- 1Department of Health Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Bío-Bío, Chillán, Chile
- 2Department of Spanish, University of Concepción, Concepción, Chile
Pupil dilation has been associated with the effort required to perform various cognitive tasks. At the lexical level, some studies suggest that this neurophysiological measure would provide objective, real-time information during word processing and lexical access. However, due to the scarcity and incipient advancement of this line of research, its applicability, use, and sensitivity are not entirely clear. This scoping review aims to determine the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in the study of lexical access by providing an up-to-date overview of research in this area. Following the PRISMA protocol, 16 articles were included in this review. The results show that pupillometry is a highly applicable, useful, and sensitive method for assessing lexical skills of word recognition, word retrieval, and semantic activation. Moreover, it easily fits into traditional research paradigms and methods in the field. Because it is a non-invasive, objective, and automated procedure, it can be applied to any population or age group. However, the emerging development of this specific area of research and the methodological diversity observed in the included studies do not yet allow for definitive conclusions in this area, which in turn does not allow for meta-analyses or fully conclusive statements about what the pupil response actually reflects when processing words. Standardized pupillary recording and analysis methods need to be defined to generate more accurate, replicable research designs with more reliable results to strengthen this line of research.
1 Introduction
Language processing is an expression of a complex cognitive function that undergoes significant changes throughout life (Marini and Andreetta, 2016). It develops rapidly in early childhood (Kuhl, 2010), remains stable in adulthood, and gradually declines with age (Peelle, 2019). During this cycle, lexical access occurs as a smooth and effortless process despite the different levels of processing involved (Goral et al., 2007; Shafto et al., 2010; Abrams and Davis, 2016). Lexical access allows us to accurately recognize (understand) or retrieve (produce) a word from the thousands in our vocabulary, allowing us to act effectively in the environment with the speed and accuracy that communicative exchange requires (Abrams and Farrell, 2011; James and Goring, 2018; Davis, 2020; Rojas et al., 2022). In this sense, lexical processing has become a frequent research focus in cognitive neuroscience over the last 40 years.
Lexical processing occupies a central place among language comprehension and production processes. It is a veritable meeting point between peripheral processes related to acoustic or visual perception (input signal) and speech or writing production (output signal). Moreover, it is associated with higher-order processes related to contextual meaning assignment, syntax, and discourse (Igoa, 2009; Abrams and Farrell, 2011; Rojas et al., 2022). On the other hand, lexical skills (i.e., activation-selection of lexical competitors and activation of core meanings) are described as an organized set of ‘subpersonal’ cognitive processes, which means that they are not under the voluntary control of the individual (Igoa, 2009; Cuetos et al., 2015). Generally, there is a consensus that lexical access can be divided into three levels of processing: (1) word recognition, (2) semantic activation, and (3) lexical retrieval.
First, at the word recognition level, a weighting (or comparison) is made between a physical stimulus from the environment (acoustic or visual signal) and a lexical representation stored in memory (Luce and Pisoni, 1998). This similarity weighting is performed between the input signal and a limited set of lexical competitors (recognition targets). Each competitor contains phonological, orthographic or morphological features similar to each other and the input signal (Igoa, 2009; Cuetos et al., 2015). Thus, the competitor that accumulates the highest activation level (due to its similarity to the input signal) is selected and suppresses the rest of the activated competitors (Balota and Chumbley, 1984).
On the other hand, the level of semantic activation involves the recognition of the semantic features of the selected lexical piece (i.e., features of shape, size, color, and other properties), the activation potential of which depends on variables such as the frequency of use of the word, its degree of imaginability, or the age of acquisition (Cuetos et al., 2015). Finally, the level of lexical retrieval (word production) begins with the activation of the concept that the speaker wishes to convey (Levelt, 1989). This process involves the activation of several competing lexical competitors (similar in form or meaning). Thus, the competitor that best represents the conceptualized idea is selected, and the rest are deleted (Igoa, 2009; Cuetos et al., 2015). Subsequently, the selected word is encoded at the syllabic, phonemic or graphemic level for its production.
In general, lexical access studies have very different objectives and can address any of the three levels of processing mentioned above (Perea and Rosa, 1999), which allows research ranging from the activation and selection of lexical competitors (recognition) to the selection of lemmas and subsequent phonological encoding (retrieval). Moreover, they incorporate all kinds of populations and age groups. For example, there are studies on the role of lexical access during language acquisition in early childhood (Dale and Fenson, 1996; Dromi, 1999) or studies on lexical availability in school and university students (Barbeiro et al., 2011; Richter et al., 2013; Ludewig et al., 2022). Many studies also document word retrieval difficulties in old age (Rojas et al., 2023) or verbal fluency difficulties in individuals with cognitive impairment or dementia (Henderson et al., 2023).
Various approaches are employed for its study (Perea and Rosa, 1999), including both traditional offline methods (chronometric tasks and psychometric tests) and online procedures using advanced instrumental techniques (e.g., electroencephalography/EEG, magnetic resonance imaging/MRI, and eye movements). In this context, it is worth mentioning that there has been a substantial increase in the utilization of pupillometry, particularly pupillary dilation, as a neurophysiological measure in cognitive language processing tasks at various levels over the past decade (Kuchinke et al., 2007; Goldinger and Papesh, 2012; Kuchinsky et al., 2013; Hepach, 2023).
