
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
REVIEW article
Front. Psychol. , 07 February 2025
Sec. Emotion Science
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1525517
Empathy is a pivotal capacity that is essential for human interaction. It encompasses cognitive empathy, which is the ability to understand another individual’s emotional state, and affective empathy, which is to express an appropriate affective response to another person’s emotional state. Recent advancements in empathy research have highlighted the contextual nature of both cognitive and affective empathy, signifying their susceptibility to modulation by situational factors. Despite this progress, a comprehensive mechanistic understanding of empathy as a form of situated cognition that integrates both state and trait dimensions remains scarce. This review outlines the interplay of trait and state empathy and how state empathy emerges from a dynamic interplay between bottom-up processes and top-down control mechanisms. It further covers which situational factors increase versus decrease state empathy. In addition, to assist in selecting appropriate measurement tools for measuring trait and/or state empathy, the review categorizes existing empathy measurement instruments. Taken together, this review provides a roadmap for enhancing the efficacy of future empathy studies by: (1) outlining the current theoretical and methodological considerations for disentangling trait and state empathy; (2) organizing existing empathy measurement tools to aid researchers in selecting appropriate tools for future studies; (3) describing the interplay between bottom-up processes and top-down control mechanisms for state and trait empathy; and (4) reviewing factors that increase or decrease state empathy to prevent their potential interference and enable a more accurate assessment of empathy.
According to a recent umbrella review “empathy is to understand, feel, and share what someone else feels with a clear self-other differentiation” (Håkansson Eklund and Summer Meranius, 2021, p. 306). This definition integrates two essential components, namely, how well an individual can perceive and understand the emotions of another individual (cognitive empathy), and the affective state an individual feels in response to others’ emotions (affective empathy). The long-term coexistence of multiple definitions may explain why a variety of measurement tools are available to assess empathy (Hall and Schwartz, 2019). Thus far, however, situational factors that modify empathy, thereby addressing the situatedness of empathy, have yet to be considered with regard to empathy for complex emotions. Following the work of Nezlek et al. (2001), Cuff et al. (2016) recently emphasized the importance of considering the interaction between trait capacities and state-related factors for individual empathic responses. Indeed, this may be crucial when establishing a comprehensive framework to explain the processes of empathy. The authors propose that an individual’s capacity for empathy consists of a stable trait component, akin to personality factors, and a state component influenced by specific situational factors like acute stress, pain, or mood (Cuff et al., 2016). Although the selection of different measurement tools for different aspects of empathy (e.g., self-report measures for trait empathy and task-based performance measures for state empathy) has become an implicit practice by researchers, standardized measures of empathy do not always integrate both aspects and sometimes even ignore this distinction. This results in ambiguity when interpreting whether differences in empathic responses reflect (a) trait capacities of an individual, or (b) state empathy influenced by the specific situation or natural condition in which the study was conducted. A comprehensive assessment of the various components of empathy that incorporates contextual factors that impact state empathy is necessary to address this issue. While studies have considered situational factors such as motivation for enhancing empathy (Weisz et al., 2021), a structured approach has yet to be taken to differentiate trait and state empathy.
The aim of the present review, therefore, is to comprehensively examine current theoretical and methodological considerations and empirical evidence for differentiating trait and state empathy. It comprises four main steps: (1) outlining the current theoretical and methodological considerations for disentangling trait and state empathy; (2) organizing existing empathy measurement tools to aid researchers in selecting appropriate tools for future studies; (3) describing the interplay between bottom-up processes and top-down control mechanisms for state and trait empathy; and (4) reviewing factors that increase or decrease state empathy to prevent their potential interference and enable a more accurate assessment of empathy.
The Perception-Action Model (PAM; Preston and de Waal, 2002), one of the leading theoretical frameworks relating to empathy, is particularly relevant for establishing a theoretical definition of trait and state empathy. The PAM depicts a bottom-up process of empathy, meaning that initially both neural and cognitive representations of the emotional state of another person emerge within the individual. The framework was further developed to include top-down regulation processes that moderate the initial automatic component of empathy (de Vignemont and Singer, 2006; Preston et al., 2020), and that relate to executive functions, self-regulation mechanisms, or attention (Preston et al., 2020). Transferring this idea to trait versus state differentiation, we argue that the bottom-up process of empathy may be the result of the initial activation of trait empathy, while the top-down regulation may shape the final state empathy reaction. In general, trait factors are considered traits only when they demonstrate stability over time and consistency across contexts, state components are generally characterized by their variability in response to acute situations. They change over the lifespan and are influenced by contextual factors (Roberts and DelVecchio, 2000). In previous work conducted by van der Graaff et al. (2016), Zhao et al. (2021), and Zhao et al. (2022), trait empathy is considered the general ability to express empathy, while state empathy responses depend on the immediate context. In line with this hypothesis, Hall and Schwartz (2019) consider that empathy originates from a general capacity that is further influenced by situational or motivational factors, thereby emphasizing that the distinction between trait and state components of empathy has been proposed by various researchers.
On an experimental level, a small number of studies have recently approached empathy from a trait versus state perspective and investigated the extent to which the two concepts coincide (van der Graaff et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2022; Zhao et al., 2021). For instance, in response to sadness, and thereby including the valence of the stimulus material, trait empathy [measured using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) Davis, 1983] was positively associated with state empathy (measured by the rating of emotional film clips) both for cognitive and affective empathy measures (van der Graaff et al., 2016). The same pattern occurred in response to happiness, though less consistently (van der Graaff et al., 2016). Regarding the culture-sex interaction in empathy, both ethnic-group bias and sex-group favor (adapted according to the sex of the stimulus) were reported to vary for different groups (Caucasian versus Asian participants) in task-based state empathy measures (single-character portraits and documentary photos with emotional background) (Zhao et al., 2021). In their study, state empathy was measured using two sets of stimuli: single-character portraits and documentary photos with emotional background, with each set consisting of 24 pictures (depicting the following stimulus features 2 cultures, 2 sexes, and 6 emotions [happiness, anger, sadness, surprise, and fear, and neutral-peacefulness of the protagonists]). Data revealed significant two-way, three-way, and four-way interactions of the factors sex of the participant, and stimulus sex, as well as culture of the participant and stimulus culture, resulting in specific in-group vs. out-group relations. Furthermore, the effects partly varied depending on the specific emotion quality. The measurement tools to assess trait empathy, the IR and the Empathy Quotient (EQ, Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004) only allowed examining the effects of the participants´ culture (only found in females, with higher scores in Australian than Chinese women) and the participants´ sex (only found in Australians with higher scores in Australian women than Australian men). Future studies should consider the assessment of culture-sex interaction in both, state and trait measures of empathy.
This highlights the interaction of individuals’ trait characteristics (measured by self-report questionnaires) and state factors (Zhao et al., 2021). The trait versus state dissociation of empathy was further investigated from a neuroscientific perspective in a female sample: the resting-state activity patterns of the state empathy neural network consisting of the bilateral supplementary motor area and the middle cingulate cortex, as well as the left anterior insula and the inferior frontal gyrus, could predict trait empathy measures (Zhao et al., 2022). In essence, while the interplay between trait and state empathy is present, these nuanced distinctions underscore the need to continue investigating the relationship between the two dimensions.
Some studies have attempted to identify factors that influence empathy without differentiating between state and trait empathy. Cuff et al. (2016) compiled factors that vary within the individual, such as current cognitive load (Rameson et al., 2012), perceived power (Galinsky et al., 2006), perceived need to emphasize (Lishner et al., 2011), blame (Rudolph et al., 2004), mood (Pithers, 1999) or observer-target similarity (Håkansson Eklund et al., 2009) to the empathic response of the observer. Further factors that affect an observer’s expression of empathy are acute stress (Nitschke and Bartz, 2023; von Dawans et al., 2021; Wolf et al., 2015), attention (Zaki, 2014), emotions or affective states (Tamir, 2016; Thompson et al., 2019), motivation (Zaki, 2014) or the different hormonal phases in women’s menstrual cycles (Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021b). This will be discussed in more depth later in this review. Interestingly, other psychological constructs, for example, anxiety, have already been thoroughly investigated concerning the difference between trait and state, and distinct measurement tools have been developed (for an example, see the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory by Spielberger et al. (1971)). This indicates that such a distinction may increase our understanding of such psychological constructs. It is important to further view this differentiation from a developmental perspective: Jarvis et al. (2024) recently published a meta-analysis indicating that affective empathy may increase with age. Cognitive empathy seems to be relatively stable once individuals reach adulthood, only starting to decline after the age of 65 (Dorris et al., 2022). Yet, both affective and cognitive empathy seem to vary across the lifespan, thereby indicating that state measures of those concepts are susceptible to context-dependent changes. Although beyond the scope of this review, it should be acknowledged that investigating empathy, and more precisely trait and state empathy from a clinical perspective holds the potential to unravel nuances in trait and state empathy (for a comparison see Preston et al. (2020)).
