Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol. , 18 February 2025

Sec. Sport Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1517718

This article is part of the Research Topic Determinants of Achievement in Top Sport View all 16 articles

Mental imagery and stress: the mediating role of self-efficacy in competitive martial arts athletes

  • 1Department of Sport Sciences, Kore University, Enna, Italy
  • 2Department of Biomedical and Biotechnological Sciences, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
  • 3Department of Psychology, Kore University, Enna, Italy
  • 4Department of Social and Humanistic Sciences in Sport, Faculty of Sport, University of Ljubljana, Ljubljana, Slovenia

Introduction: Martial arts are diverse systems of combat practices, each with its unique techniques, philosophies, and histories. Mental imagery is a multisensory experience that integrates various senses to create vivid mental representations, evoking the physical attributes of people, places, or objects not currently within our perception. In sports contexts, studies have indicated that martial arts often use imagery to enhance performance.

Methods: This study aimed to investigate the mediating role of self-efficacy in the relationship between static (visualizing still scenes or concepts) and dynamic (focusing on motion and action) vividness, controllability (manipulating the imagery experience), and the degree of stress, in a group of 110 martial arts athletes (61 males and 49 females), aged between 21 and 23 years old (M = 22.47, SD = 0.75). Participants were competitive athletes of karate, taekwondo and judo, having a minimum of 13 years of training skill in the sport. They completed the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire, the Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire-2, the Mental Image Transformation Task, The General Self-Efficacy Scale, and Measuring Psychological Stress Test.

Results: A 2 × 3 (gender × sport type) between groups MANOVA showed significant differences in imagery dimensions by sport type. The results of the mediation analysis showed that self-efficacy plays a mediating role between imagery and stress.

Discussion: The study advanced exploration in the field of mental imagery training, providing additional evidence for its importance in improving performance and managing stress in athletes.

Introduction

Martial arts encompass a variety of combat practices, and can be broadly categorized into traditional styles (like Karate, Taekwondo, and Judo), modern self-defense systems (like Krav Maga), and mixed martial arts. Each style has its own techniques, philosophies, and training methods. However, they share several general characteristics that define their essence and purpose. At their core, martial arts emphasize discipline, respect, physical fitness, and self-defense (Gavagan and Sayers, 2017; Barnamehei et al., 2020). Practitioners not only learn physical skills but also cultivate mental strength, confidence, skill of visualization, and adaptability to stressors. Specifically, they often use visualization techniques to complement their physical training, creating a powerful synergy between the mind and body, enhancing performance, and skill acquisition (Lindsay et al., 2023). Mental imagery is a multisensory experience that integrates various senses to create vivid mental representations, evoking the physical attributes of people, places, or objects not currently within our perception. It is crucial for executing movements and plays a significant role in overall human functioning (Commodari et al., 2024). In sports contexts, imagery is described as a state where individuals visualize themselves, third or first person, performing skills necessary for upcoming tasks or enhancing their performance (Cumming et al., 2023). This imagery can arise from both conscious and unconscious recall processes, allowing a person to “see” an image or “experience” a movement in their mind without actually experiencing it in reality. Imagery is crucial in this context, as it enhances performance in motor tasks. By mentally rehearsing movements, athletes can refine their skills, boost confidence, and improve overall execution (Simonsmeier et al., 2021). In mental imagery, dynamic (the body’s perceptual involvement during movement and action) and static (the visual fixed aspects of what a person sees) are the most commonly used sensory methods for creating mental images (Moran and O’Shea, 2020). These approaches help individuals enhance their performance by integrating bodily sensations and kinaesthetic actions with visual representations (Guillot et al., 2024). Imagery is indeed a complex construct. Several studies have identified a link between high athletic performance and proficiency in mental rotation. They also highlight a strong connection between motor practice and imagery ability, which is connected to mental rotation processes (Voyer and Jansen, 2017; Geisen et al., 2024; Di Corrado et al., 2020).

The skill of generating vivid mental images differs from the ability to control and manipulate those images. In this context, cognitive and sports psychology have developed tools to differentiate mental imagery ability into two components: controllability, defined as the ability to mentally manipulate an image accurately, and vividness, which refers to the sensory richness of an image (Cumming and Eaves, 2018).

Vividness is typically assessed through self-report questionnaires that capture individuals’ subjective perceptions of static and dynamic images quality, which may not reflect the actual quality of the imagery (Bedir and Erhan, 2021). In contrast, controllability can be evaluated using objective criteria, such as mental rotation tasks that require cognitive manipulation and spatial transformation of imagined objects (Saimpont et al., 2015). Sport psychology presents mixed findings regarding the relationship between the two components of imagery - vividness and controllability - and their impact on athletic performance. An individual may excel in one type of imagery task while showing lower abilities or none in others. This variability underscores the nuanced nature of imagery skills, where strengths can differ across different tasks or contexts (Smith and Wakefield, 2015). In martial arts, vivid imagery can enhance performance by allowing practitioners to visualize techniques and scenarios effectively; on the contrary, the ability to manipulate mental images of objects is crucial for spatial awareness, movement planning, and executing techniques (Munroe-Chandler and Guerrero, 2018).

