Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 06 June 2024
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Depression: Social Stress, Inflammation, Neuromodulatory, and Neural Network Perspectives View all 8 articles

Neuromodulation of safety and surprise in the early stages of infant development: affective homeostatic regulation in bodily and mental functions

  • 1Department of Medical, Surgical and Health Sciences, Cattinara Hospital, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, Institute for Maternal and Child Health, IRCCS “Burlo Garofolo”, Trieste, Italy
  • 3College of Veterinary Medicine, Washington State University, Pullman, WA, United States

Developing a sense of internal safety and security depends mainly on others: numerous neuromodulators play a significant role in the homeostatic process, regulating the importance of proximity to a caregiver and experiencing feelings that enable us to regulate our interdependence with our conspecifics since birth. This array of neurofunctional structures have been called the SEPARATION DISTRESS system (now more commonly known as the PANIC/ GRIEF system). This emotional system is mainly involved in the production of depressive symptoms. The disruption of this essential emotional balance leads to the onset of feelings of panic followed by depression. We will focus on the neuropeptides that play a crucial role in social approach behavior in mammals, which enhance prosocial behavior and facilitate the consolidation of social bonds. We propose that most prosocial behaviors are regulated through the specific neuromodulators acting on salient intersubjective stimuli, reflecting an increased sense of inner confidence (safety) in social relationships. This review considers the neurofunctional link between the feelings that may ultimately be at the base of a sense of inner safety and the central neuromodulatory systems. This link may shed light on the clinical implications for the development of early mother-infant bonding and the depressive clinical consequences when this bond is disrupted, such as in post-partum depression, depressive feelings connected to, addiction, neurofunctional disorders, and psychological trauma.

Early neuroregulations of the relational world of the infant

Adaptation is the main task of the brain and mental apparatus: it requires mainly internal adjustments to novel conditions and aims to maintain the self-organization of any living being. There are today a compelling and growing body of evidence (Beauregard, 2007) that suggest the view that the subjective nature and the intentional emotional content of mental processes (e.g., thoughts, feelings, beliefs, volition, all experienced from the “first-person perspective”) significantly influence the various levels of brain functioning (e.g., molecular, cellular, neural circuit) and brain plasticity. These findings indicate that subjective variables must be seriously considered to reach a correct understanding of the neural bases of behavior in humans.

Below, we first illustrate the theory of emotional homeostasis from a particular neuroscientific perspective. In the next section, we describe the one supported by psychoanalysis. In both descriptions, the base regulatory principle, as we will show, is focused on the homeostatic maintenance of a maximum rate of an inner sense of safety (given by the absence of any drive or affect); in other terms, the minimization of inner free energy (given when all the subject’s needs and expectancies are met). In joining these two views, this paper aims to offer an integrated neuropsychoanalytic account.

Psychoanalytic view on the development of a sense of safety

Psychoanalysis may be defined as the discipline that studies the laws underlying human subjective life, i.e., this discipline’s scientific purposes concern intrapsychic processes. Neuroscientists tend to avoid tackling these questions, considering them as not belonging to their methodological field (e.g., Kinsbourne, 2001). Even many psychoanalysts believe the knowledge of neuroscience as irrelevant to the psychoanalytic scientific endeavor (Blass and Carmeli, 2007, 2015; Michels, 2010). Still, most psychoanalysts would assume that their discipline’s assumptions on mental processes will have to be confirmed, or at least paralleled by comparison, with the other scientific branch that deals with brain processes (for a review, see Canestri, 2015; Yovell et al., 2015).

Therefore, in our discussion of the neuromodulatory aspects of the primary regulatory principle of safety, Joffe and Sandler (1967), for example, showed that psychoanalytic psychology is a psychology of adaptation, dealing in particular with the subjective adaptations to changes in feeling states. This model implies that even the most refined and abstract cognitive representations may be activated only if feelings are associated with them. Specifically, a primary task of the mental apparatus during its maturation and development is to manage feelings: the prominent role of learning from experience is to widen the tolerable range of (conscious or unconscious) experiences without disrupting the fundamental feeling tone of safety (inner sense of mastery over one’s feelings, security, and absence of external threatening situations) and well-being (lack of anxiety and distress). Therefore, the neurological and mental apparatuses are constantly concerned with maintaining feelings of self-preservation (i.e., safety), largely maintained by monitoring perceptual constancy and congruence. The successful performance of such an act of integration is accompanied by a definite feeling tone, a fundamental ego tone, as Sandler (1960) puts it, that might be figured as a constant background state of mastery over unpleasant feelings, even though such activities often occur outside our usual sphere of awareness. Safety seeking is a fundamental survival skill that enables organisms to actively search for and identify potential sources of safety within their environment. This adaptive ability not only involves the detection and recognition of available resources but also entails the utilization of these resources to address pressing inner needs. By leveraging their innate safety seeking abilities, organisms can effectively navigate their environment to ensure their safety and well-being. It yields an internal sense of comfort, a function related to, but distinct, from the well-studied ability to learn about new dangers (FEAR avoidance; Panksepp, 1998; LeDoux, 2000, 2003).

In human clinical settings, the concept of safety has been studied by developmental psychologists and psychodynamic psychologists. Several authors (especially attachment theorists, and psychoanalysts, as for instance Sandler, 1960; Joffe and Sandler, 1967; Bowlby, 1970, 1988; Ainsworth, 1989; Fonagy, 2018) emphasized the role of safety, security and related feelings as critical concepts in the refined attunement of the interaction between the mother and the infant and, henceforth, in the early development of personality and its psychopathology. The idea of inner safety was developed in the ‘60s or ‘70s in different contexts, mainly developmental attachment theorists on one side and psychoanalysts on the other, converging on the child’s early attachment processes. The concept has various meanings in these two contexts, and the term safety is used differently in the two related disciplines. Traditionally, attachment theorists refer more to secure attachment (objective safety), meaning the child’s experience with its mother in the history of their mutual relationship, as it may be objectified by an external observer (Belsky, 1999; Fonagy, 2018). In psychoanalysis (see for example Sandler, 1960; Winnicott, 1965) the same concept is applied, but to emphasize subjective safety, an intrapsychic condition achieved through the conscious and unconscious elaboration of affective drives. Unconscious motivational processes (from basic needs to conflictual urges) aim at the perception (see the concept of identity of perception and transference; Sandler, 1976), a re-representation of early relational significant and reassuring experiences of the child with its caregivers.

According to attachment theorists (Bowlby, 1970; Ainsworth, 1989), security is the core regulatory process promotes healthy prosocial psychological development from infancy. Bowlby (1988) developmental notion of a “secure attachment” and a “secure base” were implicitly based on a conception of safety–related processes. Still, differently from the psychoanalytic use of the concept, the dissimilarity may be found in the diverse possible origins of the threat (external for attachment theorists, internal for psychoanalysis). Psychoanalysts (Sandler and Joffe, 1968; Fonagy, 2005) stress the importance of intrapsychic adaptation, avoidance of feelings of destabilization, and the development and consolidation of internal working models (representations of the caregiver in a specific relation with the subject) to achieve a sense of safety. Attachment theorists and psychoanalysts agree that security and safety are based on innate functions that operate soon after birth and govern long-term psychological development and emotional responsivity. Thus, both attachment theory and psychoanalytic models emphasize the primary status and biological function of intimate emotional bonds between individuals: we want to add to the discussion that the maintaining of these emotional intersubjective bonds is controlled by specific neurobiological systems situated within the deeper parts of central nervous system (CNS). Parent-infant affective exchanges rely on the activation of ancient and deep evolutionarily preserved brainstem networks: the infant’s separation distress behavior is mediated by the PANIC/GRIEF system, which promotes reunion with the caregiver, while the complementary parental CARE system (Panksepp, 1998) processes feeling of tenderness: both sustain early attachment development. These basic emotional systems are modulated by various neuroamines and neuropeptides that converge on opioid-mediated processes (Panksepp et al., 1980). The positive interactions between parent and infant facilitate the development of a positive working model of self-other relationships (or internal representations of interactions that have been generalized in an implicit or explicit memory process; Stern, 2014).

Moreover, Panksepp (2010) conceptualized an intermediate layer in the learning processes during the developmental age in the child: he termed this level the secondary process. At this level, there is the consolidation in a procedural form of the relationships to attachment figures (introjection; Olds, 2012). These secondary processes take the form of relational implicit memories that have a vital role in the developmental interplay between the basic emotional systems (primary process) and higher cognitive functions (tertiary processes) (see also Solms and Panksepp, 2012). This hierarchical model evolved in the brain to sustain its fundamental principle of minimizing disorganization (or free energy; see Friston et al., 2006), warranting the organism to remain in the vital homeostatic bounds of inner safety.