Despite these research efforts, little is known about how current evidence relates to the distinction between trait and state empathy. Interestingly, research widely considers that empathy consists of two facets: cognitive versus affective empathy. This distinction, however, fails to explicitly acknowledge empathy as a situated capacity; nevertheless, the differentiation between cognitive and affective empathy may play a role in conceptualizing trait and state empathy. Situatedness or a situated capacity is the idea that contexts plays a pivotal role in expressing capacities and that a skill (or in the case of situated cognition a thought) is specific to a situation (Brown et al., 1989; Newen et al., 2018). We look at cognitive and affective empathy as both traits and states, and highlight the differences between empathy and its related constructs. We will review exemplary findings that suggest a distinction between cognitive and affective empathy from a trait versus state perspective. Of note, the factors reported to influence the different facets of empathy are exemplary as well. A comprehensive review of factors increasing and decreasing empathy will be provided at a later point.
As mentioned above, definitions of empathy cover both cognitive and affective empathy (Cuff et al., 2016; Singer and Lamm, 2009). It has been shown that cognitive and affective empathy partly rely on different neural circuits (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009). This suggests that cognitive and affective empathy are independent to some extent. However, their specific interaction and interdependencies are still debated (Cuff et al., 2016; Preston et al., 2020).
It has recently been argued that affective empathy can occur without cognitive empathy but not vice versa because affective representations in the bodily state are necessary to understand the emotional state of another person (Preston et al., 2020). Notably, affective empathy is considered the result of an initial bottom-up process wherein the perception of external stimuli, e.g., the emotional state of another person, induces a representation in the observer itself (de Waal and Preston, 2017; Preston et al., 2020). On the contrary, cognitive empathy is thought to be a top-down process in which internal stimulation leads to cognitive empathy processes. Despite this difference in the origin of stimulation (external versus internal), and the sequence of processes (bottom-up to top-down process for affective empathy and top-down to bottom-up process for cognitive empathy), both affective and cognitive empathy activate affective representations in the observer, leading to similar representations in bodily states (de Waal and Preston, 2017; Preston et al., 2020).
The distinction between cognitive and affective empathy may also be crucial when discussing situational variables affecting state empathy. In this regard, twin studies estimating genetic and environmental portions suggest that affective empathy is more heritable than cognitive empathy, as demonstrated by a recent meta-analysis (Abramson et al., 2020). From this finding, one might conclude that affective empathy is more strongly determined by genetic factors than cognitive empathy, and in turn, that affective empathy may be more stable and cognitive empathy more situated. Cognitive empathy may thus be more susceptible to the potential effects of detrimental environmental factors, but also more responsive to interventions meant to increase empathy.
Preliminary findings contradict these conclusions. For example, Wolf et al. (2015) did not find changes in cognitive empathy measures after acute stress. In contrast, however, they report an increase in affective empathy following acute stress. Other studies report beneficial effects of acute stress on emotion recognition, a subcomponent of cognitive empathy (Domes and Zimmer, 2019). The apparent inconsistency in these results can be attributed in part to differences in the selection of measurement tools for empathy. For example, Wolf et al. (2015) implemented the Multifaceted Empathy Test in its Condensed and Revised Version (MET-core-2; Dziobek et al., 2011) to measure empathy, a task that includes the identification and the affective sharing of complex emotions. Alternatively, participants in a study by Domes and Zimmer (2019) were asked to decide whether one of two basic emotions or a non-emotional condition (angry, happy, or neutral) was present on pictorial stimuli. Such methodological disparities likely contribute to the mixed findings, an explanation that will be elaborated later in the manuscript.
The issue becomes even more complex when one considers that both cognitive and affective empathy lead to changes in bodily states (de Waal and Preston, 2017). Thus, the construct of embodiment must be recognized when discussing empathy. Embodiment is a psychological construct that refers to the idea that our thoughts, emotions, and behaviors are shaped by the physical body and its interactions with the environment (Newen et al., 2018). In the context of empathy, embodiment implies that bodily physical sensations influence the way we experience and respond to others’ emotions (Niedenthal, 2007). One implication of this approach is that perceiving another person’s emotions depends on the interaction between your bodily state and the emotions the other person displays (Niedenthal, 2007). This idea also implies that perceiving an emotion is always situated, and empathy should be considered an embodied process instead of a purely cognitive one.
In sum, because empathy is conceptualized as a complex and dynamic process influenced by a variety of internal and external factors, it is imperative to establish guidelines in measurement standards for empathy research. The recent theoretical and empirical developments in empathy research lead us to conclude that trait empathy represents an individual’s overall capacity for both cognitive and affective empathy. In contrast, state empathy is the specific expression of this capacity in a given situation. In this regard, the definition of Håkansson Eklund and Summer Meranius (2021) appears incomplete and requires expansion for clarity. We propose the following addition (see italics) to this definition: The general construct of trait empathy includes four factors: understanding, feeling, and sharing another person’s emotion with a clear self-other differentiation. The expression of empathy is situated within both psychological and physiological factors that influence the empathic response.
Past research on empathy has not only been challenged by methodological issues but also theoretical ones, specifically in differentiating empathy from its related constructs (Cuff et al., 2016). A central factor in discriminating empathy from other constructs is self-other differentiation (de Vignemont and Singer, 2006). In (both trait and state) empathy, the observer is aware that the affective state of interest originates in another person and not in oneself. In contrast, when experiencing emotional contagion for example, the observer acknowledges an affective state but is unable to determine where it originated. Thus, the observer believes that the observed affective state comes from themself and fails to differentiate between sources of origin (Cuff et al., 2016).
It is also important to disentangle state empathy from pro-social behavior. Pro-social behavior is broadly defined as any action or behavior that promotes welfare in others (Pfattheicher et al., 2022). State empathy is the situated expression of empathy that can, but does not have to, be expressed in behavior. The question then arises of how state empathy and prosocial behavior are connected.
It has been suggested that (state) empathy constitutes the basis for showing prosocial behavior (Stevens and Taber, 2021). However, empathy and prosocial behavior do not seem to act on each other directly. Instead, it has been proposed that empathy resulting in prosocial behavior is mediated by compassion (Stevens and Taber, 2021). Compassion is defined as the ability to understand when another person is suffering and feeling emotionally connected to that person (Strauss et al., 2016). Furthermore, the observer can understand the common ground of this emotion, tolerate the (potentially negative) emotions that result in themself, and finally act or develop the motivation to act (Strauss et al., 2016). Stevens and Taber (2021) postulate that while compassion leads to prosocial behavior, empathy does not do this automatically. Regardless, empathy is considered an essential component of the emergence of compassionate feelings and thus prosocial behavior (Lim and DeSteno, 2016).
Two more distinctions are important to note. First and foremost, the difference between emotion recognition and empathy plays a central role in empathy research. Emotion recognition is a cognitive ability that enables the recognition of basic and complex emotions in others (Domes and Zimmer, 2019; Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021a). It is generally considered to be one facet of cognitive empathy (Cuff et al., 2016; von Dawans et al., 2021). Beyond this, cognitive empathy includes further processes such as mentalizing (von Dawans et al., 2021). Basic emotions typically cover happiness, sadness, fear, anger, surprise, and disgust, and are recognized across cultures (Fridenson-Hayo et al., 2016). In contrast, complex emotions are culturally dependent and rely on the context in which they occur (Fridenson-Hayo et al., 2016). Second, for complex emotions, it is worthwhile making a distinction between empathy for pain vs. empathy for other complex emotions. Empathy for pain has been researched extensively in behavioral paradigms (Gonzalez-Liencres et al., 2016; Lamm et al., 2007; Tomova et al., 2017), but only a few studies consider state empathy measures that target more complex, situated emotions as assessed by the MET-core-2 (e.g., highly satisfied, relaxed, or jubilant as positive emotions, and terrified, frustrated or desperately unhappy as negative emotions) while measuring both, cognitive and affective empathy (Drimalla et al., 2019; Dziobek et al., 2011; Dziobek et al., 2008; Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021a; Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021b; Wolf et al., 2015). Findings from previous studies targeting empathy for pain have been generalized to the wider construct of empathy (Lamm et al., 2007; Tomova et al., 2017). However, Timmers et al. (2018) showed that there are unique neural correlates of empathy for pain and that because a distinction between empathy for pain and empathy for other emotions exists, any generalizations should be made with caution (Timmers et al., 2018). These different components reveal that empathy is a nuanced concept and that measurement tools must be adjusted to the specific aspect of the research question. To the extent that it is possible, this review focuses on measurements of empathy for complex emotions.
Previous studies have tried to measure empathy via self-report measures, task-based performance / behavioral measures, or neuroimaging. However, because studies have yet to systematically match the theoretical definition of empathy to the measurement tool used to assess empathy or an empathy-related construct, Hall and Schwartz (2019) call for an adapted multitrait-multi-method approach to assess empathy in order to increase homogeneity and “accommodate both empathic traits and empathic states” (Hall and Schwartz, 2019, p. 235). This review considers the extent to which the distinction between trait and state empathy is reflected in methodological approaches used thus far. The following chapter outlines how the most prominent tools for measuring the empathy construct currently address the distinction.
