Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 13 June 2024
Sec. Cognition

Priors and prejudice: hierarchical predictive processing in intergroup perception

  • 1School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 2Brain & Cognition Group, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands

Hierarchical predictive processing provides a framework outlining how prior expectations shape perception and cognition. Here, we highlight hierarchical predictive processing as a framework for explaining how social context and group-based social knowledge can directly shape intergroup perception. More specifically, we argue that hierarchical predictive processing confers a uniquely valuable toolset to explain extant findings and generate novel hypotheses for intergroup perception. We first provide an overview of hierarchical predictive processing, specifying its primary theoretical assumptions. We then review evidence showing how prior knowledge influences intergroup perception. Next, we outline how hierarchical predictive processing can account well for findings in the intergroup perception literature. We then underscore the theoretical strengths of hierarchical predictive processing compared to other frameworks in this space. We finish by outlining future directions and laying out hypotheses that test the implications of hierarchical predictive processing for intergroup perception and intergroup cognition more broadly. Taken together, hierarchical predictive processing provides explanatory value and capacity for novel hypothesis generation for intergroup perception.

Introduction

Insights from neuroscientific research have improved understanding of how intergroup bias is formed, strengthened, and maintained (Cikara and Van Bavel, 2014; Bagnis et al., 2019; McGovern and Vanman, 2020; Amodio and Cikara, 2021 for reviews). At the same time, emergent literature suggests computational accounts of social cognition could explain how intergroup attitudes are encoded and how they impact behavior (Freeman and Ambady, 2011; Xiao et al., 2016; Hackel and Amodio, 2018; Amodio, 2019). Computational frameworks provide insight into how prejudice or stereotypes can shape early cognitive processing, such as intergroup perception. One influential framework combining knowledge from neuroscience and computational approaches is hierarchical predictive processing (from here on predictive processing (Friston, 2010; Hohwy, 2013). Here, we suggest that predictive processing provides a cohesive account of biased intergroup perception. This framework can offer a degree of theoretical parsimony not offered by competing frameworks and enables novel hypothesis generation (see Table 1). Adoption of this framework, in turn, may enable an improved understanding of biased intergroup perception, offering space for the creation of novel interventions for reducing intergroup bias.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Outline of possible experimental setups and predictions arising from a predictive processing framework.

Hierarchical predictive processing

Bayes theorem suggests that in estimating the probability of any event, one should consider the current evidence for that event but also the prior knowledge about the event. Predictive processing is a theory that translates this insight to cognition, specifically focusing on perception (Joyce, 2019). In this framework, a perceiver has an internal “model” of the outside world and continuously updates this model based on new information (Kersten et al., 2004; Knill and Pouget, 2004; Bar, 2007; Friston and Kiebel, 2009; Friston, 2010; Clark, 2013; Hohwy, 2013, 2017; Otten et al., 2017; Williams, 2018).

The predictive processing framework assumes that any perceptual process, where the organism acquires new information about the outside world in the form of sensory information, results in an inference about the cause of that sensory data, a so-called posterior. Each posterior is a combination of the likelihood and prior. The likelihood is a representation of the data sampled in the sensorium, which includes both the sensory input and an estimate of the reliability of that sensory data. The prior is the initial prediction (before any sensory information is encountered) about the cause of the sensory data. The prior is often shaped by memory and thus by previous experiences.

In hierarchical predictive processing—namely, active inference under deep generative models—there are priors at every level of the implicit model. Prior beliefs at intermediate levels are known as ‘empirical’ priors because they are updated by evidence from lower levels. In short, empirical prior beliefs depend upon experience. This is often reflected in the description of belief updating, which uses today’s posteriors as tomorrow’s priors.

Priors can be held with greater or lesser precision. A very precise conviction or commitment to a prior belief (e.g., innate subpersonal priors that underwrite homeostasis) is clearly less amenable to updating. Conversely, prior beliefs at higher levels are generally held with less precision or confidence and are more amenable to updating, through perceptual inference, or long-term revision, through learning and experience-dependent plasticity.

The predictive processing framework has been successfully applied to understanding visual perception, supported by a wealth of research in recent years, suggesting that it offers a plausible account for how visual information is encoded, represented, and predicted in cognitive systems (Mumford, 1992; Rao and Ballard, 1999; Spratling, 2010, 2017; Rauss et al., 2011; Bastos et al., 2012; Clark, 2013; Kok and de Lange, 2015; Hindy et al., 2016; Schellekens et al., 2016; Shipp, 2016; Edwards et al., 2017; Homann et al., 2017; Alexander and Brown, 2018; Friston, 2018; Hogendoorn and Burkitt, 2018; Keller and Mrsic-Flogel, 2018; Pereira et al., 2019). It is worth noting a recent review that found mixed support for key tenets of predictive processing (Walsh et al., 2020). In particular, the review found partial support that the process of minimizing prediction error occurs via an inferential hierarchy (see below), moderate support for the notion that expectations attenuate sensory processing, and modest support for the notion that error signals should be larger insofar as sensory data departs from expectation. For what follows, it should thus be noted that predictive processing still has several empirical hurdles to clear to be considered a comprehensive approach to brain function, and future work remains to test and refine key tenets of predictive processing. Acknowledging these limitations, we nonetheless move forward with the assumption that predictive processing at least offers a useful framework for conceptualizing brain functioning and perception.

Predictive processing departs from classical notions of the brain as a passive perceiver of information, recasting perception as the brain’s “best guess” of the hidden causes of data in the sensorium (see Friston, 2010). Predictive processing assumes that the trade-off between priors, likelihood, and posteriors is all done in the service of minimizing prediction error—the divergence between expected and sampled sensory data. In this process, the brain generates a model of the world in the service of prediction error minimization. The modeling of the world is thus generative in the sense that it optimizes the inferred causes of sensory signals based on experience and internally generates a set of predictions (priors) about what kinds of sensory encounters it is likely to sample thereafter. Hierarchical predictive processing (or coding) is also known as active inference and is distinguished from predictive coding accounts of perceptual synthesis by emphasizing the enactive and situated aspect of sense-making (e.g., active vision or, more generally, active inference that involves active sampling of the sensorium).

Predictive processing has been applied to understand a range of cognitive processes, from psychiatric disorders (Corlett et al., 2019; McGovern et al., 2022) to emotion perception (Ransom et al., 2020). A growing body of research suggests that perceptions (even if incorrect) depend upon prior learning (Gilbert et al., 2001; Knill, 2007; Powers et al., 2017) and local context (Palmer, 1975; Bar, 2004, 2007; Snyder et al., 2015 for an overview). For example, seeing lips move a certain way makes people likely to hear a word even if there is no actual audio input (Haarsma et al., 2020). Similarly, hearing a tone alongside a visual stimulus makes people more likely to report hearing the tone alongside the visual stimulus even if it no longer remained (Powers et al., 2017). Samaha et al. (2018) found that verbal cues increased recognition and discrimination of ambiguous sensory data. Moreover, improved discrimination was predicted by increased power of the posterior alpha band, suggesting that the previously learned knowledge tuned the brain for future perception (via modulation of alpha-band oscillations), which attenuated early visual attention and thus perception (Samaha et al., 2018, pp. 1; see also Mayer et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2021). This rests upon electrophysiological evidence that ascending prediction errors are conveyed by fast gamma activity while descending predictions are mediated at slower frequencies (Bastos et al., 2015; Michalareas et al., 2016; Pefkou et al., 2017). These spectral asymmetries are just one facet of the empirical evidence for predictive coding in the brain that complements other anatomical and physiological studies (please see Shipp, 2016; Hohwy, 2020; Haarsma et al., 2022; Hodson et al., 2024 for review).