Some studies indicate that individuals with high vividness, as assessed by self-report methods, learn new movements more quickly than those with lower vividness abilities. Furthermore, these individuals tend to perform movements with greater accuracy (Parnabas et al., 2015; Cunanan et al., 2018).

Conversely, other research shows that superior performance among athletes is linked to controllability rather than vividness. For example, Moreau et al. (2010), found no significant differences in vividness across varying levels of expertise in fencing, judo, and wrestling, while controllability showed positive effects. In contrast, Di Corrado et al. (2014), reported that dancers and karate practitioners scored higher in both vividness and controllability compared to a control group of non-athletes. This highlights the complexity of the relationship between these components and athletic performance. Additionally, research has highlighted that mental imagery not only improves sports performance and outcomes during actual competitions, but also helps reduce pre-match stressful symptoms. Stress is the feeling of being overwhelmed by mental or emotional pressure. It is also a significant factor that can adversely affect an individual’s cognitive and physical capabilities (Budnik-Przybylska et al., 2023; Di Corrado et al., 2021). Nevertheless, it is an inevitable part of life and sport, characterized as a physical, mental, or emotional demand that disrupts the body’s homeostasis. While a certain level of stress can be beneficial - providing the pressure and demands needed to enhance performance - excessive stress can be detrimental.

Stress can manifest as acute, episodic, or chronic, with episodic stress being the most common. If acute stress is not managed effectively, it can lead to long-term negative effects on performance (Di Corrado et al., 2021; Wearne et al., 2019).

When athletes perceive stress as unmanageable, they often experience a decline in performance levels, which can range from mild to severe (Cunanan et al., 2018). Interestingly, many studies have recognized that maintaining positive functioning can be a normative response even in the face of emotional distress. This refers to an adaptive process where people use cognitive efforts to cope with adversity, leading to well-being and adaptability to stressors (Kellmann et al., 2018). Self-efficacy is seen as an essential resource and a key factor for success, emphasizing its role as a protective element or mediator that fosters more adaptive responses to stress and adversity. It refers to an individual’s belief in their capacity to utilize motivational, and cognitive resources to address situational demands (Di Corrado et al., 2022). Research has confirmed its significant role in adaptive coping styles during stressful situations, showing that individuals with high self-efficacy tend to have greater control over their thoughts and exhibit more stability in goal attainment (Peers et al., 2020; Popovych et al., 2020; Di Corrado et al., 2023). Referring to mental visualization techniques used in sports, in the current study, we have explored traditional styles of martial arts, including judo, karate, and taekwondo. Each discipline incorporates different elements that can influence the effectiveness of imagery. At this regard, studies have indicated that judoka often use imagery to enhance performance in techniques, timing, and situational awareness; karate practitioners frequently use imagery to visualize complex movements and technique precision; taekwondo athletes often employ imagery to visualize competitive scenarios and dynamic movements (Piedade et al., 2022; Russo and Ottoboni, 2019: Beheshti et al., 2021).

The choice of imagery techniques may depend on the specific skills and strategies emphasized in each discipline. The relationship between mental imagery, self-efficacy, and stress, respectively, in judo, karate, and taekwondo has been a focus of various studies, revealing interesting insights into how imagery can influence athletes’ mental states and performance. Research indicated that imagery can significantly enhance self-efficacy in karate athletes (Simonsmeier et al., 2021; Khodabandelou and Salehian, 2023). By visualizing successful performances, athletes can boost their confidence, leading to improved execution of techniques during competitions and also managed stress. Di Corrado et al. (2019), revealed that both vividness and controllability in imagery significantly predicted self-efficacy. In judo, athletes who engaged in imagery reported higher levels of self-efficacy, making athletes feel more prepared for matches and more resilient (Andreato et al., 2022). A study by Silva et al. (2018), found that elite judoka used imagery to enhance their mental preparation before matches. They reported using imagery to visualize specific techniques and scenarios, which helped improve their confidence and performance under pressure.

Julvanichpong et al. (2022), have shown that vivid mental rehearsal of techniques and sparring scenarios boosted confidence in taekwondo athletes, feeling more capable and focused during competitions.

Despite the large amount of research in the strong positive relationship between imagery and self-efficacy conducted separately in all three sports, very little attention has been devoted to the precise relationship between each dimension of imagery vividness (static and dynamic), controllability (i.e., the athlete’s ability to manipulate and direct their mental imagery), and stress levels. Given the limited research, the purpose of this study was to test the mediating role of self-efficacy in the association between Static Imagery, External dynamic Imagery, and Internal dynamic Imagery, Controllability, and the degree of Stress among judo, karate, and taekwondo athletes.

More specifically, we hypothesized the following:

H1: There are significant relationships among vividness, controllability, and stress levels, across different types of martial arts.

H2: Self-efficacy mediates the relationship between each dimension of imagery and stress.

