Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 10 July 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology

Is it really a neuromyth? A meta-analysis of the learning styles matching hypothesis

Virginia Clinton-Lisell
&#x;Virginia Clinton-Lisell*Christine LitzingerChristine Litzinger
  • Department of Education, Health, and Behavior Studies, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks, ND, United States

Learning styles have been a contentious topic in education for years. The purpose of this study was to conduct a meta-analysis of the effects of matching instruction to modality learning styles compared to unmatched instruction on learning outcomes. A systematic search of the research findings yielded 21 eligible studies with 101 effect sizes and 1,712 participants for the meta-analysis. Based on robust variance estimation, there was an overall benefit of matching instruction to learning styles, g = 0.31, SE = 0.12, 95% CI = [0.05, 0.57], p = 0.02. However, only 26% of learning outcome measures indicated matched instruction benefits for at least two styles, indicating a crossover interaction supportive of the matching hypothesis. In total, 12 studies without sufficient statistical details for the meta-analysis were also examined for an indication of a crossover effect; 25% of these studies had findings indicative of a crossover interaction. Given the time and financial expenses of implementation coupled with low study quality, the benefits of matching instruction to learning styles are interpreted as too small and too infrequent to warrant widespread adoption.

Introduction

Learning styles have been the topic of ongoing debate in education. Teacher education textbooks often state matching instruction to students’ preferred style will optimize learning outcomes (i.e., the matching hypothesis; Cuevas, 2015; Wininger et al., 2019). In contrast, cognitive scientists have argued there is a lack of empirical evidence to support the claims of the matching hypothesis (Kirschner, 2017; Willingham, 2018). Because of the lack of known empirical evidence supporting the matching hypothesis, there is understandable concern that perpetuating the concept of learning styles could lead to wasting resources (namely, educator time and effort) to match instruction as well as stereotyping students into restrictive categories (Newton and Miah, 2017). However, a meta-analysis aggregating findings compiled from an exhaustive search for studies on matching instruction to learning styles has not been conducted. Such a meta-analysis could be very helpful in informing this ongoing debate between educational practitioners and researchers. The purpose of this study was to conduct a meta-analysis of learning outcomes comparing conditions in which instruction is matched to students’ preferred learning styles to when instruction is unmatched to students’ preferred learning styles.

Literature review

It is not controversial that there are substantial individual differences in student learning—teacher education and cognitive science scholars agree on this concept. There is substantial empirical evidence that students’ academic performance and learning vary due to background knowledge, motivation, and study strategies (Fong et al., 2021; Smith et al., 2021), just to name a few examples. However, the concept in learning styles theories that is controversial is the meshing or matching hypothesis in which students learn better when their instruction matches their preferred learning style (Pashler et al., 2008; Cuevas, 2015; Lyle et al., 2023). A key aspect of the matching hypothesis is that there is a crossover interaction (Kirschner, 2017), also known as a qualitative interaction, in which a particular treatment (in the case of learning styles, a particular modality of instruction) is effective for at least one subgroup but a different treatment is effective for another subgroup (Qiu and Wang, 2019). Generally speaking, these crossover treatment interactions are rare (Petticrew et al., 2012; Preacher and Sterba, 2019), but important to determining optimal treatments for individuals (Olsen et al., 2019; Qiu and Wang, 2019).

There are numerous learning styles (Dunn, 1990) as well as cognitive styles in which the preferred order of processing information varies (Calcaterra et al., 2005; Fiorina et al., 2007). The most prevalent are preferred modalities for learning information (Dekker et al., 2012; Brown, 2023). Learners are generally categorized through self-reports of preferred modalities (An and Carr, 2017), such as the VAK typology (visual, auditory, and kinesthetic; Fallace, 2023a,b). An example of accommodating these styles would be to provide information for learners categorized as “visual” in pictures, learners categorized as “auditory” would process the same information best aurally, and learners categorized as kinesthetic would have a hands-on activity (Dunn and Dunn, 1975). Then, a read/write category was added making it the VARK typology for learners who were thought to best process information through reading verbal information (as opposed to visual learners who better processed pictures; Fleming and Mills, 1992).

A typology similar to the VARK for categorizing learning styles is the verbalizer/visualizer approach (Riding and Rayner, 1998). According to this framework, verbalizers tend to mentally represent information in words whereas visualizers (also called imagers) tend to mentally represent information in mental pictures or diagrams (Riding and Sadler-Smith, 1992; Knoll et al., 2017). Subsequently, the developers of this framework argue that verbalizers better learn the material presented in text and images better learn the material presented in images (Riding and Sadler-Smith, 1992). This is analogous to the visual and read/write learners in the VARK model. Importantly, both the VARK and the verbalizer/visualizer approach advocate matching the modality of the instruction to the students’ learning style.

Adapting instruction based on modality learning styles may be conflated with multimodal instruction. Multimodal instruction is providing information to students in more than one modality, such as a text with relevant pictures or diagrams (Bouchey et al., 2021). The rationale for providing students with multiple modalities is grounded in dual coding in which visual and verbal information are processed in separate channels or pathways in the architecture of human cognition (Paivio, 1991; Reed, 2006). Having information presented in two modalities (and subsequently two channels) allows for more information to be processed at a given time (Mayer and Anderson, 1992; Mayer, 2011). Multimodal instruction has been found to benefit learning for students (Mayer, 2017; Noetel et al., 2022). However, it should be noted there are individual differences in the degree of benefit, such as students with lower levels of background knowledge tend to have more benefit from adding visuals to verbal information compared to their peers with higher levels of background knowledge (Mayer, 2017). This is distinct from learning styles in that certain students learn better than others in multimodal instruction, but there is not a crossover in which students receive harm or benefit from multimodal instruction. Individuals who support learning styles have been found to also support multimodal instruction (Nancekivell et al., 2021). However, matching instruction to learning styles is more time-consuming as it involves assessing for styles and purposefully assigning modalities, rather than providing multiple options available for all students.

There are concerns that matching instruction to learning styles relates to psychological essentialism, which is the belief that categories of people are innate and biologically based (Gelman, 2003; Nancekivell et al., 2020). An essentialist view of learning styles would be that, for example, visual learners are born with a predisposition to learning visually and that this limits what they can learn through other modalities. Indeed, essentialist and non-essentialist believers in learning styles have been identified (Nancekivell et al., 2020). Essentialist belief in learning styles may explain why visual learners are perceived as more intelligent and better performing academically than kinesthetic, “hands on,” learners (Sun et al., 2023). Relatedly, learners who are told they have a particular style may have a self-fulfilling prophecy in which they believe they can only learn in a particular modality and subsequently do not develop necessary skills in modalities outside of their style (Vasquez, 2009).

Given the resources involved and potential consequences relevant to psychological essentialism, learning styles would logically need to demonstrate remarkable efficacy to justify their use in education. In a review of learning styles efficacy, a team of cognitive scientists focused on student learning explained the criteria for validating the matching or meshing hypothesis (Pashler et al., 2008). One is to categorize learners based on a measure of learning style into at least two groups. A second is that participants need to be randomly assigned to receive instruction in a minimum of two methods (e.g., visual compared to auditory information). Learners need to be assessed in the same manner across styles and conditions. Finally, there needs to be a crossover in which there is an interaction between the learning style group and instruction in which matched instruction has higher learning gains than unmatched instruction for each of the learning style groups. This avoids the possibility that the instruction intended to be matched for a particular style is simply better across style groups. For example, college students who were prompted to visualize statements (visual matching) remembered more statements than their peers who were prompted to consider the sounds in the statements (auditory matching) across learning style categories (Cuevas and Dawson, 2018).