This leaves us two important implications for what follows. First, prior expectations can have a large effect on perceptual processing (Knill and Pouget, 2004; Friston, 2010; Hohwy, 2013, 2017). Perhaps most importantly, expectations (i.e., priors) can result in perceptions not afforded by the actual sensory data, meaning people can see or hear things that are not actually present.

Hierarchical predictive processing in intergroup perception: review of evidence

In what follows, we outline evidence of how priors can influence social and intergroup perception. We assess three lines of evidence. First, we assess the influence of group-based priors on face perception. Second, we assess how group-based priors influence emotional perception. Third, we assess how group-based priors may influence object perception. In synthesizing these complementary lines of evidence, we aim to illustrate the tangible influences that group-based priors can enact on the perpetual process. Note the intention at this point is not to say that this evidence points only to hierarchical predictive processing (this is discussed in the section following the evidence review where we argue how predictive processing may offer the most parsimonious explanations of these findings), before offering predictions and questions for future work (see Table 1 for summary).

Social priors and face perception: early and late processing

Social knowledge about others can modulate early visual perception (Galli et al., 2006; Anderson et al., 2011; Otten et al., 2017 for review). Social knowledge can be based on explicit information about other individuals, such as group membership (Anderson et al., 2011). Stereotypes and prejudices held toward other groups can alter even the earliest phases of intergroup perception (Golby et al., 2001; Hugenberg and Bodenhausen, 2003; Freeman et al., 2011; Freeman and Ambady, 2011; Van Bavel et al., 2011; Ratner and Amodio, 2013; Xiao et al., 2016; Villiger, 2023). In one example, Levin and Banaji (2006) showed that when a face with Black features or White features was shown with identical luminance, participants reported the face with the Black features as darker than the White face. This suggests that the features of the face activated priors about skin color, which, in the absence of conclusive sensory data, influenced the posterior percept representing skin color (see Firestone and Scholl, 2015).

Further evidence of the constraining effect of priors comes from evidence assessing face perception (Trapp et al., 2018; Pereira et al., 2019; Walsh et al., 2020 for an overview). Neural signals involved in early processing relate to the visual processing of faces, such as the N170. The N170 is a neuronal signal with a negative peak occurring over the occipital lobe around 170 ms following stimulus onset (Rossion and Jacques, 2012; Hinojosa et al., 2015). This component is increasingly thought to reflect prediction errors, such that an increased amplitude corresponds to an unexpected stimulus in the visual field (Johnston et al., 2016, 2017; Robinson et al., 2020; Baker et al., 2022). Johnston et al. (2016) had participants belonging to a more frequently presented identity, a less frequently presented identity, and a random identity. Low frequency and randomly presented identities induced an N170 response over the occipital lobe. Johnston et al. (2016) suggest that the visual system detects mismatches (here between the expected and actual identity of a face or prediction errors) between expected and sampled visual data, in line with predictive processing.

The N170 component is implicated in social (Johnston et al., 2016; Walla et al., 2020) and even intergroup perception (Ito et al., 2004; Galli et al., 2006; Bentin et al., 2007; Ibáñez et al., 2010). If the N170 component indeed (at least partially) represents prediction error, then stereotype-inconsistent face information should solicit an N170 response. An extensive literature lends support to this notion (Ito and Urland, 2003; Herrmann et al., 2007; Stahl et al., 2008; Ibáñez et al., 2010; Ofan et al., 2011). Ibáñez et al. (2010), for example, found that outgroup faces produced an increased N170 response when paired with positive words but not negative words. The fact that the brain detects a departure between expectations and sampled data in the sensorium (a.k.a. prediction error) this early in the visual process lends support to the idea that group-based priors can shape even the earliest stages of visual perception.

Group stereotypes and emotion perception

It is not only person-level knowledge that influences early perception. Social knowledge about groups, such as stereotypes, modulates how individuals from that stereotyped group are subsequently perceived and acted toward (Payne, 2001; Correll et al., 2002; Eberhardt et al., 2004; Unkelbach et al., 2008; Villiger, 2023). The effect of social knowledge on intergroup perception occurs across multiple processes, including quicker detection of anger in ambiguous facial expressions for Black compared to White faces (Hugenberg and Bodenhausen, 2003; Hutchings and Haddock, 2008), and differential mental representations of emotions displayed by Black and White faces (Otten and Banaji, 2012). This is in line with the stereotype-based association between Black men and aggression (Hugenberg, 2005; Ackerman et al., 2006; Hutchings and Haddock, 2008; Kang and Chasteen, 2009; Shapiro et al., 2009; Zebrowitz et al., 2010; Neuberg and Schaller, 2016; Kleider-Offutt et al., 2018; Thiem et al., 2019; Raissi and Steele, 2021). Stolier and Freeman (2016a) found that brain areas involved in social category representations, the right fusiform gyrus, and the occipital frontal lobe were activated most when viewing Black and Male faces, both of which are linked to the “dangerous” stereotype (also see Stolier and Freeman, 2016b). The authors suggested this was due to the fact these groups share similar stereotypes, impacting early perceptual processes and neural activation.

Related literature shows an increased probability of misperceiving anger in Black compared to White faces (Hugenberg, 2005; Ackerman et al., 2006; Hutchings and Haddock, 2008; Kang and Chasteen, 2009; Shapiro et al., 2009; Zebrowitz et al., 2010; Kleider-Offutt et al., 2018; Thiem et al., 2019; Raissi and Steele, 2021) particularly for those higher in safety concern (Cook et al., 2018) and implicit prejudice (Hutchings and Haddock, 2008). Those with higher safety concerns and self-protection motivation are also more likely to perceive anger in Arabian versus White faces (Becker et al., 2011).

The link between stereotypes and emotion perception is not limited to racial or ethnic stereotypes. Perceived gender also impacts emotion perception (Craig and Lee, 2020, for review). Female faces are faster to be perceived as happy (Becker et al., 2007; Aguado et al., 2009; Tipples, 2019), whereas male faces are faster to be perceived as angry (Le Gal and Bruce, 2002; Smith et al., 2017; Taylor, 2017; Primbs et al., 2022). Craig and Lee (2020) suggest that the tendency to see happiness in female faces is linked to the (implicit) association between female and positive words (Bijlstra et al., 2010; Craig and Lipp, 2018). Similarly, the effects of stereotypes on emotion perception are not limited to facial expressions; the perception of posture is also influenced. Faces are not the only source of social information. Karmali and Kawakami (2023) found that Black targets with an expansive body posture were perceived as more aggressive than White targets with the same expansive body posture. Taken together, these findings suggest that (higher-order) group-level knowledge, such as racial or sex stereotypes, bears influence on subsequent emotion perception.

Group stereotypes influence the perception of objects and bodies

Group-based social priors do not just alter the perception of faces or the perception of emotion in those faces; there is also evidence that object perception is altered. For example, when shown images of a Black or White face, followed by an image of a weapon or a tool, people are quicker to and more often correctly identify targets as weapons after Black faces are presented compared to when White faces are presented, and even misperceive tools as weapons (Eberhardt et al., 2004; Payne et al., 2005; Payne, 2006; Kidder et al., 2018). These effects of race primes on object perception extend beyond the categorization of the object itself (gun vs. not a gun); they are also visible in decisions to shoot or not shoot in a first-person shooter game based on the presumed presence of a gun (Correll et al., 2002, 2007; Plant and Peruche, 2005; Sadler et al., 2012; Mange et al., 2015). In these experiments, participants should only shoot when the person on the screen carries a gun and not shoot when the person is carrying a different object, such as a phone. These decisions to shoot differ depending on the race of the person that is on screen: participants are faster to shoot a Black gun-carrying target (compared to a White gun-carrying target) while they are slower to not shoot a Black unarmed target compared to White unarmed target (see Mekawi and Bresin, 2015 for a meta-analysis), particularly in participants who are led to associate Black faces with danger (Correll et al., 2002).