In self-report measures, participants are asked to evaluate their empathic abilities or situational expression by choosing a response based on how much they agree or disagree with an item (e.g., “I really get involved with the feelings of the characters in a novel.”; Davis, 1983). Hall and Schwartz (2019) have recently gathered an overview of the most frequently used self-report empathy measurement instruments. Similarly, Yu and Kirk (2009) have listed available measurement tools. Based on these two reviews, we have compiled an overview of self-report measures of empathy in Table 1. Note that to refrain from a subjective bias when selecting the list of measurement tools included in this review, we decided to base our list solely on the systematic review by Yu and Kirk (2009) and the quantitative review by Hall and Schwartz (2019). Thereby, the list is not exhaustive but aims to summarize the most common approaches. It is important to acknowledge, that by using this approach to select measurement tools, some tools that are also used in research are not included in this review [e.g., the Emotional Contagion Scale by Doherty (1997) and the Emotional Empathy Scale by Mehrabian and Epstein (1972)]. To facilitate the choice of an appropriate measurement tool, it is essential to know which tools address which facets of empathy (cognitive versus affective versus both), and if they distinguish between trait and state empathy.
In addition, Vieten et al. (2024) recently published a very concise and comprehensive overview of both, empathy and compassion measurement tools. While these authors have focused on including both constructs, the present review solely focuses on the work by Hall and Schwartz (2019) and Yu and Kirk (2009) and thereby on empathy measurement tools and the distinction of trait and state empathy.
Performance measures, in contrast to self-report measures, require participants to make a (behavioral) forced choice between different alternatives with one of the alternatives being the correctly identified emotion. This opens up the possibility of measuring empathy based on responses as participants do not subjectively have to rate their empathic skills. In the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test, participants are instructed to identify the emotional state of the protagonist by evaluating the expression of a pair of eyes on a picture and choosing one of four alternatives (RMET; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). Other frequently used tests are the Pictorial Empathy Test (PET; Lindeman et al., 2016) and the Multifaceted Empathy Test, both in its original version (MET; Dziobek et al., 2008) and a Condensed and Revised Version (MET-core-2; Drimalla et al., 2019; Dziobek et al., 2011). In line with the questions raised in the previous chapter on self-report measures, we provide a systematic categorization of empathy measurements. The tools are (1) categorized based on whether they assess cognitive empathy, affective empathy, both constructs, or neither, (2) further classified as a self-report measure or performance measure, and (3) grouped on whether they specifically address trait empathy, state empathy, both constructs, or neither. In addition to the tools addressed by Yu and Kirk (2009) and Hall and Schwartz (2019), we also included the MET (Dziobek et al., 2008), the MET-core-2 (Drimalla et al., 2019; Dziobek et al., 2011), the BES (Jolliffe and Farrington, 2006), the PET (Lindeman et al., 2016) and the RMET (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) in Figure 1 based on the frequency of their use in research. Where possible, we have included a citation of the sentence upon which we based our decision in Appendix Table A. We based the categorization not on how these measures have been used in the past, but solely on what is stated in their respective manuals. This caused certain challenges; for example, we categorized the RMET (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) as “not addressed” even though it has been used as a state measure in most studies. Similarly, the IRI (Davis, 1983) is frequently used as a trait measure of empathy but the original manuscript did not state specifically if this tool is used to measure trait or state empathy. As we believe this to be the most objective approach, we ask that researchers address the distinction between trait and state empathy in their manuals. Lastly, we want to point out that some information displayed in the table was adapted and extended based on the work by Yu and Kirk (2009).
Figure 1. Diagram to select the appropriate Empathy Measurement Tool. This diagram sorts current empathy measurement tools by three factors: (1) whether the original manuscript addresses either cognitive or affective empathy separately or combined or if it does not address these components (pink boxes); (2) whether it is a self-report or a performance measure (yellow boxes) and (3) whether the original manuscript addresses if the tool measures empathy as a trait, as a state, or both trait and state combined or if it does not address it (green boxes). A red cross indicates that, to the best of our knowledge, there is no current empathy measurement tool available for this category. Please note that the categorization of the Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale (BEES; Mehrabian, 1996) and the Perception of Empathy Inventory (PET; Wheeler, 1990) are not included in this figure despite their original listing in Yu and Kirk (2009) or Hall and Schwartz (2019) due to the lacking availability of the original manuscript. The asterisks indicate: *Category (either cognitive/affective or trait/state) was not directly addressed in the manuscript but could indirectly be concluded. **Cognitive and Affective Empathy was addressed but is not being measured separately. BLRI, Barrett-Lennard Relationship Inventory; BES, Basic Empathy Scale; CIDC, Carkhuff Indices of Discrimination & Communication; CVEDS, Child Victim Empathy Distortions Scale; CARE, Consultation and Relationship Empathy; EIS, Emotional Intelligence Scale; ECRS, Empathy Construct Rating Scale; EQ, Empathy Quotient; HES, Hogan Empathy Scale; IRI, Interpersonal Reactivity Index; JSPE, Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy; LET, Layton Empathy Test; MET, Multifaceted Empathy Test; MET-core-2, Multifaceted Empathy Test Condensed and Revised Version; PET, Pictorial Empathy Test; QCAE, Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy; QMEE, Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy; SEE, Scale of Ethnocultural Empathy; SEQ, Socio-emotional Questionnaire; TEQ, Toronto Empathy Questionnaire; RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test. Created with Biorender.com.
As depicted in Figure 1, a high number of self-report measures cover trait empathy. In contrast, to our knowledge, only one self-report measure has been developed to approach state empathy, namely the BLRI (Barrett-Lennard, 1962), and no self-report measure specifically targets both, trait and state empathy in one tool. While Batson et al. (1987) mentioned in their manual of the Batson scale that it is advisable to establish a clear conceptual distinction between variations in empathic emotion experienced in specific situations, and that the construct gauged by self-report indicates a more general concept, the authors did not include two separate measures for trait and state empathy.
Figure 1 outlines that most measurement tools fail to define whether the self-report measure targets trait or state empathy. Although this can be indirectly inferred from the wording used in some of the items of the questionnaires, we want to emphasize that the authors should address the issue specifically to evolve and further define the correct use of the respective measurement tool. Correspondingly, performance measures were classified as a trait measurement tool, a state measurement tool, or did not address the distinction at all. We consider performance measures an appropriate means for assessing situated empathy that is influenced by contextual factors, e.g., the testing environment. The extent to which a performance measure can assess trait empathy via a repeated-measure design deserves further investigation.
One of the few studies to have addressed the difference between trait and state empathy was conducted by Zhao et al. (2021). They used both the IRI (Davis, 1983) and the EQ (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004) as measures for trait empathy, and an adapted version of a task-based empathy measure by Neumann et al. (2013) to assess state empathy. Similarly, van der Graaff et al. (2016) measured trait empathy using the IRI (Davis, 1983) and state empathy using an adapted task-based empathy assessment that included watching emotionally loaded film clips, a subjective rating, and identification of the emotion. The selected videos for the state empathy task included four different clips representing either happiness or sadness and were taken from Dutch documentary films (van der Graaff et al., 2016). In addition, prior to the task as well as in between emotional video clips, participants watched fragments of an aquatic video fostering relaxation (van der Graaff et al., 2016). Notably, both studies used a self-report measure for trait empathy and a performance measure for state empathy; a division that we favor as well.
Given the classification, tools such as the MET (Dziobek et al., 2008) or the RMET (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) have further limitations. For example, the extent to which existing performance measures of empathy can distinguish simple emotion recognition from cognitive empathy has been questioned (Preston et al., 2020). Both the RMET and the MET ask participants to recognize the emotional state of a target on pictorial stimuli but fail to include higher-level processing steps (Preston et al., 2020).
At this point, it is important to note that in addition to self-report and performance measures, neural activity might serve as a further category to assess empathy. To the best of our knowledge, only one study has specifically targeted the distinction between trait and/or state empathy from a neuroimaging perspective. Zhao et al. (2022) identified a neural network representing state empathy that included the bilateral middle cingulate cortex, the bilateral supplementary motor area, the left inferior frontal gyrus, and the anterior insula. The authors could link intrinsic brain activity in these regions to trait empathy measures conducted using the IRI (Davis, 1983). Yet, as far as we know, no neural network representing trait empathy distinctively from state empathy has been identified. We decided not to include neuroimaging measures as a third category in this review as studies approaching empathy from this perspective usually combine their measures with a self-report and/or a task-based approach to measure empathy.
It is important to apply caution if considering most measures as indicators of trait empathy. Performance and self-report results may be subject to the influence of situational or contextual factors during data collection. Therefore, it is crucial to interpret such measurements with care and account for potential sources of variability. In sum, Figure 1 shows that indications, of whether existing tools measure trait and/or state empathy are lacking, a problem that may partially explain inconsistencies in past empathy research and that can easily be addressed. Our above categorization enables researchers to select appropriate tools for their research questions and study designs, and facilitates the comparison of findings across studies.
In addition to differentiating between trait and state empathy, we further summarized whether respective measurement tools address the cognitive and/or affective domains of empathy. Older empathy measurement instruments in particular tended not to address this difference. This might be due in part to more recent theoretical developments since the differentiation between cognitive and affective empathy may have been established after the development of older tools.
Taken together, the question remains whether or not, and to what extent, existing measurement tools should be adapted. We argue that separating trait versus state empathy does not necessarily lead to new measurement tools. Instead, two advances should be made: (1) authors should clearly state if their experiment targets trait and/or state empathy, choose the appropriate tool, and argue why the tool is suited for the trait and state dimension; and (2) when performing empathy experiments, authors should gather information on situated influences that potentially modulate state empathy and include them as co-variances in the analyses. For this to happen, a consensus needs to be reached on which individual factors modify state empathy. This topic will be discussed in the next chapter.