In our hypothesized model (Figure 1), the total effects included a direct effect pathway (path c’) of each dimension of imagery vividness (Static Imagery, External dynamic Imagery, and Internal dynamic Imagery), and Controllability on the stress levels, and a total indirect pathway (mediated: path ab) of each level of imagery vividness and controllability, on the stress levels through self-efficacy.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized mediation model explaining the potential mediating effect of self-efficacy on the relationship between each of dimension of imagery vividness and controllability, and stress levels.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

Group sample sizes were decided by power analysis, with G*Power being used in a one-way between-subjects ANOVA (Faul et al., 2007). The input parameters were effect size (ŋ2 = 0.70), alpha level of 0.05, power (0.95%) and the number of groups sampled (three groups). Finally, the result revealed the need for 36 participants in each of the three groups (108 in all). Therefore, a total of n = 110 participants (61 males and 49 females), aged between 21 and 23 years old (M = 22.47, SD = 0.75), were recruited to take part in the research. The athletes were recruited according to the style they practiced: taekwondo (n = 36), karate (n = 34) and judo (n = 40) group. They were active competitors and had a minimum of 13 years of training skill in the sport (M = 15.05, SD = 1.18). They usually trained five or six times per week (M = 9.6 h, SD = 1.3). The inclusion criteria for participants were: (a) at least 18 years old; (b) availability for the entire data collection period; (c) no motor or neurological deficits; and (d) ability to provide informed consent. Participants were excluded if they did not meet these criteria. Prior to the study, all participants received a comprehensive explanation of the protocols and signed an informed consent form for all tests. The procedures adhered to the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and ethical approval was granted by the University Enna Kore Internal Review Board for research (UKE-IRBPSY-09.23.11). Measurements were conducted individually in groups of four participants, in a secluded area near the training facilities, and not immediately before or after competitions to reduce external distractions. All participants completed a series of tests under the supervision of two researchers, with confidentiality of their responses guaranteed. The descriptive characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample.

Measures

Imagery vividness assessment

Static imagery

The Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (Marks, 1973) in the Italian adapted version (Antonietti and Crespi, 1995) was used to assess the participant’s Static imagery. The instrument is a 16-item self-report tool designed to assess the vividness of visual mental imagery across four subscales, with four items dedicated to each. Participants are prompted to visualize scenes, such as characteristics of a friend or parent, the climate, or a country, and then rate the clarity and vividness of these images on a five-point scale: 1: “No image at all (only a sense of knowing the object)”; 2: “Vague and dim”; 3: “Moderately clear and vivid”; 4: “Clear and fairly vivid”; and 5: “Perfectly clear and vivid, just like normal perception.” Higher scores reflect greater vividness. The instrument has demonstrated a mean Cronbach’s α of 0.89, indicating strong internal consistency (McKelvie, 1995). The internal consistency for the current study is good (α = 0.86).

Dynamic imagery

The Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire-2 (Roberts et al., 2008) is a self-report questionnaire for measuring Dynamic imagery and includes 12 items. Participants evaluate their ability to visualize or kinaesthetically imagine a movement using a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 indicates “perfectly clear and vivid” and 5 indicates “no image at all, only a sense of knowing you are ‘thinking’ of the skill.” The instrument assesses three distinct types of movement imagery: 1. External dynamic imagery, which refers to the visualization of movement from an outsider’s perspective; 2. Internal dynamic imagery, which involves visualizing oneself performing the movement; 3. Kinaesthetic dynamic imagery, which captures the somatosensory experience of executing the task, as if one were truly engaged in the activity. The tool has demonstrated a Cronbach’s α of 0.91, indicating excellent internal consistency. The internal consistency for the current study is very good (α = 0.88). To obtain the prevalence of different types of viewpoint imagination, our current study focused on two kinds of movement imagery: external viewpoint and internal viewpoint.

Imagery controllability assessment

Subtraction of parts

It consists of mentally taking away a part of an image in order to compose or decompose complete figures. This task was extracted from Mental Imagery Test (Di Nuovo et al., 2014). Participants were shown two 17x12cm cards representing a full-lighted digital clock on the first card, while the second card contained the part(s) of the picture that needed to be taken away (Figure 2). Participants were asked to image complete clock without the subtracting parts and name the resulting image. Specifically, participants received the following verbal instructions: “Look at this (complete clock) without this (subtracting parts). What does this become?” Participants received three trials and were tested individually in a single session lasting about 10 min. The subtest has reported an alpha coefficient of r = 0.68.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mental image transformation task subtraction of parts.

Stress levels

The level of stress was assessed using the Italian version (Di Nuovo et al., 2000) of the Psychological Stress Measure (PSM – Lemyre et al., 1990), a self-administered questionnaire consisting of 49 items. It is based on different aspects related to the perception that the individual has of its condition. The choice of responses is made of a scale (Likert-type) whose possible answers are 1–4 (from “null” to “much”). A total score of stress level can be found by sum of all scores. The internal consistency measured for our data was good, showing a α > 0.89.