Together with the studies on race-priming on gun identification, these studies show that group-based priors can shape how people perceive and respond to objects and that this can confer detrimental real-world consequences. Of course, this work is not as conclusive on whether top-down knowledge would exert its effects in the absence of sensory cues. Future work should continue to disambiguate whether top-down knowledge exerts effects on weapon awareness (i.e., whether we are faster to be aware of weapons when primed with a Black but not White face; see Stein et al., 2023) or whether the effect of race only matters after initial perceptual processing (Freeman and Ambady, 2011; Firestone and Scholl, 2015).

Advantages of predictive processing

Having discussed evidence of the effect of top-down knowledge on intergroup perception, we now turn to the discussion of the advantages of conceptualizing such findings through a predictive processing lens. We start by comparing predictive processing to alternative theories of social perception, including grounded cognition (Barsalou, 2008) and the dynamic interactive theory of person construal (DIPT) (Freeman and Ambady, 2011). We then outline the theoretical advantages conferred by predictive processing before outlining hypotheses and directions for future work.

Hierarchical predictive processing compared to other models of social perception

Work dating back to the 1980s (McCauley et al., 1980) has utilized the Bayesian approach to understanding intergroup perception (Hinton, 2017; Otten et al., 2017; Villiger, 2023), social cognition (Freeman and Ambady, 2011; Shafto et al., 2012; Gershman and Cikara, 2020; Vélez and Gweon, 2021), and impression formation (Kunda and Thagard, 1996; Van Rooy et al., 2003; Willis and Todorov, 2006). The challenge remaining here, then, is to explain why predictive processing, as a particular implementation of the Bayesian Brain Hypothesis, offers explanatory and hypotheses-generating advantages.

Predictive processing departs from alternate frameworks in several ways. Alternate frameworks often assume that social knowledge comes online only after the percept reaches awareness (Barsalou, 2008). Grounded cognition, for example, posits that social cognition is an internal simulation of re-enactment and implements shared knowledge from various modalities during the perceptual process (Barsalou, 2008). Contrary to predictive processing, grounded cognition posits that these internal simulations are only influential following the initial processing of sensory input of some target stimulus (Barsalou, 2003, 2008; Decety and Grèzes, 2006; Goldman, 2006; Meyer and Damasio, 2009). A shortcoming of this framework becomes apparent when considering binocular rivalry findings. These experiments show effects of motivation (Balcetis and Dunning, 2006; Balcetis et al., 2012; Radel and Clément-Guillotin, 2012), affective state (Jolij and Meurs, 2011), and expectation (Lupyan and Ward, 2013; Pinto et al., 2015; Seth, 2015) on the eventual percept. These internally generated priors not only influenced what participants were likely to report but also whether the stimulus reached consciousness at all. If it were the case that internal models are important only following perception, as suggested by grounded cognition, it remains unclear how internal expectations can influence what is perceived before the perceiver is even conscious of the percept. Predictive processing, on the other hand, posits that percepts arise from internally generated predictions rather than a faithful read out of sense data (Seth, 2013; Barrett and Simmons, 2015). Resultingly, expectations based on experience play a crucial role in what information is searched for and is preferred in visual search space. In turn, predictive processing suggests that no initial stimulus needs to be there to generate a percept in line with these expectations. This would explain why predictions can be generated in the absence of conscious awareness and do not require sensory stimuli. Indeed, the phenomena of dreaming—during rapid eye movement sleep—speaks of the fact that the brain can generate percepts while sequestered from the sensorium [please (see Hobson, 2009) for treatment of this under predictive processing].

Another influential theory that has arisen in recent years is Dynamic Interactive Theory of Personal Construal (from here on DIPT). DIPT builds on ideas from connectionism (Read and Miller, 1993; Kunda and Thagard, 1996; Read et al., 1997; Smith and DeCoster, 1998, 1999; Zebrowitz et al., 2003; Van Overwalle, 2007; Van Overwalle and Labiouse, 2013) and dynamical systems theory (Rumelhart et al., 1986; Smolensky, 1990; Van Gelder and Port, 1995; Rogers and McClelland, 2004). In essence, DIPT assumes that perception of others is gradually built up via cyclical interactions between “categories, high-level cognitive states, and the low-level processing of facial, vocal, and bodily cues” (Freeman and Ambady, 2011, pp. 250). Once a perceptual cue is presented, it begins a cascade of interaction between neural nodes of mutual inhibition and excitation that encode social categories (i.e., if there is a male visual cue, then this might excite the angry category and inhibit the happy category), the net process of which is a percept that integrates bottom-up social input and top-down social knowledge (Freeman and Ambady, 2011). However, this is also difficult to reconcile with research that purposely exposes participants to ambiguous stimuli. In such cases, participants frequently rely on existing priors and can arrive at a percept without clear sensory evidence belonging to a clear category (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974; Correll et al., 2002, 2014; Eberhardt et al., 2004; Neth and Gigerenzer, 2015). The fact that perceptual inferences and judgments can be determined without categorical cues offers support to a predictive processing framework, given the fact that the cognitive system (in this circumstance) can generate a percept without a clear stimulus to kickstart the bottom-up/top-down integration proposed by DIPT. Predictive processing suggests that when there is ambiguous sensory data at hand, the brain reverts to priors in attempting to generate predictions about what is causing the sensory data. We would thus expect that when there are unclear perceptual cues (i.e., sensory data that does not clearly belong to any particular category), resulting percepts would be most likely to be consistent with the social agents prior (i.e., existing expectations based on past experience). More precisely, if (under DIPT) there is highly ambiguous data at hand, it does not offer an explanation of how the brain then makes inference on what category the sensory data belongs to (e.g., whether a conspecific whom we cannot see well belongs to one race over another). In this situation, predictive processing suggests that the category (e.g., what racial category does this person belong to) under ambiguous circumstances can be arrived at purely based on prior experience. As such, prior experience tilts the perceptual judgement arrived at toward that consistent with prior experience.

Perhaps a key departure of predictive processing (and why it carries explanatory advantage) is the emphasis placed on attention (Friston, 2010). DIPT outlines that once a percept reaches awareness, it begins a cascading sequence of interactions between excitatory/inhibitory nodes (Freeman and Ambady, 2011), which enables a convergence toward an eventual social percept. However, predictive processing suggests that agentic motives, the social environment, and social knowledge tune the types of information being attended to or sought out in the first instance (Hung, 2023). This is particularly important in explaining how ambiguous stimuli are more likely to be perceived in line with preexisting stereotypes (Hugenberg and Bodenhausen, 2003; Rahman and Sommer, 2012).

Why is prejudice hard to undo? Attention, precision, and prediction errors

As discussed above, environmental cues can influence what percepts form during social perception, and agents’ stronger priors can be uniquely difficult to undo. This is a particular problem for prejudice, as the most strongly prejudiced beliefs could be the most difficult to undo (Hung, 2023). Indeed, more strongly held beliefs are less amendable to update than those less strongly held (Kunda and Sinclair, 1999; Sinclair and Kunda, 1999; Jost et al., 2017), decreasing the probability of belief update, independent of their accuracy (Kunda and Sinclair, 1999; Sinclair and Kunda, 1999). Recent research underscores how motivation can influence early perceptual and subsequent categorization processes—deemed motivated perception (Leong et al., 2019). Participants were instructed to complete a task of visual categorization. Each participant was presented with a composite image of a face within a visual scene, and participants were instructed to say whether the scene consisted of more face, or more background scene. Subsequently, participants were told that they could win money if the next image was of a particular category (i.e., face or scene), to shift participant motivation to observe a scene or a face. After this, participants were more likely to label ambiguous images as those with the reward, and this was the case regardless of whether their perceptions were incorrect. These results suggest that agentic motivations heavily influence what visual information is attended to, but that agentic beliefs can remain rigid even when contradictory evidence is shown.

