2012). Behavioral investigation of the influence of social categorization on empathy for pain: a minimal group paradigm study. Front. Psychol. 3:389. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00389
Monto, M. A., and Supinski, J. (2014). Discomfort with homosexuality: a new measure captures differences in attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. J. Homosex. 61, 899–916. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2014.870816
Morrison, T. G., Kenny, P., and Harrington, A. (2005). Modern prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women: assessing the viability of a measure of modern homonegative attitudes within an Irish context. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 131, 219–250. doi: 10.3200/MONO.131.3.219-250
Morrison, M. A., and Morrison, T. G. (2003). Development and validation of a scale measuring modern prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women. J. Homosex. 43, 15–37. doi: 10.1300/j082v43n02_02
Morrison, M. A., Morrison, T. G., and Franklin, R. (2009). Modern and old-fashioned homonegativity among samples of Canadian and American university students. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 40, 523–542. doi: 10.1177/0022022109335053
Murphy, B. A., and Lilienfeld, S. O. (2019). Are self-report cognitive empathy ratings valid proxies for cognitive empathy ability? Negligible meta-analytic relations with behavioral task performance. Psychol. Assess. 31, 1062–1072. doi: 10.1037/pas0000732
Norris, W. P. (1992). Liberal attitudes and homophobic acts: the paradoxes of homosexual experience in a Liberal institution. J. Homosex. 22, 81–120. doi: 10.1300/J082v22n03_04
O’Handley, B. M., Blair, K. L., and Hoskin, R. A. (2017). What do two men kissing and a bucket of maggots have in common? Heterosexual men’s indistinguishable salivary α-amylase responses to photos of two men kissing and disgusting images. Psychol. Sex. 8, 173–188. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2017.1328459
Olatunji, B. O. (2008). Disgust, scrupulosity and conservative attitudes about sex: evidence for a mediational model of homophobia. J. Res. Personal. 42, 1364–1369. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.001
Parrott, D. J., and Gallagher, K. E. (2008). What accounts for heterosexual women’s negative emotional responses to lesbians? Examination of traditional gender role beliefs and sexual prejudice. Sex Roles 59, 229–239. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9436-0
Peterson, C. H., Dalley, L. M., Dombrowski, S. C., and Maier, C. (2017). A review of instruments that measure LGBTQ affirmation and discrimination constructs in adults. J. LGBT Issues Couns. 11, 230–246. doi: 10.1080/15538605.2017.1380555
Pettigrew, T. F., and Tropp, L. R. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 922–934. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.504
Poteat, P. V., DiGiovanni, C. D., and Scheer, J. R. (2013). Predicting homophobic behavior among heterosexual youth: domain general and sexual orientation-specific factors at the individual and contextual level. J. Youth Adolesc. 42, 351–362. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9813-4
Ratcliff, J. J., Lassiter, G. D., Markman, K. D., and Snyder, C. J. (2006). Gender differences in attitudes toward gay men and lesbians: the role of motivation to respond without prejudice. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 1325–1338. doi: 10.1177/0146167206290213
Rattazzi, A. M. M., and Volpato, C. (2003). Social desirability of subtle and blatant prejudice scales. Psychol. Rep. 92, 241–250. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2003.92.1.241
Romero, D. H., Morera, O. F., and Wiebe, J. S. (2015). Assessing the gender invariance of the modern homonegativity scale. J. Homosex. 62, 1539–1559. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2015.1073034
Rye, B. J., and Meaney, G. J. (2010). Measuring homonegativity: a psychometric analysis. Can. J. Behav. Sci. Rev. Can. Sci. Comport. 42, 158–167. doi: 10.1037/a0018237
Sessa, P., Meconi, F., Castelli, L., and Dell’Acqua, R. (2014). Taking one’s time in feeling other-race pain: an event-related potential investigation on the time-course of cross-racial empathy. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 454–463. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst003
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 132, 617–627. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn279
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Goldsher, D., Berger, B. D., and Aharon-Peretz, J. (2004). Impairment in cognitive and affective empathy in patients with brain lesions: anatomical and cognitive correlates. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 26, 1113–1127. doi: 10.1080/13803390490515531
Shields, S. A., and Harriman, R. E. (1984). Fear of male homosexuality: cardiac responses of low and high homonegative males. J. Homosex. 10, 53–67. doi: 10.1300/J082v10n01_04