Self-efficacy

The Italian version of the General Self Efficacy Scale (Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995; Di Nuovo and Magnano, 2013) is a 10-item scale designed to assess optimistic self-beliefs to cope with demands in life and reach goals (e.g., “Even if someone opposes, I can find the means and strength to achieve what I desire”; “I am confident that I can cope adequately with unexpected events”). Response options range on a 5-point Likert format from (5) Completely agree to (1) Completely disagree. Higher scores indicate a stronger sense of self-efficacy. In the present sample, Cronbach’s α was 0.89.

Statistical analyses

Data were expressed as means (M) ± standard deviations (SD) and the range. A 2 × 3 (gender × sport type) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted to compare the mean in main effects and interactions of gender (male and female) and sport type (taekwondo/karate/judo) on the data of the study variables. Follow-up analysis of variance (ANOVA) and t-test with Bonferroni correction were then used to examine the significant differences. Gender was included as a control variable in the analysis to account for potential differences between male and female participants. Furthermore, Pearson’s correlation was used to determine the relationships between the selected variables. An a-priori power analysis was run with G*Power (Faul et al., 2007). To examine the significance of mediation, the recommendations put forth by Baron and Kenny (1986) were followed. Their Steps 1, 2 and 3 involve testing the significance of the relationship between the following: (1) the independent and the dependent variables (i.e., vividness/controllability and stress); (2) the independent and the mediator variables (i.e., vividness/controllability and self-efficacy); (3) the mediator and the dependent variables (i.e., self-efficacy and stress). If these steps are passed, one should determine in Step 4 whether the mediator variable reduces or eliminates the link between the independent variable and the dependent variable. For this purpose, a mediation analysis was conducted using Hayes (2013) PROCESS version 3.1 (Hayes PROCESS macro-Model 4 – Ohio, USA) computational tool for SPSS. This tool enables the estimation of path coefficients, standard errors, and different indexes of effect size, as well as the significance of the indirect effects obtained through the bootstrapping method with 5,000 repetitions, with a confidence interval (CI) of 95% (Preacher and Kelley, 2011). Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. Statistical analyses were processed using SPSS version 27.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

Findings revealed significant multivariate effects only for sport type, Wilks λ = 0.002, F (12, 198) = 368.705, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.957, power = 1.000. Means and standard deviations of study variables are reported in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean ± SD for the study variables.

Follow up ANOVAs revealed significant univariate differences (p = 0.001) between sport type on all variable scores. Specifically, athletes of karate reported higher scores on Static Imagery: F (2, 107) = 2652.205, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.981, power = 1,000 (taekwondo M = 43.61; karate M = 76.41; judo M = 71.18); Controllability: F (2, 107) = 303.230, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.854, power = 1,000 (taekwondo M = 3.19; karate M = 7.56; judo M = 5.23); Stress: F (2, 107) = 224.849, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.812, power = 1,000 (taekwondo M = 56; karate M = 107.71; judo M = 77.43), than athletes of other sports. Conversely, athletes of taekwondo reported higher scores on External dynamic Imagery: F (2, 107) = 654.865, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.926, power = 1,000 (taekwondo M = 53.42; karate M = 22.24; judo M = 44.48); Internal dynamic Imagery: F (2, 107) = 377.573, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.879, power = 1,000 (taekwondo M = 52.50; karate M = 23.26; judo M = 44.85); Self-efficacy: F (2, 107) = 498.140, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.905, power = 1,000 (taekwondo M = 45.78; karate M = 24.62; judo M = 33.42), than athletes of other sports. Moreover, there was no significant difference (p > 0.05) across groups concerning gender.

Then, the relationships among variables were analysed with Pearson’s correlation (Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson’s correlation coefficients.

As can be observed, Dynamic Imagery (External/Internal) is positively associated with self-efficacy, and negatively associated with stress level. Conversely, Static Imagery and Controllability are negatively associated with self-efficacy, and positively associated with stress level. Lastly, self-efficacy is negatively associated with stress level.

Based on the correlation results, a mediation analysis was executed by entering the score of self-efficacy as mediator in the vividness/controllability and stress relationship. The mediation results are presented in Table 4, which contains the standardized β, indicating the intensity of the effect, and the 95% CIs, indicating the significance of the effect with a 5% probability of error (CIs that do not contain 0 are significant).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effects of imagery vividness on anger expression through resilience (standardized β).

As can be observed, the results showed that Static Imagery had a significant indirect effect, mediated by self-efficacy, on stress levels (β = 1.037, SE = 0.02, p < 0.001). Thus, the relationship between Static imagery and stress was fully mediated by self-efficacy, with a decrement of stress levels. Furthermore, mediational analyses showed that External dynamic Imagery, Internal dynamic Imagery, and Controllability had both direct and indirect effects through self-efficacy on stress. Thus, these relationships were partially mediated by self-efficacy. The direct effect path-way of External dynamic Imagery (β = −0.918, SE = 0.02, p < 0.001) and Internal dynamic Imagery (β = −0.636, SE = 0.02, p < 0.001) had a significant and negative impact on stress levels, indicating Dynamic imagery as a significant predictor of stress.