What accounts for this inability to update priors even when clear contradictory information is available? Active inference presumes that social agents do not come into any social perceptual process as passive perceivers of information. Motivations, context, and intentions can bias which priors are being acted upon and tested during the perceptual process (i.e., what information is sought out and thus attended to (Otten et al., 2017)). Put simply, prior precision influences what is being attended to. Moreover, what is being attended to is more likely to solicit a stronger prediction error signal when there is a mismatch between prediction and sense data (Friston, 2010). However, when attention toward a stimulus increases, error signals will be given more weight, and when attention is decreased, error signals are given less weight (Friston, 2010; Hung, 2023). This means that prediction errors not being attended to during perception will be ascribed less precision and thus be less likely to influence the posterior belief. In terms of group-based priors, this means that counter-stereotypical information is down-weighted during the perceptual process due to the increased attention on stereotype-consistent information—particularly for stronger (i.e., more precise) prejudicial priors (Hung, 2023). In a complementary fashion, the relative imprecision of sensory evidence (e.g., ambiguous stimuli) also allows priors to dominate. This follows because the relative contribution of the prior and (likelihood) of sensory evidence rests upon their relative precision. In the predictive processing literature, Bayesian belief updating is often described as minimizing precision-weighted prediction error (Hohwy, 2012). This is important because the precision per se has to be predicted, leading to a formal notion of attention as affording a high degree of confidence or precision to various sources of evidence (i.e., prediction errors). The posterior belief is therefore more likely to fall in line with the initial group-based prior (i.e., stereotype-consistent information). Over time, this may be what renders group-based priors and stereotypes difficult to undo.

In summary, we suggest some strengths of predictive processing, which are as follows: (1) its ability to explain biased perceptions in conditions of uncertainty, (2) it can explain how people arrive at biased perceptions of ambiguous stimuli, (3) it explains well how contextual conditions push perception towards biased perceptions, and (4) it provides a comprehensive account of why the most prejudiced people would be the least likely to revise biased beliefs. Below, we outline future directions and predictions that could test the predictive processing account of prejudice.

Future directions

We have thus far outlined evidence for top-down effects of motivation, environment, and strength of priors for biased social processing and outlined why predictive processing may be a robust framework for explaining such findings. Below, we discuss future research avenues that could test tenets of predictive processing in intergroup perception.

Perceptual versus post-perceptual effects

The studies reviewed here show that people report seeing different emotions or objects or respond differently to different faces or objects, depending on their existing knowledge about social groups and individuals. This suggests that stereotypes and prejudice that priors shape initial precepts in the earliest stages of intergroup perception, and more so when sense data is ambiguous (see above). Nonetheless, the observed effects of stereotypes could rely on changes in how the participants respond to these precepts.

To establish whether behavioral response patterns indicating the influence of group-based priors are the result of perceptual or response-related effects, reaction time and accuracy data could be analyzed using methods that can distinguish between response biases and perceptual effects. Signal detection theory (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999) analyses the patterns of responding (hits, misses, correct rejections, and false alarms) to determine how easily two categories of stimuli can be perceptually distinguished from each other (d’) and how far the responders are biased to respond with one of the two response options (the criterion). For example, if the presence of a Black face prime makes it easier to perceptually distinguish guns from tools, then this should be reflected in a shift in d’ compared to white face primes (see, for example, Greenwald et al., 2003). However, if participants are more likely to respond “gun” after seeing a Black face compared to a white face, this will only shift the criterion (Payne et al., 2005; Klauer and Voss, 2008). Some research suggests that, at least with respect to the gun/tool task, there are no perceptual effects (Klauer and Voss, 2008; Burke, 2015; Payne and Correll, 2020). Future research, however, could apply this paradigm to face and emotion perception. For example, to see whether the presence of a black face makes one more likely to perceive anger, and vice versa.

Drift diffusion modeling (Ratcliff and McKoon, 2008) considers patterns of responding related to reaction times to estimate perceptual processing and response biases. From the resulting parameters, the drift rates and the starting point are particularly relevant to distinguish perceptual effects from response bias. On the one hand, the drift rate indicates how quickly sensory evidence is collected toward a specific response (say, identifying a face as angry). On the other hand, the starting point shows whether the system is already biased toward one response option (angry) over the other (happy) before any sensory information is processed. Again, this could be employed to see whether a prejudicial starting point pushes people to learn an association more quickly between some negative feature or emotion for outgroup members. Because active inference provides a formal (mathematical) account of sentient responses, it is possible to model empirical choice behavior or responses under a suitable generative model. By adjusting the priors of this model, one can estimate the prior beliefs held by a subject by optimizing the priors in the generative model to maximize the likelihood of a subject’s responses. This is known as computational phenotyping (Schwartenbeck and Friston, 2016) and has been used to phenotype psychiatric cohorts and neurotypical subjects in a variety of paradigms (Smith et al., 2019, 2020).

Future neuroscientific could also provide insights into the perceptual effects of group-based priors. Electrophysiology, with its high temporal resolution, can illuminate whether group-based priors alter early perceptual or later evaluation and response-related processing. An example of this approach could be seen whether people are more likely and quicker to collect evidence for anger in a Black face than in a White face. If people are put into a fearful or anger-inducing context, it could be that a Black face could be more likely to be inferred from an ambiguous face, an effect which may be modulated by an enhanced alpha response (Mayer et al., 2015).

Another way to employ neuroscience to pinpoint perceptual effects is through decoding the representations of sensory stimuli in the brain. Multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA; Haxby et al., 2014) identifies patterns of activation related to specific categories of stimuli, for example, distinguishing between the patterns of brain activity related to angry and happy faces. With regard to the effects of group-based priors on social perception, classifiers could be employed to test whether the sensory representation of prior-consistent sensory input is sharpened (Kok et al., 2014) or whether established categories (for example, the neural patterns associated with angry or happy facial expressions) generalize to stereotype-induced perceptual categories [for example, ambiguous white and Black faces that appear more or less angry based on the race of the face (Hugenberg and Bodenhausen, 2003)].

Testing prior probabilities

In predictive processing, if the top-down signal can account for the bottom-up prediction error, the prediction error is resolved or ‘explained away’ by inhibitory synaptic mechanisms (Bastos et al., 2012). If the prediction error cannot be explained away, it propagates the hierarchy to update posterior beliefs. These updates then furnish descending predictions that resolve prediction errors until they are explained away—and there is no further drive for updating. Alternatively, if the error signal cannot be explained, it propagates all the way up the perceptual hierarchy, and the internal generative model is updated to account for this error. This error can also be resolved by active inference (see above). In more formal terms, this process is described as Bayesian belief updating, where message passing between neurons consists of an ongoing reciprocity of matching top-down predictions with bottom-up prediction errors (Friston et al., 2017). This ongoing exchange of cascading neural signaling throughout the cortex is therefore the process of building—i.e., inverting and learning—a hierarchically generative model of the world. This process allows the cognitive system to update what it ‘expects’ to sample during perception and to update its model so that it can minimize prediction errors in the future (Friston, 2010).