Sidanius, J., Kteily, N., Sheehy-Skeffington, J., Ho, A. K., Sibley, C., and Duriez, B. (2013). You’re inferior and not worth our concern: the interface between empathy and social dominance orientation: empathy and social dominance orientation. J. Pers. 81, 313–323. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12008
Sonnby-Borgström, M., Jönsson, P., and Svensson, O. (2008). Gender differences in facial imitation and verbally reported emotional contagion from spontaneous to emotionally regulated processing levels. Scand. J. Psychol. 49, 111–122. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00626.x
Spreng, R. N., McKinnon, M. C., Mar, R. A., and Levine, B. (2009). The Toronto empathy questionnaire: scale development and initial validation of a factor-analytic solution to multiple empathy measures. J. Pers. Assess. 91, 62–71. doi: 10.1080/00223890802484381
Stathi, S., Humayun, S., Stoddart Isaac, R., and McCarron, D. M. (2021). Psychopathy and prejudice: the mediating role of empathy, social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism. J. Theor. Soc. Psychol. 5, 530–541. doi: 10.1002/jts5.116
Stępień-Nycz, M., Bosacki, S., and Białecka-Pikul, M. (2021). Theory of mind in adolescence: developmental change and gender differences. J. Early Adolesc. 41, 424–436. doi: 10.1177/0272431620919164
Suleiman, R., Yahya, R., Decety, J., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2018). The impact of implicitly and explicitly primed ingroup–outgroup categorization on the evaluation of others pain: the case of the Jewish–Arab conflict. Motiv. Emot. 42, 438–445. doi: 10.1007/s11031-018-9677-3
Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., and Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: a predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personal. Individ. Differ. 49, 587–592. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.024
Totan, T., Doğan, T., and Sapmaz, F. (2012). The Toronto empathy questionnaire: evaluation of psychometric properties among Turkish university students. Eurasian J. Educ. Res. 46, 179–198.
Toussaint, L., and Webb, J. R. (2005). Gender differences in the relationship between empathy and forgiveness. J. Soc. Psychol. 145, 673–685. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.145.6.673-686
Trawalter, S., and Hoffman, K. M. (2015). Got pain? Racial bias in perceptions of pain. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 9, 146–157. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12161
Turner, J. C., and Reynolds, K. J. (2010). “The story of social identity,” in Rediscovering Social Identity. eds. T. Postmes and N. R. Branscombe (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 13–32.
Vachon, D. D., and Lynam, D. R. (2016). Fixing the problem with empathy: development and validation of the affective and cognitive measure of empathy. Assessment 23, 135–149. doi: 10.1177/1073191114567941
van Baaren, R. B., Decety, J., Dijksterhuis, A., van der Leij, A., and van Leeuwen, M. L. (2009). “Being imitated: consequences of non-consciously showing empathy” in The social neuroscience of empathy (Cambridge: Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology), 31–42.
Vandello, J. A., Bosson, J. K., Cohen, D., Burnaford, R. M., and Weaver, J. R. (2008). Precarious manhood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 1325–1339. doi: 10.1037/a0012453
Vanman, E. J. (2016). The role of empathy in intergroup relations. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 11, 59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.06.007
Vera Cruz, G., and Mullet, E. (2019). Empathy and forgiveness among Mozambican wives. Int. Perspect. Psychol. 8, 124–143. doi: 10.1037/ipp0000110
Wang, Y., Song, J., Guo, F., Zhang, Z., Yuan, S., and Cacioppo, S. (2016). Spatiotemporal brain dynamics of empathy for pain and happiness in friendship. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:45. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00045
Watt, D. (2007). Toward a neuroscience of empathy: integrating affective and cognitive perspectives. Neuropsychoanalysis 9, 119–140. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2007.10773550
Wilkinson, H., Whittington, R., Perry, L., and Eames, C. (2017). Examining the relationship between burnout and empathy in healthcare professionals: a systematic review. Burn. Res. 6, 18–29. doi: 10.1016/j.burn.2017.06.003
Keywords: empathy, sexual prejudice, gender, sexual orientation, prejudice
Citation: Winward SB and Itier RJ (2025) The influence of sexual prejudice and gender on trait and state-level empathy. Front. Psychol. 16:1527124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1527124
Received: 12 November 2024; Accepted: 20 March 2025;
Published: 01 April 2025.
Edited by:
Qing Zhao, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), ChinaReviewed by:
Shishir Baliyan, National University of Distance Education (UNED), SpainCopyright © 2025 Winward and Itier. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Seth B. Winward, c3dpbndhcmRAdXdhdGVybG9vLmNh
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.