Discussion

Imagery plays a critical role in enhancing self-efficacy and managing stress in martial arts, contributing to athletes’ overall mental preparedness and performance. The aim of this study was to test the mediating role of self-efficacy in the association between Static Imagery, External dynamic Imagery, and Internal dynamic Imagery, Controllability, and the degree of Stress among judo, karate, and taekwondo athletes. We hypothesized that all the variables of imagery would be related to stress levels through the mediation of self-efficacy.

Data analysis partially confirmed the research hypotheses. In line with H1, findings showed that karate athletes reported significant higher scores on Static Imagery, Controllability and stress levels. On the other hand, results showed that taekwondo athletes reported significant higher scores on External dynamic imagery, Internal dynamic imagery, and self-efficacy.

According with our findings, Volgemute et al. (2023), found that athletes using dynamic imagery techniques reported higher self-efficacy and lower pre-competition anxiety compared to those using static imagery. This indicates that dynamic imagery may be more effective in high-pressure situations. Dynamic imagery plays a vital role in visualizing sparring scenarios and executing techniques effectively.

Research indicates that athletes who experience vivid dynamic imagery tend to feel more confident, as it enables them to rehearse and enhance their performance (Denis, 1985; Di Corrado et al., 2020). Moreover, it helps manage stress by preparing athletes for fast-paced situations, making them feel more capable of handling the unpredictability of competition (Di Corrado et al., 2014). Each sport employs a mix of both types of imagery, tailored to their specific skills and performance demands.

In judo, karate, and taekwondo, both static and dynamic imagery are utilized, but the emphasis varies depending on the nature of the sport (Stergiou et al., 2019). Specifically, karate practitioners primarily use static imagery to visualize the positions and postures required for katas (forms), which helps them understand stance, balance, and form. Dynamic imagery is employed when visualizing techniques in action (Franklin, 2022). Judoka use both static and dynamic imagery: static imagery to visualize grip positions and dynamic imagery to envision movements in response to their opponents. Lastly, static imagery is used less frequently by taekwondo athletes, who heavily rely on dynamic imagery to visualize high kicks, fast movements, and sparring scenarios. This approach helps them anticipate actions and enhance their performance under pressure (Fu et al., 2014; Patti et al., 2018).

Additionally, Pearson’s correlation analysis revealed very strong positive relationships between scores for self-efficacy and dynamic imagery variables (both external and internal), as well as a negative association with stress levels. In confirmation of such findings, research has shown that this positive correlation could likely be due to the fact that dynamic imagery can help athletes visualize themselves successfully completing a task, thereby increasing their self-confidence and belief in their ability to succeed.

Moreover, dynamic imagery can cultivate essential mental skills, including focus, concentration, and relaxation, which are vital for optimizing athletic performance (Lochbaum et al., 2023; Jekauc et al., 2023; Lochbaum et al., 2022).

Our findings also revealed a negative correlation between static imagery and controllability with self-efficacy, while positively linking them to stress levels. This can lead to a heightened stress response, both physically and mentally. Research supports this notion, indicating that focusing on negative or uncontrollable aspects of a situation through imagery can inadvertently exacerbate stress levels (Budnik-Przybylska et al., 2023; Zapała et al., 2021; Wearne et al., 2019). In other words, respondents who reveal higher physiological activation during imagery tasks are more likely to experience emotional instability, and stress. This could be explained using Yerkes and Dodson’s (1908) first law: with low worry, imagery did not play any role; however, when worry was high then imagery was moderate or high.

Lastly, a mediation analysis partially confirmed the research hypothesis (H2), highlighting that only the relationship between Static imagery and stress was fully mediated by self-efficacy, with a decrement of stress levels. Furthermore, mediational analyses showed that relationships between External dynamic Imagery, Internal dynamic Imagery, Controllability, and stress were partially mediated by self-efficacy. In line with our findings, previous research indicated that athletes who have higher self-efficacy use all imagery abilities more than athletes who have lower self-efficacy level (Volgemute et al., 2021; Mills et al., 2000). Specifically, athletes who feel confident in their abilities are better equipped to handle competition-related pressures, leading to improved focus and performance. Overall, our finding suggest that imagery serves multiple roles - enhancing skill acquisition, reducing anxiety, improving emotional regulation, and ultimately leading to better performance. These techniques are essential to mental preparation and contribute significantly to an athlete’s success in competitions.

A limitation of the present study could be that the cross-sectional nature of the work prevents any conclusions of causal relationships existing between variables. To establish causal relationships, future research could utilize a longitudinal design or prospective cohort studies. By following participants over time, researchers can track changes in variables and observe how one may influence the other. This would help establish causality by capturing temporal relationships between the variables.

Secondly, all data were obtained through self-reported questionnaires, which could present response bias. The participants might have underestimated or overestimated the relationship between the study variables.