The review by Pashler et al. (2008) concluded that there was a lack of empirical evidence to support matching instruction to student’s learning styles that met their criteria for validating the matching hypothesis. Since this time, there have been other reviews similarly concluding that there is a lack of empirical support for matching instruction to students’ learning styles (Cuevas, 2015; Klitmøller, 2015; Aslaksen and Lorås, 2018). However, there has not been a meta-analysis aggregating effects across studies to provide an estimate of magnitude. Such an approach provides more precision that can be deduced from individual studies and more power to detect effects that may be provided by a single study sample (Deeks et al., 2023). Moreover, meta-analyses may help resolve controversies based on conflicting study findings (Deeks et al., 2023).

Potential moderators

The modality of instruction for matching to learning styles should be considered when considering effects. For example, verbalizer or read/write learners may have their matched instruction involve reading and auditory learners would receive the same information aurally (e.g., Rogowsky et al., 2015, 2020; Lehmann and Seufert, 2020). However, reading comprehension is somewhat better than listening comprehension for inferential understanding in which readers need to connect ideas from the text (Clinton-Lisell, 2022). However, listening may be more effective than reading when accompanied by relevant visual representations, such as pictures or diagrams (Noetel et al., 2022). In addition, non-verbal images (pictures) tend to be remembered better than the same information presented in words (Paivio and Csapo, 1973).

The modality of the assessment should be considered as a potential moderator. Pashler et al.’s (2008) criteria understandably require the learning assessment to be the same modality in order to make comparisons between matched and unmatched instructions based on learning styles. However, this typically involves one method of instruction being in the same modality as the assessment and the comparison method of instruction being in a modality different than the assessment. For example, a listening task would be considered matched for auditory learners and a reading task would be considered matched for read/write or verbal learners and the assessment would be in writing, which is the same modality as the matched instruction for read/write or verbal learners. It is possible that there is a modality-match effect in which having the same modality at learning and assessment would affect results (Mulligan and Osborn, 2009). Encoding and producing information in the same modality may be easier than in different modalities (Staudigl and Hanslmayr, 2019), and subsequently, whether the instruction modality and assessment modality were the same should be considered.

Experimental studies comparing instructions matching and unmatched to learning styles have been conducted with between-subjects and within-subjects designs. With a between-subjects design, participants are in separate groups and only experience one condition. In the case of learning styles, participants would be placed in a group to either receive instruction matched or unmatched to their categorized learning style. An advantage to between-subject designs is that participants are unaware of conditions they were not assigned to thereby preventing carryover effects from other conditions as well as practice effects (Charness et al., 2012). However, different individuals are compared by condition, and subsequently, prior group differences could confound effects thought to be due to condition (Gray et al., 2003; Adesope et al., 2017). In contrast, a within-subjects design involves participants experiencing both instruction matched and instruction unmatched to their learning style with different materials and counterbalanced to prevent order effects. With a within-subjects design, each participant serves as their own control, which prevents prior group differences at baseline to confound results (Charness et al., 2012). Because these research designs are comparable, but not identical, it is recommended that the study design be tested as a moderator in meta-analyses (Borenstein et al., 2009).

Study quality is an important consideration in meta-analyses as it is possible for treatment effects to vary as a function of study quality (Sterne et al., 2001; Feeley, 2020). However, removing low-quality studies from analyses may lead to missing valuable data and meta-analyses should strive to be as inclusive as possible to have an accurate understanding of the accumulated evidence (Weaver, 2011; Feeley, 2020). Narrow inclusion criteria themselves may bias meta-analytic findings. Moreover, studies in social sciences and education (such as the ones for the current meta-analysis) tend to receive low-quality ratings due to methodological details (particularly internal consistency) not being reported (Singer and Alexander, 2017; Feeley, 2020). However, the potential influence of study quality should be considered by coding the quality of each study using predetermined quality criteria and including study quality as a moderator to assess its potential contribution to varying effects (Pigott and Polanin, 2020; see Austin et al., 2019; Zhu et al., 2021; Lam and Zhou, 2022).

The current study

The purpose of this study was to conduct a meta-analysis of matching instruction to modality learning styles. In doing so, the criteria from Pashler et al. (2008) are generally followed. One exception is that non-randomized quasi-experiments are included given the valuable information they provide due to their external validity in education research (Waddington et al., 2022). Three research questions guide this inquiry:

1. What is the aggregated effect of matching instruction to learning styles compared to unmatched instruction on learning outcomes?

2. How frequent is the crossover of matching instruction by style? In other words, is there an interaction indicating benefits to matched instruction over unmatched instruction for at least two of the styles examined?

3. Does the study design (between or within subjects), type of styles, modality of instruction, or study quality moderate the effects of matching instruction to learning styles?

Methods

The data extracted from the included studies and R code used for analyses are available on the Open Science Framework (Clinton-Lisell, 2023b).

Author positionality

Following guidance from Castillo and Babb (2024), information about the authors’ backgrounds and identities is shared in this section.

The first author learned a cognitive approach to educational psychology during her doctoral and postdoctoral studies. During these times, she was taught that there was a lack of empirical evidence to support the concept of learning styles and that it was a common myth of education. Furthermore, she is aware that learning styles have origins rooted in ethnocentrism in which white scholars developed the concept based on condescending attitudes toward children of color (Fallace, 2019). As a white woman, this is a history she works to be mindful of not repeating.

The second author has a master’s degree in school counseling and was influenced by behavioral and school counseling theories. The career aspects of school counseling education supported the use of learning style inventories during the time she received her training. As a researcher, the second author became aware of learning styles research that did not support the career education practices being utilized in the educational setting. The second author shifted practices in her work away from using learning style inventories as part of career education because of the current research on the topic. As a white woman and first-generation college student, she works to be mindful of the social/cultural underpinnings that could influence the understanding of research in learning styles.

Inclusion criteria

Following Pashler et al.’ (2008) guidelines, studies for the learning styles meta-analysis were included if they met the following criteria: (1) participants were categorized in at least two types of learning styles (e.g., visual and auditory), (2) there was at least one condition with instruction and/or learning materials matching to the participants’ learning styles and at least one condition with instruction and/or learning materials not matching to the participants’ learning styles, (3) the unmatched condition for one type of learning style was considered a matched condition for another learning style (so that a crossover interaction could be examined), (4) there was a measurement of learning that was identical across conditions and styles, (5) the study was disseminated in English because of the linguistic skills of the research team, and (6) descriptive statistics were reported to calculate effect sizes or the author of the study provided these upon request.

Systematic search

The first step in the systematic search for relevant articles included a broad search of the databases Web of Science, Scopus, PsycInfo/EBSCOhost, ERIC, and ProQuest Dissertations and Theses using the search terms such as “learning style*” and “learning preference*.” Dissertations and theses were important to include in the search as they are less likely to be influenced by publication bias in which journal articles are more likely to get published when reporting significant results (Paez, 2017). A total of 6,299 citations were found (see Figure 1 for a flow chart of the systematic search process). After duplicates were removed, 1,810 remained. These citations were screened based on titles and abstracts by at least two researchers working independently using Abstrackr (Wallace et al., 2012). Based on this screening, 40 reports were selected for full-text screening. Of these studies, 12 were selected for inclusion (see Figure 1 for reasons for exclusion). A backwards search of the citations in these 12 reports was conducted but did not yield additional studies. A forwards search of the 12 reports yielded an additional 8 reports. The citations of previous reviews were examined (Pashler et al., 2008; Cuevas, 2015; Aslaksen and Lorås, 2018; Dinsmore et al., 2022), which yielded one more report. This led to a total of 21 reports of 21 independent studies in this meta-analysis.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart of the systematic review process.