Pupil size variation (dilations and contractions) is considered a basic neurophysiological indicator of central nervous system activity (Hyönä et al., 1995). However, research also suggests a connection between pupil size variation and the processing of complex cognitive information (Partala and Surakka, 2003; Liu et al., 2017; Ryals et al., 2021). The reason for this size pupil variation is that the sympathetic nervous system is activated during a complex cognitive task due to the effort put into solving it (Liu et al., 2017). Traditionally, the pupillary response has been thought to respond to modulation of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus (LC) in the brainstem, as its activity would release the norepinephrine (NE) necessary to produce generalized effects on central and peripheral nervous system function. As a result, physiological and cognitive activation would occur simultaneously (Samuels and Szabadi, 2008; Sara, 2009), generating a pupillary response evoked by the effort associated with the task being performed (Steinhauer et al., 2004; Liu et al., 2017).
However, recent studies suggest that pupillary responses are modulated by three brainstem neural systems (Joshi and Gold, 2020; Joshi, 2021), which in turn can be classified into different hierarchical levels of operation (Strauch et al., 2022). At the basic level, the pretectal olivary nucleus (PON), composed of parasympathetic and sympathetic circuits, would be responsible for controlling the pupillary reflex to light and focal distance (when fixating from far to near or vice versa; Joshi and Gold, 2020) through direct innervation of the pupillary muscles. On the other hand, the intermediate level of operation would consist of two partially overlapping neural circuits that are part of a global attentional network: (1) the locus coeruleus (LC)-centered circuit that generates pupil changes associated with attentional states (including pupil changes to outstanding stimuli); and (2) the superior colliculus (SC)-centered circuit that mediates pupil orienting responses (Strauch et al., 2022). Finally, at a higher hierarchical level, the locus coeruleus (LC)-norepinephrine (NE) neuromodulatory system would be responsible for generating responses to sensory control information, executive processing, and higher-level cognitive tasks (Strauch et al., 2022).
In this regard, the increase in pupillary diameter (or pupillary dilation) would be associated with the effort required for the cognitive task. For example, pupillary dilation is associated with the presentation of arousing stimuli or information, whether positive or negative, compared to neutral stimuli. Similarly, it is also connected to changes in cortical and subcortical activity related to cognitive behavior, such as attention lapses, learning of relevant information, and decision-making (Larsen and Waters, 2018; de Winter et al., 2021). Furthermore, pupillary dilation has been linked to processes of language acquisition and processing (Schmidtke, 2018), information storage, and retrieval (Eckstein et al., 2017).
Pupillary response has clear advantages over other cognitive measures, particularly behavioral measures. For example, it would provide information about the time (or precise timing) required for cognitive activation during highly complex tasks. Furthermore, the pupillary response could respond to cognitive processes that are partially activated but do not reach the level required for overt behavior or conscious awareness (Laeng et al., 2012). In addition, since it does not involve motor or verbal responses, it would prevent the conscious influence of executing these responses during processing. Therefore, pupillometry would be an appropriate method to assess cognitive processes during tasks that elicit high arousal, alertness, and emotional control (Ariel and Castel, 2014; Gomes et al., 2015; Kafkas and Montaldi, 2015; Larsen and Waters, 2018; de Winter et al., 2021).
In sum, it is possible to observe that many cognitive processes could cause pupil dilation (Laeng et al., 2012; Kafkas and Montaldi, 2015). In this sense, some studies note that pupil dilation could be an applicable and valuable tool for studying language processing (Schmidtke, 2018; Hepach, 2023). Pupillometry as a research paradigm in language and cognition has been used for quite some time but has only really taken off in the last decade. Linguistic-pupillometric studies have focused on three broad areas: (1) auditory and auditory–visual processing, (2) orthographic language processing, and (3) speech production. These have shown that pupillometry can provide researchers with new and creative ways to test hypotheses and thus advance knowledge of cognitive language processing (Schmidtke, 2018). However, it is not as well developed as other techniques used in this area, and there are still gaps regarding the meaning of this physiological response and its actual viability as a dependent variable in linguistic-cognitive research.
In this context, pupillometry could provide objective and real-time information about the cognitive effort or performance an individual experiences when processing words, for example, (1) when recognizing, inhibiting, and selecting words (McLaughlin et al., 2022), (2) when activating meanings (Chapman and Hallowell, 2015) or (3) during word retrieval and their phonological encoding (Granholm et al., 1998). In addition, pupillometry could reflect a person’s level of effort and surprise when the person must process ambiguous words, activate multiple meanings, or retrieve specific information (Pluchino et al., 2014). However, due to the limited and incipient advancement of this line of research in experimental psycholinguistics (in fact, most of the research developed is along the lines of auditory processing and speech production rather than lexical processing; Schmidtke, 2018), its applicability, use, and sensitivity are not entirely clear. Therefore, this scoping review aims to assess the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in the study of lexical access.
1.1 Research questions
Based on the background presented so far, this scoping review formulates the following research questions: What is the applicability of pupillometry in the study of lexical access? What is the usefulness of this neurophysiological measure, and what are the specific aims of this area of research? What psycholinguistic methods, tasks, and procedures are used? Are the results of lexical-pupillometric research sensitive, reliable and replicable? What are the projections and limitations of this research area?
2 Methods
2.1 Context
Our approach to answering the research questions involved creating a protocol (Moher et al., 2015) that aligns with the objectives of a scoping review. This type of review aims to determine the scope and methodology of the literature on a specific topic and then provide a comprehensive summary of the available evidence (Munn et al., 2018). We developed eligibility criteria considering the research context and used critical concepts for the initial search phase. Finally, although the amount of research on lexical processing and pupillometry appears to be scarce, no grey literature was included in this review.