To reduce response bias and to address potential inaccuracies in self-reports, researchers could employ multiple data collection methods or using different methods to verify the same information. For example, comparing self-reported data with other objective or third-party assessments. By applying these solutions, the study’s findings could be more robust and reliable, providing clearer insights into causal relationships and minimizing biases.

Future research could provide a more comprehensive understanding of how different types of imagery may specifically influence stress levels in athletes, with self-efficacy acting as a mediator. By examining these factors in greater detail, we could gain a more nuanced understanding of the interplay between mental strategies, stress management, and performance across diverse athletic settings.

Conclusion

Athletes who believe in their skills are more likely to engage deeply with dynamic imagery, enhancing their performance and managing pre-performance stress. In conclusion, results demonstrated that vividness of imagery and controllability skills are correlated, while self-efficacy plays a crucial mediating role. Effective stress management through enhanced self-efficacy can lead to better mental performance in martial arts, highlighting the importance of psychological factors in physical training and competition. By enhancing self-efficacy through imagery strategies, coaches can create programs that focus on helping athletes manage challenges and reduce stress responses.

Moreover, by incorporating imagery strategies into regular training routines, coaches can help athletes manage stress more effectively, through imagery exercises where athletes visualize themselves succeeding in stressful situations, such as performing under pressure, recovering from mistakes, or staying focused during high-stakes moments.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the University Enna Kore Internal Review Board for research (UKE-IRBPSY-09.23.11). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PT: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. MC: Data curation, Writing – original draft. MG: Data curation, Writing – original draft. MT: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. MP: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer VR declared a shared affiliation with the author(s) MC to the handling editor at the time of review.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Andreato, L. V., dos Santos, M. G., and Andrade, A. (2022). What do we know about the effects of mental training applied to combat sports? A systematic review. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 63:102267. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonietti, A., and Crespi, M. Analysis of three questionnaires for assessing the vividness of mental image. Department of Psychology Catholic University of the Sacred Heart Technical Report Milano, (1995). Available at: http://www.neurovr.org/psylab/ricerca_pc_im.html (accessed on 22 February 2019).

Google Scholar

Barnamehei, H., Khazaee, F., Safaei, M. A., Jabari, H., Golfeshan, N., Barnamehei, M., et al. (2020). Motor learning and training strategy effect on motor control; comparison between taekwondo and karate front kick (Ap Chagi and Mae Geri). Int. J. Martial Arts 6, 48–65. doi: 10.51222/injoma.2020.11.6.48

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedir, D., and Erhan, S. E. (2021). The effect of virtual reality technology on the imagery skills and performance of target-based sports athletes. Front. Psychol. 11:2073. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02073

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beheshti, A., Zandi, H. G., Fathirezaie, Z., and Heidari, F. (2021). Mental toughness and performance strategies of martial artists in practice and competition. Exerc Qual Life 13, 13–21. doi: 10.31382/eqol.210602

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Budnik-Przybylska, D., Syty, P., Kaźmierczak, M., Łabuda, M., Doliński, Ł., Kastrau, A., et al. (2023). Exploring the influence of personal factors on physiological responses to mental imagery in sport. Sci. Rep. 13:2628. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-29811-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Commodari, E., Sole, J., Guarnera, M., and La Rosa, V. L. (2024). Mental imagery in education: what impact on the relationships with visuospatial processing and school performance in junior high school students? Think. Ski. Creat 54:101667. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2024.101667

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, J., Bird, G., Brown, K., Kolitsida, M., and Quinton, M. (2023). “Imagery” in Routledge handbook of applied sport psychology (Routledge: London, UK), 543–552.

Google Scholar

Cumming, J., and Eaves, D. L. (2018). The nature, measurement, and development of imagery ability. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 37, 375–393. doi: 10.1177/0276236617752439

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunanan, A. J., DeWeese, B. H., Wagle, J. P., Carroll, K. M., Sausaman, R., Hornsby, W. G., et al. (2018). The general adaptation syndrome: a foundation for the concept of periodization. Sports Med. 48, 787–797. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0855-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Denis, M. (1985). Visual imagery and the use of mental practice in the development of motor skills. Can. J. Appl. Sport Sci. 10, 45–165.

Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Buscemi, A., Magnano, P., Maldonato, N. M., Tusak, M., and Coco, M. (2021). Mood states and performance in elite canoe polo players: the mediating role of stress. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:4494. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18094494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Guarnera, M., Guerrera, C. S., Maldonato, N. M., Di Nuovo, S., Castellano, S., et al. (2020). Mental imagery skills in competitive young athletes and non-athletes. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00633

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Guarnera, M., and Quartiroli, A. (2014). Vividness and transformation of mental images in karate and dance. Percept. Mot. Skills 119, 764–773. doi: 10.2466/22.24.PMS.119c30z6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Guarnera, M., Vitali, F., Quartiroli, A., and Coco, M. (2019). Imagery ability of elite level athletes from individual vs. team and contact vs. no-contact sports. PeerJ 7:e6940. doi: 10.7717/peerj.6940