The neuropsychology of specific safety signals

The neurobiology of processes associated with the affective states related to safety has only recently received experimental neuropsychological attention (Rogan et al., 2005). These researchers have tried to identify and localize neural networks connected to conditions of safety and security, not simply to be considered as states of lack of fear of dangers but as the result of specific comforting signals, which depart from subcortical motivational networks. Other studies (Eisenberger et al., 2011) have tried identifying affective cortico-limbic systems connected to safety modulation. Fear learning and safety-related conditions are undoubtedly interconnected since safety signals reduce conditioned fear responses (Grillon and Ameli, 2001), indicating the existence of an interaction with the neural circuitry underlying fear conditioning, of which the amygdala is a crucial structure (LeDoux, 2000). The critical areas of the safety processing system in the brain appear in specific areas of the dorsal striatum (Rogan et al., 2005) and portions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Eisenberger et al., 2011). Both animal and human research have shown that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) is an essential component of neural mechanisms underlying perceived safety in fear learning (Schiller et al., 2008). However, the correlation between animal and human studies is not univocal (Josselyn et al., 2004). A cascade of fear-related brain networks, including the periaqueductal grey, amygdala, and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, seems to be deactivated in response to safety signals that decrease threat value (Mobbs et al., 2010). Notably, all the structures mentioned above seem to be mainly involved in safety regulation, and they possess a high quantity of oxytocin (OT) receptors on the surface of their neuronal cells (Gimpl and Fahrenholz, 2001). OT seems to modulate the responses to stress and increase the subjective feelings of comfort and safety. Studies on natural variations of oxytocin receptor polymorphism confirm the linkage between OT and feeling of safety. For instance, a specific homozygotic configuration of oxytocin receptor enhances self-confidence and optimism in healthy individuals (Saphire-Bernstein et al., 2011) in conjunction with lower levels of perceived loneliness and stress responses (Lucht et al., 2009) compared to individuals who carry the heterozygote alleles. Positive affectivity, a sense of being in a “safe” social environment, and the related ability to seek social resources are considered protective factors for depressive disorders. However, the effects of oxytocin appear to be stronger when the baseline condition of the subject is already “safe.” For instance, Declerck et al. (2010) found that people behave more cooperatively under the influence of oxytocin only when interacting with familiar targets. Thus, future studies should aim to investigate the association between OT polymorphism and responsiveness to OT administration in depressed patients and, eventually, the association between OT and the testing environment.

Brains and minds as safety-seeking agencies

According to the theory of brain information processing, the brain is an inferential machine, meaning a sophisticated device capable of generating and testing hypotheses about the causes of its internal states (of the body and the mind) to self-maintain and perpetuate its primary functional organization over time (Friston, 2005). In a previous paper, one of us (Clarici et al., 2019) focused on how this self-organization follows the evolutionary laws of self-preservation and is fundamentally regulated in organic and psychological terms by achieving a basic safety necessary for the organism’s survival. The underlying principle is the Free Energy Principle (Friston et al., 2006). It argues that for an organism to self-preserve, it must first minimize the number of surprising events occurring in time. Thus, surprise refers to the quantity of unexpected “informational disorder” or the degree of dissipation of its energy. If this dissipation is too high, the organism will gradually disorganize, cease to function, ultimately disintegrate, and consequently die. In physics (and in particular in information theory), “disorder” in this context is formalized in terms of the degree of this measure of dissipation (or entropy) in the system.1 It is, therefore, crucial for every organism to possess specific characteristics that allow constant monitoring of its state and the extent of its disorder and to implement measures capable of correcting any excessive deviation from optimal ranges of homeostatic regulation.2 In an attempt to simplify the complexities of the mathematical and theoretical formulations underlying information theory, we can summarize that to minimize the degree of surprise (and thus maximize the system’s inner state of safety and security), the organism must, first and foremost, have (1) boundaries that delineate the inside from the outside (a Markov blanket); (2) a sensory-perceptual apparatus capable of receiving, encoding, and translating the possible causes of external states into an internal representation (internal states); (3) a motor apparatus capable of altering the origin of these possible external causes; and (4) an apparatus capable of recording the outcomes of these “experimental tests” deriving from the interaction of the System with the non-System (the external environment) and updating this internal model, by constantly modifying it to adapt it to its needs or the context. Finally, a self-maintaining system must be capable of (5) assessing the reliability of a specific series of events, enhancing the salience of certain stimuli that are more significant for the organism’s survival and preservation. This assessment of reliance (or confidence) (a) in incoming relevant signals (predictive errors) or (b) in our internal expectations (prior predictions) projected outward is inherently a conscious activity linked to the effects of feelings. First and foremost, the world must be felt as we navigate through our senses to perceive it. With each of these five components in place, we can assume that an organism can resist its dissipation according to the functioning of homeostasis, surviving for as long as the interaction with its environment maintains its organization in dynamic balance to perpetuate and reproduce its “informational heritage.”

Neuromodulators and their adaptive role in infant development

Given the regulatory role of safety over anxiety argued by Freud (1938[1940])3, we could wonder whether the safety feelings are inversely correlated to social separation-distress processes that Panksepp (1998) postulated stemming from what he called the PANIC/GRIEF system or to more general anxiety responses that help the child avoid dangers and external threatening situations (such as the FEAR system). As suggested by Panksepp (2005a,b), the genesis of panic attacks (i.e., the behavioral analog of feeling ultimately unsafe and utterly lost) is closely related to our mammalian capacity to have social bonds. As conceptualized initially by Panksepp (1992), oxytocin and the concept of safety are strongly associated. Panksepp states in this respect that the vicissitudes of the perception of the proximity of a caregiver are regulated by various neuromodulators (e.g., oxytocin, together with other neuromodulators such as endogenous opioids and prolactin) that create and maintain a sort of affective background sense of safety and security. The setting of these bounds on the homeostasis of parent-infant attachment and bond is established in the critical first stages of development.

We are herewith reviewing the hypothesis that the neuroanatomical system supporting the achievement of maladjusted attachment styles and those supporting adaptation and mature behavior may be connected to a standard safety and motivational regulatory homeostatic system involving, to some extent, oxytocin. The origin of such feelings seems rooted in the supportive social behaviors of a central attachment figure (or caregiver), for the mother provides the infant with a warm and intimate sense of psychological safety and security (Heinrichs et al., 2009). However, the infant’s sense of inner safety and security constitutes the base for confidence in oneself and trust in others. This sense of confidence is constructed with brain chemistries that carry such experiences of social commitment and support. Very early in life, the infant’s attachment-seeking behaviors come to be directed toward individuals who sustain such rewarding and comforting feelings. The first brain chemistries demonstrated to mediate such effects were endogenous opioids (for reviews, see Panksepp et al., 1980, 1985; Panksepp, 1981), but in the 1990s, great interest was extended to the analysis of oxytocin (OT) in the mediation of social affects and attachments (Panksepp, 1992; Carter, 1998; Uvnäs-Moberg, 1998), and the previous decade observed an expansion of work to humans through the use of intranasal oxytocin, which was found to penetrate the brain (Born et al., 2002). The dopaminergic system and its interactions with oxytocin have been implicated in many aspects of motivational states and affiliative behavior (Leckman et al., 2005). There is a significant anatomical overlap between the oxytocin and dopamine neuron populations. Projections of oxytocinergic neurons in the medial preoptic area to the ventral tegmental area (the origin of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine pathway that mediates behavioral responses to stimuli of salience) are associated with the regulation and maintenance of social bonds, including human parenting and affiliative behavior (Young and Wang, 2004). In this perspective, the critical effect of oxytocin is to promote feelings of safety and, hence, social confidence, which are of foremost importance in affectively positive human relations, from mother–infant bonding to adult attachments and experiences of friendships (Heinrichs et al., 2009). Even though it is also clear that endogenous opioids are very robust mediators of similar psychological processes, we here focus on oxytocin because of the great potential for immediate applications to various human clinical problems ranging in adults (from addiction to depression to suicidality; Stevens et al., 2013; Yovell et al., 2016) and in child development (Quattrocki and Friston, 2014). One point often lost in these discussions is the anatomy of oxytocin systems, concentrated in ancient subcortical “animalian” regions of mammalian brains (Panksepp, 1998). Hence, it is essential to recognize that these emotional foundations of human minds may need to be discussed at many distinct levels of analysis — from primary processes we share with animals to higher-order ones, some of which are unique to humans (for a discussion of this levels of analysis issues, see Panksepp, 2011a,b). Suppose we use terminologies that are generally more critical for human interactions, such as the “trust” needed in financial negotiations. In that case, we may focus on social cognitive processes mediated by oxytocin and be confronted by paradoxical results such as intranasal oxytocin promoting financial “trust” in many humans but reducing it among individuals with borderline personality disorders (Bartz J. et al., 2010). At the same time, both effects may make sense if we understand how higher mental processes might respond to elevations of primary affective processes such as a sense of safety, security, and confidence. Such feelings may ramify widely in higher regions of the brain, leading at times to love, at times to trust, social congeniality, and other good social feelings that require “propositional objects” — namely specific cognitive representations of events or of significant others from which the details of our lives are constructed.

The particular role of oxytocinergic neuromodulation in constituting prosocial life in the infant

Numerous findings in animals (Boer, 1993; Bales and Carter, 2003) and humans (Kendrick, 2000) have now converged to highlight the role of the evolutionarily ancient neurohormone oxytocin (Gimpl and Fahrenholz, 2001) in promoting parental urges and mediating parent-infant affective exchanges (Pedersen et al., 1982; Panksepp, 1992, 1998; Carter, 1998) in moderating social stress responses (Panksepp, 1992; Heinrichs et al., 2003), and also facilitating adult pair-bonding (Insel and Young, 2001; Domes et al., 2007; Ditzen et al., 2009).

In humans, OT was thought to promote prosocial behaviors: following intranasal OT administration, individuals show enhanced trust (Baumgartner et al., 2008), promote the understanding of others (Domes et al., 2007), and various “compassionate” behaviors (Barraza et al., 2011). These seemingly diverse forms of human prosocial behaviors at their fundamental levels rely on ancient basic emotional subcortical systems and their neurochemistry (Panksepp, 1998). Because of the diversity of behaviors observed, it now seems reasonable to inquire whether a general oxytocin-induced improvement of the “psycho-relational” behaviors that have been studied in humans may share a common denominator. We hypothesize that this basic affective signal of enhanced comfort may be associated with the basic feeling of safety, which opens the door to social confidence (brought about by the absence of feelings of PANIC/GRIEF). One of the most recent theories for how oxytocin modulates behavior is the “social salience” hypothesis. According to Love (2014), many of the several oxytocin-induced changes in mammals’ social behavior may be under control of dopamine reward system. The location of oxytocin receptors in the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system places oxytocin in an ideal position to influence a wide range of motivated behaviors.