Coding

To prepare the studies for analyses, two researchers coded the methodological and bibliographic information about each study (see Table 1; κ = 0.89). Specifically, the sample, study design, learning styles examined, measures of learning, content of instruction/materials, and assessment were recorded to describe studies (see Appendix A for codebook). Study quality was determined based on What Works Clearinghouse (2022) criteria and categorized as meeting standards, meeting standards with reservations, or not meeting standards (see Appendix A for details). Based on these standards, a study must be a randomized experiment to meet standards (although not all randomized experiments meet standards). In randomized experiments, the chance of students being the control or intervention should be equal. In contrast, quasi-experiments involve naturally occurring groups, typically classes in educational research, or controls matched through propensity score matching or regression discontinuity design. Whether a study had randomization (for between subjects) or counterbalancing (for within subjects) is noted in the summary of studies in Table 1. Other study quality criteria such as the face validity for each outcome measure, reliability standards for each outcome measure, and whether there was consistent data collection across conditions are reported in Appendix Table B1. As can be seen in Appendix Table B2, five studies were determined to WWC standards and the remainder did not meet WWC standards.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of studies.

Statistical procedures

The effect sizes for each learning outcome comparing matched and unmatched instruction were calculated. Hedges’ g was used as an effect size calculated using Meta-Essential tools (Suurmond et al., 2017). A positive Hedges’ g indicates better learning outcomes for matched than unmatched instruction. To account for multiple effect sizes within each study, a robust variance estimation (RVE) was used. An RVE is a statistical technique that accounts for dependencies within studies while still allowing for the unique contribution of each effect size to be considered (Tanner-Smith et al., 2016). Each of the study effect sizes is shown in Table 2, and a forest plot is in Figure 2. Learning outcomes indicating a crossover interaction as articulated in Pashler et al. (2008) in which at least two styles had higher learning outcomes with matched instruction are bolded in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Effect sizes with variances and number of participants.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot of effect sizes.

Results

The overall main effect of matching instruction to learning styles on learning outcomes was estimated using RVE based on 21 studies and 101 effect sizes and assumed dependency (intercorrelation of dependent effects within studies) of ρ = 0.8. The findings indicated an overall positive effect on learning outcomes for matching instruction to learning styles compared to unmatched instruction, g = 0.32, SE = 0.12, 95% CI = [0.07, 0.57], p = 0.01. There was substantial variability with a τ2 of 0.77 and I2 of 91.17. A sensitivity analysis was conducted with a range of dependent effect size correlations. As can be seen in Table 3, the effect was consistent across assumed dependencies.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Sensitivity analyses for the assumed dependency of effect sizes.

Publication bias

Publication bias was examined to see whether there was overreporting of positive effects. A funnel plot was generated using the “metafor” package in R (Viechtbauer, 2010; see Figure 3). Based on a visual inspection of the funnel plot, the distribution of effect sizes was approximately symmetrical with smaller and larger studies having similar distances away from the mean (indicated by the vertical line; Lin and Chu, 2018). Egger’s test of the intercept was not significant, b = −0.058, 95% CI [−0.52, 0.41], p = 0.11. Taken together, the funnel plot and Egger’s test indicate that publication bias does not appear to be the reason for the positive effect of matching instruction to learning styles.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Funnel plot of effect sizes.

Crossover interactions

The number of crossover interactions (at least two styles benefited from matched instruction within a learning outcome measure) was calculated. Based on the tally of the bolded effect sizes in Table 2, there are 11 learning outcome measures in which matched instruction benefited at least 2 learning styles as indicated by Hedges’ g greater than 0. This was out of a total of 42 learning outcome measures that were compared indicating that 26.19% had the type of crossover interaction necessary to support the meshing hypothesis as articulated by Pashler et al. (2008).

As indicated in Figure 1, five that had their full texts screened did not have sufficient statistics to calculate the effect sizes reported. In addition, seven reports identified through other searches did not have sufficient statistics to calculate effect sizes but otherwise met inclusion criteria. Based on the descriptions of the findings of these 12 studies in Tables 3, 4 of these studies indicated a crossover interaction (25.00%). Therefore, the findings from the studies without sufficient statistics reported appear to be similar to the studies included in the meta-analysis in terms of crossover interactions.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Studies without sufficient descriptive statistics reported to calculate effect sizes.

Moderator analysis

To estimate whether these potential moderators varied the effect of matching instruction to learning style, the package “robumeta” in R was used (Fisher and Tipton, 2015). The study design (between or within subjects), modality of instruction/materials (visual, verbal, or auditory), whether the assessment was in the same modality as the instruction, and study quality (does not meet WWC standards or meets WWC standards) were all coefficients estimated in the meta-regression model. For consistency across studies, “visual” matched instruction that was text-based was coded as “verbal or read/write.” Based on the output of the meta-regression model, none of the moderators were significant (see Table 5). Therefore, it is unclear what the source of variability in the aggregate findings is.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Meta-regression model.

Sensitivity analysis

A sensitivity analysis was conducted to examine whether altering the inclusion criteria changed the results. There was only one study in which instruction was adopted to a kinesthetic learning style (Papanagnou et al., 2016). Removing this study from the RVE analyses indicated an overall aggregated benefit of matched instructions to learning styles compared to unmatched, g = 0.34, SE = 0.13, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.60], p = 0.01 with 20 studies and 98 effect sizes. This finding is similar to the findings when the kinesthetic learning intervention was included. There were two studies that were quasi-experiments without random assignment (Moussa-Inaty et al., 2019; Mujtaba et al., 2022). This was a concern given that they did not demonstrate baseline equivalence in the study quality coding (see Appendix Table B1). Therefore, an RVE was conducted with the two quasi-experiments removed from the analyses. The results of the RVE were similar to the quasi-experiments removed in that there was an overall aggregated benefit of matched instruction to learning styles compared to unmatched, g = 0.33, SE = 0.13, 95% CI = [0.05, 0.61], p = 0.02 with 19 studies and 91 effect sizes.

Discussion

Educational researchers consider the concept of better learning through matching instruction to learning styles to be a neuromyth that completely lacks empirical evidence (Brown, 2023). However, the findings from this meta-analysis indicated a small, but statistically reliable benefit of matching instruction based on learning styles. This aligns with the majority of educators’ perspectives (Dekker et al., 2012; Nancekivell et al., 2020; Eitel et al., 2021) but conflicts with the conclusions of previous reviews by educational researchers (Cuevas, 2015; An and Carr, 2017; Aslaksen and Lorås, 2018; Dinsmore et al., 2022; Yan and Fralick, 2022). What distinguishes this meta-analysis from previous reviews is (1) its singular focus on studies comparing instruction matched and unmatched to modality learning styles and (2) its systematic approach to gathering relevant studies and aggregating findings. The lack of evidence noted in previous reviews may be due to a lack of power in individual studies. The cumulative evidence of aggregated effects appeared to have sufficient power to detect an effect. However, the majority of learning outcomes did not indicate a crossover interaction that would validate accommodation to learning styles. However, a non-trivial minority of learning outcomes did indicate the crossover interaction indicative of supporting the matching hypothesis based on Pashler et al. (2008). An important caveat is that most of the studies indicating a crossover interaction did not meet quality standards as determined by the What Works Clearinghouse (2022). Taken together, these findings may be interpreted that it is too much of an overreach to insist learning styles should be incorporated into instructional practices.