This tenant of hierarchical predictive processing also makes it easy to see how stereotypes can be used as explanations for otherwise ambiguous sensory data in intergroup perception. As such, what follows from this framework is that only when encountering information highly contradictory to group-based priors do perceptually implemented stereotypes/prejudices become amendable to update. Higher-order beliefs drive the process of perception and belief generation. As such, it is not surprising that (depending on an agent’s motivations and attentional preferences discussed above) higher-order priors may not shift and change as rapidly as those lower on the hierarchy (Kiebel et al., 2008; Eil and Rao, 2011; Huntenburg et al., 2018). For example, when people are asked to guess the probability they will get cancer, they tend to ignore information that goes against their existing belief structures (Sharot et al., 2011). This shows that if information goes against higher order beliefs, and run contrary to an agent’s motivations or desires, they are less likely to be updated. The crucial point, then, is that not only does this predictive process occur in a hierarchical manner, but that higher order priors can be less amendable to update if the existing higher order predictions are positive for the agent and the incoming evidence is negative for the agent. This could be tested further in experimental settings. Researchers could alter prior probabilities of stereotypes to groups and then implement reversal learning paradigms to see how the brain updates the stereotype-group link under varying degrees of prior invariance (see Table 1 for more suggestions). This would allow researchers to assess the degree to which the brain updates the stereotype-group link and why it may or may not do Bayes optimally.

Summary and conclusion

Here, we proposed that predictive processing can offer a framework to explain how prejudiced perception unfolds, develops, and maintains itself over time and why prejudiced priors are difficult to undo. Here, we outlined how biased priors can prejudice early intergroup perceptual processes. We then outlined how these findings fit within a predictive processing framework, applying this framework to understanding intergroup perception and cognition, and highlighted its explanatory power when compared with other prominent theories. We then outlined how future work could test the predictive processing hypothesis in the context of intergroup perception, offering specific hypotheses. While intergroup neuroscience is still somewhat new (Amodio, 2014; Amodio and Cikara, 2021), we contend that the adoption of novel frameworks such as predictive processing will offer substantive contributions to the area of intergroup perception, providing novel avenues forward for continued theoretical and empirical advancement.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

HM: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MO: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The first author was supported by an Australian Government Research Training Program (RTP) Scholarship.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2006). They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry) from out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychol. Sci. 17, 836–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01790.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguado, L., García-Gutierrez, A., and Serrano-Pedraza, I. (2009). Symmetrical interaction of sex and expression in face classification tasks. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 9–25. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.1.9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, W. H., and Brown, J. W. (2018). Frontal cortex function as derived from hierarchical predictive coding. Sci. Rep. 8:3843. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-21407-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amodio, D. M. (2014). The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 670–682. doi: 10.1038/nrn3800

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amodio, D. M. (2019). Social cognition 2.0: an interactive memory systems account. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 21–33. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amodio, D. M., and Cikara, M. (2021). The social neuroscience of prejudice. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 439–469. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050928

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, E., Siegel, E. H., Bliss-Moreau, E., and Barrett, L. F. (2011). The visual impact of gossip. Science 332, 1446–1448. doi: 10.1126/science.1201574

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagnis, A., Celeghin, A., Mosso, C. O., and Tamietto, M. (2019). Toward an integrative science of social vision in intergroup bias. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 102, 318–326. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.04.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, K. S., Yamamoto, N., Pegna, A. J., and Johnston, P. (2022). Violated expectations for spatial and feature attributes of visual trajectories modulate event-related potential amplitudes across the visual processing hierarchy. Biol. Psychol. 174:108422. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108422

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balcetis, E., and Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: motivational influences on visual perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 612–625. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.612

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balcetis, E., Dunning, D., and Granot, Y. (2012). Subjective value determines initial dominance in binocular rivalry. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 122–129. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.08.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar, M. (2004). Visual objects in context. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 617–629. doi: 10.1038/nrn1476

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 280–289. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 419–429. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Lang. Cogn. Proc. 18, 513–562. doi: 10.1080/01690960344000026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastos, A. M., Usrey, W. M., Adams, R. A., Mangun, G. R., Fries, P., and Friston, K. J. (2012). Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron 76, 695–711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastos, A. M., Vezoli, J., Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J. M., Oostenveld, R., Dowdall, J. R., et al. (2015). Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron 85, 390–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, D. V., Kenrick, D. T., Neuberg, S. L., Blackwell, K. C., and Smith, D. M. (2007). The confounded nature of angry men and happy women. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 179–190. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.179

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, D. V., Mortensen, C. R., Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Anderson, U. S., Sasaki, T., et al. (2011). Signal detection on the battlefield: priming self-protection vs. revenge-mindedness differentially modulates the detection of enemies and allies. PLoS One 6:e23929. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023929

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., Taylor, M. J., Rousselet, G. A., Itier, R. J., Caldara, R., Schyns, P. G., et al. (2007). Controlling interstimulus perceptual variance does not abolish N170 face sensitivity. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 801–802. doi: 10.1038/nn0707-801

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bijlstra, G., Holland, R. W., and Wigboldus, D. H. (2010). The social face of emotion recognition: evaluations versus stereotypes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 657–663. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, C. T. (2015). Process dissociation models in racial bias research: Updating the analytic method and integrating with signal detection approaches. Group Proc. Intergroup Relat. 18, 402–434. doi: 10.1177/1368430214567763

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cikara, M., and Van Bavel, J. J. (2014). The neuroscience of intergroup relations: an integrative review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 245–274. doi: 10.1177/1745691614527464

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 181–204. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000477

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, C. L., Li, Y. J., Newell, S. M., Cottrell, C. A., and Neel, R. (2018). The world is a scary place: individual differences in belief in a dangerous world predict specific intergroup prejudices. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 21, 584–596. doi: 10.1177/1368430216670024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corlett, P. R., Horga, G., Fletcher, P. C., Alderson-Day, B., Schmack, K., and Powers, A. R. (2019). Hallucinations and strong priors. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 114–127. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Correll, J., Hudson, S. M., Guillermo, S., and Ma, D. S. (2014). The police officer's dilemma: a decade of research on racial bias in the decision to shoot. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 8, 201–213. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12099

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., and Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police officer's dilemma: using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 1314–1329. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., and Wittenbrink, B. (2007). The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1102–1117. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.450

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, B. M., and Lee, A. J. (2020). Stereotypes and structure in the interaction between facial emotional expression and sex characteristics. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 6, 212–235. doi: 10.1007/s40750-020-00141-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, B. M., and Lipp, O. V. (2018). The influence of multiple social categories on emotion perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 75, 27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.11.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Grèzes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Res. 1079, 4–14. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eberhardt, J. L., Goff, P. A., Purdie, V. J., and Davies, P. G. (2004). Seeing black: race, crime, and visual processing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 876–893. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.876

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, G., Vetter, P., McGruer, F., Petro, L. S., and Muckli, L. (2017). Predictive feedback to V1 dynamically updates with sensory input. Sci. Rep. 7:16538. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16093-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eil, D., and Rao, J. M. (2011). The good news-bad news effect: asymmetric processing of objective information about yourself. Am. Econ. J. 3, 114–138. doi: 10.1257/mic.3.2.114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Firestone, C., and Scholl, B. J. (2015). Can you experience ‘top-down’ effects on perception?: The case of race categories and perceived lightness. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 694–700. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0711-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, J. B., and Ambady, N. (2011). A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychol. Rev. 118, 247–279. doi: 10.1037/a0022327

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, J. B., Penner, A. M., Saperstein, A., Scheutz, M., and Ambady, N. (2011). Looking the part: social status cues shape race perception. PLoS One 6:e25107. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2018). Does predictive coding have a future? Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1019–1021. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0200-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., and Kiebel, S. (2009). Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond B Biol. Sci. 364, 1211–1221. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0300