As discussed earlier, current theoretical approaches consider empathy to be a capacity with expressions that are situated within and influenced by contextual factors (Hall and Schwartz, 2019). Both affective and cognitive empathy are assumed to include a bottom-up process as well as a top-down control mechanism (de Waal and Preston, 2017; Hall and Schwartz, 2019; Preston and de Waal, 2002; Singer and Lamm, 2009). Empathy for pain is the main area considered in terms of neural correlates of empathy. In this context, studies show that neural networks related to empathy processes (mainly insula and anterior cingulate cortex, ACC) are activated even when participants are not explicitly asked to emphasize. This is understood as the initial bottom-up process that cannot be controlled by individuals (Singer and Lamm, 2009). Only later can the empathy process be regulated by top-down control mechanisms. Although a clear distinction between bottom-up and top-down processes is challenging to determine empirically, a theoretical distinction between the two holds value by providing testable predictions and hypotheses for future studies. Furthermore, several studies illustrate that the empathic reaction after stimulus onset alters over time, indicating that there are fast, intuitive, and slow deliberative processes influencing the final expression of empathy (for an overview of examples for both processes in more detail see Singer and Lamm, 2009).
Empirical studies on empathy for more complex emotions are scarce. Despite this, it can be expected that complex emotional mechanisms work similarly. Building on the approach taken by Thompson et al. (2019), we conceptualize empathy as a process by adding the trait versus state perspective. We propose the following idea: when an empathic reaction is initiated (for example by an external stimulus like a crying family member), the trait empathy measure equals the general ability and functions as a stable multiplicator that determines the magnitude (steepness) of the initial, intuitive bottom-up process of the empathic reaction. In addition to trait empathy, various situational factors influence the intensity of the initial, intuitive bottom-up empathic reaction. For example, Morel et al. (2012) report contextual influences on face perception as early as 60 ms. Considering the short time frame, this influence can be attributed to the bottom-up process. Concerning top-down control mechanisms, different emotion regulation strategies reportedly either increase or decrease state empathy (Jauniaux et al., 2020; Thompson et al., 2019). Furthermore, similar to the bottom-up process, contextual factors can influence the top-down process; these situational factors may lead either to an increase or decrease in the initiated intensity of an empathic reaction. Consequently, the result of this process is the situated expression of trait empathy, measured in most studies investigating empathy (state empathy). We consider this approach to work equally for both cognitive and affective empathy processes. For a visualization, see Figure 2.
Figure 2. Situated framework for trait versus state empathy. The figure is a graphic description of the temporal sequence of the bottom-up process and the top-down control process. Further incorporated is the intensity of the empathic reaction as well as the trait versus state component. The x-axis represents the temporal domain starting with the onset of an empathic reaction. On the y-axis, the intensity of the empathic reaction is displayed. During the first process, the bottom-up process, trait empathy determines the intensity of the empathic reaction as initiated by the different dotted lines. Starting during the bottom-up process and extending to the second process of the empathic reaction, namely the top-down control process, situational factors influence the steepness of the initial empathic reaction and have the potential to both increase and decrease the empathic reaction, hereby indicated through the red arrows and red lines. During the third process, the empathic reaction is displayed, which is considered state empathy as it resulted from the process described above. Created with Biorender.com.
The presented idea implies that there is an inherent difference between measuring the empathic reaction shortly after the stimulus onset or after a longer time. The first measure reflects mainly the bottom-up process consisting of the general trait capacity while the latter represents the outcome of both the bottom-up process and the top-down regulation. Thus, the crucial variable for measuring task-based empathy is the time frame for indicating an individual’s empathic response. In a recent study, a 5 s time pressure was identified as a valid method to induce intuitive thinking in decision-making paradigms (Isler and Yilmaz, 2023). Although response time is usually recorded in empathy studies, it is rarely interpreted, and its impact is generally underestimated. Measurement standards for short-term empathic measures (for example <5 s) and long-term empathic measures (for example >10 s) are needed; for a comparison see Isler and Yilmaz (2023). Both approaches are important for understanding the different aspects of empathy. However, researchers must decide beforehand which aspect they intend to measure and design their experiment accordingly. We believe that the lack of time control contributes to the large variance between results in studies measuring empathy, and that mitigating this shortcoming could be a simple and cost-efficient way to improve empathy research.
The presented theoretical framework and its implications are in line with the following results: in two studies, individuals with self-reported high trait empathy were compared to individuals with low trait empathy as measured with the EQ (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Rameson et al., 2012). Participants were instructed to either look at photos and empathize with the people in the photo, or look at photos while remembering an eight-digit number (passive condition but with a high cognitive load). Remarkably, participants with high levels of trait empathy exhibited heightened expressions of empathy under cognitive load compared to participants characterized by lower trait empathy (Rameson et al., 2012). This suggests that under conditions of high cognitive load, individuals with high-trait empathy report higher intensities of empathic reactions than individuals with low-trait empathy. Considering our approach, the steepness of the level of intensity that reflects trait empathy determines the level of intensity of the state empathy reaction. Although the assumption regarding time control remains untested, its validity could be determined if future studies implement two groups in task-based empathy measures like the MET-core-2. The first group would be forced to respond within 5 s of stimulus onset while the second group could not respond until 10 s after stimulus onset. This would ensure that the empathetic reaction of the first group was primarily based on the bottom-up process while the second group’s response was based on a mixture of the bottom-up and top-down processes. If the responses between groups varied significantly, the results would suggest that the distinction between the two processes is valuable and should be made in future research.
It is critical to note that trait and state empathy may not be entirely independent of one another, contrary to what might be inferred from the model presented above. Determining the relationship between trait and state processes—whether they are interdependent or distinct—requires experimental intervention studies. Crucially, such studies must employ comprehensive measurement tools, as previously emphasized, that assess both trait and state components of empathy.
Finally, it is important to have a basic understanding of the factors that increase or decrease the empathetic response when planning an experiment to reduce the risk of opposing factors falsely attenuating the effects of one another. Thus, the following chapters briefly outline increasing and decreasing factors that have been identified in previous studies.
This chapter examines internal/intraindividual and external situational factors that govern the presence of state empathy and serve as increasing factors.
Motivation can be understood as a cognitive process that initiates purposeful goal-directed behavior (Wasserman and Wasserman, 2020). It is thought to influence the interpretation and appraisal of a given situation, thereby facilitating the emergence of an empathic response (Nitschke and Bartz, 2023; Preston et al., 2020).
However, it is still debated how exactly motivation shapes empathy. The literature differentiates between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation is generally understood as a form of motivation where a person does an activity simply because of the activity itself (Ryan and Deci, 2020); it thus arises in the person and is not based on the environment (Hendijani et al., 2016). Transferring this to empathy means that someone is empathic simply because they find intrinsic pleasure in the empathic action. According to the self-determination theory by Ryan and Deci (2000), autonomy is a critical driving factor increasing intrinsic motivation. Strikingly, this effect has been replicated and established in different contexts (Ryan and Deci, 2020). Empathy has been shown to encourage more helping behavior the more autonomous motivation occurs (Pavey et al., 2012). Motivation is currently regarded as the bridge between empathy and prosocial behavior. However, research is scarce on experimental approaches that connect autonomous motivation and the occurrence of empathy. Future research should aim to investigate the extent to which the self-determination theory parameter also applies to the occurrence of empathy.
Extrinsic motivation comes from external factors such as rewards, recognition, or pressure from others (Hendijani et al., 2016). The expectation of reward (Hendijani et al., 2016) can be considered an extrinsic motivation based on the situation or environment. Only a few studies have addressed the influence of reward on different aspects of empathy. Sims et al. (2012) showed that conditioned reward associated with different faces influenced the level of facial mimicry expressed, with higher reward inducing higher levels of facial mimicry. Facial mimicry is a subcomponent of empathy and consists of the facial mimic expression of emotions, or at least the emotional valence of an empathized emotion, that matches the emotion expressed by the counterpart (Drimalla et al., 2019). Haffey et al. (2013) not only found a similar effect of reward on mimicry, but further showed that trait empathy, measured using the EQ (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004), predicted the level of automatic mimicry. In line with this, the mirror neuron system is reportedly influenced by reward (Trilla Gros et al., 2015). Notably, these studies used social rewards (e.g., faces or hands of human individuals) and mimicry as subcomponents of empathy or as markers of activity in the mirror neuron system. Taken together, it would be valuable to examine whether these effects are restricted to the subcomponents of empathy or apply to the broader concepts of cognitive and affective empathy as well.