Given the time and resources required for matching instruction to learning styles coupled with the potential for harm through psychological essentialism (Vasquez, 2009; Fallace, 2019, 2023a,b; Nancekivell et al., 2020; Sun et al., 2023), we stated in the literature review that accommodating instruction to learning styles would need to have substantial benefits to mitigate their potential for harm. To consider this issue, it may be helpful to compare the effect size noted in this meta-analysis (Hedges’ g = 0.32) with those from other methods of adapting instruction. For example, the modality effect, in which listening to verbal information while viewing visual representations, rather than reading the same verbal information alongside visuals has Hedges’ g of 0.70 (Noetel et al., 2022); that is, the benefit of listening, rather than reading verbal information that accompanies visual representations across all students, appears to have twice the effect than was noted in this meta-analysis matching instruction to learning styles and would be less time-consuming and expensive to implement. Removing interesting or irrelevant information included with the lesson has Hedges’ g of 0.33 (Sundararajan and Adesope, 2020). Segmenting instruction into meaningful learner-paced units has a benefit of Hedges’ g of 0.32 compared to continuous information (Rey et al., 2019). Finally, an overall application of multimedia principles to learning has Hedge’s g of 0.28 (Noetel et al., 2022).

When examining an overview of meta-analysis on multimedia design for learning, accommodating instruction based on learning styles in the current meta-analysis is generally about the same size or smaller than various multimedia designs (e.g., signaling important information, animation, and pleasant colors/anthropomorphic; Noetel et al., 2022). However, all of the multimedia design principles reviewed involved having students each receive the same instructional changes, whereas accommodating instruction based on learning styles by definition involves at least two types of instruction (Noetel et al., 2022). In addition, 85% of the studies in the current meta-analysis did not include all participants in the final sample because their learning styles scores did not allow for confident categorization and matching/unmatching to instructional modality. Therefore, we, the authors, deeply question whether the found benefits of learning styles in this meta-analysis warrant accommodating instruction, especially for widespread use. Based on previous studies, well-structured instructional design may be more effective across all students and would involve less time than accommodating to learning styles.

Participant expectations may be relevant to interpreting the findings from the studies in this meta-analysis (Vasquez, 2009; Sun et al., 2023). Generally, participants were asked about their modality preferences and then engaged in a learning activity shortly thereafter (only three studies specifically stated participants were asked to complete learning style measures after the learning activity; see Table 1). If participants were aware of their learning styles prior to engaging in a task that matched or unmatched their style, they may have had different expectations for success and engagement that affected their learning (Vasquez, 2009). For example, one study categorized students based on fake/induced learning styles (Moser and Zumbach, 2015). In other words, students took a learning styles assessment and were told (incorrectly) that they scored as visualizers or verbalizers. Students scored higher when their instructional materials “matched” their fake/induced learning style compared to the unmatched conditions, but there were no benefits to matching based on their actual categorizations based on the learning styles assessment (Moser and Zumbach, 2015). This is described in the learning styles genesis model in which appraisal and decision processes based on external feedback about learning styles along with previous experiences with modalities shape learning outcomes (Moser and Zumbach, 2018). Moreover, participants may have had a situational interest in the content triggered by immediately receiving instruction in their stated preferences (Bernacki and Walkington, 2018). This likely would not continue long term as maintained interest requires a personal connection (Høgheim and Reber, 2017).

Learning styles may be conflated with modality-specific skills (An and Carr, 2017). In other words, participants may simply be better at reading if they indicate a read/write style or listening if they indicate an auditory style. This results in a jangle fallacy in which two similar constructs (e.g., modality skill and learning styles) are considered different because they have different terminology (Kelley, 1927; Beisley, 2023). It should be noted, however, that if this is the case, skills in a modality may have been developed because of preferences in that modality, which would, in turn, lead to more practice and more skill in a particular modality. It would be extremely difficult to disentangle the initiating factor in this (hypothetical) perpetual cycle of skill and preference. However, more inquiry into fake/induced learning styles such as that by Moser and Zumbach (2015) would be a useful means of testing whether the skill is confounded with style given that fake/induced styles would be randomly assigned and subsequently modality skills should be similar across “styles.”

A key feature of the matching hypothesis is a crossover interaction in which matched instruction benefits learning for only the group for which it is matched. This matched instruction differs depending on the learning style of the student. In the current meta-analysis, approximately one-fourth of the learning measures indicated a crossover interaction in which there were positive effect sizes for matched instruction for two different learning styles (Kassaian, 2007; Chen and Sun, 2012; Hazra et al., 2013; Kam et al., 2020; Lehmann and Seufert, 2020; Chui et al., 2021; Tadayonifar et al., 2021). This raises the question of what characteristics of these studies and learning measures may be responsible for the crossover interaction. However, these studies are quite heterogeneous. Samples include young adult college students, elementary school students, and aircraft pilot trainees. The learning styles and their inventories varied and included the Styles of Processing Scale (Childers et al., 1985), VARK questionnaire (Fleming, 2001), Index of Learning Styles Scores (Felder and Soloman, 1997), and Caption Reliance Test (Leveridge and Yang, 2014). Content and learning measures were also varied such as memory and recall of history (Hazra et al., 2013), multiple-choice questions about energy education (Chen and Sun, 2012), and flight simulator performance (Chui et al., 2021). Therefore, there does not seem to be any consistent feature across these studies based on the information coded for this meta-analysis that would elucidate the mechanism behind the crossover interaction. Subsequently, the lack of understanding of what circumstances could foster a crossover interaction is an additional reason for caution in implementing the matched instruction for learning styles. Without knowing what features are conducive to effective matched instruction, it is extremely difficult to have effectively matched instruction across identified learning styles.

Implications

We advise extreme caution if using the findings from this meta-analysis to justify matching instruction to learning styles. If choosing to incorporate learning styles, then learning styles should never be ascribed as a feature of a cultural group, especially by individuals outside of that group, as this leads to unwarranted and potentially harmful expectations based on group membership (Gutiérrez and Rogoff, 2003; Fallace, 2023a,b). Moreover, learning style interventions are costly in terms of both time and money (Pashler et al., 2008). By definition, matching instruction based on learning styles requires multiple versions of instruction or materials to be developed.

If learning styles are incorporated into education, we strongly recommend that they be implemented in the context of multimodality for learning. By providing information in more than one modality, such as text with visuals, the same materials could arguably appeal to both verbal and visual learning styles while grounded in theories of human cognition such as dual coding (Noetel et al., 2022). Engaging multiple senses is generally beneficial for learning (Nguyen et al., 2022). In addition, providing students with audio-assisted text may also be beneficial, particularly for learning beyond one’s native language (Clinton-Lisell, 2023a), and logically appeal to auditory and verbal preferences. Not only is multimodality known to be effective for learning but even individuals with strong essentialist beliefs about learning styles support multimodal learning as effective (Nancekivell et al., 2021). Moreover, offering multiple modalities for learning provides an inclusive education for students with perceptual disabilities to have access to the content (Thomas et al., 2015; Griful-Freixenet et al., 2017).

Limitations and future directions

Limitations to the studies were included in the meta-analysis. As indicated in the study quality coding, the majority of the outcome measures did not have reliability metrics reported. The lack of information about reliability, as noted in the study quality scoring, leads to challenges in determining the validity of the findings. Indeed, the primary issue with study quality is due to an inability to assess reliability due to a lack of reporting across multiple studies. Unfortunately, a lack of reporting reliability statistics is a common issue across multiple social science and education disciplines (Barry et al., 2014; Lovejoy et al., 2014; Han, 2016; Parsons et al., 2019; Flake, 2021). This illustrates the need to ensure that reliability is reported throughout the peer review and publication process. Indeed, publication reporting standards in psychology, through the American Psychological Association (Appelbaum et al., 2018), state that the reliability of measures should be reported.