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Parr, T., and de Vries, B. (2017). The graphical brain: belief propagation and active inference. Network Neurosci. 1, 381–414. doi: 10.1162/NETN_a_00018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Galli, G., Feurra, M., and Viggiano, M. P. (2006). “Did you see him in the newspaper?” electrophysiological correlates of context and valence in face processing. Brain Res. 1119, 190–202. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gershman, S. J., and Cikara, M. (2020). Social-structure learning. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 29, 460–466. doi: 10.1177/0963721420924481

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, C. D., Sigman, M., and Crist, R. E. (2001). The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron 31, 681–697. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00424-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D., Chiao, J. Y., and Eberhardt, J. L. (2001). Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 845–850. doi: 10.1038/90565

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Oakes, M. A., and Hoffman, H. G. (2003). Targets of discrimination: effects of race on responses to weapons holders. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 39, 399–405. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00020-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haarsma, J., Knolle, F., Griffin, J. D., Taverne, H., Mada, M., Goodyer, I. M., et al. (2020). Influence of prior beliefs on perception in early psychosis: effects of illness stage and hierarchical level of belief. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 129, 581–598. doi: 10.1037/abn0000494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haarsma, J., Kok, P., and Browning, M. (2022). The promise of layer-specific neuroimaging for testing predictive coding theories of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 245, 68–76. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.10.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackel, L. M., and Amodio, D. M. (2018). Computational neuroscience approaches to social cognition. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 24, 92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Connolly, A. C., and Guntupalli, J. S. (2014). Decoding neural representational spaces using multivariate pattern analysis. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 37, 435–456. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170325

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, M. J., Schreppel, T., Jäger, D., Koehler, S., Ehlis, A. C., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2007). The other-race effect for face perception: an event-related potential study. J. Neural Transm. 114, 951–957. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0624-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hindy, N. C., Ng, F. Y., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2016). Linking pattern completion in the hippocampus to predictive coding in visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 665–667. doi: 10.1038/nn.4284

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinojosa, J. A., Mercado, F., and Carretié, L. (2015). N170 sensitivity to facial expression: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 55, 498–509. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinton, P. (2017). Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: cognition and culture in “biased” person perception. Palgrave Commun. 3, 1–9. doi: 10.1057/palcomms.2017.86

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobson, J. A. (2009). REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 803–813. doi: 10.1038/nrn2716

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodson, R., Mehta, M., and Smith, R. (2024). The empirical status of predictive coding and active inference. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 157:105473. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105473

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogendoorn, H., and Burkitt, A. N. (2018). Predictive coding of visual object position ahead of moving objects revealed by time-resolved EEG decoding. NeuroImage 171, 55–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohwy, J. (2012). Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Front. Psychol. 3:96. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohwy, J. (2013). The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Hohwy, J. (2017). Priors in perception: top-down modulation, Bayesian perceptual learning rate, and prediction error minimization. Conscious. Cogn. 47, 75–85. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohwy, J. (2020). New directions in predictive processing. Mind Lang. 35, 209–223. doi: 10.1111/mila.12281

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Homann, J., Koay, S. A., Glidden, A. M., Tank, D. W., and Berry, M. J. (2017). Predictive coding of novel versus familiar stimuli in the primary visual cortex. BioRxiv :197608. doi: 10.1101/197608

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hugenberg, K. (2005). Social categorization and the perception of facial affect: target race moderates the response latency advantage for happy faces. Emotion 5, 267–276. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hugenberg, K., and Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: implicit prejudice and the perception of facial threat. Psychol. Sci. 14, 640–643. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1478.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, T. W. (2023). Why human prejudice is so persistent: a predictive coding analysis. Soc. Epistemol. 37, 779–797. doi: 10.1080/02691728.2023.2237942

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntenburg, J. M., Bazin, P. L., and Margulies, D. S. (2018). Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchings, P. B., and Haddock, G. (2008). Look black in anger: The role of implicit prejudice in the categorization and perceived emotional intensity of racially ambiguous faces. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1418–1420. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.05.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibáñez, A., Gleichgerrcht, E., Hurtado, E., González, R., Haye, A., and Manes, F. F. (2010). Early neural markers of implicit attitudes: N170 modulated by intergroup and evaluative contexts in IAT. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:188. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00188

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, T. A., Thompson, E., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Tracking the timecourse of social perception: The effects of racial cues on event-related brain potentials. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 1267–1280. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264335

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, T. A., and Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 616–626. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.616

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, P., Overell, A., Kaufman, J., Robinson, J., and Young, A. W. (2016). Expectations about person identity modulate the face-sensitive N170. Cortex 85, 54–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, P., Robinson, J., Kokkinakis, A., Ridgeway, S., Simpson, M., Johnson, S., et al. (2017). Temporal and spatial localization of prediction-error signals in the visual brain. Biol. Psychol. 125, 45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.02.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolij, J., and Meurs, M. (2011). Music alters visual perception. PLoS One 6:e18861. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jost, J. T., Becker, J., Osborne, D., and Badaan, V. (2017). Missing in (collective) action: ideology, system justification, and the motivational antecedents of two types of protest behavior. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 99–108. doi: 10.1177/0963721417690633

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joyce, J. (2019). “Bayes’ theorem” in The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. ed. E. N. Zalta (Spring). Stanford University.

Google Scholar

Kang, S. K., and Chasteen, A. L. (2009). The moderating role of age-group identification and perceived threat on stereotype threat among older adults. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 69, 201–220. doi: 10.2190/AG.69.3.c

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karmali, F., and Kawakami, K. (2023). Posing while black: the impact of race and expansive poses on trait attributions, professional evaluations, and interpersonal relations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 124, 49–68. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, G. B., and Mrsic-Flogel, T. D. (2018). Predictive processing: a canonical cortical computation. Neuron 100, 424–435. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kersten, D., Mamassian, P., and Yuille, A. (2004). Object perception as Bayesian inference. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 55, 271–304. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidder, C. K., White, K. R., Hinojos, M. R., Sandoval, M., and Crites, S. L. Jr. (2018). Sequential stereotype priming: a meta-analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 22, 199–227. doi: 10.1177/1088868317723532

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiebel, S. J., Daunizeau, J., and Friston, K. J. (2008). A hierarchy of time-scales and the brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4:e1000209. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000209

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klauer, K. C., and Voss, A. (2008). Effects of race on responses and response latencies in the weapon identification task: a test of six models. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1124–1140. doi: 10.1177/0146167208318603

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleider-Offutt, H. M., Bond, A. D., Williams, S. E., and Bohil, C. J. (2018). When a face type is perceived as threatening: using general recognition theory to understand biased categorization of Afrocentric faces. Mem. Cogn. 46, 716–728. doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0801-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knill, D. C. (2007). Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception. J. Vision 7:13. doi: 10.1167/7.8.13

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knill, D. C., and Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends Neurosci. 27, 712–719. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, P., and de Lange, F. P. (2015). Predictive Coding in Sensory Cortex. An Introduction to Model-Based Cognitive Neuroscience. New York, NY: Springer 221–244.

