Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 14 May 2024
Sec. Cognitive Science

Interruption in visual search: a systematic review

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
  • 2School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Dementia Research and Nursing Science, University for Continuing Education Krems, Krems an der Donau, Austria

Visual search, the process of trying to find a target presented among distractors, is a much-studied cognitive task. Less well-studied is the condition in which the search task is interrupted before the target is found. The consequences of such interruptions in visual search have been investigated across various disciplines, which has resulted in diverse and at times contradictory findings. The aim of this systematic review is to provide a more cohesive understanding of the effects of interruptions in visual search. For this purpose, we identified 28 studies that met our inclusion criteria. To facilitate a more organized and comprehensive analysis, we grouped the studies based on three dimensions: the search environment, the interruption aftermath, and the type of the interrupting event. While interruptions in visual search are variable and manifest differently across studies, our review provides a foundational scheme for a more cohesive understanding of the subject. This categorization serves as a starting point for exploring potential future directions, which we delineate in our conclusions.

Introduction

Imagine you are driving on an unfamiliar highway, on your way to a dinner party, while searching for your exit from the main road you are on. Your gaze shifts between the road and the overhead signs, trying to match them with the exit number you have memorized. Just when you think you have spotted your exit in the distance, a ring of your phone interrupts your focus. You briefly glance at your phone and discover it is not important. This interruption, although brief, causes you to miss your exit, disrupting your journey and increasing the likelihood of being late for the party.

The scenario above, searching for your exit, exemplifies visual search, the act of locating a target among distractors. Visual search is a common daily behavior; for example, recognizing a street sign in a busy cityscape. Experimentally, this behavior has been explored through various tasks like finding a target letter among a set of other letters (Horowitz and Wolfe, 1998; Höfler et al., 2014), identifying a word on a list (Lawrence, 1971; Radhakrishnan et al., 2022), or locating one specific image among a collection of images (Yang and Zelinsky, 2009; Höfler and Hübel, 2018; Stankov et al., 2021).

Traditionally, interruptions have been conceptualized as temporary cessations of a primary task, typically to divert attention to an alternative task, with the anticipation that the original task will be resumed at a later time, usually once the alternative task is resolved (Boehm-Davis and Remington, 2009). Moreover, since the first pioneering works studying the effect of interruptions (Freeman, 1930), the literature has consistently emphasized the prevailing negative impacts of interruptions on the interrupted task. For our review, we chose to employ a wider definition of interruption: an event that disrupts the search task without necessarily terminating it or initiating a new one.

Scientific research aimed at understanding the impact of interruptions on search tasks has employed a variety of methodologies. These include conducting experiments where participants search for a specific letter and are interrupted by a memory recall task (Beck et al., 2006), interrupting their search with a secondary task under simulated real-world conditions (Kujala, 2013), and naturalistic observation of interruptions in real-world settings (Cades et al., 2010). In the realm of experimental research on interruptions, it is common to let participants resume the interrupted task after interruption. However, in cases of frequent or extended interruptions, returning to the original task might involve substantial cognitive costs and require reengagement of attentional and memory processes (Hirsch et al., 2023). Furthermore, there might be instances where resumption of the original task becomes impossible due to the nature of the interruption. For example, interruptions such as time constraints can make resuming the original task unfeasible due to the irreversible loss of the allocated time for task completion (Höfler et al., 2011; Nachtnebel et al., 2023).

Literature on interruptions often underscores their negative impact on task performance, such as reduced accuracy (Wynn et al., 2018) and longer completion times in the interrupted task (Crews and Russ, 2020). However, it is important to note that interruptions are not necessarily detrimental, and that this prevailing characterization overlooks instances where interruptions may be beneficial. In some instances, well-timed interruptions can enhance productivity by providing necessary mental breaks, thereby improving focus and creativity upon task resumption (Mark et al., 2014). Moreover, interruptions in the form of queries, where additional information is actively sought out, might be necessary for the successful completion of the originally interrupted task (Jin and Dabbish, 2009).

Although the role of interruptions in visual search has sparked some scholarly interest, the subject does not form a cohesive field. Instead, research is spread across various disciplines, each contextualized within its broader domain. For instance, this topic has been explored in the fields of human-machine interaction (Brazzolotto and Michael, 2021), medical imaging (Drew et al., 2018), airport security (Rieger et al., 2021), and basic research in cognitive psychology (Lleras et al., 2005). Despite this fragmented landscape, to the best of our knowledge, no current studies aim to consolidate the scattered insights on the impact of interruptions in visual search. This lack of synthesis underscores the need for a review that collects and consolidates these diverse findings. Therefore, our objective is to present a review that acts as a foundational step in fostering a more cohesive understanding of this subject. By integrating these isolated works, we aim to work toward a unified perspective on how interruptions affect visual search tasks.