The studies were all single sessions in duration and subsequently claims about long-term effects cannot be determined from the meta-analysis. Moreover, there was substantial variation in the findings across outcomes that was not explained in the meta-regression. This could be due to insufficient power to identify moderators in the meta-regression analyses (Schmidt, 2017). Furthermore, the studies were limited to those disseminated in English due to the linguistic limitations of the research team. It is possible the inclusion of more languages would have led to different outcomes. In addition, all but two of the reviewed studies were from journal articles. Although the publication analyses did not indicate publication bias, it is still an issue to consider given that only two studies were from the gray literature in which non-significant findings are more likely to be reported (Cairo et al., 2020). There is also possible bias when considering studies as several authors were contacted with requests for data to calculate effect sizes, but only some of the authors provided this information. There may be response bias regarding the findings that were calculated based on author-provided data. However, it should be noted that authors frequently do not respond to requests for data (Tedersoo et al., 2021).

The studies in this meta-analysis all categorized their participants based on learning styles, but the methods of categorization varied. There were a range of measures used and the cutoff for categorization of learning styles differed by study as well. This makes the generalizability of the findings challenging. Moreover, there was substantial variability in the outcome measures. Only 21 studies were identified that met the criteria for testing the matching hypothesis and reported sufficient statistics to conduct effect sizes. In particular, there were not enough studies to examine whether having the learning styles assessment before or after the learning activity varied the findings. This is unfortunate given the concerns about self-fulfilling prophecies and findings from Moser and Zumbach (2015) with fake, induced learning styles.

Conclusion

Learning styles are a controversial topic in education. In this meta-analysis, we sought to inform the controversy with aggregated findings based on a comprehensive search for studies. An overall small, positive effect was noted. However, this aggregated effect should be interpreted with caution given that most studies did not indicate a crossover interaction. Such a crossover interaction would have been necessary to support the claim that matching instruction to learning styles benefits students from different learning styles. Given the high amount of variability in the findings and infrequent crossover interactions, it is far from conclusive that there is actually a benefit to matching the modality of instruction to students’ learning styles. Teaching with multiple modalities may be preferable to the costly and labor-intensive practice of matching instruction to learning styles given the empirical evidence for benefits across students for multimodal instruction.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/5heum/.

Author contributions

VC-L: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. CL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. We thank the University of North Dakota Alumni Foundation for supporting this research by VC-L through the Rose Isabella Kelly Fischer Professorship.

Acknowledgments

We thank Maylynn Riding In for her assistance screening titles and abstracts.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1428732/full#supplementary-material

References

* included in meta-analysis

Google Scholar

Adesope, O. O., Trevisan, D. A., and Sundararajan, N. (2017). Rethinking the use of tests: a meta-analysis of practice testing. Rev. Educ. Res. 87, 659–701. doi: 10.3102/0034654316689306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

An, D., and Carr, M. (2017). Learning styles theory fails to explain learning and achievement: recommendations for alternative approaches. Personal. Individ. Differ. 116, 410–416. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.050

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Appelbaum, M., Cooper, H., Kline, R. B., Mayo-Wilson, E., Nezu, A. M., and Rao, S. M. (2018). Journal article reporting standards for quantitative research in psychology: the APA publications and communications board task force report. Am. Psychol. 73, 3–25. doi: 10.1037/amp0000191

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslaksen, K., and Lorås, H. (2018). The modality-specific learning style hypothesis: a mini-review. Front. Psychol. 9:1538. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01538

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Aslaksen, K., and Lorås, H. (2019). Matching instruction with modality-specific learning style: effects on immediate recall and working memory performance. Educ. Sci., 9:32. doi: 10.3390/educsci9010032

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Austin, C. R., Wanzek, J., Scammacca, N. K., Vaughn, S., Gesel, S. A., Donegan, R. E., et al. (2019). The relationship between study quality and the effects of supplemental reading interventions: a meta-analysis. Except. Child. 85, 347–366. doi: 10.1177/0014402918796164

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bareither, M. L., Arbel, V., Growe, M., Muszczynski, E., Rudd, A., and Marone, J. R. (2013). Clay modeling versus written modules as effective interventions in understanding human anatomy. Anat. Sci. Educ. 6, 170–176. doi: 10.1002/ase.1321

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, A. E., Chaney, B., Piazza-Gardner, A. K., and Chavarria, E. A. (2014). Validity and reliability reporting practices in the field of health education and behavior: a review of seven journals. Health Educ. Behav. 41, 12–18. doi: 10.1177/1090198113483139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beisley, A. H. (2023). The jingle-jangle of approaches to learning in prekindergarten: a construct with too many names. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 35:79. doi: 10.1007/s10648-023-09796-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernacki, M. L., and Walkington, C. (2018). The role of situational interest in personalized learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 110:864. doi: 10.1037/edu0000250

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., and Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Hoboken, New Jersey, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Google Scholar

Bouchey, B., Castek, J., and Thygeson, J. (2021). “Multimodal learning” in Innovative learning environments in STEM higher education: Opportunities, challenges, and looking forward. eds. J. Ryoo and K. Winkelmann (Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 35–54.

Google Scholar

Brown, S. B. R. E. (2023). The persistence of matching teaching and learning styles: a review of the ubiquity of this neuromyth, predictors of its endorsement, and recommendations to end it. Front. Educ. 8:1147498. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1147498

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Burns, L. (n.d.). Visual versus auditory processing preference and mode of presentation: Differences in confidence, attention, and recall performance in online learning. Undergraduate capstone project at the University of Missouri-Columbia. Available at: https://osf.io/5heum/?view_only=510f56642e974fbeb000dc8a8b214b68

Google Scholar

Cairo, A. H., Green, J. D., Forsyth, D. R., Behler, A. M. C., and Raldiris, T. L. (2020). Gray (literature) matters: evidence of selective hypothesis reporting in social psychological research. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 46, 1344–1362. doi: 10.1177/0146167220903896

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Calcaterra, A., Antonietti, A., and Underwood, J. (2005). Cognitive style, hypermedia navigation and learning. Comput. Educ. 44, 441–457. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2004.04.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castillo, W., and Babb, N. (2024). Transforming the future of quantitative educational research: a systematic review of enacting QuantCrit. Race Ethn. Educ. 27, 1–21. doi: 10.1080/13613324.2023.2248911

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Charness, G., Gneezy, U., and Kuhn, M. A. (2012). Experimental methods: between-subject and within-subject design. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 81, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2011.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Chen, C.Y. (2020). The influence of representational formats and learner modality preferences on instructional efficiency using interactive video tutorials. J. Educ. Train. 7, 1–17. doi: 10.5296/jet.v7i2.17415

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Chen, C.M., and Sun, Y.C. (2012). Assessing the effects of different multimedia materials on emotions and learning performance for visual and verbal style learners. Comput. Educ., 59, 1273–1285. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.05.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Childers, T. L., Houston, M. J., and Heckler, S. E. (1985). Measurement of individual differences in visual versus verbal information processing. J. Consum. Res. 12, 125–134. doi: 10.1086/208501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chislet, V., and Chapman, A. (2005). VAK learning styles self-test.

Google Scholar

*Chui, T. K., Molesworth, B. R., and Bromfield, M. A. (2021). Feedback and student learning: matching learning and teaching style to improve student pilot performance. Int. J. Aerospace Psychol., 31, 71–86. doi: 10.1080/24721840.2020.1847650

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clinton-Lisell, V. (2022). Listening ears or reading eyes: a meta-analysis of reading and listening comprehension comparisons. Rev. Educ. Res. 92, 543–582. doi: 10.3102/00346543211060871

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clinton-Lisell, V. (2023a). Does reading while listening to text improve comprehension compared to reading only? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Theory Pract. 34, 133–155.