As already stated, endogenous opioids operate with other neuropeptide systems like oxytocin to enhance the feeling of comfort and reduce feelings of PANIC/GRIEF. We here develop the idea that OT may contribute to helping mediate feelings of intrapersonal safety, which at the psychobehavioral level is expressed as increased confidence, prosocial behaviors and other higher-order cognitive expressions of such primal processes, such as trust, social cognition, empathy, love and compassion (Panksepp, 1992, 2009). Of note, disorders in mother-infant primary interaction and mental pain experienced in Postpartum Depression share standard anatomical and functional neural systems, one of the most studied being the PANIC separation-distress system (Panksepp, 2005a,b). This system also appears to be amply regulated through oxytocinergic pathways. This assumption has implications for the clinical management of social bonding and related disorders in humans and animals (Panksepp, 2005a,b). In diverse human cultures, people talk about losing a loved one in terms of painful feelings. Evidence suggests that localized electrical stimulation of subcortical brain areas involved in regulating pain can provoke separation cries, and endogenous neuroendocrinological substances regulate these responses, indicating an evolutionary relationship between social and physical pain systems. Additional areas of painful feelings related to PANIC/GRIEF include the anterior cingulate, the bed-nucleus of the stria terminalis, the ventral septal and dorsal preoptic areas, the dorsomedial thalamus, and the Periaqueductal Gray (PAG) of the brainstem (Panksepp, 1998). Notably, most of these areas coincide with the Central Oxytocinergic System (see above Gimpl and Fahrenholz, 2001). The effects of OT on the emotional response of PANIC/GRIEF were more robust than on physical pain (Panksepp and Watt, 2011). Along this way, it was also discovered that other important chemistries robustly quelled PANIC/GRIEF, such as endogenous opioids and prolactin, as well as brain chemistries that intensify feelings of distress, such as CRF and glutamate (Panksepp, 2005a,b). A critical question is whether and to what extent OT brain pathways mediate affective and maternal urges.

In line with recent theories of its modulating role (instead of promoting one), OT does not appear essential for mothering: its removal through OT antagonist substances does not interfere with mothering behavior (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998). Oxytocin helps develop a secure attachment by facilitating and extending the security period during bonding formation. Maternal attachment patterns have been found to impact the caregiving practices of mothers significantly. Specifically, mothers with secure attachment patterns during interactions with their infants tend to produce a higher level of oxytocin, known as the “love hormone,” which increases the reward experience. This, in turn, may contribute to the mother’s ability to provide consistent and nurturing care. These findings highlight the importance of maternal attachment patterns in developing healthy mother-infant relationships and may have implications for promoting positive parenting practices (Strathearn et al., 2009).

Nevertheless, OT may not be implicated only in “positive” rewarding or prosocial basic emotional processes. Another hypothesis is that oxytocin may be a neuropeptide involved in the codification and informative function in the CNS of conditions of achieved emotional homeostasis. Incidentally, this may be why OT effects are so subtle and not perceivable even after administration of high doses in the CNS. This passage discusses the hypothesis that homeostatic processes do not inherently promote prosocial or developmental novel states but instead aim to maintain a dynamic stationary state. This suggests that OT may also be involved in psychopathological processes if it is present to maintain a maladaptive state of “well-being,” and its release is linked to any developmental new issue experienced as a disruption. Ultimately, this could inform the psychic apparatus of a potential deviation from the maladaptive but stable state. (Yong, 2012; Miller, 2013).

Intranasal oxytocin has shown an anxiolytic effect, as measured by decreased corticosterone in men during a test involving a stressful public speaking task (Heinrichs et al., 2003). Women viewing pictures of loved ones have high brain activity in dopaminergic pathways associated with reward, which is supposed to be modulated by the highly represented OT and arginine-vasopressin receptors in the same areas (Bartels and Zeki, 2004), as do people who describe themselves as being “intensely in love” (Fisher and Aron, 2005). All these studies confirm a positive effect of OT on prosocial mental processes and a substantial decrement in anxiety signals along with elevated feelings of safety. In women, various studies have been conducted using intravenous infusions and the measurement of plasma levels of OT during labor and postpartum. Such research shows that oxytocin plasma levels are positively correlated with higher self-reported feelings of social attachment (Tops et al., 2007) and also associated with decreased negative feelings (Mezzacappa and Katlin, 2002). Presumably, the OT-induced affective shifts are also manifested and further processed at higher cognitive levels, but these influences can only be inferred from human self-reports.

Psychopathologies connected to altered neuromodulation

This review will present in the following sections some disorders that appear to be related to disturbances or alterations in the fundamental principle of inner safety maximization (Sandler and Joffe, 1969) or free energy minimization (as Friston, 2010 puts it). In fact, according to Friston (2023), from a computational psychiatry perspective states:

if psychology — read as belief updating in the brain — can be cast as a computational process of inference, it follows that psychopathology just is false inference. False inference is meant in the usual sense of false positives (i.e., type I errors); namely, inferring something is there when it is not. Cardinal examples here include hallucinations, delusions and other features of reality distortion seen in psychosis. False negatives (i.e., type II errors) mean inferring something is not there when it is; for example, dissociative disorders, neglect syndromes, derealization phenomena, et cetera. Indeed, when one thinks about psychiatric and neurological disorders, most can be framed as false inference. (p. 257)

We will begin with a description of stress-related disturbances, then move on to describe some psychopathologies deeply related to stress, trauma, or relational issues (functional neurological disorders, pathological addictions), and finally, mood disorders deeply interrelated to relational dynamics. Throughout the discussion, relationships with oxytocin will be highlighted.

Stress-related diseases and neuromodulation of depressive feelings

Research has shown that the hypothalamic–pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA axis) functions differently in individuals with certain psychological and psychiatric conditions, such as depression, anxiety disorders, and PTSD (Baskerville and Douglas, 2010; Walker and McGlone, 2013; Herman et al., 2016). These conditions are often associated with an altered response to stress. The mechanisms behind maladaptive reactions to chronic stress are still being understood. However, the complex relationship between prolonged HPA axis activation and neural pathway activation in the brain is known. In normal conditions, the stress response “[…] represents an integrated reaction to stressors, broadly defined as real or perceived threats to homeostasis or wellbeing.” (Herman et al., 2016). The HPA axis recruits the energies needed to respond to stressors, whether it is a reactive response or an anticipatory response. After the active phase, the HPA axis activity decreases due to a negative feedback mechanism regulated by glucocorticoids. However, if this system is repeatedly or continuously activated, it can lead to various problems. Studies suggest that prolonged or repeated stress during childhood, caused by negative social experiences, abuse, or parental neglect, can result in more prominent reactions to stressors later in life (Walker and McGlone, 2013). However, genetic components shape personal responses to stress by influencing the presence and density of specific receptors, which will define the neurobiological response patterns to stressors in conjunction with early childhood expressions. According to research, individuals who have experienced emotional trauma or have PTSD have significantly lower levels of oxytocin (OT) in their bodies (Donadon et al., 2018). Some studies suggest that oxytocin receptors located in the amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex may play a role in regulating stress responses, as these areas are also involved in the physiology of PTSD (see for example Sack et al., 2017; Takayanagi and Onaka, 2021; Jin et al., 2023).

Oxytocin exerts a significant anti-stress function by suppressing both the secretory and behavioral reactions to stress within the HPA axis due to its actions within the PVN (paraventricular nucleus), which inhibits the activity of corticotrophin-releasing hormone neurons. It seems that oxytocin also has a role in the modulation of anxiety, acting in the diverse regions involved in these disorders, namely the PVN, SON (supraoptic nucleus), and amygdala. In chronic stress, magnocellular oxytocinergic neurons are inhibited by corticosterone, decreasing the amount of central oxytocin and preventing an anxiolytic and anti-stress effect (Di et al., 2005). The anxiolytic effect of oxytocin would be given by an action via serotoninergic pathways, which are also involved in depressive disorders; these findings are inherent in animal models only (Baskerville and Douglas, 2010). The anti-stress effect of oxytocin would also be due, in synergy with dopamine, to the rewarding part of social and addictive behavior. Other evidence from animal models suggests how crucial dopaminergic pathways and the alteration of their plasticity are in determining depressive spectrum disorders (Lamanna et al., 2022). In this regard, the relationships between the dopaminergic system and oxytocin could be significant in treating these disorders.

Functional neurological disorders and neuromodulation of depressive feelings

The absence of an organic etiopathogenesis characterizes functional neurological disorders. Interpretation of the establishment and development of these disorders is complex given the variety of symptom manifestations and neurological or psychiatric comorbidities Current theoretical systems recognize several possible causes, predisposing factors and triggering events that can be primarily traced back to the individual’s developmental history, i.e., individual and family history of illness, past and present trauma, organic and genetic vulnerabilities, and others (Edwards et al., 2012; Ludwig et al., 2018; Edwards, 2021). The interpretative model we refer to here is based on the Bayesian model of the brain and the role of attentional components (exteroception, interoception, and proprioception) in establishing of the disorder. In particular, the role of emotional activation is relevant, influencing attentional orientation and salience (see Edwards et al., 2012; Solms and Friston, 2018, for an account of attention and affective precision).

The absence of an organic etiopathogenesis characterizes functional neurological disorders. These disorders are difficult to interpret due to various symptom manifestations and neurological or psychiatric comorbidities (Edwards et al., 2012; Espay et al., 2018; Gilmour et al., 2020; Patron et al., 2022). Various theoretical systems acknowledge several possible causes, predisposing factors, and triggering events that can primarily be traced back to the individual’s developmental history (Edwards et al., 2012; Ludwig et al., 2018; Edwards, 2021). This includes their individual and family history of illness, past and present trauma, organic and genetic vulnerabilities, and more. The interpretative model that we are referring to is based on the Bayesian model of the brain and the role of attentional components such as exteroception, interoception, and proprioception in the establishment of the disorder (Edwards et al., 2012; Edwards and Bhatia, 2012; Edwards, 2021). Emotional activation plays a significant role in influencing attentional orientation and salience. The role of attention and affective precision is explained in detail in works by Edwards et al. (2012) and Solms and Friston (2018).