Grasping the nuances of interruptions in visual search is vital not only for academic research but also for its practical applications in fields as diverse as defense (Rice and Trafimow, 2012), healthcare (Williams and Drew, 2017) and digital interface design (Kujala and Saariluoma, 2011). In these areas, understanding how interruptions influence visual search can lead to significant improvements in performance and safety. Through our work, we intend to highlight current knowledge gaps, and propose potential avenues for future research. We hope our review will not only spark interest but also inspire further studies. This, in turn, would contribute to the advancement of our understanding of the consequences of interruptions in visual search.

Methods

We employed a systematic review methodology, conforming to the guidelines of the PRISMA framework (Moher et al., 2009). Our literature search was conducted in October 2023, across the following databases: APA PsycNet, IEEE Xplore, PubMed, Sage Journals, ScienceDirect, Scopus, SpringerLink, and Web of Science. We used the keywords “visual search” AND (“interruption” OR “interruptions”) as queries. We focused exclusively on peer-reviewed articles from scholarly journals and did not apply any publication year filters. The initial results were uploaded to EndNote (The EndNote Team, 2013) for duplicate checking and subsequently imported into Rayyan (Ouzzani et al., 2016), an online tool designed for abstract screening.

After removing duplicates, the initial set of 167 records was reduced to 109. These remaining abstracts were further evaluated for eligibility by AJCD, SJN, and MH based on pre-established inclusion criteria: (i) the study employed an experimental approach; (ii) the paper was authored in English; (iii) adult human subjects participated in the research; and (iv) no clinical samples were part of the study. Additionally, it is important to note that, while some of the studies reviewed included experiments without interruptions, our analysis concentrated exclusively on experiments that deliberately incorporated interruptions into their design. Following the application of these inclusion criteria, resulting discrepancies among the authors were resolved through further discussion.

Twenty-two articles were initially selected for full-text review. Following the comprehensive full-text review, four of these articles were excluded because visual search did not constitute the main experimental task of the experiments. Additional manual searches, which involved examining the reference lists of these selected articles, led to the identification of ten more articles for potential inclusion. The final corpus for review comprised 28 articles. A PRISMA flow diagram (Moher et al., 2009) depicting the review process is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram for the study selection procedure.

Results

We collected and compiled data from each of the chosen studies, including the reference, sample size, country of data collection, main search task, implementation of interruption, interruption onset, interruption frequency main results, all of which are detailed in Table 1. The sample size ranged from 1 to 150 participants. Half (14) of the studies reviewed were conducted in the United States, followed by seven in Canada, two in Austria, two in France, two in Germany and one in the United Kingdom.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Synopsis of the included studies in alphabetical order.

The impact of interruption on the visual search task was evaluated using metrics commonly employed in visual search paradigms. These included search accuracy and response time, along with measures of oculomotor behavior, such as the duration and frequency of fixations. Additionally, specific metrics designed for experimental paradigms involving interruptions, like resumption lag (i.e., the time taken to resume the search task after the interruption), were utilized.

To categorize the reviewed studies, we employed a hierarchical scheme based on three key aspects (Figure 2). At the first level, studies were categorized based on the search environment, distinguishing between “artificial” environments, common in laboratory visual search studies (18/28 papers), and “natural” environments, representing those utilizing more ecologically valid search settings (10/28 papers). The second level addresses the interruption aftermath, specifically, whether the original search was resumed or ceased after the interruption. Most studies reviewed (22/28) implemented interruptions that disrupted the search task only momentarily, allowing participants to resume the search afterwards. The remaining six studies investigated scenarios where interruptions led to a complete cessation of the search, rendering it impossible for participants to resume their search post-interruption. Finally, the third level classifies studies based on whether the interrupting event necessitated participant action: task-required events (13), such the appearance and subsequent solving of an arithmetic task (Nachtnebel et al., 2023), non-task events (14), such as the brief disappearance of the search display (Thomas and Lleras, 2009), and hybrid events (1), which involve a combination of action-required and non-action-required events (Shen and Jiang, 2006).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Categorization scheme by search environment (artificial vs. natural), interruption aftermath (search resumed vs. search ceased), and interrupting event (task vs. no task).

Figure 3A illustrates the distribution of search tasks utilized in the reviewed studies. A majority of research in artificial environments (10/18) was conducted with the task of identifying a specific letter among distractors (Lleras et al., 2005, 2007; Beck et al., 2006; Van Zoest et al., 2007; Jungé et al., 2009; Lleras and Enns, 2009; Thomas and Lleras, 2009; Höfler et al., 2011; Godwin et al., 2013; Mereu et al., 2014). Furthermore, five of the studies asked participants to identify a target shape (Olds et al., 2000a,b,c, 2001; Olds and Punambolam, 2002) while one study each explored finding a picture (Alonso et al., 2021), categorizing numbers (Ratwani and Trafton, 2008) and tracking multiple objects (Labonté and Vachon, 2021). In contrast to the more homogenous distribution of search tasks in studies conducted in artificial environments, natural environment search tasks exhibited greater heterogeneity. They encompassed four medical image scanning tasks (Williams and Drew, 2017; Drew et al., 2018; Wynn et al., 2018; Radović et al., 2022), one change detection task in natural scenes (Shen and Jiang, 2006, all experiments except 5b), one search in the real world (Nachtnebel et al., 2023),one scanning an aerial map (Rice and Trafimow, 2012), and one X-ray screening task (Rieger et al., 2021).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparative distribution of search tasks (A) and interrupting events (B) present in the reviewed studies.