Google Scholar

Clinton-Lisell, V. (2023b). Learning styles meta-analysis. Available at: https://osf.io/5heum/?view_only=510f56642e974fbeb000dc8a8b214b68

Google Scholar

Cohen, A.D., Oxford, R.L., and Chi, J.C. (2019) Learning style survey: Assessing your own learning styles. Available at: http://carla.umn.edu/maxsa/documents/LearningStyleSurvey_MAXSA_IG.pdf

Google Scholar

Cuevas, J. (2015). Is learning styles-based instruction effective? A comprehensive analysis of recent research on learning styles. Theory Res. Educ. 13, 308–333. doi: 10.1177/1477878515606621

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Cuevas, J., and Dawson, B. L. (2018). A test of two alternative cognitive processing models: learning styles and dual coding. Theory Res. Educ., 16, 40–64. doi: 10.1177/1477878517731450

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deeks, J. J., Higgins, J. P. T., and Altman, D. G. (2023). “Chapter 10: analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses” in Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.4. eds. J. P. T. Higgins, J. Thomas, J. Chandler, M. Cumpston, T. Li, and M. J. Page (Cochrane Statistical Methods Group).

Google Scholar

Dekker, S., Lee, N., Howard-Jones, P., and Jolles, J. (2012). Neuromyths in education: prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers. Front. Psychol. 3:429. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinsmore, D. L., Fryer, L. K., and Parkinson, M. M. (2022). The learning styles hypothesis is false, but there are patterns of student characteristics that are useful. Theory Pract. 61, 418–428. doi: 10.1080/00405841.2022.2107333

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, R. (1990). Understanding the Dunn and Dunn learning styles model and the need for individual diagnosis and prescription. J. Read. Writ. Learn. Disabil. Int. 6, 223–247. doi: 10.1080/0748763900060303

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, R., and Dunn, K. (1975). Educator’s self-teaching guide to indiviudalizing instructional programs. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, USA: Parker Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Eitel, A., Prinz, A., Kollmer, J., Niessen, L., Russow, J., Ludäscher, M., et al. (2021). The misconceptions about multimedia learning questionnaire: An empirical evaluation study with teachers and student teachers. Psychol. Learn. Teach. 20, 420–444. doi: 10.1177/14757257211028723

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fajari, L. E., Sarwanto,, and Chumdari,. (2020). Improving elementary school’s critical thinking skills through three different learning media viewed from learning styles. J. E Learn. Knowledge Soc. 16, 55–65. doi: 10.20368/1971-8829/1135193

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallace, T. (2019). The ethnocentric origins of the learning style idea. Educ. Res. 48, 349–355. doi: 10.3102/0013189X19858086

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallace, T. (2023a). Herman Witkin and the rise and fall of the black learning style idea, 1960–2003. Teach. Coll. Rec. 125, 67–94. doi: 10.1177/01614681231178589

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallace, T. (2023b). The long origins of the visual, auditory, and kinesthetic learning style typology, 1921–2001. Hist. Psychol. 26, 334–354. doi: 10.1037/hop0000240

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeley, T. H. (2020). Assessing study quality in Meta-analysis. Hum. Commun. Res. 46, 334–342. doi: 10.1093/hcr/hqaa001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Felder, R. M., and Soloman, B. A. (1997). Index of learning styles questionnaire. Cochrane Statistical Methods Group.

Google Scholar

Fiorina, L., Antonietti, A., Colombo, B., and Bartolomeo, A. (2007). Thinking style, browsing primes and hypermedia navigation. Comput. Educ. 49, 916–941. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2005.12.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, Z., and Tipton, E. (2015). Robumeta: An R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis. ArXiv Preprint ArXiv 1503:02220. doi: 10.48550/arXiv.1503.02220

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flake, J. K. (2021). Strengthening the foundation of educational psychology by integrating construct validation into open science reform. Educ. Psychol. 56, 132–141. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2021.1898962

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, N. D. (2001). Teaching and learning styles. Christchurch, New Zealand: VARK strategies

Google Scholar

Fleming, N. D., and Mills, C. (1992). Not another inventory, rather a catalyst for reflection. Improve Acad. 11, 137–155. doi: 10.1002/j.2334-4822.1992.tb00213.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fong, C. J., Krou, M. R., Johnston-Ashton, K., Hoff, M. A., Lin, S., and Gonzales, C. (2021). LASSI's great adventure: a meta-analysis of the learning and study strategies inventory and academic outcomes. Educ. Res. Rev. 34:100407. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2021.100407

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ge, Z. G. (2021). Does mismatch between learning media preference and received learning media bring a negative impact on academic performance? An experiment with e-learners. Interact. Learn. Environ., 29, 790–806. doi: 10.1080/10494820.2019.1612449

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Gray, N. S., Snowden, R. J., Peoples, M., Hemsley, D. R., and Gray, J. A. (2003). A demonstration of within-subjects latent inhibition in the human: limitations and advantages. Behav. Brain Res. 138, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00181-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Griful-Freixenet, J., Struyven, K., Verstichele, M., and Andries, C. (2017). Higher education students with disabilities speaking out: perceived barriers and opportunities of the universal Design for Learning framework. Disabil. Soc. 32, 1627–1649. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2017.1365695

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez, K. D., and Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways of learning: individual traits or repertoires of practice. Educ. Res. 32, 19–25. doi: 10.3102/0013189X032005019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C. (2016). Reporting practices of rater reliability in interpreting research: a mixed-methods review of 14 journals (2004-2014). J. Res. Design Statistics Linguist. Commun. Sci. 3, 49–75. doi: 10.1558/jrds.29622

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hazra, A.K., Patnaik, P., and Suar, D. (2013). Relation of modal preference with performance in adaptive hypermedia context: An exploration using visual, verbal and multimedia learning modules. 2013 IEEE fifth international conference on Technology for Education (T4e 2013), 163–166

Google Scholar

Höffler, T. N., and Schwartz, R. N. (2011). Effects of pacing and cognitive style across dynamic and non-dynamic representations. Comput. Educ. 57, 1716–1726. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2011.03.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Høgheim, S., and Reber, R. (2017). Eliciting mathematics interest: new directions for context personalization and example choice. J. Exp. Educ. 85, 597–613. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2016.1268085

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, T.-C. (2019). Do different learning styles make a difference when it comes to creativity? An empirical study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 100, 252–257. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kam, E. F., Liu, Y.-T., and Tseng, W.-T. (2020). Effects of modality preference and working memory capacity on captioned videos in enhancing L2 listening outcomes. ReCALL, 32, 213–230. doi: 10.1017/S0958344020000014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kassaian, Z. (2007). Learning styles and lexical presentation modes. Estud. Linguistica Inglesa Aplicada, 7, 53–78

Google Scholar

Kelley, T. L. (1927). Interpretation of educational measurements. Chicago, Illinois, USA: World Book.