The purpose of this article is not to delve into the details of this model. Instead, we will focus on the potential role of neuromodulators, specifically oxytocin, in certain disorders. Studies have shown that patients with functional neurological disorders (FND) often have comorbid mood disorders such as dysthymia and depression, anxiety disorders, and stress-related problems (Ludwig et al., 2018; Liang et al., 2021). Other studies have found alterations in the limbic-motor axis, with abnormal amygdala activations and a possible suppression effect on unwanted memories, and, more generally, an abnormal emotional activation (Aybek et al., 2014, 2015; Liang et al., 2021). Moreover, patients affected by FND complain of high levels of pain, both acute and chronic (Espay et al., 2018; Gilmour et al., 2020; Liang et al., 2021). There are very few references in the literature regarding FNDs and the efficacy of drug treatments, especially in randomized controlled trials (Perjoc et al., 2023). These aspects suggest an imbalance in the regulation or expression of neurotransmitters that regulate such emotional and stress aspects in the brain. However, the literature has no complete agreement on this (Liang et al., 2021).

It is essential to conduct further investigation to fully comprehend the role of monoamines such as norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin. These monoamines are involved in various affective and adaptive mechanisms, including mood regulation, fear management, pain experience, reward mechanisms, motor programming/regulation, and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). It is also crucial to explore the role of oxytocin in modulating specific affects. Additionally, it is necessary to understand any alterations in the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the role of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) and cortisol in patients with functional neurological disorder (FND). These mechanisms appear to be closely related to the neurodynamic of oxytocin. The uncertainty regarding the use and effectiveness of pharmacologic treatments in FND leads to a need for more agreement and knowledge among experts. While antidepressants, antipsychotics, and neuroleptics are used for symptom management, their mechanisms of action concerning symptoms are not yet fully understood. Additionally, therapy cannot act on the neural mechanisms of the disease (Gelauff, 2020; Perjoc et al., 2023).

To summarize, there is much more to learn about functional neurological disorders (FNDs). However, recent research suggests that there may be a connection between altered emotional regulation and bodily experience in patients with FNDs. This has led to increased study of the neural systems, such as the motor, limbic, and executive systems, and the associated neuromodulators (Espay et al., 2018; Liang et al., 2021; Perjoc et al., 2023).

Neuromodulation of functional disorders connected to attachment styles

Further exploration is needed to understand the complex factors contributing to functional symptoms fully. The age-old debate between nature and nurture is relevant in this context, as research by Torgersen (1986) suggests that both genetic and environmental factors influence the inheritance of somatoform disorders within families in a complex interdependence. Although the concordance rates in monozygotic (MZ) twins were higher (29%) compared to dizygotic (DZ) twins (10%), the difference was not statistically significant. This indicates that although genetics may play a role, it is not the only factor in developing of functional neurological disorders. The study acknowledges the limitations imposed by the small sample size and the shared childhood environments, which were found to be correlated, albeit weakly, with the concordance rates for both MZ and DZ twins. This nuanced interpretation suggests that the transmission of functional disorders within families could be attributed to genetic predispositions and shared environmental influences. The findings highlight the need for a more comprehensive understanding of the factors contributing to somatoform disorders, considering the biological underpinnings and the environmental contexts in which these disorders manifest. On the same leitmotiv, a recent study (Hur et al., 2019) conducted on a sample of 1,754 South Korean adolescent and young adult twins aimed to estimate the heritability of somatization and to explore the shared genetic and environmental influences between somatization and Hwabyung (HB), an anger syndrome. The sample included 367 monozygotic males, 173 dizygotic males, 681 monozygotic females, 274 dizygotic females, and 259 opposite-sex dizygotic twins. Participants completed self-report questionnaires on HB symptoms and somatization scales through telephone interviews.

The findings revealed that 43% of the variance in somatization was attributable to additive genetic factors. In comparison, the remaining 57% was due to individual-specific environmental influences, including measurement error, with no significant sex differences observed. A notable phenotypic correlation of 0.53 between HB and somatization was identified, suggesting a significant relationship. Bivariate model-fitting analyses indicated a genetic correlation of 0.68 between HB and somatization, signifying a substantial genetic overlap. However, the individual-specific environmental correlation, including correlated measurement errors, was 0.41, suggesting that while there is a shared genetic basis, the environmental factors contributing to the development of HB and somatization may be largely independent.

This study’s findings emphasize the complex interplay of genetic and environmental factors in the manifestation of somatization and HB, underscoring the importance of considering both in understanding and treating these conditions. The significant genetic correlation between the two suggests potential shared biological pathways, which could inform future research and treatment strategies. However, the distinct environmental influences highlight the role of individual experiences and contexts in developing these conditions, pointing to the need for personalized approaches in treatment and prevention.

Adult attachment styles (AAS) may play a significant role in further explaining the relationship between genetic and environmental factors. A recent fMRI study (Vrtička et al., 2012) delves into the modulation of social emotion perception and regulation by AAS. Conducted with 19 healthy adults, the research investigates how avoidant (AV) and anxious (AX) attachment styles affect neural responses to social emotions. AV individuals displayed increased activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and left amygdala during cognitive reappraisal of unpleasant social stimuli, indicating a potential inefficiency in emotion regulation strategies. Additionally, during the suppression of positive social emotions, these individuals showed heightened activity in the supplementary motor area (SMA) and ventral caudate, suggesting a more significant effort to inhibit emotional expressions.

Conversely, AX individuals exhibited increased activation in the right amygdala and left parahippocampal cortex in response to negative and positive social stimuli, respectively, but only during spontaneous emotional judgments. This pattern suggests a heightened sensitivity to social cues without the corresponding difficulty in emotion regulation that AV individuals experience. The study’s findings underscore how AAS influences the neural underpinnings of social emotion regulation. Avoidant individuals may struggle with cognitive reappraisal and exhibit more significant neural effort during emotion suppression, while anxious individuals display heightened neural sensitivity to social stimuli.

In 2012, Vrticka and Vuilleumier conducted a comprehensive overview of the relationship between neuroscience, human social interactions, and adult attachment styles (AAS) (Vrtička and Vuilleumier, 2012). They offered a unique perspective on how individual differences in attachment impact emotional and social cognition through neurobiological processes. The authors recognized the critical role of attachment theory (AT) in understanding the development and maintenance of social bonds. They explored the neurobiological factors influencing attachment orientations and how they impact various social and emotional behaviors.

The review proposes a framework that combines current neuroscience with relational and attachment theories. This framework has two key components. The first component is a system that allows for rapid and automatic affective appraisals. This system encodes basic dimensions of safety versus threat or approach versus aversion tendencies in social contexts. It relies on limbic cortico-subcortical areas. The second component is a controlled processing system that engages in mentalizing processes such as theory of mind, self-reflection, and emotion regulation. This system uses frontotemporal areas.

This review explains how two different systems, called affective appraisal and emotional arousal, work together in social situations. These systems affect people differently, depending on their attachment styles. People with an avoidant attachment style tend to ignore or downplay positive social signals. Those with an anxious attachment style are more sensitive to negative cues, which can cause emotional arousal. These findings help us understand functional disorders, which are physical symptoms that do not have a clear medical cause. These disorders are affected by psychological factors, including how people process and regulate emotions (Edwards et al., 2012; Solms and Friston, 2018). Attachment styles play a significant role in emotional regulation and processing (see for example Liu and Ma, 2019; Eilert and Buchheim, 2023). People with an insecure attachment style may have trouble controlling their emotions (see for example Tammilehto, et al., 2022) and this may lead to experience physical symptoms which, in turn, could become predisposing factors for the functional disorders (Edwards, et al., 2012; Espay et al., 2018; Gilmour et al., 2020; Liang et al., 2021).

In conclusion, attachment styles can affect how people process and regulate emotions. This can lead to physical symptoms that are part, as triggers or predisposing factors, of functional disorders. Understanding this connection can help us develop better treatments for these disorders.

Addictions and neuromodulation of depressive feelings

Addiction is a complex issue that arises due to the misuse of various substances, which triggers a specific type of brain plasticity that is common across all addictive substances. While we have a good understanding of the neurobiology of addiction, the exact role of certain neuromodulators, such as serotonin and dopamine, is still not entirely clear. Dopamine is the neurotransmitter that is most involved in the mechanisms of addiction.

Numerous medications can stimulate the release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens, a crucial brain nucleus within the reward circuit. Typically, the prefrontal cortex receives a steady dopamine flow due to the ongoing activity of dopaminergic neurons that originate in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and connect to the cortex. However, when an enriching experience or an extremely unpleasant event occurs, the dopaminergic neurons become much more active and fire rapidly. This sudden increase in activity causes a temporary but significant spike in dopamine levels. It is widely believed that these heightened dopamine levels during this rapid discharge phase are crucial for dopamine’s reward effect (Uhl et al., 2019; Becker-Krail et al., 2022).