Regarding the interruption aftermath, in most of the studies conducted in artificial environments (13/18), participants typically were allowed to continue their search after an interruption without needing to complete additional tasks (Olds et al., 2000a,b,c, 2001; Olds and Punambolam, 2002; Lleras et al., 2005, 2007; Van Zoest et al., 2007; Jungé et al., 2009; Lleras and Enns, 2009; Thomas and Lleras, 2009; Godwin et al., 2013; Mereu et al., 2014). However, in three studies, participants were required to perform another task following the interruption before they could resume their search (Ratwani and Trafton, 2008; Alonso et al., 2021; Labonté and Vachon, 2021). Additionally, in two instances where the interruption marked the end of the search, a further task followed the interruption (Beck et al., 2006; Höfler et al., 2011).

Most studies carried out in natural environments (6/10) allowed for search resumption, however this was always dependent on the completion of a task during the interruption (Williams and Drew, 2017; Drew et al., 2018; Wynn et al., 2018; Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020, 2021; Radović et al., 2022). In natural scenarios where the interruption ended the search, participants were required to complete a task in two studies (Rice and Trafimow, 2012; Nachtnebel et al., 2023) while in one study, they were not required to perform any task post-interruption (Rieger et al., 2021). Finally, the study by Shen and Jiang (2006) exhibited a hybrid pattern: in experiments 1–3, completing a task was not required to resume the search, while in experiments 4–6, it was necessary.

When we assessed whether the interruption event was considered a task or not, we observed that 14 studies did not involve a task as the interruption (Olds et al., 2000a, 2000b, 2000c, 2001; Olds and Punambolam, 2002; Lleras et al., 2005, 2007; Van Zoest et al., 2007; Jungé et al., 2009; Lleras and Enns, 2009; Thomas and Lleras, 2009; Godwin et al., 2013; Mereu et al., 2014; Rieger et al., 2021); and all but one of these studies (Rieger et al., 2021) were conducted in artificial environments. In contrast, in the 13 studies where the interruption was a task, five were conducted in artificial environments (Beck et al., 2006; Ratwani and Trafton, 2008; Höfler et al., 2011; Alonso et al., 2021; Labonté and Vachon, 2021) and eight in natural ones (Rice and Trafimow, 2012; Williams and Drew, 2017; Drew et al., 2018; Wynn et al., 2018; Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020, 2021; Radović et al., 2022; Nachtnebel et al., 2023). Within these 13 studies, two involved interrupting event tasks that were directly related to the interrupted search, requiring participants to use information obtained during the incomplete search to complete their tasks (Beck et al., 2006; Rice and Trafimow, 2012) whereas the tasks in the remaining nine studies were search-unrelated.

Overall, artificial environments exhibited a more limited variety of interrupting events compared to natural settings. The distribution of these events is depicted in Figure 3B. In artificial environments, seven types of events were observed, five of which were unique to these settings. Conversely, natural settings featured eight different types of events, with six exclusive to them. Tasks specific to artificial environments included the momentarily disappearance of the display (Lleras et al., 2005, 2007; Van Zoest et al., 2007; Jungé et al., 2009; Lleras and Enns, 2009; Thomas and Lleras, 2009; Godwin et al., 2013; Mereu et al., 2014), the addition of new distractors to the display (Olds et al., 2000a,b,c, 2001; Olds and Punambolam, 2002), a new search in the same display (Höfler et al., 2011), typing characters (Alonso et al., 2021), or a hybrid task requiring either and arithmetic or a spatial rotation task (Ratwani and Trafton, 2008). Tasks unique to natural environments included number discrimination (Radović et al., 2022), a prompt to respond to the interrupted search (Rice and Trafimow, 2012), a search for a letter (Wynn et al., 2018), the timeout and accompanying end of the search (Rieger et al., 2021), a hybrid task requiring either an over the phone diagnosis or the completion of a demographic survey (Drew et al., 2018) or an event that was either the momentary disappearance of the search display or passive viewing of a natural scene (Shen and Jiang, 2006). Finally, two interrupting events where common to both environments: arithmetic tasks (Williams and Drew, 2017; Brazzolotto and Michael, 2021; Labonté and Vachon, 2021; Nachtnebel et al., 2023) and a memory tasks (Beck et al., 2006; Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020).