Google Scholar

Kirschner, P. A. (2017). Stop propagating the learning styles myth. Comput. Educ. 106, 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2016.12.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klitmøller, J. (2015). Review of the methods and findings in the Dunn and Dunn learning styles model research on perceptual preferences. Nord. Psychol. 67, 2–26. doi: 10.1080/19012276.2014.997783

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoll, A. R., Otani, H., Skeel, R. L., and Van Horn, K. R. (2017). Learning style, judgements of learning, and learning of verbal and visual information. Br. J. Psychol. 108, 544–563. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koć-Januchta, M. M., Höffler, T. N., Eckhardt, M., and Leutner, D. (2019). Does modality play a role? Visual-verbal cognitive style and multimedia learning. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 35, 747–757. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koć-Januchta, M. M., Höffler, T. N., Prechtl, H., and Leutner, D. (2020). Is too much help an obstacle? Effects of interactivity and cognitive style on learning with dynamic versus non-dynamic visualizations with narrative explanations. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 68, 2971–2990. doi: 10.1007/s11423-020-09822-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kollöffel, B. (2012). Exploring the relation between visualizer–verbalizer cognitive styles and performance with visual or verbal learning material. Comput. Educ. 58, 697–706. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2011.09.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraemer, D. J. M., Schinazi, V. R., Cawkwell, P. B., Tekriwal, A., Epstein, R. A., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2017). Verbalizing, visualizing, and navigating: the effect of strategies on encoding a large-scale virtual environment. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 43, 611–621. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000314

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, K. K. L., and Zhou, M. (2022). Grit and academic achievement: a comparative cross-cultural meta-analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 114, 597–621. doi: 10.1037/edu0000699

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lehmann, J., and Seufert, T. (2020). The interaction between text modality and the learner’s modality preference influences comprehension and cognitive load. Front. Psychol., 10:2820, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02820

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leveridge, A. N., and Yang, J. C. (2014). Learner perceptions of reliance on captions in EFL multimedia listening comprehension. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 27, 545–559. doi: 10.1080/09588221.2013.776968

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, L., and Chu, H. (2018). Quantifying publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 74, 785–794. doi: 10.1111/biom.12817

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovejoy, J., Watson, B. R., Lacy, S., and Riffe, D. (2014). Assessing the reporting of reliability in published content analyses: 1985–2010. Commun. Methods Meas. 8, 207–221. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2014.937528

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyle, K. B., Young, A. S., Heyden, R. J., and McDaniel, M. A. (2023). Matching learning style to instructional format penalizes learning. Computers Edu. Open 5:100143. doi: 10.1016/j.caeo.2023.100143

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Massa, L. J., and Mayer, R. E. (2006). Testing the ATI hypothesis: should multimedia instruction accommodate verbalizer-visualizer cognitive style? Learn. Individ. Differ. 16, 321–335. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2006.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E. (2011). “Chapter three—applying the science of learning to multimedia instruction” in Psychology of learning and motivation. eds. J. P. Mestre and B. H. Ross, vol. 55 (Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Academic Press), 77–108.

Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E. (2017). Using multimedia for e-learning. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 33, 403–423. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12197

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E., and Anderson, R. B. (1992). The instructive animation: helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 444–452. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.444

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E., and Massa, L. J. (2003). Three facets of visual and verbal learners: cognitive ability, cognitive style, and learning preference. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 833–846. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.833

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moser, S., and Zumbach, J. (2015). Explicit and implicit measuring of visual and verbal learning styles. Paper presented at the European Association for Research in learning and instruction (EARLI) conference 2015, 25.-29.08.2015 in Limassol, Cyprus.

Google Scholar

*Moser, S., and Zumbach, J. (2018). Exploring the development and impact of learning styles: An empirical investigation based on explicit and implicit measures. Comput. Educ., 125, 146–157. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2018.05.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Moussa-Inaty, J., Atallah, F., and Causapin, M. (2019). Instructional mode: a better predictor of performance than student preferred learning styles. Int. J. Instr., 12, 17–24. doi: 10.29333/iji.2019.1232a

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Mujtaba, M.S., Parkash, R., Kaur Mehar Singh, M., and Kamyabi Gol, A. (2022). The effect of computer-mediated feedback on L2 accuracy. Does the difference in learners’ perceptual style moderate the effectiveness of the feedback? Comput. Sch., 39, 99–119. doi: 10.1080/07380569.2022.2041891

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulligan, N. W., and Osborn, K. (2009). The modality-match effect in recognition memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35, 564–571. doi: 10.1037/a0014524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nancekivell, S. E., Shah, P., and Gelman, S. A. (2020). Maybe they’re born with it, or maybe it’s experience: toward a deeper understanding of the learning style myth. J. Educ. Psychol. 112, 221–235. doi: 10.1037/edu0000366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nancekivell, S. E., Sun, X., Gelman, S. A., and Shah, P. (2021). A slippery myth: how learning style beliefs shape reasoning about multimodal instruction and related scientific evidence. Cogn. Sci. 45:e13047. doi: 10.1111/cogs.13047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Newton, P. M., and Miah, M. (2017). Evidence-based higher education–is the learning styles ‘myth’ important? Front. Psychol. 8:444. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00444

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, N. N., Mosier, W., Hines, J., and Garnett, W. (2022). Learning styles are out of style: shifting to multimodal learning experiences. Kappa Delta Pi Rec. 58, 70–75. doi: 10.1080/00228958.2022.2039520

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noetel, M., Griffith, S., Delaney, O., Harris, N. R., Sanders, T., Parker, P., et al. (2022). Multimedia design for learning: An overview of reviews with meta-meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 92, 413–454. doi: 10.3102/00346543211052329

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsen, M. K., Stechuchak, K. M., and Steinhauser, K. E. (2019). Comparing internal and external validation in the discovery of qualitative treatment-subgroup effects using two small clinical trials. Contemp. Clin. Trials Commun. 15:100372. doi: 10.1016/j.conctc.2019.100372

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paez, A. (2017). Gray literature: An important resource in systematic reviews. J. Evid. Based Med. 10, 233–240. doi: 10.1111/jebm.12266

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: retrospect and current status. Can. J. Psychol. 45, 255–287. doi: 10.1037/h0084295

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paivio, A., and Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: imagery or dual coding? Cogn. Psychol. 5, 176–206. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90032-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Papanagnou, D., Serrano, A., Barkley, K., Chandra, S., Governatori, N., Piela, N., et al. (2016). Does tailoring instructional style to a medical student’s self-perceived learning style improve performance when teaching intravenous catheter placement? A randomized controlled study. BMC Med. Educ., 16:205. doi: 10.1186/s12909-016-0720-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, S., Kruijt, A. W., and Fox, E. (2019). Psychological science needs a standard practice of reporting the reliability of cognitive-behavioral measurements. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2, 378–395. doi: 10.1177/2515245919879695

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D., and Bjork, R. (2008). Learning styles: concepts and evidence. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 9, 105–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01038.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petticrew, M., Tugwell, P., Kristjansson, E., Oliver, S., Ueffing, E., and Welch, V. (2012). Damned if you do, damned if you don’t: subgroup analysis and equity. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 66, 95–98. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.121095

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pigott, T. D., and Polanin, J. R. (2020). Methodological guidance paper: high-quality Meta-analysis in a systematic review. Rev. Educ. Res. 90, 24–46. doi: 10.3102/0034654319877153

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Sterba, S. K. (2019). Aptitude-by-treatment interactions in research on educational interventions. Except. Child. 85, 248–264. doi: 10.1177/0014402918802803

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, X., and Wang, Y. (2019). Composite interaction tree for simultaneous learning of optimal individualized treatment rules and subgroups. Stat. Med. 38, 2632–2651. doi: 10.1002/sim.8105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rassaei, E. (2018). Computer-mediated textual and audio glosses, perceptual style and L2 vocabulary learning. Lang. Teach. Res., 22, 657–675. doi: 10.1177/1362168817690183

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rassaei, E. (2019). Computer-mediated text-based and audio-based corrective feedback, perceptual style and L2 development. System, 82, 97–110. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2019.03.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, S. K. (2006). Cognitive architectures for multimedia learning. Educ. Psychol. 41, 87–98. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep4102_2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, J. M. (1987). The learning style preferences of ESL students. TESOL Q. 21, 87–111. doi: 10.2307/3586356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rey, G. D., Beege, M., Nebel, S., Wirzberger, M., Schmitt, T. H., and Schneider, S. (2019). A Meta-analysis of the segmenting effect. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 31, 389–419. doi: 10.1007/s10648-018-9456-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riding, R. J. (1991). Cognitive styles analysis. Birmingham, United Kingdom: Learning and Training Technology.