Zaki (2014) identified six key motives (three avoidance-related, three approach-related) that modify the empathic outcome: (1) avoiding pain, (2) avoiding costs, (3) avoiding interferences, (4) approaching capitalizations, (5) approaching affiliation, and (6) approaching desirability. Such motives are intertwined with different emotion regulation strategies that allow for coping with a situation. An emotion regulation strategy is a method to modulate one’s emotional state to ensure optimal functioning in an environment and to uphold and improve well-being (Stevens and Taber, 2021). Emotion regulation strategies have recently been proposed as both increasing and decreasing factors for state empathy, depending not only on the specific strategy but also on the valence of the emotion (Jauniaux et al., 2020). Investigating complex emotions using an emotion regulation strategy that up-regulates the intensity of one’s emotional state (e.g., by taking the first-person perspective in a cognitive reappraisal process) led to higher state empathy, compared to a method that down-regulates one’s emotional state. In addition, state empathy was higher for negative social stimuli compared to positive social stimuli (Jauniaux et al., 2020). Weisz and Zaki (2017) emphasize that identifying the core motives driving empathic behavior can aid in designing effective empathy training programs, thereby enhancing the development and efficacy of such interventions. We take their approach a step further by suggesting that incorporating motive assessment during empathy experiments could offer valuable explanations for mixed findings in previous studies. This consideration can contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of empathy-related research outcomes.
Attention is a key concept in psychology, involving selectively focusing on specific aspects over others (Posner and Petersen, 1990). Mindfulness is defined as non-judgmental awareness (Donald et al., 2019) and is considered one method for guiding one’s attention to the given moment and to the thoughts and emotions of an ongoing situation. As such, mindfulness has been explored for its potential to enhance state empathy (Donald et al., 2019). Recent research suggests a connection between mindfulness and empathy although formal mindfulness training has not consistently resulted in increased empathy (Cooper et al., 2020). Authors of one meta-analysis point out that methodological difficulties must be considered when interpreting their finding that meditation can boost empathy, and that future approaches should clarify any inconsistencies before building upon these results (Kreplin et al., 2018). Although prosocial behavior and empathy are distinct, it is notable that Luberto et al. (2018) identified a positive impact of meditation on prosocial behavior. It would be interesting to investigate the extent to which this impact could be expanded to empathy.
Emotions are central in social interactions and are one of the most prominent topics in psychological research (Tamir et al., 2016). Emotions encompass physiological components, appraisals, expressions, and behaviors that shape an individual’s relationship with their environment (Tamir et al., 2016). Understanding how emotions influence state empathy is challenging due to the versatility of the expression of emotions and their subjective perception. One affective state discussed in the context of empathy is compassion. Personal distress, a potentially decreasing factor of empathic reaction (to be discussed later in the manuscript) (Kim and Han, 2018), may be counteracted by compassion. Whereas empathy is conceptualized as a self-directed emotion, compassion is considered an other-related emotion and leads to positive feelings such as love (Lantos et al., 2023). Interestingly, the potential for compassion training to increase positive affect (Klimecki et al., 2014), and in turn state empathy, has recently been acknowledged. More precisely, compassion training led to a decrease in activity in the respective neural regions connected to empathy for pain (Klimecki et al., 2014). In addition, a recent study used loving-kindness meditation training as a form of compassion-based training to increase self-report empathy measures, namely the JSPE (Hojat et al., 2001). As part of a broader approach used to assess the effect of emotional states on empathy, Trilla Gros et al. (2021) showed that an egocentric bias exists when perceiving ambiguous faces; if participants were happy, they were more likely to identify a facial expression as happy. This mood-congruency bias in emotion perception may be an important variable when designing empathy studies and calls for further examination.
Acute stress has been shown to modify information-processing steps, higher cognitive functions, and empathy (Hermans et al., 2014; Nitschke and Bartz, 2023; Shields et al., 2016). Stress, defined as a response occurring when external demands surpass one’s resources, triggers changes in affective, neural, cardiovascular, and hormonal processes (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). A recent review by Nitschke and Bartz (2023) discusses the influence of acute stress on empathy. The authors report that evidence on enhanced empathy in the context of stress has been gathered in healthy samples and is debated under the term “tend-and-befriend.” The term describes increased prosocial activities due to stress that may also extend to empathic behavior (Taylor, 2006). Increased empathy in the aftermath of acute stress was also reported by Gonzalez-Liencres et al. (2016); participants undergoing the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; Kirschbaum et al., 1993) evaluated pain experienced by a third person as more unpleasant compared to control participants without pre-experience of stress. In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, Tomova et al. (2017), extended these findings by showing increased activity in brain structures associated with automatic empathy for others’ pain (e.g., the anterior insula, the anterior midcingulate cortex, the primary somatosensory cortex) after exposing male participants to a common fMRT stress-induction paradigm. It is important to note that both studies only targeted empathy for pain (Gonzalez-Liencres et al., 2016; Tomova et al., 2017). Future studies should address empathy for complex (positive and negative) emotions under stress as well.
As mentioned, acute stress is known to impair higher cognitive functions and adaptive behavior, compelling individuals to allocate their cognitive resources toward coping with the stressor (Hermans et al., 2014; Shields et al., 2016). This rationale leads to the expectation that acute stress reduces cognitive empathy (Nitschke and Bartz, 2023). Interestingly, empirical findings paint a mixed picture. Studies investigating simple emotion recognition as a key component of cognitive empathy endorse the beneficial effects of acute stress on cognitive empathy (Domes and Zimmer, 2019), though this may be restricted to positive emotions (von Dawans et al., 2020) or emotions expected to be more salient under stress such as disgust and surprise (Daudelin-Peltier et al., 2017). In contrast, authors such as Smeets et al. (2009), Wingenfeld et al. (2018), and Graumann et al. (2021) report null findings, while Wolf et al. (2015) found no effect of acute stress on cognitive empathy but a stress-induced enhancement of affective empathy. These inconsistencies between studies may arise due to the varying complexity and ecological validity of the different tasks used to assess emotion recognition (Nitschke and Bartz, 2023).
To add even more complexity, sex-specific effects have been reported, with female participants showing impaired or unaffected empathy under rising cortisol levels while male participants seemed to benefit from higher cortisol reactivity (Nitschke et al., 2022; Smeets et al., 2009). Because men generally exhibit a higher cortisol response to stress, direct comparisons are challenging (Nitschke and Bartz, 2023). Speculating on how cortisol might affect empathy on a mechanistic level, Nitschke and Bartz (2023) provide a framework for interpreting contradictory results. The authors suggest that cortisol may specifically target brain areas responsible for a meaningful self-other distinction. As such, enhanced empathy under stress may result from a failure to distinguish how far the perceived affect concerns one’s own emotional state, or the emotional state of another individual. The authors highlight a need for further research to identify additional mediators of the effects of acute stress on empathy beyond cortisol and other stress markers.
Taken together, the evidence shows that state empathy is context-dependent and amenable to various facilitative factors. These factors should be considered when devising empathy measurement tools and designing empathy intervention programs. This is crucial when tailoring interventions for specific groups such as individuals with autism spectrum disorder who may exhibit lower levels of trait empathy. Investigating the impact of these factors, whether individually or in combination, on improving the capacity to express state empathy is a promising avenue for future research and practice.
Many of the aforementioned factors have the potential not only to heighten state empathy but also to diminish it. Consequently, in the following section, we summarize experimental conditions employed to reduce state empathy. Results provide valuable insights into the dynamics of state empathy and contribute to the development of more effective empathy measurement tools and interventions.
Attention, or more precisely, the focus of attention, is one of the most prominent factors affecting state empathy. Gu and Han (2007) showed that attentional focus influences the activity of the neural network involved in empathy for pain. When participants focused on rating the painful experience of a person in a picture, the neural network related to empathy was active. If, however, participants were asked to count a specific aspect of the photos they saw (e.g., to count the number of identical hands), their attention shifted away from the emotional response, and the activity of the neural affective empathy network was decreased. Similarly, Fan and Han (2008) supported the assumption that attention has a moderating effect on the occurrence of empathy for pain. Specifically, redirecting cognitive resources away from someone else’s emotional signals can affect the initial perceptual aspect of empathy, leading to a diminished emotional reaction in the observer (Fan and Han, 2008). To the best of our knowledge, no study has adopted a similar approach to Gu and Han (2007) to investigate the influence of attentional focus on empathy in the context of more complex emotions.
Thus far, personal distress is one of the few emotional states investigated in terms of its direct influence on empathy (Kim and Han, 2018). Personal distress is defined as the tendency to experience negative feelings and discomfort when faced with the suffering of others. It is an emotional response that arises from empathy (Kim and Han, 2018) and results in the tendency to withdraw oneself from a stressor (Batson et al., 2009). As Preston et al. (2020) outline, impaired affective empathy may result from high levels of personal distress rather than a psychopathological deficit. Moreover, personal distress may occur if the self-regulation process after experiencing a shared affect is unsuccessful (Stevens and Taber, 2021). In this sense, experiencing personal distress leads to experiencing stress (Batson et al., 2009) and subsequent physical arousal. Deuter et al. (2018) recently investigated the influence of physiological arousal on affective empathy and found a negative relationship between arousal and self-reported affective empathy. They concluded that physiological arousal may diminish empathy (Deuter et al., 2018). Future research should investigate this connection to better understand the influence of physical arousal on empathy, particularly in experimental settings.
It has been suggested that facial mimicry helps to better understand the perception of the emotional state of another person for basic and complex emotions (Drimalla et al., 2019). Blocking facial mimicry (e.g., by biting a pen or chewing gum) has been reported to decrease empathic processes (Stel and van Knippenberg, 2008). Several studies report that emotion recognition, an important component of empathy, is slower when facial mimicry is inhibited (Niedenthal, 2007; Stel and van Knippenberg, 2008) for a more comprehensive discussion see Hess and Fischer (2013). Thus, facial mimicry can be considered an embodied mediating factor and an example of situated emotional influence on state empathy.