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Muzii, B., Magnano, P., Coco, M., La Paglia, R., and Maldonato, N. M. (2022). The moderated mediating effect of Hope, self-efficacy and resilience in the relationship between post-traumatic growth and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Healthcare 10, 1091–1106. doi: 10.3390/healthcare10061091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Quartiroli, A., and Coco, M. (2021). Editorial: psychological and motor associations in sports performance: a mental approach to sports. Front. Psychol. 12:629944. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.629944

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado, D., Sagone, E., Buscemi, A., and Coco, M. (2023). The relationship between anger expression and performance score in parents and coaches: the mediating role of self-efficacy and assertiveness. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20, 5372–5385. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20075372

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Nuovo, S., Castellano, S., and Guarnera, M. (2014). Mental imagery test. Florence, Italy: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Di Nuovo, S., and Magnano, P. (2013). Transversal competencies and educational choices (Competenze Trasversali e Scelte formative). Trento, Italy: Erickson.

Google Scholar

Di Nuovo, S., Rispoli, L., and Genta, E. (2000). Measuring stress. The M.S.P. Test and other tools for integrated assessment (Misurare lo stress. Il test M.S.P. E altri strumenti per una valutazione integrata). Milan: Franco Angeli.

Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, E. (2022). Dynamic alignment through imagery. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Fu, A. S., Kho, A., Li, J., Sun, X. H., and Chan, C. C. (2014). Imagery perspective among young athletes: differentiation between external and internal visual imagery. J Sport Health Sc. 5, 1–8.

Google Scholar

Gavagan, C. J., and Sayers, M. G. (2017). A biomechanical analysis of the roundhouse kicking technique of expert practitioners: a comparison between the martial arts disciplines of Muay Thai, karate, and taekwondo. PLoS One 12:e0182645. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182645

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geisen, M., Raab, M., Jansen, P., and Klatt, S. (2024). Embodied mental rotation ability in open-and closed-skill sports: pilot study with a new virtual paradigm. Ex brain res. 242, 653–664. doi: 10.1007/s00221-023-06753-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guillot, A., Rienzo, F. D., Frank, C., Debarnot, U., and MacIntyre, T. E. (2024). From simulation to motor execution: a review of the impact of dynamic motor imagery on performance. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17, 420–439. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2021.2007539

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, USA: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Jekauc, D., Fiedler, J., Wunsch, K., Mülberger, L., Burkart, D., Kilgus, A., et al. (2023). The effect of self-confidence on performance in sports: a meta-analysis and narrative review. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1, 1–27. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2023.2222376

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Julvanichpong, T., Pattanamontri, C., Charoenwattana, S., and Singnoy, C. (2022). The effect of a psychological skill training package on the mental readiness of taekwondo athletes. Psychol 13, 1670–1684. doi: 10.4236/psych.2022.1312104

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellmann, M., Bertollo, M., Bosquet, L., Brink, M., Coutts, A. J., Duffield, R., et al. (2018). Recovery and performance in sport: consensus statement. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 13, 240–245. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2017-0759

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khodabandelou, M., and Salehian, M. H. (2023). The effect of psychological toughness and self-efficacy in the acquisition of the martial arts leg kick skill. Ido Mov Culture. J Martial Arts Anthrop 23, 6–12.

Google Scholar

Lemyre, L., Tessier, R., and Fillion, L. (1990). La Mesure du stress psychologique: manuel d’utilisation. Québec, Que: Behaviora.

Google Scholar

Lindsay, R. S., Larkin, P., Kittel, A., and Spittle, M. (2023). Mental imagery training programs for developing sport-specific motor skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Phys Educ Sport Pedagogy 28, 444–465. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2021.1991297

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lochbaum, M., Sherburn, M., Sisneros, C., Cooper, S., Lane, A. M., and Terry, P. C. (2022). Revisiting the self-confidence and sport performance relationship: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:6381. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19116381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lochbaum, M., Sisneros, C., Cooper, S., and Terry, P. C. (2023). Pre-event self-efficacy and sports performance: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Sports 11:222. doi: 10.3390/sports11110222

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br. J. Psychol. 64, 17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McKelvie, S. J. (1995). The VVIQ as a psychometric test of individual differences in visual imagery vividness: a critical quantitative review and plea for direction. J Mental Imagery 19, 1–106.

Google Scholar

Mills, K. D., Munroe, K. J., and Hall, C. R. (2000). The relationship between imagery and self-efficacy in competitive athletes. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 20, 33–39. doi: 10.2190/FC65-AP4T-E94J-5W7P

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, A., and O’Shea, H. (2020). Motor imagery practice and cognitive processes. Front. Psychol. 11:394. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00394

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreau, D., Clerc, J., Mansy-Dannay, A., and Guerrien, A. (2010). Assessing movement imagery ability: self-report questionnaires vs. performance-based tests. EJOP 6, 93–109. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v6i4.225

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Munroe-Chandler, K., and Guerrero, M. (2018). “Imagery in sport, exercise and performance”, in sport, exercise, and performance psychology. London, UK: Routledge, 279–301.