Google Scholar

Riding, R. J., and Ashmore, J. (1980). Verbaliser-imager learning style and children's recall of information presented in pictorial versus written form. Educ. Stud. 6, 141–145. doi: 10.1080/0305569800060204

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Riding, R., and Douglas, G. (1993). The effect of cognitive style and mode of presentation on learning performance. Br. J. Educ. Psychol., 63, 297–307. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1993.tb01059.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riding, R., and Rayner, S. (1998). Cognitive styles and learning strategies: Understanding style differences in learning and behaviour. London, United Kingdom: David Fulton Publishing.

Google Scholar

Riding, R., and Sadler-Smith, E. (1992). Type of instructional material, cognitive style and learning performance. Educ. Stud. 18, 323–340. doi: 10.1080/0305569920180306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rogowsky, B. A., Calhoun, B. M., and Tallal, P. (2015). Matching learning style to instructional method: effects on comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol., 107, 64–78. doi: 10.1037/a0037478

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rogowsky, B. A., Calhoun, B. M., and Tallal, P. (2020). Providing instruction based on students’ learning style preferences does not improve learning. Front. Psychol., 11:164. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00164

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rundle, S., and Dunn, R. (2010). Learning styles: Online learning style assessments and community. Available at: http://www.learningstyles.net

Google Scholar

Sankey, M. D., Birch, D., and Gardiner, M. W. (2011). The impact of multiple representations of content using multimedia on learning outcomes across learning styles and modal preference. Int. J. Educ. Dev. Using Inf. Commun. Technol. 7, 18–35.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, F. L. (2017). Statistical and measurement pitfalls in the use of meta-regression in meta-analysis. Career Dev. Int. 22, 469–476. doi: 10.1108/CDI-08-2017-0136

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, L. M., and Alexander, P. A. (2017). Reading on paper and digitally: what the past decades of empirical research reveal. Rev. Educ. Res. 87, 1007–1041. doi: 10.3102/0034654317722961

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Slack, N., and Norwich, B. (2007). Evaluating the reliability and validity of a learning styles inventory: a classroom-based study. Educ. Res. 49, 51–63. doi: 10.1080/00131880701200765

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., Snow, P., Serry, T., and Hammond, L. (2021). The role of background knowledge in Reading comprehension: a critical review. Read. Psychol. 42, 214–240. doi: 10.1080/02702711.2021.1888348

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Staudigl, T., and Hanslmayr, S. (2019). Reactivation of neural patterns during memory reinstatement supports encoding specificity. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 175–185. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2019.1621825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne, J. A. C., Egger, M., and Smith, G. D. (2001). Investigating and dealing with publication and other biases in meta-analysis. BMJ 323, 101–105. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7304.101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, X., Norton, O., and Nancekivell, S. E. (2023). Beware the myth: learning styles affect parents’, children’s, and teachers’ thinking about children’s academic potential. NPJ Sci. Learn. 8:1. doi: 10.1038/s41539-023-00190-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundararajan, N., and Adesope, O. (2020). Keep it coherent: a Meta-analysis of the seductive details effect. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 32, 707–734. doi: 10.1007/s10648-020-09522-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suurmond, R., van Rhee, H., and Hak, T. (2017). Introduction, comparison and validation of Meta-essentials: a free and simple tool for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 8, 537–553. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1260

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Tadayonifar, M., Entezari, M., and Valizadeh, M. (2021). The effects of computer-assisted L1 and L2 textual and audio glosses on vocabulary learning and reading comprehension across different learning styles. J. Lang. Educ., 7, 223–242. doi: 10.17323/jle.2021.11020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanner-Smith, E. E., Tipton, E., and Polanin, J. R. (2016). Handling complex meta-analytic data structures using robust variance estimates: a tutorial in R. J. Dev. Life-Course Criminol. 2, 85–112. doi: 10.1007/s40865-016-0026-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedersoo, L., Küngas, R., Oras, E., Köster, K., Eenmaa, H., Leijen, Ä., et al. (2021). Data sharing practices and data availability upon request differ across scientific disciplines. Scientific Data 8:192. doi: 10.1038/s41597-021-00981-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, P. R., and McKay, J. B. (2010). Cognitive styles and instructional design in university learning. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.01.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, C. N., Van Garderen, D., Scheuermann, A., and Lee, E. J. (2015). Applying a universal design for learning framework to mediate the language demands of mathematics. Read. Writ. Q. 31, 207–234. doi: 10.1080/10573569.2015.1030988

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasquez, K. (2009). “Learning styles as self-fulfilling prophecies” in Getting culture. eds. R. A. R. Gurung and L. R. Prieto (New York, NY, USA: Routledge), 55–63.

Google Scholar

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Waddington, H. S., Wilson, D. B., Pigott, T., Stewart, G., Aloe, A. M., Tugwell, P., et al. (2022). Quasi-experiments are a valuable source of evidence about effects of interventions, programs and policies: commentary from the Campbell collaboration study design and Bias assessment working group. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 152, 311–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2022.11.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, B. C., Small, K., Brodley, C. E., Lau, J., and Trikalinos, T. A. (2012). Deploying an interactive machine learning system in an evidence-based practice center: Abstrackr. Proceed. ACM Int. Health Inform. Symp., 819–824. doi: 10.1145/2110363.2110464

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weaver, A. J. (2011). A Meta-analytical review of selective exposure to and the enjoyment of media violence. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media 55, 232–250. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2011.570826

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

What Works Clearinghouse (2022). WWC version 5.0 procedures and standards handbook. Available at: https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/handbooks#procedures

Google Scholar

Willingham, D. T. (2018). Ask the cognitive scientist: does tailoring instruction to “learning styles” help students learn? Am. Educ. 42:28.

Google Scholar

Wininger, S. R., Redifer, J. L., Norman, A. D., and Ryle, M. K. (2019). Prevalence of learning styles in educational psychology and introduction to education textbooks: a content analysis. Psychol. Learn. Teach. 18, 221–243. doi: 10.1177/1475725719830301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, V. X., and Fralick, C. M. (2022). “Consequences of endorsing the individual learning styles myth: helpful, harmful, or harmless?” in Learning styles, classroom instruction, and student achievement. eds. D. H. Robinson, V. X. Yan, and J. A. Kim (Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 59–74.

Google Scholar

Zhu, J., Racine, N., Xie, E. B., Park, J., Watt, J., Eirich, R., et al. (2021). Post-secondary student mental health during COVID-19: a Meta-analysis. Front. Psych. 12:777251. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.777251

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: learning styles, meta-analysis, modality, systematic review, crossover interaction

Citation: Clinton-Lisell V and Litzinger C (2024) Is it really a neuromyth? A meta-analysis of the learning styles matching hypothesis. Front. Psychol. 15:1428732. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1428732

Received: 06 May 2024; Accepted: 17 June 2024;
Published: 10 July 2024.

Edited by:

Alessandro Antonietti, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Reviewed by:

Mojtaba Tadayonifar, Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand
Stephen B. R. E. Brown, Red Deer Polytechnic, Canada

Copyright © 2024 Clinton-Lisell and Litzinger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Virginia Clinton-Lisell, virginia.clinton@und.edu

ORCID: Virginia Clinton-Lisell, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4705-2217

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.