2.2 Search strategy and procedure
This scoping review was designed, conducted, and reported following the PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist and explanation (Tricco et al., 2018), and was supervised by one of the researchers (GL). The process included four stages: (1) search strategy definition, (2) literature screening, (3) data extraction, and (4) data synthesis and analysis.
2.2.1 Search strategy
The search took place from November 25 to December 03, 2023, and included articles published in English from 2017 to date. The reasons for including articles since 2017 were: (1) before the search strategy phase, we refined and tested with different keywords, Boolean operators, and search years. As a result, we found that only a few articles before 2017 presented titles and objectives somewhat close to the purposes of the present review. However, we noted that these studies mainly focused on the role of pupillometry in acoustic speech perception and others on speech production, not necessarily on lexical access. Coincidentally, we also note that Schmidtke’s (2018) review, which focuses on language processing and pupillometry, is also dominated by auditory processing and speech production studies. Only a limited number of lexical-pupillometric studies from earlier years are mentioned, but they used acoustic stimuli as input signals (exclusion criterion for the present review, see Literature screening). (2) The inclusion of articles published in the last eight years allows us to make a very up-to-date first approach to lexico-pupillometric studies, identifying the essential research elements, recent gaps, future challenges, and current aspects that need to be better defined (essential objectives of a scoping review). However, beyond the decision to include articles from 2017 onwards, we cannot exclude the possibility that there are no lexico-pupillometric studies from previous years.
The search covered four databases: Web of Science (WoS), Science Direct, Scopus, and PubMed. The search strategy took a sensitive approach and included iterative processes using a combination of keywords, index terms, Boolean operators and search strings. The basic syntax and terms included were (((“Pupillometry” OR “Pupil dilation”) AND (“Words”))), which correspond to the natural language terms in this area of study (not included in MeSH) needed to generate the search strategy (Table 1). PubMed was used for the initial search, with subsequent adjustments for each database. The initial search was expanded to capture alternative words and phrases for each key term so that the search terms were tested and refined to ensure relevant and complete results. In this regard, “Lexical Access” was discarded and replaced with “Words” to avoid generalization during the identification phase. The literature was manually searched in databases and downloaded in a digital (.ris) format, then processed with the Rayyan platform.1
2.2.2 Literature screening
All collected articles were reviewed by title and abstract. For this phase, two authors (CR and JC) utilized the Rayyan platform (a free AI web application; Ouzzani et al., 2016) to conduct the initial screening independently and autonomously using a double-blind function. Each reviewer labeled the identified articles in two categories: (1) included and (2) not included. In order to make this decision, the reviewers relied on the following inclusion criteria: (1) studies with an experimental, quasi-experimental, clinical trial, or cohort designs; (2) studies using pupillometry as a method of data collection; (3) studies examining changes in pupillary dilation associated with the following terms (tasks or levels): visual word recognition, word retrieval, lexical competitors, lexical activation, semantic activation or meaning activation; (4) studies in normal population or with cognitive impairments (congenital or acquired) and throughout the life cycle. The following exclusion criteria were used: (1) studies in pupillary dilation associated with the concepts: “speech recognition,” “auditory word comprehension,” “speech analysis,” “acoustic speech analysis,” or “speech perception”; (2) conferences, dissertations and theses.
The reasons for including articles with only visual (and not auditory) input were: (1) a large number of psycholinguistic studies with auditory stimuli focus on pupil responses in speech perception processes, phonetic-phonological signal analysis, acoustic analysis, and central auditory processing, levels that are not the focus of the present review; (2) the selection of only one type of sensory input allows greater control over the methodological and procedural characteristics of the included studies; (3) it also allows the analyzed pupillary response (results) to be more “homogeneous” when responding exclusively to visual lexical tasks; and (4) some hypotheses suggest that the sensory analysis of auditory stimuli presents problems of variability, segmentation and coarticulation, leading to greater use of cognitive resources. In contrast, the visual signal is relatively uniform and invariant, facilitating access to the lexicon and other higher cognitive processes.
Finally, if authors had conflicting decisions on an article, a third author (YEV) resolved the dispute and determined whether the article was included.
2.2.3 Data extraction
Full-text versions of the selected articles were downloaded for analysis. Four authors independently reviewed the texts (CR, JC, YEV, MGC). Through discussion, the whole group resolved any discrepancies in the decision include/exclude any article. The final articles were chosen because they met the criterion of being consistent with the aim of the scoping review. Tables were designed in Word format to extract information on (1) objectives, (2) population and sample, (3) general characteristics of the assessed task, (4) method and design, (5) results, and (6) main conclusion. The primary reviewer (CR) confirmed that the included articles met the review’s purpose and inclusion/exclusion criteria previously controlled. Due to the varied methods, populations, and objectives of the studies included, meta-analysis was not the appropriate approach for this review. Finally, considering the scope of the topic and the purpose of a scoping review, no critical evaluation was performed on the sources of evidence included.
2.2.4 Data synthesis and analysis
In order to present the results in a more systematic way, the included studies were categorized into three groups based on the levels of lexical processing reported in the literature. These groups were thus as follows: (1) studies on lexical recognition, (2) studies on semantic processing and activation, and (3) studies on lexical retrieval or word production. Finally, one of the authors (YS) was responsible for the review and consistency of the literature necessary for the introduction and subsequent discussion of the results.
3 Results
The initial search identified 430 records. A total of 177 duplicate articles were removed. A total of 253 articles were reviewed, of which 221 were initially excluded (theoretical articles, articles that did not meet the purpose of the review, or inclusion/exclusion criteria). Thirty-two articles met the eligibility criteria, of which 16 were excluded (6 because they included auditory input in their methodology or assessed speech perception; 6 assessed non-linguistic constructs, and 4 assessed another linguistic level). The number of reports included in this review was 16 (Figure 1).