Similar to other factors, acute stress may lead to a decrease, as well as an increase in state empathy. Because stress represents a state characterized by a reallocation of cognitive resources to stimuli other than the stressor, one might assume that available cognitive resources are predominantly needed to cope with the stressor in question. It is conceivable that under stress, available cognitive resources are invested in regulating one’s own emotional state rather than in showing empathy for the emotions of others. This is evident in Buruck et al. (2014) who found reduced empathy for pain in participants having undergone a TSST. However, this relation was moderated by participants’ emotion regulation capacities. Participants with stronger emotion regulation skills showed even higher deficits in empathic sharing. Initially, this may seem counterintuitive. One might assume that those skilled in emotion regulation require fewer cognitive resources for handling their own emotions, leaving resources available for empathizing with others. However, this does not guarantee a willingness to share others’ emotions under stress. Empathizing might amplify arousal and emotion regulation costs. Additionally, and as mentioned above, Smeets et al. (2009), Wingenfeld et al. (2018), and Graumann et al. (2021) report null findings, suggesting that acute stress may not consistently increase or decrease state empathy and that other factors play a role as well.
Up to this point, we have discussed internal factors operating within the observer and thus shaping the expression of state empathy. Several external variables moderate the extent to which a person exhibits empathy in a given moment. For instance, factors such as the emotional valence of the stimulus (Drimalla et al., 2019; Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021a; Wolf et al., 2015), the observer’s relationship with the target including in-group and out-group biases (Cikara et al., 2014), and/or sex of the target (e.g., interaction with sex-hormone status of the female observer for cognitive empathy and affective empathy) may increase or decrease state empathy (Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021a; Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021b). Moreover, the ethnicity of the target (Xu et al., 2009) can modulate empathy depending on the valence of the context (Neumann et al., 2013). While a detailed discussion of these factors exceeds the scope of this review, they warrant consideration as potential covariates in future empirical studies.
It becomes apparent that while the distinction between increasing and decreasing factors is helpful for systemization, precise analyses are necessary to account for factors that can have both effects depending on nuanced individual and/or contextual differences. To account for this, all factors are summarized in Table 2.
Considering the complex nature of empathy and the multitude and versatility of factors influencing empathic responses described in the previous chapters, one can easily conclude that measuring empathy poses challenges. To mitigate them, this review developed simple and cost/time-efficient ways to enhance the validity of empathy measurements: (1) Measurement choice: as outlined in Chapter 2, studies investigating empathy frequently use empathy measurements without considering the inherent differences of the accessed concepts. The choice diagram depicted in Figure 1 provides a tool that can be used to decide what measurement should be used in a study to optimally quantify the specific aspect of empathy under investigation. (2) Time control: as outlined in Chapter 3, the time frame within which participants must indicate their empathic response can cause significant differences in results. To overcome this limitation, we recommend a simplified distinction between bottom-up and top-down processes in measuring empathy. To assess a bottom-up process, studies should integrate a forced response time for the empathic response, triggering a fast, intuitive reaction. To assess the general empathic response (consisting of bottom-up and top-down processes) studies should implement a time period during which participants cannot respond, ensuring that the top-down process has time to occur. (3) Confounding factors: as outlined in Chapter 4, the measurement of empathy is sensitive to several factors that can increase or decrease the empathic response. Since there is a risk that factors act in opposition to each other (meaning that factors that both increase and decrease empathic responses are present) causing null effects, it is important to control for the confounding effects at least to some degree. To facilitate the selection of appropriate study designs and measurements, Table 2 provides researchers with an overview of how different factors can influence empathy. We believe that the consideration of the three aforementioned elements (measurement choice, time control, and confounding factors) will enhance the validity and generalizability of empathy research results.
One interesting approach that more explicitly integrates biopsychological indicators would be to establish standardized test batteries that allow for the measurement of both cognitive and affective empathy through self-report (for trait empathy), performance-measures (for state empathy), and CNS-correlates (such as fMRI- or EEG-measures). One such battery is the so-called EmpaToM task (Kanske et al., 2015) for use in fMRI. The EmpaToM, which assesses cognitive and affective empathy and compassion, was shown to discriminate between the three corresponding types of neural pathways crucial for understanding others within the same functional-imaging task (initially developed in German; Kanske et al., 2015). Concretely, the neuronal correlates of affective empathy, ToM (theory of mind, i.e., cognitive empathy), and compassion are assessed while using dynamic video sequences of neutral and negative valence (related to suffering). The EmpaToM is now also available in English (Lantos et al., 2023). We would add that it is important to extend its scope by examining complex emotions of both positive and negative (and neutral) valence rather than focusing only on negative emotions (and neutral controls).
This review aimed to outline the benefits of differentiating between trait and state empathy on a theoretical and methodological level. In addition, we challenged current methodological approaches for measuring empathy. We elaborated on the theoretical aspects of trait and state empathy and discussed both increasing and decreasing factors of state empathy. Finally, we highlighted three factors that should be taken into consideration when designing future empathy studies.
We are aware that the current review comes with limitations. It is noteworthy that more aspects than those listed in this review shape empathy. For example, Weisz and Zaki (2017) summarize that expectations of the emphasizer due to their gender play a pivotal role, but only when the gender-related expectation is made conscious. In addition, and as stated above, other studies report that the valence of emotions interacts with the female’s menstrual cycle stage and therefore specifically affects empathy (Gamsakhurdashvili et al., 2021b; Wolf et al., 2015). While this paper’s selection of individual factors is literature-driven, it is not conceptualized as systematic due to the lack of previous approaches taken to address the topic. Despite these limitations, the review opens the possibility for future researchers to assess the distinction between trait and state empathy. Future research should aim to clarify inconsistencies in methodological approaches used to measure empathy.
In conclusion, we believe that our approach is a valuable addition to the theoretical development of the construct. Empathy can be understood not only as a trait but a state. We call for researchers to consider that (both cognitive and affective) empathy is the result of both, bottom-up processes and top-down control mechanisms that are influenced by increasing and decreasing situational factors. Lastly, we highlight three efficient steps for improving existing trait and state empathy measures. Namely, researchers should choose the appropriate measurement tool, implement a time control during performance tasks, and control for confounding factors. Through this, we hope to increase the validity and generalizability of results in empathy research.
KH: Conceptualization, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RS: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. LSP: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. SO: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. OG: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. US: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. KH was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – project number GRK-2185/2 (DFG Research Training Group Situated Cognition)/Gefoerdert durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG)—Projektnummer GRK-2185/2 (DFG-Graduiertenkolleg Situated Cognition). The contribution of LSP was supported by the DFG within project B4 of the Collaborative Research Center (SFB) 874 “Integration and Representation of Sensory Processes” [project number 122679504].
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1525517/full#supplementary-material
Abramson, L., Uzefovsky, F., Toccaceli, V., and Knafo-Noam, A. (2020). The genetic and environmental origins of emotional and cognitive empathy: review and meta-analyses of twin studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 114, 113–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.023
Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022607.19833.00
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., and Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1962). Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in therapeutic change. Psychol. Monogr. Gen. Appl. 76, 1–36. doi: 10.1037/h0093918
Batson, C. D., Decety, J., and Ickes, W. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Soc. Neurosci. 3–15. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262012973.001.0001
Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., and Schoenrade, P. (1987). Distress and empathy: two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. J. Pers. 55, 19–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00426.x
Beckett, R., and Fisher, D. (1994). Community-based treatment for sex offenders: An evaluation of even treatment programmes.