Google Scholar

Kok, P., Failing, M. F., and de Lange, F. P. (2014). Prior expectations evoke stimulus templates in the primary visual cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1546–1554. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00562

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunda, Z., and Sinclair, L. (1999). Motivated reasoning with stereotypes: activation, application, and inhibition. Psychol. Inq. 10, 12–22. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1001_2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunda, Z., and Thagard, P. (1996). Forming impressions from stereotypes, traits, and behaviors: a parallel-constraint-satisfaction theory. Psychol. Rev. 103, 284–308. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.284

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Gal, P. M., and Bruce, V. (2002). Evaluating the independence of sex and expression in judgments of faces. Percept. Psychophys. 64, 230–243. doi: 10.3758/BF03195789

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leong, Y. C., Hughes, B. L., Wang, Y., and Zaki, J. (2019). Neurocomputational mechanisms underlying motivated seeing. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 962–973. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0637-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, D. T., and Banaji, M. R. (2006). Distortions in the perceived lightness of faces: the role of race categories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 501–512. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupyan, G., and Ward, E. J. (2013). Language can boost otherwise unseen objects into visual awareness. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 110, 14196–14201. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1303312110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mange, J., Sharvit, K., Margas, N., and Sénémeaud, C. (2015). Do I shoot faster because I am thinking about an outgroup or a threatening outgroup? Soc. Psychol. 47, 29–37. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000255

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, A., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Wibral, M., Singer, W., and Melloni, L. (2015). Expecting to see a letter: alpha oscillations as carriers of top-down sensory predictions. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3146–3160. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv146

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCauley, C., Stitt, C. L., and Segal, M. (1980). Stereotyping: from prejudice to prediction. Psychol. Bull. 87, 195–208. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.87.1.195

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGovern, H. T., De Foe, A., Biddell, H., Leptourgos, P., Corlett, P., Bandara, K., et al. (2022). Learned uncertainty: The free energy principle in anxiety. Front. Psychol. 13:943785. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.943785

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGovern, H. T., and Vanman, E. J. (2020). Pathogens and intergroup relations. How evolutionary approaches can inform social neuroscience. Evol. Psychol. Sci. 7, 200–210. doi: 10.1007/s40806-020-00269-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mekawi, Y., and Bresin, K. (2015). Is the evidence from racial bias shooting task studies a smoking gun? Results from a meta-analysis. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 61, 120–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.08.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, K., and Damasio, A. (2009). Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends Neurosci. 32, 376–382. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalareas, G., Vezoli, J., Van Pelt, S., Schoffelen, J. M., Kennedy, H., and Fries, P. (2016). Alpha-beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron 89, 384–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumford, D. (1992). On the computational architecture of the neocortex: II The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biol. Cybern. 66, 241–251. doi: 10.1007/BF00198477

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neth, H., and Gigerenzer, G. (2015). “Heuristics: tools for an uncertain world” in Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences. Eds. R. A. Scott and S. M. Kosslyn (New York, NY: Wiley Online Library), 1–18.

Google Scholar

Neuberg, S. L., and Schaller, M. (2016). An evolutionary threat-management approach to prejudices. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.06.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ofan, R. H., Rubin, N., and Amodio, D. M. (2011). Seeing race: N170 responses to race and their relation to automatic racial attitudes and controlled processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3153–3161. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Otten, M., and Banaji, M. R. (2012). Social categories shape the neural representation of emotion: evidence from a visual face adaptation task. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:9. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Otten, M., Seth, A. K., and Pinto, Y. (2017). A social Bayesian brain: how social knowledge can shape visual perception. Brain Cogn. 112, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, T. E. (1975). The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects. Mem. Cogn. 3, 519–526. doi: 10.3758/BF03197524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: the role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 181–192. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. K. (2006). Weapon bias: Split-second decisions and unintended stereotyping. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 287–291. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00454.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. K., and Correll, J. (2020). “Race, weapons, and the perception of threat” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 62 (Academic Press), 1–50.

Google Scholar

Payne, B. K., Shimizu, Y., and Jacoby, L. L. (2005). Mental control and visual illusions: toward explaining race-biased weapon misidentifications. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.05.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pefkou, M., Arnal, L. H., Fontolan, L., and Giraud, A. L. (2017). θ-Band and β-band neural activity reflects independent syllable tracking and comprehension of time-compressed speech. J. Neurosci. 37, 7930–7938. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2882-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, M. R., Barbosa, F., de Haan, M., and Ferreira-Santos, F. (2019). Understanding the development of face and emotion processing under a predictive processing framework. Dev. Psychol. 55, 1868–1881. doi: 10.1037/dev0000706

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, Y., van Gaal, S., de Lange, F. P., Lamme, V. A., and Seth, A. K. (2015). Expectations accelerate entry of visual stimuli into awareness. J. Vision 15:13. doi: 10.1167/15.8.13

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plant, E. A., and Peruche, B. M. (2005). The consequences of race for police officers' responses to criminal suspects. Psychol. Sci. 16, 180–183. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00800.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, A. R., Mathys, C., and Corlett, P. R. (2017). Pavlovian conditioning–induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science 357, 596–600. doi: 10.1126/science.aan3458

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Primbs, M. A., Rinck, M., Holland, R., Knol, W., Nies, A., and Bijlstra, G. (2022). The effect of face masks on the stereotype effect in emotion perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 103:104394. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104394

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Radel, R., and Clément-Guillotin, C. (2012). Evidence of motivational influences in early visual perception: hunger modulates conscious access. Psychol. Sci. 23, 232–234. doi: 10.1177/0956797611427920

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahman, R. A., and Sommer, W. (2012). Knowledge scale effects in face recognition: an electrophysiological investigation. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 161–174. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0063-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raissi, A., and Steele, J. R. (2021). Does emotional expression moderate implicit racial bias? Examining bias following smiling and angry primes. Soc. Cogn. 39, 570–590. doi: 10.1521/soco.2021.39.5.570

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ransom, M., Fazelpour, S., Markovic, J., Kryklywy, J., Thompson, E. T., and Todd, R. M. (2020). Affect-biased attention and predictive processing. Cognition 203:104370. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104370

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, R. P., and Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 79–87. doi: 10.1038/4580

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliff, R., and McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation 20, 873–922. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratner, K. G., and Amodio, D. M. (2013). Seeing “us vs. them”: minimal group effects on the neural encoding of faces. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 298–301. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauss, K., Schwartz, S., and Pourtois, G. (2011). Top-down effects on early visual processing in humans: a predictive coding framework. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1237–1253. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Read, S. J., and Miller, L. C. (1993). Rapist or" regular guy": Explanatory coherence in the construction of mental models of others. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 19, 526–540. doi: 10.1177/0146167293195005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Read, S. J., Vanman, E. J., and Miller, L. C. (1997). Connectionism, parallel constraint satisfaction processes, and gestalt principles:(Re) introducing cognitive dynamics to social psychology. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 1, 26–53. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, J. E., Breakspear, M., Young, A. W., and Johnston, P. J. (2020). Dose-dependent modulation of the visually evoked N1/N170 by perceptual surprise: a clear demonstration of prediction-error signalling. Eur. J. Neurosci. 52, 4442–4452. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13920

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, T. T., and McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic Cognition: A Parallel Distributed Processing Approach. (Cambridge, Massechussetts: MIT Press).

Google Scholar

Rossion, B., and Jacques, C. (2012). “The N170: understanding the time course of face perception in the human brain” in The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. eds. S. J. Luck and E. S. Kappenman (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 115–141.