The dopamine receptors (D1) stimulate both reward and conditioning mechanisms in which the amygdala, medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and hippocampus are involved, thus enhancing memory and creating memories associated with substance use (Uhl et al., 2019). Memory processes allow the creation of an automatic association, i.e., a prediction of satisfaction (reward). In subjects with addiction, however, dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens are lower than levels found in healthy subjects. This would be given by the tolerance/habituation mechanisms, which lead to behaviors (due to increased dopamine in the dorsolateral striatum) of habit and are no longer goal-directed, as in the early period of addiction (McSweeney et al., 2005; Lipton et al., 2019; Uhl et al., 2019). Over time, a change leads to goal-directed, habit behaviors, as the connections between the dorsolateral striatum and basolateral amygdala, which processes the cues that trigger habit behaviors, are reinforced (Lipton et al., 2019). Then, a reduced response of the reward system is generated. Concurrently, the absence of the psychoactive substance triggers adverse reactions, i.e., withdrawal (Uhl et al., 2019). The brain systems that regulate reward and stress responses are intimately connected. In the absence of the substance (e.g., opioids, alcohol or stimulants), a negatively perceived reaction is generated in the individual by the release of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and dynorphin in the amygdala (Knoll and Carlezon, 2010; Zorrilla et al., 2014; Baumgartner et al., 2022). In turn, CRF stimulates the production of precisely Adrenocorticotropic Hormone (ACTH) in the pituitary gland, which stimulates cortisol production (Panksepp and Biven, 2012; Zorrilla et al., 2014; Uhl et al., 2019; Lightman et al., 2020). The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis coordinates neuroendocrine stress response systems (Herman et al., 2016).

A renewed abuse reduces stress-related reactions, ending the aversive reactions of withdrawal. However, there are alternative ways to reduce blood cortisol levels. It appears, for example, that serotonin may also play a role in inhibiting reward establishment and aversive states (Uhl et al., 2019).

This review emphasizes that oxytocin plays a role in reducing cortisol levels. First, studies show how dense the interactions between dopamine and oxytocin are in the context of social behaviors such as sex and pair bonding. The prefrontal cortex receives important oxytocin innervations, and receptors for dopamine and oxytocin are found in large populations in both the prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens. This evidence indicates how oxytocin plays a role in the expression of complex social behaviors and how, conversely, may shape such behaviors. In people with addiction, reduced dopamine receptors (due to prolonged overstimulation over time given by nerve pathways projecting from the VTA into the prefrontal cortex) resulting in less activation of these areas is partly responsible for impulsive and compulsive behaviors.

There exists a body of evidence which suggests that the neurotransmitters dopamine and oxytocin work in tandem to regulate addiction. Specifically, oxytocin is believed to facilitate abstinence while mitigating mechanisms of tolerance and dependence. This would be due to its relationship with dopamine: endogenous opioids in natural social situations control the expression of oxytocin neurons. Opioid-based drugs, therefore, act on oxytocin neurons by inhibiting them since they express opioid receptors. In addiction and abstinence, therefore, the oxytocin system, as well as the dopaminergic system, would be altered; this evidence prompts further investigation into the therapeutic role that OT might play in the treatment of addiction, particularly in the withdrawal phase (Baskerville and Douglas, 2010).

When managing stress, oxytocin is known for reducing stress-related reactions. Social support can also increase the production of oxytocin and lower cortisol levels. In addition, taking oxytocin directly through the nose has been shown to have an even more significant impact in reducing the neuroendocrine responsible for adverse stress reactions (Love, 2018). Recent findings show how oxytocin can reduce drug-seeking and drug-induced behaviors. This could be because oxytocin may have a direct role in glutamatergic pathways involved in addiction (Sundar et al., 2021). This evidence leads us to discuss the role oxytocin plays in prolonged and acute stress contexts.

Neuromodulation of depression and anxiety

Interdisciplinary studies, as reviewed above, may help better understand many psychiatric problems. For instance, since social loss is one of the most robust precipitants of depression (Nelson and Panksepp, 1998) and oxytocin administration reduces PANIC/GRIEF, we could anticipate that oxytocinergic facilitators should be effective treatments for sadness and depression.

Postpartum depression is a clinical condition that is accompanied subjectively by conflictual ambivalent feelings of a mother toward her baby (Green, 1986; Lemaitre-Sillère and Bennett, 1998). Compared with non-lactating women, postpartum mothers (2 days after birth) who have received OT during labor (just after birth) have significantly lower scores on anxiety and aggression self-report scales and also score higher on a socialization scale, with the result remaining constant at 2 and 6 months postpartum (Jonas et al., 2008). Plasma OT and pregnancy OT plasma levels are very stable throughout all trimesters of pregnancy. They are positively associated with maternal–fetal attachment as measured by a prenatal attachment scale administered to the mothers (Levine et al., 2007). Feldman et al. (2007) analyzed OT levels in the first trimester and early postpartum period and found a positive correlation with certain maternal bonding behaviors, such as gazing at the infant, vocalizations to the infant, and affectionate touch. Of course, many measures of peripheral OT levels can be criticized since the correlation between central and peripheral oxytocin remains unclear in all human experiments, and the results are mixed in animal experiments. Nevertheless, it is worth noting that viewing images of one’s children activates dopaminergic pathways in the mothers’ brains associated with reward, where there are also high levels of OT receptors (Bartels and Zeki, 2004).

The findings are consistent with clinical studies showing an association between specific adult attachment styles and depression (Carnelley et al., 1994). In particular, of the four categories of attachment style, only insecure attachment is linked with the risk of developing depression (Mickelson et al., 1997; Murphy and Bates, 1997). Recent studies suggest interactions between central OT and attachment style (Bartz J. A. et al., 2010; Rockliff et al., 2011). For instance, Bartz and colleagues found that intranasal OT promotes the recollection of negative representation of the mother in individuals with insecure attachment styles, with opposite effects in individuals with secure attachment styles (Bartz J. et al., 2010). These findings contrast the hypothesis that oxytocin would increase positive feelings (Yong, 2012).

On the other hand, a possible interpretation of these findings would be that OT facilitates the disclosure of one’s representations by enhancing a basic feeling of safety and comfort. This evidence is in line with our hypothesis that OT may be an essential neuromodulator of a homeostatic system related to safety conditions reflecting the experience by the subject, without distinction of the fact these same experiences were positive or negative in terms of wellbeing. As well known in clinical practice, the feeling of safety is the prerequisite for sharing personal experiences and representing oneself and others. Indeed, considering that safety cuts across both social and non-social domains, this primal “common denominator” may help explain various paradoxical and fragile specific social effects seen with oxytocin (Bartz et al., 2011). Modern anti-depressants have been employed in postpartum depression. Now that there are ways of administering exogenous OT via intranasal airways, which are medically approved in Europe, have no known addiction liability, and seem to be fundamentally free of side effects, there is a role for off-label use of neuropeptides in treating this type of depressions, with positive effects that can occur promptly as opposed to over several weeks of treatment with a classical antidepressant. Our group has started the evaluation of whether intranasal administration of oxytocin can alleviate feelings of despair in those mothers who experience depression soon after the birth of their children. Such a project, in the context of psychoanalytic therapy, has been initiated (Ozkarar and Clarici, 2008; Pellizzoni et al., 2009; Clarici and Pellizzoni, 2013) with preliminary results showing a facilitatory role of OT in maintaining a positive therapeutic alliance in the course of a dynamically oriented psychotherapy in postpartum depressed mothers (Clarici et al., 2015). OT’s pharmacologic properties make it most attractive as a potential antidepressant drug in postpartum depression, with the main aim of improving the quality of infant-mother interaction through natural endogenous substances and psychosocial interventions.

In depressed patients, the hyperactivation of HPA axis, and consequently, high cortisol level, is well established. Additionally, plasma oxytocin is negatively associated with scores of depression symptoms (Scantamburlo et al., 2007; Cyranowski et al., 2008). Central OT administration has been shown to have anxiolytic properties on endocrine and behavioral systems. This seems to be due to the effect of the neuropeptide on the HPA system (Windle et al., 1997; Bale et al., 2001). However, it is still unclear if it has a direct effect on the axis or an indirect effect through other structures, such as the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Dunn, 1987) or the amygdala (Dunn and Berridge, 1990). OT does not decrease salivary cortisol levels in men with a history of early parental separation as in control males, indicating a possible abnormal HPA response to OT in these subjects (Meinlschmidt and Heim, 2007). This study confirms a relation between the attachment system and possible dysregulation of these neural systems due to a premature loss experience. OT may exert its effects by directly inhibiting the amygdala (Davis, 1992; Adolphs et al., 1995). Further, recent contributions show that long-term anxiety and depressive states associated with development are associated with greater amygdala volume (van Elst et al., 2003). In the laboratory, the amygdala activation is reduced by intranasal administration of OT (Dębiec, 2005).

From this perspective, the role of the amygdala in major depressive and anxiety disorder is most interesting (Pezawas and Meyer-Lindenberg, 2005). Functional imaging studies observed an increased metabolism in the amygdala in subjects with major depressive disorder (Fales et al., 2009). Related to this, studies have shown that OT reduces both social anxiety (Heinrichs and Domes, 2008) and amygdala responses to fearful faces (Kirsch et al., 2008), implying a possible trade-off of OT and amygdala responsivity and, in turn, a sensation to trust the other and to create a bond with people and possibly offspring.