Bramham, J., Morris, R. G., Hornak, J., Bullock, P., and Polkey, C. E. (2009). Social and emotional functioning following bilateral and unilateral neurosurgical prefrontal cortex lesions. Journal of Neuropsychology 3, 125–143. doi: 10.1348/174866408X293994
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., and Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educ. Res. 18, 32–42. doi: 10.3102/0013189X018001032
Buruck, G., Wendsche, J., Melzer, M., Strobel, A., and Dörfel, D. (2014). Acute psychosocial stress and emotion regulation skills modulate empathic reactions to pain in others. Front. Psychol. 5:517. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00517
Carkhuff, R. R. (1969). The prediction of the effects of teacher- counselor education: The development of communication and discrimination selection indexes. Counselor Education and Supervision 8, 265–272. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6978.1969.tb01340.x
Cikara, M., Bruneau, E., van Bavel, J. J., and Saxe, R. (2014). Their pain gives us pleasure: how intergroup dynamics shape empathic failures and counter-empathic responses. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 55, 110–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.007
Cooper, D., Yap, K., O’Brien, M., and Scott, I. (2020). Mindfulness and empathy among counseling and psychotherapy professionals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mindfulness 11, 2243–2257. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01425-3
Cuff, B. M., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., and Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: a review of the concept. Emot. Rev. 8, 144–153. doi: 10.1177/1754073914558466
Daudelin-Peltier, C., Forget, H., Blais, C., Deschênes, A., and Fiset, D. (2017). The effect of acute social stress on the recognition of facial expression of emotions. Sci. Rep. 7:1036. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01053-3
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
Deitz, S. R., and Byrnes, L. E. (1982). Attribution of responsibility for sexual assault: The influence of observer empathy and defendant occupation and attractiveness. The Journal of Psychology 108, 17–29. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1981.9915241
de Vignemont, F., and Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.008
de Waal, F. B. M., and Preston, S. D. (2017). Mammalian empathy: Behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 498–509. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.72
Deuter, C. E., Nowacki, J., Wingenfeld, K., Kuehl, L. K., Finke, J. B., Dziobek, I., et al. (2018). The role of physiological arousal for self-reported emotional empathy. Autonomic Neurosci. 214, 9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2018.07.002
Doherty, R. W. (1997). The emotional contagion scale: A measure of individual differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 21, 131–154. doi: 10.1023/A:1024956003661
Domes, G., and Zimmer, P. (2019). Acute stress enhances the sensitivity for facial emotions: a signal detection approach. Stress 22, 455–460. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2019.1593366
Donald, J. N., Sahdra, B. K., van Zanden, B., Duineveld, J. J., Atkins, P. W. B., Marshall, S. L., et al. (2019). Does your mindfulness benefit others? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the link between mindfulness and prosocial behaviour. Br. J. Psychol. 110, 101–125. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12338
Dorris, L., Young, D., Barlow, J., Byrne, K., and Hoyle, R. (2022). Cognitive empathy across the lifespan. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. 64, 1524–1531. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.15263
Drimalla, H., Landwehr, N., Hess, U., and Dziobek, I. (2019). From face to face: the contribution of facial mimicry to cognitive and emotional empathy. Cognit. Emot. 33, 1672–1686. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1596068
Dziobek, I., Preissler, S., Grozdanovic, Z., Heuser, I., Heekeren, H. R., and Roepke, S. (2011). Neuronal correlates of altered empathy and social cognition in borderline personality disorder. NeuroImage 57, 539–548. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.005
Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., et al. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 464–473. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x
Fan, Y., and Han, S [Shihui] (2008). Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: an event-related brain potential study. Neuropsychologia, 46, 160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.023
Fridenson-Hayo, S., Berggren, S., Lassalle, A., Tal, S., Pigat, D., Bölte, S., et al. (2016). Basic and complex emotion recognition in children with autism: cross-cultural findings. Molecular Autism. 7, 52–63. doi: 10.1186/s13229-016-0113-9
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., and Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives not taken. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1068–1074. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01824.x
Gamsakhurdashvili, D., Antov, M. I., and Stockhorst, U. (2021a). Facial emotion recognition and emotional memory from the ovarian-hormone perspective: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 12:641250. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.641250
Gamsakhurdashvili, D., Antov, M. I., and Stockhorst, U. (2021b). Sex-hormone status and emotional processing in healthy women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 130:105258. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105258
Gonzalez-Liencres, C., Breidenstein, A., Wolf, O. T., and Brüne, M. (2016). Sex-dependent effects of stress on brain correlates to empathy for pain. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 105, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.04.011
Graumann, L., Duesenberg, M., Metz, S., Schulze, L., Wolf, O. T., Roepke, S., et al. (2021). Facial emotion recognition in borderline patients is unaffected by acute psychosocial stress. J. Psychiatr. Res. 132, 131–135. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.10.007
Gu, X., and Han, S. (2007). Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain. NeuroImage 36, 256–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.025
Haffey, A., Press, C., O'Connell, G., and Chakrabarti, B. (2013). Autistic traits modulate mimicry of social but not nonsocial rewards. Autism Res. 6, 614–620. doi: 10.1002/aur.1323
Håkansson Eklund, J., Andersson-Stråberg, T., and Hansen, E. M. (2009). “I've also experienced loss and fear”: effects of prior similar experience on empathy. Scand. J. Psychol. 50, 65–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00673.x
Håkansson Eklund, J., and Summer Meranius, M. (2021). Toward a consensus on the nature of empathy: a review of reviews. Patient Educ. Couns. 104, 300–307. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2020.08.022
Hall, J. A., and Schwartz, R. (2019). Empathy present and future. J. Soc. Psychol. 159, 225–243. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2018.1477442
Hendijani, R., Bischak, D. P., Arvai, J., and Dugar, S. (2016). Intrinsic motivation, external reward, and their effect on overall motivation and performance. Hum. Perform. 29, 251–274. doi: 10.1080/08959285.2016.1157595
Hermans, E. J., Henckens, M. J. A. G., Joëls, M., and Fernández, G. (2014). Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends Neurosci. 37, 304–314. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.03.006
Hess, U., and Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 17, 142–157. doi: 10.1177/1088868312472607
Hogan, R. (1969). Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 33, 307–316.
Hojat, M., Mangione, S., Nasca, T. J., Cohen, M. J. M., Gonnella, J. S., Erdmann, J. B., et al. (2001). The Jefferson scale of physician empathy: development and preliminary psychometric data. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 61, 349–365. doi: 10.1177/00131640121971158
Isler, O., and Yilmaz, O. (2023). How to activate intuitive and reflective thinking in behavior research? A comprehensive examination of experimental techniques. Behav. Res. Methods 55, 3679–3698. doi: 10.3758/s13428-022-01984-4
Jauniaux, J., Tessier, M.-H., Regueiro, S., Chouchou, F., Fortin-Côté, A., and Jackson, P. L. (2020). Reappraisal of others’ positive and negative emotions is related to distinctive patterns of cardiac autonomic regulation and situational empathy. PloS one. 15: e0244427. doi: 10.5683/SP2/65Z0CO
Jarvis, A. L., Wong, S., Weightman, M., Ghezzi, E. S., Sharman, R. L., and Keage, H. A. (2024). Emotional empathy across adulthood: A meta-analytic review. Psychology and Aging 39:126. doi: 10.1037/pag0000788
Jolliffe, D., and Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of the basic empathy scale. J. Adolesc. 29, 589–611. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010
Kanske, P., Böckler, A., Trautwein, F.-M., and Singer, T. (2015). Dissecting the social brain: introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain-behavior relations for empathy and theory of mind. NeuroImage 122, 6–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.082
Kim, H., and Han, S [Sumi] (2018). Does personal distress enhance empathic interaction or block it? Personal. Individ. Differ., 124, 77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.12.005
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., and Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The 'Trier social stress Test'--a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M., and Singer, T. (2014). Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 873–879. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst060
Kreplin, U., Farias, M., and Brazil, I. A. (2018). The limited prosocial effects of meditation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci. Rep. 8:2403. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20299-z
Lamm, C., Nusbaum, H. C., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2007). What are you feeling? Using functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess the modulation of sensory and affective responses during empathy for pain. PLoS One 2:e1292. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001292
La Monica, E. L. (1981). Construct validity of an empathy instrument. Research in Nursing & Health 4, 389–400. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770040406
Lantos, D., Costa, C., Briglia, M., Molenberghs, P., Kanske, P., and Singer, T. (2023). Introducing the english EmpaToM task: a tool to assess empathy, compassion, and theory of mind in fMRI studies. NeuroImage 3:100180. doi: 10.1016/j.ynirp.2023.100180
Layton, J. M. (1979). The use of modeling to teach empathy to nursing students. Research in Nursing & Health 2, 163–176.
Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company.
Lim, D., and DeSteno, D. (2016). Suffering and compassion: the links among adverse life experiences, empathy, compassion, and prosocial behavior. Emotion 16, 175–182. doi: 10.1037/emo0000144
Lindeman, M., Koirikivi, I., and Lipsanen, J. (2016). Pictorial empathy test. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 34, 421–431. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000353
Lishner, D. A., Batson, C. D., and Huss, E. (2011). Tenderness and sympathy: distinct empathic emotions elicited by different forms of need. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 614–625. doi: 10.1177/0146167211403157
Luberto, C. M., Shinday, N., Song, R., Philpotts, L. L., Park, E. R., Fricchione, G. L., et al. (2018). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of meditation on empathy, compassion, and prosocial behaviors. Mindfulness 9, 708–724. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0841-8
Mehrabian, A. (1996). Manual for the balanced emotional empathy scale (BEES): Unpublished Manuscript. Available from Albert Mehrabian 1130.
Mehrabian, A. (1997). Relations among personality scales of aggression, violence, and empathy: Validational evidence bearing on the risk of eruptive violence scale. Aggressive Behavior 23, 433–445.
Mehrabian, A., and Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 525–543.