3.1 Studies on word recognition and pupillometry
Five of the 16 articles included in this review assessed the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in visual word recognition (Table 2). In particular, 1 study (Haro et al., 2017) aimed to determine whether pupillary response is sensitive to lexical frequency and whether this effect is related to lexical access. Moreover, 2 articles focused on pupillary behavior during word recognition in bilinguals (Guasch et al., 2017; Toivo and Scheepers, 2019). Finally, 2 additional articles examined pupillary dilation (cognitive effort) in different types of readers during word recognition and subsequent reading (Shechter and Share, 2021; Shechter et al., 2022).
In the first study, Haro et al. (2017) assessed pupillary response in a traditional lexical decision task (LDT, experiment 1) and a delayed lexical decision task (LDT delayed, experiment 2). During the experiments, 60 participants (30 per group) had to discriminate between pseudowords and words while their pupillary response was monitored. The words had different lexical frequencies (low, medium, or high). The results of the study suggest that pupillary response is a sensitive indicator of word frequency, with low-frequency words causing significantly grater pupil dilation than high-frequency words, regardless of the LDT type. The authors argue that pupil dilation would represent a genuine effect of lexical processing and could be considered an objective measure for assessing word recognition, as it would not be mediated by response mechanisms accessory to LDT (i.e., executive control, inhibition or sensorimotor aspects).
Among the articles focusing on bilingualism, Guasch et al. (2017) studied how bilinguals’ pupillary response is affected by words that fulfill the cognate condition, i.e., words from different languages with similar spelling and identical meaning. Using LDT, pupil dilation was recorded in 35 bilingual native speakers (Spanish-Catalan) when they processed words in the identical cognate/non-identical cognate/non-cognate condition. The study showed that the non-cognate word condition (words with no spelling overlap but identical meaning) produced a significantly greater pupil dilation than the other experimental conditions. The research provided evidence that pupillometry is sensitive to specific word characteristics (semantic-orthographic congruence/incongruence) in bilingual native speakers. Along the same lines, Toivo and Scheepers (2019) assessed the sensitivity of the pupillary responses to words of high emotional arousal words in L1 and L2 using LDT. They included 96 monolingual and bilingual young adult participants. The study revealed that emotionally loaded words in the native language (L1) produced a stronger pupillary response (increased dilation) than emotionally loaded words in L2, possibly due to the richer semantic representation created by emotional words in L1. According to the authors, pupillometry could be an objective and valuable tool for assessing the recognition of emotionally loaded words.
Finally, 2 articles were compiled that examined the applicability and usefulness of using pupillometry as an indicator of cognitive effort in word recognition in diverse type of readers. In example, Shechter and Share (2021) examined the pupillary response during a task with known words and pseudowords (letter strings of different lengths) in 48 university students and 48 schoolchildren. Consequently, longer pseudowords resulted in greater pupillary dilation, indicating increased cognitive effort of both experienced readers (university students) and novice readers (schoolchildren), both for oral and silent reading task. The authors project that pupillometry may offer a more sensitive moment-by-moment glimpse into the dynamics of word recognition (including developmental, interindividual, and intraindividual variation). In a second study, Shechter et al. (2022) assessed the sensitivity of the pupillary response during recognition and reading aloud of a series of words in children (N = 34) and young adults (N = 34), who were further classified into fast/slow readers. The results showed that children showed more significant cognitive effort (as evidenced by a greater pupillary response) than adults during word recognition and reading aloud. However, no differences in cognitive effort were observed between fast and slow readers in any age group. According to the authors, the pupillometric index would offer objective hints about how reading ability develops starting in the earliest phases of life.
3.2 Studies on activation and semantic word processing and pupillometry
Seven of the 16 articles reviewed (Table 3) evaluated the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in activation and semantic word-processing tasks. One article relied on embodiment theory to show whether there is a significant pupillary response when processing words whose meaning is associated with a higher or lower level of brightness (Mathôt et al., 2017). Five additional articles examined the behavior of the pupillary response when establishing congruence or association judgments between pairs of words with specific semantic and orthographic features (Geller et al., 2019; Egan et al., 2020; Reilly et al., 2020; Egan et al., 2023; Haro et al., 2023). Another study (1) examined pupillary response sensitivity to determine whether the semantic priming effect is present in children under 24 months of age (Angulo-Chavira and Arias-Trejo, 2021).
In the first study reviewed, Mathôt et al. (2017) assessed pupillary response in a semantic recognition task within the framework of embodied language theory. In the experiment, participants (30 in total) were tasked with identifying words that were semantically linked to brightness, darkness, neutrality (words that did not evoke brightness or darkness), as well as animals (words whose meaning is not associated with any aspect of brightness or darkness). Participants were instructed to press the space bar when animal-related words appeared on the screen, while their pupillary response was monitored throughout the trials. The pupillary response, which reacts to changes in illumination, showed a smaller dilation when words linked to brightness were activated but a larger dilation when processing words related to darkness. The authors confirm that the pupillary response is consistent with embodied language theory, as semantic activation of word meaning was sufficient to elicit a response without needing an external light stimulus.