Conclusion

This review delves into the intricate workings of various neuromodulators in regulating various physiological and psychological functions, including safety, social bonding, and affective processes. The paper pays particular attention to how the neuromodulator oxytocin impacts attachment and prosocial behaviors, providing in-depth insights into how this hormone shapes our relationships and emotional experiences in normal development and psychopathology. Panksepp’s work is cited to emphasize the connection between oxytocin and feelings of safety, suggesting that oxytocin and other neuromodulators play a crucial role in establishing an affective background of safety and security. The focus is on the oxytocin system’s involvement in various aspects of human development, from parent-infant attachment to adult relationships, suggesting a standard regulatory homeostatic system. The discussion extends to the overlap between oxytocin and dopamine systems, highlighting their implications for motivational states and affiliative behavior. Studies have shown that the rise in oxytocin levels in the brain can improve psycho-relational behaviors. This is believed to be due to the release of a primary affective signal that enhances comfort, which is associated with the fundamental feeling of safety. As a result, individuals experience a boost in social confidence and a decrease in feelings of panic or grief. Essentially, the release of oxytocin triggers a cascade of positive emotional responses that can lead to beneficial outcomes for individuals in social situations. The oxytocinergic system plays a crucial role in regulating emotions and behaviors such as trust, social cognition, empathy, love, and compassion. It has been observed that changes in the regulatory processes involving oxytocin and various neurotransmitters connected to it can contribute to the development and persistence of depressive disorders. It is important to note that this response should not be regarded solely as a disease, but also as an adaptive mechanism of the brain in response to predictive and false inferences that trigger depressive symptoms. One notable sentence that helps connect psychopathology with the predictive coding theories of Friston (2023) is as follows:

The narrative starts with the simple premise that if psychology— read as belief updating in the brain—can be cast as a computational process of inference, it follows that psychopathology just is false inference. [page 257]

The role of oxytocin is connected to social bonding, postpartum depression and the interrelation between social and physical pain systems, with evolutionary and clinical aspects. It is a potential modulator of affective and maternal urges, affecting emotional responses to panic and grief more strongly than physical pain. Even at high doses, oxytocin’s effects are subtle, but it is involved in maintaining emotional homeostasis. Overall, this review confirms that oxytocin plays a multifaceted role in regulating emotions, social behaviors, and mental well-being throughout various stages of development.

Author contributions

AC: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MB: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. YR: Writing – review & editing. JP: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

In memoriam

This paper was written with the mentorship and editing by late professor Jaak Panksepp, and it is one of the last works before he passed away, leaving the scientific community with a deep feeling of loss. Therefore, this work suffers from the loss of the scientist who, among us authors, possessed the vastest knowledge of the brain mechanisms of neuromodulation. We believe that, even if this work has inevitably been affected by this loss, we trust that Jaak Panksepp would have approved the hypotheses on the neuromodulatory processes, which, based on his guidance, have led us to extend his conclusions to the psychiatric, psychotherapeutic and psychopathological fields. The publication is thus posthumous (one of the authors, AC obtained written permission from Prof. Panksepp’s wife, Anesa Miller, YW5lc2FtOThAZ21haWwuY29t), to maintain his name as one of the authors, as a sign and homage of our indebtment to Jaak Panksepp for his great wisdom, vast knowledge, that he perfused to us with profound intellectual and scientific honesty.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported through the contribution given to the Institute for Maternal and Child Health IRCCS Burlo Garofolo - Trieste, Italy. The contribution was given, in particular, by the Presidency of the Regione Autonoma Friuli Venezia Giulia (Italy) to the project FVG21DNF – “Meaning of the symptom and psychological support in patient with functional disorder.”

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^It should be noted that in psychiatry, for several decades now, as it is well-known, it has become common practice to refer to mental illness using the term “psychiatric disorder” (e.g., as in bipolar disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, or personality disorder, etc.), a term which is evocative of an entropic tendency affecting the psyche.

2. ^As challenging as it may appear for a reader with a psychological background to delve into these arguments drawn from theoretical physics, we want to emphasize (as we have extensively done in the mentioned article; Clarici et al., 2019) that this principle may be considered a conceptual corollary of the equally well-known principle of intrapsychic safety put forth by Sandler (in the 1960s) in the context of psychopathology. Similarly, it aligns this Safety principle with the fundamental need for a secure base present in the infant (as in many animal species) in the field of intersubjective attachment development, as proposed by Bowlby and his followers (also in the same decade of the 20th century – i.e., the 60ies).

3. ^Freud in the Outline of Psychoanalysis (1938 [1940]) states: “Just as the id is directed exclusively to obtaining pleasure, so the ego is governed by considerations of safety. The ego has set itself the task of self-preservation, which the id appears to neglect” (p. 199).

References

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. R. (1995). Fear and the human amygdala. J. Neurosci. 15, 5879–5891. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-09-05879.1995

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. Am. Psychol. 44, 709–716. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aybek, S., Nicholson, T. R., O'Daly, O., Zelaya, F., Kanaan, R. A., and David, A. S. (2015). Emotion-motion interactions in conversion disorder: an FMRI study. PLoS One 10:e0123273. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123273

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aybek, S., Nicholson, T. R., Zelaya, F., O'Daly, O. G., Craig, T. J., David, A. S., et al. (2014). Neural correlates of recall of life events in conversion disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 52–60. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2842

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bale, T. L., Davis, A. M., Auger, A. P., Dorsa, D. M., and McCarthy, M. M. (2001). CNS region-specific oxytocin receptor expression: importance in regulation of anxiety and sex behavior. J. Neurosci. 21, 2546–2552. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-07-02546.2001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bales, K. L., and Carter, C. S. (2003). Developmental exposure to oxytocin facilitates partner preferences in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav. Neurosci. 117:854. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.4.854

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barraza, J. A., McCullough, M. E., Ahmadi, S., and Zak, P. J. (2011). Oxytocin infusion increases charitable donations regardless of monetary resources. Horm. Behav. 60, 148–151. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. NeuroImage 21, 1155–1166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J., Simeon, D., Hamilton, H., Kim, S., Crystal, S., Braun, A., et al. (2010). Oxytocin can hinder trust and cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 556–563. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq085

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., and Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 301–309. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Ochsner, K. N., Bolger, N., Kolevzon, A., Ludwig, N., et al. (2010). Effects of oxytocin on recollections of maternal care and closeness. PNAS 107, 21371–21375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012669107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baskerville, T. A., and Douglas, A. J. (2010). Dopamine and oxytocin interactions underlying behaviours: potential contributions to behavioural disorders. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 16, e92–e123. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00154.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgartner, H. M., Granillo, M., Schulkin, J., and Berridge, K. C. (2022). Corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) systems: promoting cocaine pursuit without distress via incentive motivation. PLoS One 17:e0267345. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0267345

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgartner, T., Heinrichs, M., Vonlanthen, A., Fischbacher, U., and Fehr, E. (2008). Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans. Neuron 58, 639–650. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauregard, M. (2007). Mind does really matter: evidence from neuroimaging studies of emotional self-regulation, psychotherapy, and placebo effect. Prog. Neurobiol. 81, 218–236. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker-Krail, D. D., Walker, W. H. 2nd, and Nelson, R. J. (2022). The ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens as circadian oscillators: implications for drug abuse and substance use disorders. Front. Physiol. 13:886704. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.886704

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (1999). “Interactional and contextual determinants of attachment security” in Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. eds. J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 249–264.

Google Scholar

Blass, R. B., and Carmeli, Z. V. I. (2007). The case against neuropsychoanalysis: on fallacies underlying psychoanalysis' latest scientific trend and its negative impact on psychoanalytic discourse. Int. J. Psychoanal. 88, 19–40. doi: 10.1516/6NCA-A4MA-MFQ7-0JTJ

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blass, R. B., and Carmeli, Z. (2015). Further evidence for the case against neuropsychoanalysis: how Yovell, Solms, and Fotopoulou's response to our critique confirms the irrelevance and harmfulness to psychoanalysis of the contemporary neuroscientific trend. Int. J. Psychoanal. 96, 1555–1573. doi: 10.1111/1745-8315.12449

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boer, G. J. (1993). Chronic oxytocin treatment during late gestation and lactation impairs development of rat offspring. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 15, 383–389. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(93)90055-S

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Born, J., Lange, T., Kern, W., McGregor, G. P., Bickel, U., and Fehm, H. L. (2002). Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 514–516. doi: 10.1038/nn0602-849

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1970). Attachment and loss. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Canestri, J. (2015). The case for neuropsychoanalysis. Int. J. Psychoanal. 96, 1575–1584. doi: 10.1111/1745-8315.12474

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carnelley, K. B., Pietromonacó, P. R., and Jaffe, K. (1994). Depression, working models of others, and relationship functioning. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 127–140. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.127

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–818. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarici, A., and Pellizzoni, S. (2013) Oxytocin and dynamic aspects in Post partum depression. A study on the patient and on the psychotherapist. In 14th Congress of the International Society of Neuropsychoanalysis, "Clinical Applications of Neuropsychoanalysis" CapeTown, South Africa

Google Scholar

Clarici, A., Pellizzoni, S., Guaschino, S., Alberico, S., Bembich, S., Giuliani, R., et al. (2015). Intranasal adminsitration of oxytocin in postnatal depression: implications for psychodynamic psychotherapy from a randomized double-blind pilot study. Front. Psychol. 6:426. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00426

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarici, A., Zanettovich, A., and Panksepp, J. (2019). Surprise, surprise: safety and free energy principles are two sides of the same coin. Front. Psychoanalysis Neuropsychoanalysis.