In considering the effects of interruption on visual search tasks, we observed that most studies indicate a disruptive impact. However, there was extensive variation in the nature and extent of this disruption. To provide a thorough analysis, we focused on the distinct aspects of the search process influenced by these disruptions. The two most prominent findings are that interruptions impacted search accuracy and search times, as evidenced by reduced accuracy and extended response times in interrupted trials compared to uninterrupted ones (Williams and Drew, 2017; Alonso et al., 2021; Rieger et al., 2021). The timing of the interruption emerged as a key factor influencing the extent of search disruption. Notably, the first interruption led to longer response times and reduced accuracy (Drew et al., 2018) as compared to subsequent interruptions (Van Zoest et al., 2007). In this same vein, the early onset of interruptions resulted in longer response times (Olds et al., 2000c) compared to later onsets (Olds et al., 2000a, 2000b, 2001).

The impact of interruptions on search resumption was influenced by the nature of the interrupting event. Longer (Labonté and Vachon, 2021) and more difficult (Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020) interruptions significantly delayed the resumption time compared to shorter or simpler ones. Similarly, interruptions involving emotionally charged stimuli extended resumption time more than neutral interruptions (Brazzolotto and Michael, 2021). Additionally, spatial interruptions caused greater resumption lag than non-spatial ones (Ratwani and Trafton, 2008). Furthermore, the ability to quickly resume the search after an interruption, known as rapid resumption (Lleras et al., 2005), was adversely affected when interruptions involved changes to task-relevant distractors that shared features with the target (Jungé et al., 2009). However, changes to task-irrelevant distractors (Lleras et al., 2007) or the target’s location did not hinder rapid resumption (Mereu et al., 2014).

Memory, which is crucial in visual search (Hollingworth, 2006), appears to be disrupted by interruptions. When testing for recall performance, items observed shortly before an interruption were remembered best, with recall declining for items observed earlier (Beck et al., 2006). Memory for probes seemed to be robust to short interruptions but not too long ones (Thomas and Lleras, 2009). Interruptions involving moving to a new task seemed to disrupt spatial item arrangement in memory, consequently resulting in decreased search performance (Shen and Jiang, 2006). Further supporting this, participants needed more time to resume their search tasks after an interruption involving a spatial task interruption, compared to non-spatial task interruptions (Ratwani and Trafton, 2008).

Interruptions during the search task also impacted participants’ oculomotor behavior. These interruptions significantly influenced measures such as fixation duration (increased), fixation frequency (decreased) and diminished fixation-targeting accuracy (Godwin et al., 2013). Interestingly, when conducting two consecutive searches, inhibition of return, the phenomenon where individuals exhibit delayed reactions to a stimulus appearing at a location they have recently examined (Klein, 2000), was observed when the first search was interrupted, however, this inhibition was not present in scenarios where the first search was completed without interruption (Höfler et al., 2011).

Even though interruptions were generally detrimental to the ongoing visual search, some studies have provided conflicting or outright contradictory evidence, particularly in natural environments. For example, Nachtnebel et al. (2023) observed no significant difference in accuracy between interrupted and uninterrupted search conditions in a real-world setting. Additionally, some interruptions in the form of time pressure (i.e., the participant has a limited time to respond) have shown to improve response accuracy in computer-assisted searches (Rice and Trafimow, 2012) and to speed up responses without changes in accuracy (Radović et al., 2022). Furthermore, the impact of interruptions on search accuracy may vary depending on the task difficulty. For instance, Wynn et al. (2018) found that difficult searches (i.e., targets with inconspicuous features) were negatively affected by interruptions involving a new task, while easier searches (i.e., targets that are less difficult to spot) generally showed improved accuracy. In artificial environments, interruptions providing spatial information about the target’s location have been shown to decrease response times (Olds et al., 2000a; Olds and Punambolam, 2002; Lleras and Enns, 2009).

Discussion

The aim of this review was to provide a comprehensive synopsis of existing research on interruptions in visual search. It underscored the importance of identifying and bridging knowledge gaps within this field, aiming to create a more unified and thorough understanding of the phenomenon. The primary observation is the lack of uniformity in defining and executing interruptions, evident in the methodological diversity of studies. While methodological pluralism diversifies the field with a wide array of insights and perspectives, it complicates the development of a unified understanding of interruption effects in visual search, thereby hindering their integration into existing cognitive models.

Our review encompassed a range of studies from different disciplines. While not every study included was primarily focused on the effect of interruption in visual search, each integrated the aspect of interruption into its methodology, which became the central focus of our analysis. We adopted this approach with the intention of capturing the diverse ways in which interruptions manifest in visual search tasks. In doing so, we found that the diversity and complexity of these studies presents a substantial challenge for conducting a structured analysis. As a starting point, we therefore defined interruption as an event that disrupts the search task without necessarily terminating it or initiating a new search. Following this definition, we constructed a categorization scheme focused on three critical dimensions: the search environment, the aftermath of the interruption, and if the interrupting event prompted a task.

In our review, we observed that studies conducted in artificial environments often employed well-established search tasks prevalent in the visual search field, such as finding a letter among distractors or identifying a target shape. The use of methodologically rigorous and tested paradigms facilitates experimental control and enables comparisons across different studies. However, this approach may compromise ecological validity, and poses a challenge in the translation of findings to practical applications (Diaz et al., 2003). In contrast, research conducted in natural environments tends to reflect real-world search scenarios more accurately. However, these naturalistic studies often employ diverse methodological approaches, even within the same subdiscipline (Ratwani et al., 2016), leading to results that are challenging to compare across studies. Consequently, the findings are less straightforward, necessitating careful contextualization and cautious interpretation.