In the case of articles focusing on pupillary response to judgments of congruence or semantic relatedness between word pairs, Egan et al. (2020) used Event-Related Potential (ERP) and pupillometry to examine how alliteration (similar phonological form) between words improves attentional interactions during reading. Twenty adult participants were asked to make semantic judgments between semantically congruent/incongruent word pairs that also satisfied the alliteration/non-alliteration condition. The results showed that the pupil response was larger for word pairs that met the congruence condition (alliterative or non-alliterative). The research also found that the interaction between alliteration and congruence led to a continuous increase in pupil dilation when related words appeared in a sentence. According to the authors, alliteration, as measured by ERP and pupillometry, can influence semantic processing by supporting concept activation during comprehension errors. The same authors (Egan et al., 2023) evaluated the sensitivity of pupillometry to measure the influence of phonological relatedness on semantic congruence judgments in readers with dyslexia. 38 participants with (19) and without dyslexia (19) were asked to semantically process orthogonally manipulated word pairs for semantic congruence and alliteration (phonological form). Participants had to indicate whether the two words presented were meaning-related by pressing a button. As a result, pupil dilation was consistently lower in dyslexic readers. According to the authors, people with dyslexia show reduced engagement of attentional processes during reading, making pupillary response a sensitive measure for detecting differences in lexical processing among readers.
Geller et al. (2019) examined the utility of the pupillary response in a processing task of taxonomically or thematically semantically related word pairs, whose relatedness could be weak/strong. Sixty undergraduates had to make judgments about the relatedness/unrelatedness of the word pairs. The results showed greater pupil dilation in weakly related pairs than in strongly related pairs (particularly taxonomic pairs), where greater pupil dilation would require greater cognitive effort. The pupillary data would suggest that semantic control demands would be determined primarily by the strength of the semantic relationship (weak/strong) and not necessarily by the type of relationship (taxonomic or thematic). In a similar study, Haro et al. (2023) examined whether the pupillary response was sensitive to differences between polysemous and homonymous words when performing tasks involving different processing costs. The authors conducted four experiments on a sample of 117 adult subjects. Specifically, in Experiment 3 (n = 28), participants had to solve a semantic categorization task, and in Experiment 4 (n = 24) they had to determine whether a word had more than one meaning. Greater pupil dilation was only observed for those words that were congruent to the semantic category evaluated (Exp.3) and when ambiguous words were processed (Exp.4). No effects were observed when comparing polysemous and homonymous conditions. The authors point out that there is insufficient evidence to demonstrate differences in the processing of polysemous/homonymous words. However, the processing of ambiguous words and those that are of the same semantic category, would generate greater cognitive cost and therefore greater pupillary response.
In the same line, but with different aims, Reilly et al. (2020) employed pupillometry to assess the impact of transcranial stimulation on the processing of taboo and non-taboo words. Adult participants were required to read aloud words of taboo/non-taboo meaning before and after transcranial stimulation. As a result, transcranial stimulation on the right cerebral hemisphere impacts taboo word processing, which is reflected in increased pupillary dilation. In their conclusion, the authors state that the pupillary response to taboo words after transcranial stimulation results from the activation of emotional valence and the person’s unconscious inhibitory control when processing socially inappropriate words. Finally, Angulo-Chavira and Arias-Trejo (2021) explored the pupillary response when executing a semantically mediated priming task within the same level (i.e., cat [prime] - mouse [mediator] - cheese [target]) to determine whether this semantic effect is present in children younger than 24 months. Twenty-seven children observed eight experimental trials set up on a computer. The trials were divided into two conditions: the related condition (target and cue words had an indirect relation) and the unrelated condition (target and cue words had no semantic or associative relation). The pupillary response shows that children present mediated priming effects from 24 months onwards. However, the authors argue that there may be inhibitory mechanisms that limit their activation from an early age.
3.3 Studies on word retrieval and pupillometry
Four of the 16 articles found (Table 4) evaluated the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry during word retrieval. Three articles address pupillary response in verbal fluency tasks (multiple word retrieval) in various populations, and the remaining one addresses pupillary behavior in the presence of the tip-of-the-tongue states (TOTs) in adults.
El Haj et al. (2020) assessed pupillary response in a phonemic verbal fluency task (retrieval of multiple lexical items). Forty-five young adult participants were asked to retrieve all words beginning with the phoneme “p” and then to count numbers aloud while their pupillary dilation was tracked. As a result, greater pupillary dilation was obtained for the verbal fluency task compared to the counting task. The authors suggest that the increased pupil dilation during verbal fluency can be attributed to the cognitive requirement of the task, as participants were required to retrieve words, suppress irrelevant information, and explore alternative options. The same research team (El Haj et al., 2022) repeated the experiment with a group of older adults (n = 45). The results indicated that pupillary dilation increases among older adults during the verbal fluency task. For both studies, the researchers project that pupillometry could be helpful as a method of ecologically assessing lexical retrieval skills at different stages of the life cycle. In a recent publication, El Haj et al. (2024) evaluated individuals diagnosed with bvFTD compared to controls. Consequently, individuals with bvFTD showed less pupil dilation during the letter and category verbal fluency task (animals) compared to the control group. However, when comparing both tasks with counting numbers, the bvFTD group showed a greater pupillary response. The authors explore pupillometry’s applicability and ecological importance in investigating language processing in persons with dementia.
Finally, Ryals et al. (2021) explored how sensitive the pupillary response is during TOTs. They evaluated 46 undergraduates who were asked to answer pre-made questions. Participants had to report the sensation of TOTs when they could not retrieve the words associated with the requested questions. The results showed that TOTs generated a significant pupillary response (increase in diameter). The authors suggest that the increase in pupil size supports the notion that TOTs are linked to feelings of curiosity and the desire for knowledge, with the degree of pupil dilation corresponding to the amount of information retrieved. Therefore, pupillometry would be an appropriate and valuable tool for studying these linguistic phenomena.