Google Scholar

Cyranowski, J. M., Hofkens, T. L., Frank, E., Seltman, H., Cai, H.-M., and Amico, J. A. (2008). Evidence of dysregulated peripheral oxytocin release among depressed women. Psychosom. Med. 70, 967–975. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318188ade4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 353–375. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dębiec, J. (2005). Peptides of love and fear: vasopressin and oxytocin modulate the integration of information in the amygdala. BioEssays 27, 869–873. doi: 10.1002/bies.20301

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Declerck, C. H., Boone, C., and Kiyonari, T. (2010). Oxytocin and cooperation under conditions of uncertainty: the modulating role of incentives and social information. Horm Ehav. 57, 368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di, S., Malcher-Lopes, R., Marcheselli, V. L., Bazan, N. G., and Tasker, J. G. (2005). Rapid glucocorticoid-mediated endocannabinoid release and opposing regulation of glutamate and gamma-aminobutyric acid inputs to hypothalamic magnocellular neurons. Endocrinology 46, 4292–4301. doi: 10.1210/en.2005-0610

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ditzen, B., Schaer, M., Gabriel, B., Bodenmann, G., Ehlert, U., and Heinrichs, M. (2009). Intranasal oxytocin increases positive communication and reduces cortisol levels during couple conflict. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 728–731. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Michel, A., Berger, C., and Herpertz, S. C. (2007). Oxytocin improves “mind-Reading” in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 731–733. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Donadon, M. F., Martin-Santos, R., and Osório, F. L. (2018). The associations between oxytocin and trauma in humans: a systematic review. Front. Pharmacol. 9:154. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, J. D. (1987). Plasma corticosterone responses to electrical stimulation of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Brain Res. 407, 327–331. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91111-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, A. J., and Berridge, C. W. (1990). Physiological and behavioural responses to corticotropin-releasing factor administration: is CRF a mediator of anxiety or stress responses. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 15, 71–100. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(90)90012-D

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. J. (2021). Functional neurological disorder: lighting the way to a new paradigm for medicine. Brain 144, 3279–3282. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. J., Adams, R. A., Brown, H., Parees, I., and Friston, K. J. (2012). A Bayesian account of ‘hysteria’. Brain 135, 3495–3512. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. J., and Bhatia, K. P. (2012). Functional (psychogenic) movement disorders: merging mind and brain. Lancet Neurol. 11, 250–260. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70310-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eilert, D. W., and Buchheim, A. (2023). Attachment-related differences in emotion regulation in adults: a systematic review on attachment representations. Brain Sci. 13:884. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13060884

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I., Master, S. L., Inagaki, T. K., Taylor, S. E., Shirinyan, D., Lieberman, M. D., et al. (2011). Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural region and reduce pain experience. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 11721–11726. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108239108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Espay, A. J., Aybek, S., Carson, A., Edwards, M. J., Goldstein, L. H., Hallett, M., et al. (2018). Current concepts in diagnosis and treatment of functional neurological disorders. JAMA Neurol. 75, 1132–1141. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.1264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fales, C., Barch, D., Rundle, M., and Mintun, M. (2009). Antidepressant treatment normalizes hypoactivity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during emotional interference processing in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 112, 206–211. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.04.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Weller, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., and Levine, A. (2007). Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychol. Sci. 18, 965–970. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02010.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, H., and Aron, A. (2005). Romantic love: an fMRI study of a neural mechanism for mate choice. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 58–62. doi: 10.1002/cne.20772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P. (2005). An overview of Joseph Sandler's key contributions to theoretical and clinical psychoanalysis. Psychoanal. Inq. 25, 120–147. doi: 10.1080/07351692509349124

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P. (2018). Attachment theory and psychoanalysis. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1938[1940]). An outline of psycho-analysis. Int. J. Psychoanal. 23, 27–84.

Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B, Biol. Sci. 360, 815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory?. Nature reviews. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2023). Computational psychiatry: from synapses to sentience. Mol. Psychiatry 28, 256–268. doi: 10.1038/s41380-022-01743-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Kilner, J., and Harrison, L. (2006). A free energy principle for the brain. J. Physiol. 100, 70–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelauff, J. (2020). Functional motor disorders: Mechanism, prognosis and treatment. Groningen: University of Groningen.

Google Scholar

Gilmour, G. S., Nielsen, G., Teodoro, T., Yogarajah, M., Coebergh, J. A., Dilley, M. D., et al. (2020). Management of functional neurological disorder. J. Neurol. 267, 2164–2172. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-09772-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gimpl, G., and Fahrenholz, F. (2001). The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol. Rev. 81, 629–683. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.2.629

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, A. (1986). The dead mother. On private madness. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.

Google Scholar

Grillon, C., and Ameli, R. (2001). Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle and skin conductance in humans. Psychophysiology 38, 807–815. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3850807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., and Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1389–1398. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00465-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs, M., and Domes, G. (2008). Neuropeptides and social behaviour: effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans. Prog. Brain Res. 170, 337–350. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00428-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs, M., von Dawans, B., and Domes, G. (2009). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behaviour. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30, 548–557. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, J. P., McKlveen, J. M., Ghosal, S., Kopp, B., Wulsin, A., Makinson, R., et al. (2016). Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenocortical stress response. Compr. Physiol. 6, 603–621. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c150015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hur, Y. M., Jin, H. J., Lee, S., and Kim, J. W. (2019). Shared genetic etiology of Hwabyung (anger syndrome) and somatization symptoms in south Korean adolescent and young adult twins. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 114–119. doi: 10.1017/thg.2019.6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R., and Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 129–136. doi: 10.1038/35053579

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, Y., Song, D., Yan, Y., Quan, Z., and Qing, H. (2023). The role of oxytocin in early-life-stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 24:10430. doi: 10.3390/ijms241310430

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffe, W. G., and Sandler, J. (1967). On the concept of pain, with special reference to depression and psychogenic pain. J. Psychosom. Res. 11, 69–75. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90058-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonas, W., Nissen, E., and Ransjö-Arvidson, A. (2008). Influence of oxytocin or epidural analgesia on personality profile in breastfeeding women: a comparative study. Arch. Womens Ment. Health 11, 335–345. doi: 10.1007/s00737-008-0027-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Josselyn, S., Falls, W., and Gewirtz, J. (2004). The nucleus accumbens is not critically involved in mediating the effects of a safety signal on behaviour. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 17–26. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300530

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendrick, K. M. (2000). Oxytocin, motherhood and bonding. Exp. Physiol. 85, 111S–124S.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kinsbourne, M. (2001). Psychoanalysis and cognitive neuropsychology: complementary approaches to mental structure: commentary by Marcel Kinsbourne (New York). Neuropsychoanalysis 3, 24–27. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2001.10773330

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch, I., Deacon, B. J., Huedo-Medina, T. B., Scoboria, A., Moore, T. J., and Johnson, B. T. (2008). Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: a meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. PLoS Med. 5:e45. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoll, A. T., and Carlezon, W. A. Jr. (2010). Dynorphin, stress, and depression. Brain Res. 1314, 56–73. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.074

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamanna, J., Isotti, F., Ferro, M., Spadini, S., Racchetti, G., Musazzi, L., et al. (2022). Occlusion of dopamine-dependent synaptic plasticity in the prefrontal cortex mediates the expression of depressive-like behaviour and is modulated by ketamine. Sci. Rep. 12:11055. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-14694-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leckman, J. F., Carter, C. S., Hennessy, M. B., Hrdy, S. B., Keverne, E. B., Klann-Delius, C., et al. (2005). “Biobehavioural processes in attachment and bonding” in Attachment and bonding: a new synthesis. eds. C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossmann, S. B. Hrdy, M. E. Lamb, and S. W. Porges, et al. (Boston, MA: The MIT Press).

Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 23, 727–738. doi: 10.1023/A:1025048802629

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaitre-Sillère, V., and Bennett, P. (1998). The infant with a depressed mother: destruction and creation. J. Anal. Psychol. 43, 509–521. doi: 10.1111/1465-5922.00050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Feldman, R., and Weller, A. (2007). Oxytocin during pregnancy and early postpartum: individual patterns and maternal–fetal attachment. Peptides 28, 1162–1169. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2007.04.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, F., Xu, Q., Jiang, M., Feng, R., Jiang, S., Yuan, B., et al. (2021). Emotion induced monoamine neuromodulator release affects functional neurological disorders. Front. Cell Dev. Biol. 9:633048. doi: 10.3389/fcell.2021.633048

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lightman, S. L., Birnie, M. T., and Conway-Campbell, B. L. (2020). Dynamics of ACTH and cortisol secretion and implications for disease. Endocr. Rev. 41:bnaa002. doi: 10.1210/endrev/bnaa002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipton, D. M., Gonzales, B. J., and Citri, A. (2019). Dorsal striatal circuits for habits, compulsions and addictions. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 13:28. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, C., and Ma, J.-L. (2019). Adult attachment style, emotion regulation, and social networking sites addiction. Front. Psychol. 10:2352. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02352

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Love, T. M. (2014). Oxytocin, motivation and the role of dopamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 119, 49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.06.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Love, T. M. (2018). The impact of oxytocin on stress: the role of sex. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 23, 136–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.06.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucht, M. J., Barnow, S., Sonnenfeld, C., Rosenberger, A., Grabe, H. J., Schroeder, W., et al. (2009). Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and affect, loneliness and intelligence in normal subjects. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 860–866. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludwig, L., Pasman, J. A., Nicholson, T., Aybek, S., David, A. S., Tuck, S., et al. (2018). Stressful life events and maltreatment in conversion (functional neurological) disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis of case-control studies. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 307–320. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30051-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McSweeney, F. K., Murphy, E. S., and Kowal, B. P. (2005). Regulation of drug taking by sensitization and habituation. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 13, 163–184. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.3.163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meinlschmidt, G., and Heim, C. (2007). Sensitivity to intranasal oxytocin in adult men with early parental separation. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 1109–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mezzacappa, E. S., and Katlin, E. S. (2002). Breast-feeding is associated with reduced perceived stress and negative mood in mothers. Health Psychol. 21, 187–193. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.187

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michels, R. (2010). The Mind-Brain Barrier in 2010. Neuropsychoanalysis 12, 30–31. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2010.10773625

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mickelson, K. D., Kessler, R. C., and Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attachment in a nationally representative sample. Adult attachment in a nationally representative sample. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 73, 1092–1106. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1092

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, G. (2013). The promise and perils of oxytocin. Science 339, 267–269. doi: 10.1126/science.339.6117.267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mobbs, D., Yu, R., Rowe, J. B., Eich, H., FeldmanHall, O., and Dalgleish, T. (2010). Neural activity associated with monitoring the oscillating threat value of a tarantula. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 107, 20582–20586. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009076107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, B., and Bates, G. W. (1997). Adult attachment styles and vulnerability to depression. Personal. Individ. Differ. 22, 835–844. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00277-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, E. E., and Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22, 437–452. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00052-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olds, D. D. (2012). Identification: The concept and the phenomenon. Oxford: Oxford UP, 439–454.