Regardless of the search environment, in the majority of reviewed studies participants were allowed to resume search after the interruption, which is in line with the classical definition of interruption, which conceptualizes them as the temporary cessation of a primary task (Boehm-Davis and Remington, 2009). In real-life, we usually have the opportunity to return to our initial tasks after being interrupted. As such, this implementation of interruptions reflects real-world occurrences, where the process involves a pause followed by a continuation. For instance, imagine searching for a book in a library and being interrupted by a phone call; after the call, you would typically resume your search for the book. For the studies in which the interruption marked the end of the search, the interrupting event was always triggered by the expiration of allotted time (Rice and Trafimow, 2012; Rieger et al., 2021; Nachtnebel et al., 2023) or after a predetermined number of saccades (Beck et al., 2006; Höfler et al., 2011). This mirrors real-life scenarios where searches are often time-constrained, such as when we are shopping in a supermarket before it closes. Interestingly, none of the studies incorporated endogenous (i.e., participant-initiated) interruptions, which are common in daily life. For example, if we are searching for a rain jacket before an appointment and cannot find it quickly enough, we might switch our search to looking for an umbrella instead. This type of internally driven interruptions represents an area for further research.

Concerning interrupting events, clear differences were observed between artificial and natural settings. In artificial environments, interrupting events were typically predictable and consistently timed, with the aim to test clearly defined effects such as rapid resumption (Lleras et al., 2005). These interruptions often did not involve additional tasks; they were merely brief disappearances of the search display, followed by the reappearance of the same or a slightly altered display. Conversely, in natural environments, most interruptions occurred unpredictably and involved an additional task. Furthermore, the tasks prompted by these interruptions were organically connected to the preceding interrupted search, meaning they could realistically occur in such contexts—for example, a physician receiving a phone call from a patient while scanning a medical image (Drew et al., 2018). These interruptions in natural settings were designed to explore how interruptions could operate in real-world scenarios, providing valuable insights into their practical impacts.

As previously mentioned, research on the effects of interruptions has consistently emphasized their predominantly adverse consequences. Indeed, interruptions were accompanied by negative effects on the search process in all the studies included in our review, predominantly manifesting as reduced accuracy and extended response times. Interestingly, we also found that search planning was affected by search interruption. For instance, Godwin et al. (2013) observed that participants briefly continued their fixation plan during the interruption period, as evidenced by their saccades and fixations often revisiting locations observed prior to the interruption. Moreover, Höfler et al. (2011) reported that inhibition of return, i.e., a phenomenon where attention is less likely to return to a previously attended location (Klein, 2000), persists across two consecutive searches when the initial search was interrupted but it extinguishes when the initial search was completed. Nevertheless, under specific conditions, interruptions might carry a beneficial impact on visual search. For instance, Ratwani et al. (2006) demonstrated that interrupting a simple search resulted in shorter fixation durations and fewer task-critical errors compared to uninterrupted searches. Similarly, Rice and Trafimow (2012) observed that participants achieved greater accuracy in their responses when they were under time pressure compared to a control condition without such constraints. Thus, studying interruptions in visual search not only helps understand their immediate effects but also provides insights into broader aspects of search dynamics, potentially guiding the development of methods to either mitigate or capitalize on these effects.

While there are clear indications of potential benefits, the underlying mechanisms and specific contexts from which interruptions are beneficial also require further investigation. We suggest that future work in investigating interruptions in visual search tasks could draw from the methodologies and theories from studies identifying advantageous effects of interruptions in other domains (Walji et al., 2004). For instance, potential benefits in visual search tasks could be explored by implementing strategic interruptions, such as signaling alerts. These alerts could enhance efficiency and effectiveness in security-critical tasks such as baggage screening, where quick identification and processing are essential (Boskemper et al., 2022). Furthermore, interruptions could prove beneficial in human-assisting monitoring and management systems, such as those used in traffic control. For example, timely pop-ups could prompt operators to make necessary adjustments based on real-time data (Dahal et al., 2013). By taking this approach, a more nuanced range of hypotheses regarding the role of interruptions could be examined, moving beyond the commonly held view that they are primarily disruptive, toward a more comprehensive understanding that acknowledges their potential utility as well.

Our review identified a notable gap in the literature concerning the influence of individual differences on the effects of interruptions during search tasks. One study found that individuals with greater working memory capacity experienced less negative impact from interruptions on search accuracy, regardless of the duration of the interruption (Labonté and Vachon, 2021). This suggests that individual cognitive capacities might play a role in mitigating the adverse effects of interruptions. Moreover, while existing research suggests that individual expertise is associated with enhanced performance in visual search tasks (Robson et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2021), the question of whether expertise also contributes to more effective management of interruptions in visual search remains debated. For instance, when comparing the performance of experienced and novice radiologists in interpreting chest scans, Wynn et al. (2018) found no significant differences between the groups when their search was interrupted; both were equally affected, exhibiting extended scanning times and diminished response accuracy compared to the uninterrupted condition. These findings highlight the need for further research to clarify the potential moderating effects of experience and training on interruption management.