4 Discussion
This section provides a general reflection and discussion of the findings presented in the results section. We begin with a discussion of the applicability, usefulness and purpose of pupillometry in lexical access studies. This is followed by a discussion of aspects related to the research methods and procedures used. Next, the sensitivity of this measure and the reliability of the results are discussed. Some basic recommendations for the conduct of lexical-pupillometric studies are then presented. Finally, some projections and limitations are briefly discussed.
4.1 Applicability, usefulness and purposes
The general interest in cognitive neuroscience in studying systematic changes in pupillary dilation is based on the fact that it is a noninvasive, objective and automatic neurophysiological measure (Hepach, 2023), which can reflect-if well controlled-the behavior of multiple cognitive constructs in diverse populations and life cycle stages (Laeng et al., 2012; Kafkas and Montaldi, 2015). In this context, the results of this review support that the pupillary response evoked by language processing tasks-and reflecting neuronal activity-has become an emerging area of development in experimental psycholinguistics over the last decade. However, its applicability is not exclusive to language processing. Our findings suggest that around 80% of the articles recovered during the identification phase were associated with various cognitive domains such as memory, attention, motivation, and emotions, where language tasks were merely one aspect of their data-gathering strategies and did not play a significant role. Out of the total, only 20% of the studies explored language processing, covering various levels (syntax, auditory recognition, speech perception, and lexical access) across different age groups and in neurotypical individuals or with neuro-cognitive disorders (congenital and acquired).
In particular, the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry as a method to assess cognitive processes have been demonstrated in several research fields (Ariel and Castel, 2014; Gomes et al., 2015; Kafkas and Montaldi, 2015; Larsen and Waters, 2018; de Winter et al., 2021). These areas comprise surprise and expectancy paradigms (Pätzold and Liszkowski, 2019), memory and attention tasks (Larsen and Waters, 2018; de Winter et al., 2021), or arousal and motivational states (Hepach et al., 2019). Regarding language and lexical access, we found that only 16 articles fully met the established criteria. Nonetheless, the careful examination of their methods, pupillometric analysis, statistical procedures, and outcomes provides sufficient evidence to assert that pupillometry is a versatile, valuable, and sensitive method for assessing linguistic processes involved in word recognition, retrieval, and semantic activation, and that it can be applied to any population, age group, and native or bilingual speakers. Indeed, the studies demonstrated that pupillary changes – measured mainly using peaks and dilation latencies – effectively reflect lexical activity, which allowed effects to be tested in 94% of the reports analyzed.
Furthermore, the present review demonstrates that the research purposes (objectives) in lexical access and pupillometry are also very diverse and varied, ranging from pupillary response in emotional word recognition in bilinguals (Toivo and Scheepers, 2019) to pupillometric behavior in lexical fluency tasks in individuals with bvFTD (El Haj et al., 2024). The same applies to the designs, techniques, and procedures applied, where pupillometry can be effectively used for word retrieval and semantic judgment tasks, as well as LDT (or its variations). In addition, there are differences in the cleaning, analysis, and interpretation of the pupil data. These variations include using different analysis windows, the basal pupil diameter used, the pupil variable examined (maximum pupil dilation vs. maximum latency), and other factors. Thus, the emerging development and the diversity of methods observed in this field, which can be considered an advantage, do not yet allow definitive and solid conclusions to be drawn in this area.
On the other hand, although the collected background information demonstrates the usefulness of pupillometry in addressing various research problems in lexical access, it would be incorrect to assume that any pupillary response indicates lexical activation. From a methodological point of view, it is always necessary to “isolate” other cognitive factors associated with pupillary dilation, such as surprise, attention, or task-associated motivation (Hepach, 2023), as they could mask the expected pupillary response. Hence, while pupillometry is undeniably useful for studying lexical access and shows promising growth potential, it still requires improved precision and standardized conditions of use that demonstrate that it truly represents the pupillary response obtained, as we are not entirely sure whether it is an exclusive response to the cognitive effort expended in the task (Van der Wel and Van Steenbergen, 2018), or a response to the attentional component present in the cognitive function performed (Strauch et al., 2022), or simply a specific response for language processing.”
In sum, this review validates the applicability of pupillometry as a sensitive and valuable technique for assessing various lexical levels, which is a positive attribute of this neurophysiological measure. However, due to the varied purposes, objectives, and data analysis approaches of the reviewed studies, we cannot establish definitive conclusions in this area. Multiple reasons could explain this: (1) Pupillometry is a recently popularized and recognized technique; (2) pupillary dilation is a very volatile signal whose control and instrumental measurement have recently achieved better management (Hepach, 2023); (3) the interest in its application and use is limited to specific research groups rather than large collaborative networks, who turned to pupillary dilation measurement as a practical and conceptual solution to their research problem (Schmidtke, 2018). In this sense, defining more standardized purposes for applying pupillometry in lexical access would enable us to produce far more accurate research purposes, enhancing the vigor and robustness of this study area.