Google Scholar

Parnabas, V., Parnabas, J., and Parnabas, A. M. (2015). The influence of mental imagery techniques on sport performance among taekwondo athletes. EAR 11, 14729–14734.

Google Scholar

Patti, A., Messina, G., Palma, R., Barcellona, M., Brusa, J., Iovane, A., et al. (2018). Comparison of posturographic parameters between young taekwondo and tennis athletes. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 30, 1052–1055. doi: 10.1589/jpts.30.1052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peers, C., Issartel, J., Behan, S., O'Connor, N., and Belton, S. (2020). Movement competence: association with physical self-efficacy and physical activity. Hum. Mov. Sci. 70:102582. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2020.102582

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piedade, S. R., Ferreira, D. M., Cristante, A. F., Ikemoto, R. Y., and Fernandes, C. H. (2022). “Judo, karate, and taekwondo”, in specific sports-related injuries. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 315–328.

Google Scholar

Popovych, I., Blynova, O., Halian, I., and Savchuk, O. (2020). Self-efficacy of future athletes with different levels of psychological safety. J. Phys. Educ. 20, 2718–2724.

Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychol. Methods 16, 93–115. doi: 10.1037/a0022658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R., Callow, N., Hardy, L., Markland, D., and Bringer, J. (2008). Movement imagery ability: development and assessment of a revised version of the vividness of movement imagery questionnaire. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 30, 200–221. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.2.200

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, G., and Ottoboni, G. (2019). The perceptual–cognitive skills of combat sports athletes: a systematic review. Psychol Sport Ex. 44, 60–78. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saimpont, A., Malouin, F., Tousignant, B., and Jackson, P. L. (2015). Assessing motor imagery ability in younger and older adults by combining measures of vividness, controllability and timing of motor imagery. Brain Res. 1597, 196–209. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.11.050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzer, R., and Jerusalem, M. (1995) in “Generalized self-efficacy scale”, in measures in Health Psychology: A User’s portfolio. Causal and control beliefs. eds. J. Weinman, S. Wright, and M. Johnston (Windsor, UK: Nfer-Nelson), 35–37.

Google Scholar

Silva, V., Dias, C., Corte-Real, N., and Fonseca, A. M. (2018). Mental toughness attributes in judo: perceptions of athletes. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte 18, 86–101.

Google Scholar

Simonsmeier, B. A., Andronie, M., Buecker, S., and Frank, C. (2021). The effects of imagery interventions in sports: a meta-analysis. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 14, 186–207. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2020.1780627

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D., and Wakefield, C. (2015). Imagery in sport. In Sport and Exercise Psychology: Topics in Applied Psychology, 232–249. doi: 10.4324/9781315713809232–249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stergiou, M., El Raheb, K., and Ioannidis, Y. (2019). “Imagery and metaphors: from movement practices to digital and immersive environments”, in Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Movement and Computing (pp. 1–8).

Google Scholar

Volgemute, K., Vazne, Z., and Krauksta, D. (2021). The relationship between imagery and physical self-efficacy in athletes. LASE J. Sport ScI. 12, 3–10.

Google Scholar

Volgemute, K., Vazne, Z., Krauksta, D., and Cosma, G. A. (2023). The improvement of athletes' imagery ability and physical self-efficacy for the growth of athletic achievements in sport model. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 23, 748–755.

Google Scholar

Voyer, D., and Jansen, P. (2017). Motor expertise and performance in spatial tasks: a meta-analysis. Hum. Mov. Sci. 54, 110–124. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wearne, T. A., Lucien, A., Trimmer, E. M., Logan, J. A., Rushby, J., Wilson, E., et al. (2019). Anxiety sensitivity moderates the subjective experience but not the physiological response to psychosocial stress. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 141, 76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.04.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yerkes, R. M., and Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neurol. 18, 459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zapała, D., Iwanowicz, P., Francuz, P., and Augustynowicz, P. (2021). Handedness effects on motor imagery during kinesthetic and visual motor conditions. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-92467-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mental imagery, martial arts, performance, stress, competition

Citation: Di Corrado D, Tortella P, Coco M, Guarnera M, Tusak M and Parisi MC (2025) Mental imagery and stress: the mediating role of self-efficacy in competitive martial arts athletes. Front. Psychol. 16:1517718. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1517718

Received: 26 October 2024; Accepted: 04 February 2025;
Published: 18 February 2025.

Edited by:

Francesca Vitali, University of Verona, Italy

Reviewed by:

Fabrizio Sors, University of Trieste, Italy
Valentina Lucia La Rosa, University of Catania, Italy
Maria Giulia Tullo, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Di Corrado, Tortella, Coco, Guarnera, Tusak and Parisi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Donatella Di Corrado, ZG9uYXRlbGxhLmRpY29ycmFkb0B1bmlrb3JlLml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more