Mercer, S. W., Maxwell, M., Heaney, D., and Watt, G. C. (2004). The consultation and relational empathy (CARE) measure: Development and preliminary validation and reliability of an empathy-based consultation process measure. Family Practice 21, 699–705. doi: 10.1093/fampra/cmh621
Morel, S., Beaucousin, V., Perrin, M., and George, N. (2012). Very early modulation of brain responses to neutral faces by a single prior association with an emotional context: evidence from MEG. NeuroImage 61, 1461–1470. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.016
Neumann, D. L., Boyle, G. J., and Chan, R. C. K. (2013). Empathy towards individuals of the same and different ethnicity when depicted in negative and positive contexts. Personal. Individ. Differ. 55, 8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.01.022
Newen, A., de Bruin, L., and Gallagher, S. (2018). The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Nezlek, J. B., Feist, G. J., Wilson, F. C., and Plesko, R. M. (2001). Day-to-day variability in empathy as a function of daily events and mood. J. Res. Pers. 35, 401–423. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.2001.2332
Nitschke, J. P., and Bartz, J. A. (2023). The association between acute stress and empathy: a systematic literature review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 144:105003. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.105003
Nitschke, J. P., Pruessner, J. C., and Bartz, J. A. (2022). Stress and stress-induced glucocorticoids facilitate empathic accuracy in men but have no effects for women. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1783–1794. doi: 10.1177/09567976221101315
Pavey, L., Greitemeyer, T., and Sparks, P. (2012). "I help because I want to, not because you tell me to": empathy increases autonomously motivated helping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 681–689. doi: 10.1177/0146167211435940
Pfattheicher, S., Nielsen, Y. A., and Thielmann, I. (2022). Prosocial behavior and altruism: a review of concepts and definitions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 44, 124–129. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.08.021
Pithers, W. D. (1999). Empathy: definition, enhancement, and relevance to the treatment of sexual abusers. J. Interpers. Violence 14, 257–284. doi: 10.1177/088626099014003004
Posner, M. I., and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
Preston, S. D., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 1–20. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x02000018
Preston, S. D., Ermler, M., Lei, Y., and Bickel, L. (2020). Understanding empathy and its disorders through a focus on the neural mechanism. Cortex 127, 347–370. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.03.001
Rameson, L. T., Morelli, S. A., and Lieberman, M. D. (2012). The neural correlates of empathy: experience, automaticity, and prosocial behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 235–245. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00130
Reniers, R. L. E. P., Corcoran, R., Drake, R., Shryane, N. M., and Völlm, B. A. (2011). The QCAE: a questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. J. Pers. Assess. 93, 84–95. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.528484
Reynolds, W. J. (2000). The measurement and development of empathy in nursing. Ashgate, Aldershot. doi: 10.4324/9781315192499
Roberts, B. W., and DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 126, 3–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3
Rudolph, U., Roesch, S., Greitemeyer, T., and Weiner, B. (2004). A meta-analytic review of help giving and aggression from an attributional perspective: contributions to a general theory of motivation. Cognit. Emot. 18, 815–848. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000248
Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.55.1.68
Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2020). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 61:101860. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., et al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences 25, 167–177. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 132, 617–627. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn279
Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., and Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: a meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 651–668. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.038
Sims, T. B., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., and Chakrabarti, B. (2012). How reward modulates mimicry: Emg evidence of greater facial mimicry of more rewarding happy faces. Psychophysiology 49, 998–1004. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01377.x
Singer, T., and Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 81–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04418.x
Smeets, T., Dziobek, I., and Wolf, O. T. (2009). Social cognition under stress: differential effects of stress-induced cortisol elevations in healthy young men and women. Horm. Behav. 55, 507–513. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.01.011
Spielberger, C. D., Gonzalez-Reigosa, F., Martinez-Urrutia, A., Natalicio, L. F., and Natalicio, D. S. (1971). The state-trait anxiety inventory. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia/Interamerican. J. Psychol. 5, 145–158.
Spreng, R. N., McKinnon, M. C., Mar, R. A., and Levine, B. (2009). The Toronto empathy questionnaire: Scale development and initial validation of a factor-analytic solution to multiple empathy measures. Journal of Personality Assessment 91, 62–71. doi: 10.1080/00223890802484381
Stel, M., and van Knippenberg, A. (2008). The role of facial mimicry in the recognJtion of affect. Psychol. Sci. 19, 984–985. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02188.x
Stevens, F., and Taber, K. (2021). The neuroscience of empathy and compassion in pro-social behavior. Neuropsychologia 159:107925. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107925
Strauss, C., Lever Taylor, B., Gu, J., Kuyken, W., Baer, R., Jones, F., et al. (2016). What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 47, 15–27. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.004
Tamir, M. (2016). Why do people regulate their emotions? A taxonomy of motives in emotion regulation. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 20, 199–222. doi: 10.1177/1088868315586325
Tamir, M., Schwartz, S. H., Cieciuch, J., Riediger, M., Torres, C., Scollon, C., et al. (2016). Desired emotions across cultures: a value-based account. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 111, 67–82. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000072
Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 273–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.x
Thompson, N. M., Uusberg, A., Gross, J. J., and Chakrabarti, B. (2019). Empathy and emotion regulation: an integrative account. Prog. Brain Res. 247, 273–304. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.03.024
Timmers, I., Park, A. L., Fischer, M. D., Kronman, C. A., Heathcote, L. C., Hernandez, J. M., et al. (2018). Is empathy for pain unique in its neural correlates? A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of empathy. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:289. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00289
Tomova, L., Majdandžic, J., Hummer, A., Windischberger, C., Heinrichs, M., and Lamm, C. (2017). Increased neural responses to empathy for pain might explain how acute stress increases prosociality. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 401–408. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw146
Trilla Gros, I., Panasiti, M. S., and Chakrabarti, B. (2015). The plasticity of the mirror system: how reward learning modulates cortical motor simulation of others. Neuropsychologia 70, 255–262. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.033
Trilla Gros, I., Weigand, A., and Dziobek, I. (2021). Affective states influence emotion perception: evidence for emotional egocentricity. Psychol. Res. 85, 1005–1015. doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01314-3
van der Graaff, J., Meeus, W., van Boxtel, A., van Lier, P. A. C., Koot, H. M., and Branje, S. (2016). Motor, affective and cognitive empathy in adolescence: interrelations between facial electromyography and self-reported trait and state measures. Cognit. Emot. 30, 745–761. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1027665
Vieten, C., Rubanovich, C. K., Khatib, L., Sprengel, M., Tanega, C., Polizzi, C., et al. (2024). Measures of empathy and compassion: a scoping review. PLoS One 19:e0297099. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0297099
von Dawans, B., Spenthof, I., Zimmer, P., and Domes, G. (2020). Acute psychosocial stress modulates the detection sensitivity for facial emotions. Exp. Psychol. 67, 140–149. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000473
von Dawans, B., Strojny, J., and Domes, G. (2021). The effects of acute stress and stress hormones on social cognition and behavior: current state of research and future directions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 121, 75–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.11.026
Wang, Y.-W., Davidson, M. M., Yakushko, O. F., Savoy, H. B., Tan, J. A., and Bleier, J. K. (2003). The scale of ethnocultural empathy: Development, validation, and reliability. Journal of Counseling Psychology 50, 221–234. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.50.2.221
Wasserman, T., and Wasserman, L. (2020). Motivation, effort, and the neural network model. Heidelberg: Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-58724-6
Weisz, E., Ong, D. C., Carlson, R. W., and Zaki, J. (2021). Building empathy through motivation-based interventions. Emotion 21, 990–999. doi: 10.1037/emo0000929
Weisz, E., and Zaki, J. (2017). “Empathy building interventions: a review of existing work and suggestions for future directions” in The Oxford handbook of compassion science. eds. E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, L. Cameron, and J. R. Doty (New York: Oxford University Press), 399–420.
Wheeler, K. (1990). “Perception of empathy inventory,’’ in measurement of nursing outcomes: Measuring client self-care and coding skills of nursing outcomes. eds. O. Srickland and C. Waltz, vol. 4 (New York: Springer), 81–198.
Wingenfeld, K., Duesenberg, M., Fleischer, J., Roepke, S., Dziobek, I., Otte, C., et al. (2018). Psychosocial stress differentially affects emotional empathy in women with borderline personality disorder and healthy controls. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 137, 206–215. doi: 10.1111/acps.12856
Wolf, O. T., Schulte, J. M., Drimalla, H., Hamacher-Dang, T. C., Knoch, D., and Dziobek, I. (2015). Enhanced emotional empathy after psychosocial stress in young healthy men. Stress 18, 631–637. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2015.1078787
Xu, X., Zuo, X., Wang, X., and Han, S. (2009). Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. J. Neurosci. 29, 8525–8529. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2418-09.2009
Yu, J., and Kirk, M. (2009). Evaluation of empathy measurement tools in nursing: systematic review. J. Adv. Nurs. 65, 1790–1806. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2009.05071.x
Zhao, D., Ding, R., Zhang, H., Zhang, N., Hu, L., and Luo, W. (2022). Individualized prediction of females' empathic concern from intrinsic brain activity within general network of state empathy. Cognit. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 22, 403–413. doi: 10.3758/s13415-021-00964-z
Keywords: empathy, increasing and decreasing factors, bottom-up process vs. top-down control mechanisms, trait, state
Citation: Heyers K, Schrödter R, Pfeifer LS, Ocklenburg S, Güntürkün O and Stockhorst U (2025) (State) empathy: how context matters. Front. Psychol. 16:1525517. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1525517
Received: 09 November 2024; Accepted: 20 January 2025;
Published: 07 February 2025.
Edited by:
Takashi Tsukiura, Kyoto University, JapanReviewed by:
Yuri Terasawa, Keio University, JapanCopyright © 2025 Heyers, Schrödter, Pfeifer, Ocklenburg, Güntürkün and Stockhorst. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Katrin Heyers, a2F0cmluLmhleWVyc0BydWIuZGU=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.