Google Scholar

Ozkarar, G., and Clarici, A. (2008). A meta-analysis on the antidepressant effects of oxytocin. In Poster presented at the 9th International Congress of the Nuero-psychoanalytic Society. “The Self in Conflict: neuropsychoanalytic perspectives”. Montreal

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (1981). “Brain opioids: a neurochemical substrate for narcotic and social dependence” in Progress in theory in psychopharmacology. ed. S. Cooper (London: Academic Press), 149–175.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (1992). Oxytocin effects on emotional processes: PANIC/GRIEF, social bonding, and relationships to psychiatric disorders. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 652, 243–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb34359.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2005a). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 30–80. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2005b). Commentary on “integrating the psychoanalytic and neurobiological views of panic disorder”. Neuropsychoanalysis 7, 145–150. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2005.10773487

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2009). Primary process affects and brain oxytocin. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 725–727. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2010). Affective neuroscience of the emotional BrainMind: evolutionary perspectives and implications for understanding depression. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 12, 533–545. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2010.12.4/jpanksepp

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2011a). “The neurobiology of social loss in animals: some keys to the puzzle of psychic pain in humans” in Social pain: Neuropsychological and health implications of loss and exclusion. eds. G. MacDonald and L. A. Jensen-Campbell (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 11–51.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2011b). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS One 6:e21236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Biven, L. (2012). The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Herman, B. H., Vilberg, T., Bishop, P., and DeEskinazi, F. G. (1980). Endogenous opioids and social behaviour. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 4, 473–487. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(80)90036-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Jalowiec, J., DeEskinazi, F. G., and Bishop, P. (1985). Opiates and play dominance in juvenile rats. Behav. Neurosci. 99, 441–453. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.99.3.441

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Watt, D. (2011). Why does depression hurt? Ancestral primary-process separation-distress (PANIC/GRIEF) and diminished brain reward (SEEKING) processes in the genesis of depressive affect. Psychiatry 74, 5–13. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2011.74.1.5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patron, V. G., Rustomji, Y., Yip, C., and Jenkins, L. M. (2022). Psychiatric comorbidities in functional neurologic symptom disorder. Pract. Neurol. 21, 71–75. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9836030/.

Google Scholar

Pedersen, C., Ascher, J., and Monroe, Y. (1982). Oxytocin induces maternal behaviour in virgin female rats. Science 216, 648–650. doi: 10.1126/science.7071605

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellizzoni, S., Vecchiet, C., Bembich, S., Tripani, A., Natale, R., Guaschino, S., et al. (2009). Convergence between psychoanalytic and neurofunctional model of post partum depression. In 10th international congress of the neuro-psychoanalytic society. Paris

Google Scholar

Perjoc, R. S., Roza, E., Vladacenco, O. A., Teleanu, D. M., Neacsu, R., and Teleanu, R. I. (2023). Functional neurological disorder-old problem new perspective. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:1099. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20021099

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezawas, L., and Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2005). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 828–834. doi: 10.1038/nn1463

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quattrocki, E., and Friston, K. (2014). Autism, oxytocin and interoception. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 410–430. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rockliff, H., Karl, A., McEwan, K., Gilbert, J., Matos, M., and Gilbert, P. (2011). Effects of intranasal oxytocin on 'Compassion focused Imagery, vol. 11. Washington, D.C.: Emotion, 1388–1396.

Google Scholar

Rogan, M. T., Leon, K. S., Perez, D. L., and Kandel, E. R. (2005). Distinct neural signatures for safety and danger in the amygdala and striatum of the mouse. Neuron 46, 309–320. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sack, M., Spieler, D., Wizelman, L., Epple, G., Stich, J., Zaba, M., et al. (2017). Intranasal oxytocin reduces provoked symptoms in female patients with posttraumatic stress disorder despite exerting sympathomimetic and positive chronotropic effects in a randomized controlled trial. BMC Med. 15:40. doi: 10.1186/s12916-017-0801-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandler, J. (1960). The background of safety. Int. J. Psychoanal. 41, 352–356

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sandler, J. (1976). Countertransference and role-responsiveness. Int Rev Psychoanal. 3, 43–47.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sandler, J. J., and Joffe, W. G. (1968). Comments on the psychoanalytic psychology of adaptation. Int. J. Psychoanal. 49:445.

Google Scholar

Sandler, J., and Joffe, W. G. (1969). Towards a basic psychoanalytic model. Int. J. Psychoanal. 50, 79–90

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Saphire-Bernstein, S., Way, B. M., Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., and Taylor, S. E. (2011). Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is related to psychological resources. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 15118–15122. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113137108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scantamburlo, G., Hansenne, M., Fuchs, S., Pitchot, W., Maréchal, P., Pequeux, C., et al. (2007). Plasma oxytocin levels and anxiety in patients with major depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 407–410. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiller, D., Levy, I., Niv, Y., LeDoux, J. E., and Phelps, E. A. (2008). From fear to safety and Back: reversal of fear in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 28, 11517–11525. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2265-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M., and Friston, K. (2018). How and why consciousness arises. Some considerations from physics and physiology. J. Conscious. Stud. 25, 1–37. Available at: https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/10057681/1/Friston_Paper.pdf.

Google Scholar

Solms, M., and Panksepp, J. (2012). The ‘id’ knows more than the ‘ego’ admits. Brain Sci. 2, 147–175. doi: 10.3390/brainsci2020147

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, D. N. (2014). “The early development of schemas of self, other, and “self with other”” in Reflections on self psychology (psychology revivals) (London: Routledge), 49–84.

Google Scholar

Stevens, F. L., Weisman, O., Feldman, R., Hurley, R. A., and Taber, K. H. (2013). Oxytocin and behaviour: evidence for effects in the brain. J Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 25, 96–102. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.13030061

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., and Montague, P. (2009). Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 2655–2666. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundar, M., Patel, D., Young, Z., and Leong, K.-C. (2021). Oxytocin and addiction: potential glutamatergic mechanisms. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22:2405. doi: 10.3390/ijms22052405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Takayanagi, Y., and Onaka, T. (2021). Roles of oxytocin in stress responses, Allostasis and resilience. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 23:150. doi: 10.3390/ijms23010150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tammilehto, J., Bosmans, G., Kuppens, P., Flykt, M., Peltonen, K., Kerns, K. A., et al. (2022). Dynamics of attachment and emotion regulation in daily life: uni- and bidirectional associations. Cognit. Emot. 36, 1109–1131. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2022.2081534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., van Peer, J. M., Korf, J., Wijers, A. A., and Tucker, D. M. (2007). Anxiety, cortisol, and attachment predict plasma oxytocin. Psychophysiology 44, 444–449. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00510.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Torgersen, S. (1986). Genetics of somatoform disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 43, 502–505. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800050108014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhl, G. R., Koob, G. F., and Cable, J. (2019). The neurobiology of addiction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1451, 5–28. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13989

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (1998). Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 819–835. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00056-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elst, L. T., Hesslinger, B., and Thiel, T. (2003). Frontolimbic brain abnormalities in patients with borderline personality disorder: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 163–171. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01743-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vrtička, P., Bondolfi, G., Sander, D., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). The neural substrates of social emotion perception and regulation are modulated by adult attachment style. Soc. Neurosci. 7, 473–493. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.647410

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vrtička, P., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Neuroscience of human social interactions and adult attachment style. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:212. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00212

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, S. C., and McGlone, F. P. (2013). The social brain: neurobiological basis of affiliative behaviours and psychological well-being. Neuropeptides 47, 379–393. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2013.10.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Windle, R. J., Shanks, N., Lightman, S. L., and Ingram, C. D. (1997). Central oxytocin administration reduces stress-induced corticosterone release and anxiety behaviour in rats. Endocrinology 138, 2829–2834. doi: 10.1210/endo.138.7.5255

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winnicott, D. (1965). Maturational processes and the facilitating environment: Studies in the theory of emotional development. London: Hogarth Press.

Google Scholar

Yong, E. (2012). Dark side of the love hormone. New Sci. 213, 39–41. doi: 10.1016/S0262-4079(12)60377-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, L. J., and Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1048–1054. doi: 10.1038/nn1327

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yovell, Y., Bar, G., Mashiah, M., Baruch, Y., Briskman, I., Asherov, J., et al. (2016). Ultra-low-dose buprenorphine as a time-limited treatment for severe suicidal ideation: a randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 491–498. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15040535

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yovell, Y., Solms, M., and Fotopoulou, A. (2015). The case for neuropsychoanalysis: why a dialogue with neuroscience is necessary but not sufficient for psychoanalysis. Int. J. Psychoanal. 96, 1515–1553. doi: 10.1111/1745-8315.12332

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zorrilla, E. P., Logrip, M. L., and Koob, G. F. (2014). Corticotropin releasing factor: a key role in the neurobiology of addiction. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 35, 234–244. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neuromodulators of depression, neurofunctional disorders, internal and external safety, interoception, predictive coding, free energy principle, mother–infant relationship, infant development

Citation: Clarici A, Bulfon M, Radin Y and Panksepp J (2024) Neuromodulation of safety and surprise in the early stages of infant development: affective homeostatic regulation in bodily and mental functions. Front. Psychol. 15:1395247. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1395247

Received: 03 March 2024; Accepted: 22 May 2024;
Published: 06 June 2024.

Edited by:

Daniela Flores Mosri, Universidad Intercontinental, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Brian Johnson, Upstate Medical University, United States
Simon Boag, Macquarie University, Australia

Copyright © 2024 Clarici, Bulfon, Radin and Panksepp. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrea Clarici, Y2xhcmljaUB1bml0cy5pdA==

Deceased

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.