One limitation of our review is the deliberate focus on studies involving young and healthy adult populations. During our review process, we found and excluded one study that included children and one study with elderly participants. The first study, by Lleras et al. (2011), found that the ability to quickly resume searching after a brief interruption (i.e., rapid resumption) does not vary with age among children and adolescents aged 7 to 19 years. In the second study, Farrimond et al. (2006) observed that older adults, unlike younger adults, experienced a significant decline in cue detection following interruptions during a scene navigation task. They suggested that this decline could be related to a diminished capacity for self-initiated reinstatement of working memory in older age. Future research could benefit from incorporating a broader range of demographic and clinical populations to better understand how interruptions impact cognitive processes in these groups.

Our review underscores the complex nature of interruptions in visual search as we strive to establish an operational definition that captures the diversity noted in the literature. We consider that this definition was crucial for accurately categorizing the studies and developing a preliminary scheme intended as a starting point to deepen understanding of the phenomenon and facilitate comparisons across different research disciplines. Despite our efforts to define clear criteria, we are aware that our categorization scheme may be subject to critique and could require refinement or expansion in future research. For instance, parallels can be drawn between the paradigms discussed in this review and those of dual-tasks (Liesefeld et al., 2024) and task-switching (Kiesel et al., 2010). On the one hand, when search resumption is conditioned to the completion of an interruption that entails another task, this interruption can be regarded as a secondary task. On the other hand, while task-switching usually does not reflect visual search situations, it requires a quick alternation between different tasks and adaptation to the currently relevant task set. However, due to significant differences in execution and cognitive demands, studies employing these paradigms were deemed beyond the scope of our review.

In conclusion, we aimed to provide a nuanced perspective that emphasized the need for dedicated research and standardized methodologies, which are crucial for facilitating valid comparisons across studies and integrating the effects of interruptions into current visual search models. We also advocate for a reevaluation of the traditional view that interruptions are predominantly negative and encourage exploration of their potential benefits. This shift in perspective could significantly impact not only academic research but also offer broader real-world applications of these insights.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

AC-D: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SN: Investigation, Writing – review & editing, Methodology. CK: Writing – review & editing. IG: Writing – review & editing. MH: Conceptualization, Investigation, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded in whole or in part by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) [10.55776/P33074]. For open access purposes, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright license to any author-accepted manuscript version arising from this submission.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alonso, D., Lavelle, M., and Drew, T. (2021). The performance costs of interruption during visual search are determined by the type of search task. Cogn. Res. 6:58. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00322-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, M. R., Peterson, M. S., Boot, W. R., Vomela, M., and Kramer, A. F. (2006). Explicit memory for rejected distractors during visual search. Vis. Cogn. 14, 150–174. doi: 10.1080/13506280600574487

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boehm-Davis, D. A., and Remington, R. (2009). Reducing the disruptive effects of interruption: a cognitive framework for analysing the costs and benefits of intervention strategies. Accid. Anal. Prev. 41, 1124–1129. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boskemper, M. M., Bartlett, M. L., and McCarley, J. S. (2022). Measuring the efficiency of automation-aided performance in a simulated baggage screening task. Hum. Factors 64, 945–961. doi: 10.1177/0018720820983632

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazzolotto, P., and Michael, G. A. (2020). Complexity of interruptions: evidence supporting a non-interruption-based theory. Scand. J. Psychol. 61, 723–730. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12659

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazzolotto, P., and Michael, G. A. (2021). Do not interrupt me if it makes me feel something – study of the effect of the pleasantness of interruptions on performance. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 71:100623. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2021.100623

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cades, D. M., Werner, N. E., Boehm-Davis, D. A., and Arshad, Z. (2010). What makes real-world interruptions disruptive? Evidence from an office setting. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Ann. Meet. 54, 448–452. doi: 10.1177/154193121005400437

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crews, D. E., and Russ, M. J. (2020). The impact of individual differences on multitasking ability. Int. J. Prod. Perform. Mangag 69, 1301–1319. doi: 10.1108/IJPPM-04-2019-0191

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahal, K., Almejalli, K., and Hossain, M. A. (2013). Decision support for coordinated road traffic control actions. Decis. Support. Syst. 54, 962–975. doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2012.10.022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diaz, D. D., Sims, V. K., Hancock, P. A., Smith, H. S., Ellis, L. U., Clark, B., et al. (2003). Moderating factors in visual search: the role of ecological-validity. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Ann. Meet. 47, 1693–1697. doi: 10.1177/154193120304701321

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Drew, T., Williams, L. H., Aldred, B., Heilbrun, M. E., and Minoshima, S. (2018). Quantifying the costs of interruption during diagnostic radiology interpretation using mobile eye-tracking glasses. J. Med. Imaging 5:1. doi: 10.1117/1.JMI.5.3.031406