Google Scholar

Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., and McClelland, J. L. (1986). A general framework for parallel distributed processing. Parallel Distrib. Proc. 1:26. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/5236.001.0001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadler, M. S., Correll, J., Park, B., and Judd, C. M. (2012). The world is not black and white: racial bias in the decision to shoot in a multiethnic context. J. Soc. Issues 68, 286–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2012.01749.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Samaha, J., Boutonnet, B., Postle, B. R., and Lupyan, G. (2018). Effects of meaningfulness on perception: Alpha-band oscillations carry perceptual expectations and influence early visual responses. Sci. Rep. 8:6606. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25093-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellekens, W., Van Wezel, R. J., Petridou, N., Ramsey, N. F., and Raemaekers, M. (2016). Predictive coding for motion stimuli in human early visual cortex. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 879–890. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0942-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartenbeck, P., and Friston, K. (2016). Computational phenotyping in psychiatry: a worked example. Eneuro 3, ENEURO.0049–ENEU16.2016. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0049-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cognitive Sci. 17, 565–573. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A. K. (2015). Presence, objecthood, and the phenomenology of predictive perception. Cognitive Neurosci. 6, 111–117. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1026888

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafto, P., Eaves, B., Navarro, D. J., and Perfors, A. (2012). Epistemic trust: modeling children’s reasoning about others’ knowledge and intent. Dev. Sci. 15, 436–447. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01135.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, J. R., Ackerman, J. M., Neuberg, S. L., Maner, J. K., Vaughn Becker, D., and Kenrick, D. T. (2009). Following in the wake of anger: when not discriminating is discriminating. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 1356–1367. doi: 10.1177/0146167209339627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharot, T., Korn, C. W., and Dolan, R. J. (2011). How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1475–1479. doi: 10.1038/nn.2949

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipp, S. (2016). Neural elements for predictive coding. Front. Psychol. 7:1792. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01792

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, L., and Kunda, Z. (1999). Reactions to a black professional: motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 885–904. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.885

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. R., and DeCoster, J. (1998). Knowledge acquisition, accessibility, and use in person perception and stereotyping: Simulation with a recurrent connectionist network. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 21–35.

Google Scholar

Smith, E. R., and DeCoster, J. (1999). Associative and rule-based processing: “A connectionist interpretation of dual-process models” in Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology. Eds. R. F. Port and T. Van Gelder. (New York: The Guilford Press). 323–336.

Google Scholar

Smith, R., Kuplicki, R., Feinstein, J., Forthman, K. L., Stewart, J. L., Paulus, M. P., et al. (2020). A Bayesian computational model reveals a failure to adapt interoceptive precision estimates across depression, anxiety, eating, and substance use disorders. PLoS Comput. Biol. 16:e1008484. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008484

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. S., LaFrance, M., and Dovidio, J. F. (2017). Categorising intersectional targets: an “either/and” approach to race-and gender-emotion congruity. Cognit. Emot. 31, 83–97. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1081875

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., Lane, R. D., Parr, T., and Friston, K. J. (2019). Neurocomputational mechanisms underlying emotional awareness: insights afforded by deep active inference and their potential clinical relevance. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 107, 473–491. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smolensky, P. (1990). Tensor product variable binding and the representation of symbolic structures in connectionist systems. Artif. Intell. 46, 159–216.

Google Scholar

Snyder, J. S., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Vitela, A. D., and Melloni, L. (2015). How previous experience shapes perception in different sensory modalities. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:594. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00594

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spratling, M. W. (2010). Predictive coding as a model of response properties in cortical area V1. J. Neurosci. 30, 3531–3543. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4911-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spratling, M. W. (2017). A review of predictive coding algorithms. Brain Cogn. 112, 92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.11.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stahl, J., Wiese, H., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Expertise and own-race bias in face processing: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport 19, 583–587. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f97b4d

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanislaw, H., and Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 31, 137–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03207704

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, T., Ciorli, T., and Otten, M. (2023). Guns are not faster to enter awareness after seeing a black face: absence of race-priming in a gun/tool task during continuous flash suppression. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 49, 405–414. doi: 10.1177/01461672211067068

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolier, R. M., and Freeman, J. B. (2016a). Neural pattern similarity reveals the inherent intersection of social categories. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 795–797. doi: 10.1038/nn.4296

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolier, R. M., and Freeman, J. B. (2016b). Functional and temporal considerations for top-down influences in social perception. Psychol. Inq. 27, 352–357. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2016.1216034

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A. (2017). The role of fixations and face gender in facial expression categorization. Cogn. Brain Behav. 21, 101–115. doi: 10.24193/cbb.2017.21.07

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiem, K. C., Neel, R., Simpson, A. J., and Todd, A. R. (2019). Are black women and girls associated with danger? Implicit racial bias at the intersection of target age and gender. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 45, 1427–1439. doi: 10.1177/0146167219829182

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipples, J. (2019). Recognising and reacting to angry and happy facial expressions: a diffusion model analysis. Psychol. Res. 83, 37–47. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1092-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trapp, S., Schweinberger, S. R., Hayward, W. G., and Kovács, G. (2018). Integrating predictive frameworks and cognitive models of face perception. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 2016–2023. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1433-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases: Biases in judgments reveal some heuristics of thinking under uncertainty. Science 185, 1124–1131.

Google Scholar

Unkelbach, C., Forgas, J. P., and Denson, T. F. (2008). The turban effect: the influence of Muslim headgear and induced affect on aggressive responses in the shooter bias paradigm. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1409–1413. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Bavel, J. J., Packer, D. J., and Cunningham, W. A. (2011). Modulation of the fusiform face area following minimal exposure to motivationally relevant faces: evidence of in-group enhancement (not out-group disregard). J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3343–3354. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Gelder, T., and Port, R. F. (1995). “It’s about time: an overview of the dynamical approach to cognition” in Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. (Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press). vol. 1, 43.

Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F. (2007). Where is the Self in connectionism? Psychol. Inquiry London, United Kingdom: 18, 113–116. doi: 10.1080/10478400701416210

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., and Labiouse, C. (2013). Social Connectionism: A Reader and Handbook for Simulations. 1st ed. (London, United Kingdom: Psychology Press). 190–251. doi: 10.4324/9780203783115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Rooy, D., Van Overwalle, F., Vanhoomissen, T., Labiouse, C., and French, R. (2003). A recurrent connectionist model of group biases. Psychol. Rev. 110, 536–563. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.536

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vélez, N., and Gweon, H. (2021). Learning from other minds: an optimistic critique of reinforcement learning models of social learning. Curr. Opinion Behav. Sci. 38, 110–115. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Villiger, D. (2023). Stereotypes and self-fulfilling prophecies in the Bayesian brain. Inquiry, 1–25. doi: 10.1080/0020174X.2023.2166983

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walla, P., Chang, M., Schaefer, K., and Windhager, S. (2020). Social perception of faces: brain imaging and subjective ratings. Brain Sci. 10:861. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10110861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, K. S., McGovern, D. P., Clark, A., and O'Connell, R. G. (2020). Evaluating the neurophysiological evidence for predictive processing as a model of perception. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1464, 242–268. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14321

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. (2018). Predictive processing and the representation wars. Mind. Mach. 28, 141–172. doi: 10.1007/s11023-017-9441-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Willis, J., and Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychol. Sci. 17, 592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, Y. J., Coppin, G., and Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). Perceiving the world through group-colored glasses: a perceptual model of intergroup relations. Psychol. Inq. 27, 255–274. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2016.1199221

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Fellous, J. M., Mignault, A., and Andreoletti, C. (2003). Trait impressions as overgeneralized responses to adaptively significant facial qualities: evidence from connectionist modeling. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 7, 194–215. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0703_01

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Kikuchi, M., and Fellous, J. M. (2010). Facial resemblance to emotions: group differences, impression effects, and race stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 175–189. doi: 10.1037/a0017990

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y. J., Iemi, L., Schoffelen, J. M., de Lange, F. P., and Haegens, S. (2021). Alpha oscillations shape sensory representation and perceptual sensitivity. J. Neurosci. 41, 9581–9592. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1114-21.2021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: prejudice, Bayesian, intergroup, bias, social, cognition, racism

Citation: McGovern HT and Otten M (2024) Priors and prejudice: hierarchical predictive processing in intergroup perception. Front. Psychol. 15:1386370. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1386370

Received: 03 April 2024; Accepted: 20 May 2024;
Published: 13 June 2024.

Edited by:

Igor Douven, Université Paris-Sorbonne, France

Reviewed by:

Karl Friston, University College London, United Kingdom
Daniel Villiger, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2024 McGovern and Otten. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: H. T. McGovern, h.mcgovern@uqconnect.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.