4.2 Methods and procedures applied
There is a consensus that most studies in experimental psycholinguistics use small sample sizes, which contrasts with the large number of experimental trials used to favor greater statistical power (Perea and Rosa, 1999). In addition, these studies often include young, normotypical participants selected for interest, with the aim of testing hypotheses in a ‘healthy’ brain (Rojas et al., 2022). In this respect, lexical-pupillometric studies are no exception. For example, independent of the level of processing assessed, all articles exhibited bounded sample sizes in each experiment performed (i.e., Recognition: n = 35, Guasch et al., 2017; n = 96, Toivo and Scheepers, 2019; n = 68, Shechter et al., 2022. Semantic activation: n = 20, Egan et al., 2020; n = 30, Mathôt et al., 2017; n = 60, Geller et al., 2019. Retrieval: n = 45, El Haj et al., 2020, 2022; n = 46, Ryals et al., 2021), but they used a large number of experimental trials. Moreover, like the “more traditional” studies, the evaluated populations were preferably young university students with normal cognition, vision, and hearing. This situation varied when the object of study involved specific age groups or clinical conditions (n = 19 persons with dyslexia, Egan et al., 2023; n = 27 children under 24 months, Angulo-Chavira and Arias-Trejo, 2021; n = 24 patients with bvFTD, El Haj et al., 2024). Given the benefits of pupillometry (i.e., a non-invasive, objective, and free of conscious responses), sample sizes and types of participants observed in the studies reviewed, it is possible to assume that this technique could be applied in any population, particularly to individuals with complex motor, behavioral, or cognitive control, such as very young children (Hepach et al., 2019), older people and individuals with neurodegenerative diseases (El Haj et al., 2022, 2024). This fact reveals another advantage and endorses the potential of this neurophysiological tool.
Regarding the experimental tasks used in lexical-pupillometric studies, at the level of word recognition and similar to traditional studies in the field, two articles used classical LDTs (Guasch et al., 2017; Haro et al., 2017), and two additional studies used word recognition and subsequent reading methods (Shechter and Share, 2021; Shechter et al., 2022). On the other hand, regarding the activation and semantic processing level, only semantic judgment tasks were stated in several articles (Geller et al., 2019; Egan et al., 2020; Reilly et al., 2020). At the retrieval level, verbal fluency (El Haj et al., 2020, 2022, 2024) and naming by definitions (Ryals et al., 2021) tasks were preferentially used. Therefore, the assessment paradigms in the reviewed lexical-pupillometric studies are the same as the traditional ones. Additionally, they share similarities in the number of items used, the organization of each trial, the number of blocks, the stimulus presentation latencies, and the recorded behavioral responses (RT and accuracy). Similarly, lexical variables like frequency (high/low), lexicality (words/pseudowords), syllable length, polysemy, phonological form, and prime type, among others, are also subject to control. In this sense, pupillometry’s easy adaption to conventional research paradigms in experimental psycholinguistics supports its numerous uses and provides further evidence for its effectiveness in examining lexical access.
The situation becomes more confusing when analyzing and comparing the methods used to collect, control, and analyze the resulting eye data. Although, it should be recognized that basic points are consistent across articles: (1) pupillary data were recorded before, during, and after the presentation of the target stimulus; (2) areas of interest were previously identified according to the purpose of each examination, and (3) two classes of pupil measurements were preferably calculated: maximum dilation and maximum pupil latency (this is consistent with the language processing and pupillometry studies referenced by Schmidtke, 2018); it should also be noted that there are other aspects that need to be described and addressed in more detail (see point 4.4. Recommendations). In conclusion, although the necessary details for pupillometric control and analysis are reported, there are also some methodological omissions, probably made but not explicitly stated, which are necessary to replicate the studies presented here.
Hepach’s work (Hepach, 2023) defines some requirements for more effective pupillary dilation control at the experimental level. In this sense, the pupil response would not only reflect a particular cognitive response, but would also be the result of: (1) light adaptation to the environment and the focused visual field, (2) eye and head movements (Brisson et al., 2013), (3) changes in tonic arousal state (cardiac or cutaneous arousal state; Anderson and Colombo, 2009), and (4) the cognitive state of the participants (Eckstein et al., 2017). In this context, only a small number of the reviewed articles offer partial details regarding the light control of the environment, the particular visual field of focus, the participants’ head posture, and their state of arousal; except for the cognitive state, which was always stated and controlled. The lack of control of these experimental conditions and, in particular, the light verification during the application of the experiments (Schmidtke, 2018) are probably some of the main weaknesses observed in the reviewed studies.
4.3 Sensitivity of pupillometry and reliability of results
As mentioned, determining a priori that pupil size increase reflects only lexical activity (or word processing) may be risky and highly inaccurate. In contrast, pupillary responses may simultaneously encompass other cognitive processes (i.e., cognitive load, surprise, motivation; Larsen and Waters, 2018; Hepach et al., 2019; Pätzold and Liszkowski, 2019) and depend on external elements such as ambient illumination and intrinsic characteristics to the participant (Brisson et al., 2013; Eckstein et al., 2017). Therefore, the sensitivity and reliability of pupillometry will depend to a large extent on methodologically rigorous and specific experiments and their subsequent analysis. In this regard, all the articles reviewed proved to be very strict both in the approach to the lexical task and in the subsequent pupillometric analysis (although, as already described, in some cases, specific methodological parameters necessary for replication were not explicitly stated). Therefore, apart from some observed weaknesses, the analysis allows us to argue that the pupillometric results obtained seem to represent, in whole or in part, the lexical processing of words.
At the statistical level, the analyzed lexical-pupillometric results (i.e., maximum pupil dilation or maximum pupil latency) are quite clear and significant at the different levels tested. For example, in word recognition tasks, statistically significant (p < 0.05) pupillary dilations were evidenced when recognizing low-frequency words (Haro et al., 2017), words without cognate status (Guasch et al., 2017), emotional words in L1 (Toivo and Scheepers, 2019) and when processing pseudowords (Shechter and Share, 2021). Concerning semantic activation, significant pupillary dilation has been observed in the processing of words with low semantic relatedness (Geller et al., 2019), in the activation of taboo words (post-transcranial stimulation, Reilly et al., 2020), and in the processing of words that are semantically related or consistent to a semantic field (Egan et al., 2020; Haro et al., 2023). At retrieval level, significant pupillary dilation (p < 0.05) occurs when performing verbal fluency tasks and when TOTs are present (El Haj et al., 2020, 2022, 2024; Ryals et al., 2021). In sum, although the number of articles reviewed is relatively small to determine exactly how suitable pupillometry is for the study of word processing, it is striking that 94% of the articles reviewed showed the expected effects, which seems to support the idea that the technique does indeed appear to be sensitive enough for the study of lexical processing.