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrimond, S., Knight, R., and Titov, N. (2006). The effects of aging on remembering intentions: performance on a simulated shopping task. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 20, 533–555. doi: 10.1002/acp.1202

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, G. L. (1930). Changes in tonus during completed and interrupted mental work. J. Gen. Psychol. 4, 309–334. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1930.9918315

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Godwin, H. J., Benson, V., and Drieghe, D. (2013). Using interrupted visual displays to explore the capacity, time course, and format of fixation plans during visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 1700–1712. doi: 10.1037/a0032287

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, P., Moretti, L., Askin, S., and Koch, I. (2023). Examining the cognitive processes underlying resumption costs in task-interruption contexts: decay or inhibition of suspended task goals? Mem. Cogn. 52, 271–284. doi: 10.3758/s13421-023-01458-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Höfler, M., Gilchrist, I., and Körner, C. (2011). Inhibition of return functions within but not across searches. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 1385–1397. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0127-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Höfler, M., Gilchrist, I. D., and Körner, C. (2014). Searching the same display twice: properties of short-term memory in repeated search. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 76, 335–352. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0589-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Höfler, M., and Hübel, B. (2018). “Missing targets in multiple-target search” in Psychology applications and developments. ed. M. Höfler (Lisbon: inSciencePress), 247–256.

Google Scholar

Hollingworth, A. (2006). Scene and position specificity in visual memory for objects. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32, 58–69. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.58

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Horowitz, T. S., and Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual search has no memory. Nature 394, 575–577. doi: 10.1038/29068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, J., and Dabbish, L. A. (2009). “Self-interruption on the computer: a typology of discretionary task interleaving” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems. ed. J. Jin (Boston, MA: ACM), 1799–1808.

Google Scholar

Jungé, J. A., Brady, T. F., and Chun, M. M. (2009). The contents of perceptual hypotheses: evidence from rapid resumption of interrupted visual search. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 681–689. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.4.681

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., et al. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—A review. Psychological Bulletin 136, 849–874. doi: 10.1037/a0019842

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 138–147. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01452-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kujala, T. (2013). Browsing the information highway while driving: three in-vehicle touch screen scrolling methods and driver distraction. Pers. Ubiquit. Comput. 17, 815–823. doi: 10.1007/s00779-012-0517-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kujala, T., and Saariluoma, P. (2011). Effects of menu structure and touch screen scrolling style on the variability of glance durations during in-vehicle visual search tasks. Ergonomics 54, 716–732. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2011.592601

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Labonté, K., and Vachon, F. (2021). Resuming a dynamic task following increasingly long interruptions: the role of working memory and reconstruction. Front. Psychol. 12:659451. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659451

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, D. H. (1971). Two studies of visual search for word targets with controlled rates of presentation. Percept. Psychophys. 10, 85–89. doi: 10.3758/BF03214320

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liesefeld, H. R., Lamy, D., Gaspelin, N., Geng, J. J., Kerzel, D., Schall, J. D., et al. (2024). Terms of debate: Consensus definitions to guide the scientific discourse on visual distraction. Atten Percept Psychophys doi: 10.3758/s13414-023-02820-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lleras, A., and Enns, J. T. (2009). Focused spatial attention is independent of rapid resumption of an interrupted search. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 565–577. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.3.565

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lleras, A., Porporino, M., Burack, J. A., and Enns, J. T. (2011). Rapid resumption of interrupted search is independent of age-related improvements in visual search. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109, 58–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lleras, A., Rensink, R. A., and Enns, J. T. (2005). Rapid resumption of interrupted visual search: new insights on the interaction between vision and memory. Psychol. Sci. 16, 684–688. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01596.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lleras, A., Rensink, R. A., and Enns, J. T. (2007). Consequences of display changes during interrupted visual search: rapid resumption is target specific. Percept. Psychophys. 69, 980–993. doi: 10.3758/BF03193936

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mark, G., Iqbal, S., Czerwinski, M., and Johns, P. (2014). “Capturing the mood: facebook and face-to-face encounters in the workplace” in Proceedings of the 17th ACM conference on computer supported cooperative work and social computing. ed. G. Mark (Baltimore, MD: ACM), 1082–1094.

Google Scholar

Mereu, S., Zacks, J. M., Kurby, C. A., and Lleras, A. (2014). The role of prediction in perception: evidence from interrupted visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40, 1372–1389. doi: 10.1037/a0036646

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G.The PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and Meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nachtnebel, S. J., Cambronero-Delgadillo, A. J., Helmers, L., Ischebeck, A., and Höfler, M. (2023). The impact of different distractions on outdoor visual search and object memory. Sci. Rep. 13:16700. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-43679-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olds, E. S., Cowan, W. B., and Jolicoeur, P. (2000a). Partial orientation pop-out helps difficult search for orientation. Percept. Psychophys. 62, 1341–1347. doi: 10.3758/BF03212136

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olds, E. S., Cowan, W. B., and Jolicoeur, P. (2000b). The time-course of pop-out search. Vis. Res. 40, 891–912. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00248-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olds, E. S., Cowan, W. B., and Jolicoeur, P. (2000c). Tracking visual search over space and time. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 7, 292–300. doi: 10.3758/BF03212984

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olds, E. S., Jolicoeur, P., and Cowan, W. B. (2001). Interactions between search mechanisms in conjunction search. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 285–295. doi: 10.1037/h0087375

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olds, E. S., and Punambolam, R. J. (2002). The decay and interruption of interactions between search mechanisms. Vis. Res. 42, 747–760. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00314-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouzzani, M., Hammady, H., Fedorowicz, Z., and Elmagarmid, A. (2016). Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 5:210. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Radhakrishnan, A., Balakrishnan, M., Behera, S., and Raghunandhan, R. (2022). Role of reading medium and audio distractors on visual search. J. Optom. 15, 299–304. doi: 10.1016/j.optom.2021.12.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Radović, T., Rieger, T., and Manzey, D. (2022). A global and local perspective of interruption frequency in a visual search task. Front. Psychol. 13:951048. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.951048

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratwani, R. M., and Trafton, J. G. (2008). Spatial memory guides task resumption. Vis. Cogn. 16, 1001–1010. doi: 10.1080/13506280802025791

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratwani, R. M., Trafton, J. G., and Myers, C. (2006). Helpful or harmful? Examining the effects of interruptions on task performance. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Ann. Meet. 50, 372–375. doi: 10.1177/154193120605000334

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratwani, R. M., Wang, E., Fong, A., and Cooper, C. J. (2016). A human factors approach to understanding the types and sources of interruptions in radiology Reading rooms. J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 13, 1102–1105. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2016.02.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, S., and Trafimow, D. (2012). Time pressure heuristics can improve performance due to increased consistency. J. Gen. Psychol. 139, 273–288. doi: 10.1080/00221309.2012.705187

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rieger, T., Heilmann, L., and Manzey, D. (2021). Visual search behavior and performance in luggage screening: effects of time pressure, automation aid, and target expectancy. Cogn. Res. 6:12. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00280-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robson, S. G., Tangen, J. M., and Searston, R. A. (2021). The effect of expertise, target usefulness and image structure on visual search. Cogn. Res. 6:16. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00282-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, Y. J., and Jiang, Y. V. (2006). Interrupted visual searches reveal volatile search memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 32, 1208–1220. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.5.1208

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stankov, A. D., Touryan, J., Gordon, S., Ries, A. J., Ki, J., and Parra, L. C. (2021). During natural viewing, neural processing of visual targets continues throughout saccades. J. Vis. 21:7. doi: 10.1167/jov.21.10.7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

The EndNote Team (2013). EndNote.

Google Scholar

Thomas, L. E., and Lleras, A. (2009). Inhibitory tagging in an interrupted visual search. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 1241–1250. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.6.1241

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Zoest, W., Lleras, A., Kingstone, A., and Enns, J. T. (2007). In sight, out of mind: the role of eye movements in the rapid resumption of visual search. Percept. Psychophys. 69, 1204–1217. doi: 10.3758/BF03193956

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walji, M., Brixe, J., Johnson-Throop, K., and Zhang, J. (2004). A theoretical framework to understand and engineer persuasive interruptions. In: Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science Society.

Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Wang, L., Lin, S., Cong, W., Xue, J., and Ochieng, W. (2021). Effect of working experience on air traffic controller eye movement. Engineering 7, 488–494. doi: 10.1016/j.eng.2020.11.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. H., and Drew, T. (2017). Distraction in diagnostic radiology: how is search through volumetric medical images affected by interruptions? Cogn. Res. 2:12. doi: 10.1186/s41235-017-0050-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wynn, R. M., Howe, J. L., Kelahan, L. C., Fong, A., Filice, R. W., and Ratwani, R. M. (2018). The impact of interruptions on chest radiograph interpretation. Acad. Radiol. 25, 1515–1520. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2018.03.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., and Zelinsky, G. J. (2009). Visual search is guided to categorically-defined targets. Vis. Res. 49, 2095–2103. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: interruption, systematic review, visual search, visual cognition, interrupting event

Citation: Cambronero-Delgadillo AJ, Nachtnebel SJ, Körner C, Gilchrist ID and Höfler M (2024) Interruption in visual search: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 15:1384441. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1384441

Received: 09 February 2024; Accepted: 24 April 2024;
Published: 14 May 2024.

Edited by:

Adrian Von Mühlenen, University of Warwick, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Alejandro Lleras, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, United States
Florian Georg Jentsch, University of Central Florida, United States

Copyright © 2024 Cambronero-Delgadillo, Nachtnebel, Körner, Gilchrist and Höfler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alejandro J. Cambronero-Delgadillo, YS5jYW1icm9uZXJvLWRlbGdhZGlsbG9AdW5pLWdyYXouYXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.