However, the same cannot be said for the reliability of pupillometric measures. With the evidence reviewed in the present review, it cannot yet be determined whether pupil dilation is specifically and uniquely related to language processing. It is also uncertain whether the pupil dilation levels obtained in the applied tasks are not at least partially influenced by other cognitive processes, such as motivation, arousal, and attention, as evidence from other fields shows (Hepach et al., 2019; Pätzold and Liszkowski, 2019). In this sense, the influence of other cognitive processes on pupillometric measures is not in itself a problem; many of the linguistic processes are not exclusively linguistic and a large number of factors are involved in both the comprehension and production of language. However, it is necessary to determine the relative weight of each factor in the pupillometric measures obtained in order to better isolate specific aspects of language processing.
In this context, we hypothesize that the different neuromodulatory systems responsible for pupil activity (PON/SC/LC, Joshi and Gold, 2020; Joshi, 2021) operate as interconnected systems that generate specific pupil responses in the presence of different cognitive functions (i.e., attention, language, emotion, working memory). Therefore, we think word processing (or lexical access) may generate pupil dilation patterns specific to this function (i.e., reflected in specific temporal dilation window, mean dilation diameters or particular dilation peaks). Thus, it is likely that there are distinct dilation components for linguistic tasks related to word recognition, lexical retrieval, and semantic activation. However, these specific pupil dilation components need to be isolated and distinguished from other response components (similar to the behavior of Evoked response potential/ERP components), such as cognitive effort, surprise, attention, or memory, which are likely to manifest themselves through pupil response patterns that are different and distinct from the ‘linguistic’ pupil response. Thus, determining which cognitive-neural processes are actually represented by changes in pupil size has become one of the major current challenges for cognitive neuroscience.
One way to ensure the reliability of lexical-pupillometric results is for studies using this measure to generate their own experimental designs. In other words, these experimental designs should be intended as pupillometric studies from the initial stage of the experimental design and not at later stages (Hepach, 2023). Hepach (2023) mentions that it would not be advisable to ‘salvage’ data from a completed (and for other purposes) eye movement study to perform a supplementary or secondary pupillometric analysis, as there are numerous artifacts and experimental manipulations that could alter the results. Consequently, any study examining pupillary changes during word retrieval, semantic activation, or recognition should be designed from the beginning as a pupillometric study. This includes considerations for sample size, basal pupil diameter definition, analysis window duration, control over blink frequency, saccades, and other artifacts, control over illumination (i.e., matching the luminance of the stimuli or using the same image in all experimental conditions), excitability and cognitive characteristics of the persons assessed, among other aspects.
4.4 Basic recommendations for conducting lexical-pupillometric studies
The results of the present review allow us to propose some basic recommendations for using pupillometry in lexical access tasks. First of all, considering the multiple applications of this neurophysiological measure, its different forms of measurement and the different alternatives in data analysis, we suggest that, before formulating the experiments, the researcher should: (1) deepen his or her physiological knowledge of the pupillary response; (2) inquire about technical and procedural aspects already established in lexical-pupillometric experiments; and (3) formulate or replicate objectives based on previous studies in the field, which will allow improving the consistency and stability of research purpose in different populations and contexts.
Secondly, we have found certain methodological and procedural weaknesses in several lexical-pupillometric studies that should be described and addressed in more detail. For example, more precise control over the number of blinks, saccadic movements, and other artifacts is recommended. It is known that data ‘contaminated’ by blinks (N > 5) and saccades (N > 5) in five or more trials throughout the experiment can lead to erroneous results (Kafkas and Montaldi, 2015; Carbajal et al., 2018; McKee et al., 2018). Similarly, the differences in measurement units used for pupil dilation (arbitrary chamber units vs. metric units) and the lack of explanation for choosing one measurement over another must be addressed. It is also recommended to comprehensively describe the methodology used to calculate the mean number of eye fixations and the mean fixation duration for each trial. In addition, it is recommended to provide further explanation on how the basal pupil diameter was calculated: by averaging the anterior pupil diameter during the target appearance or while the fixation cross was displayed? In this sense, for future research in this area, we propose to review the methodological considerations for using pupillometry in the study of language processing, according to Schmidtke (2018).
Thirdly, there is an urgent need to replicate already published studies, increase sample sizes and standardize specific parameters of pupillary analysis in order to increase the reliability of this neurophysiological measurement (Hepach, 2023). Thus, greater and better replicability of studies and standardization of the forms of recording, administration and analysis of pupillometric results will favor the knowledge and reliability of the procedure and allow access to more consistent language processing data.
Finally, as general recommendations, Mathôt and Vilotijević (2023) proposed a practical guide for the general design of cognitive experiments using pupillometry. In their paper, they propose six basic principles for experimental design: (1) stimuli used in experiments should be constant across conditions; (2) eye position should also be constant across conditions; (3) tests should be presented at a slow presentation rate; (4) pupil size should be assessed while participants are doing nothing; (5) ambient lighting should be intermediate and adapted to the brightness of the screen; and (6) data should be stored in a single file per participant (for further details see Mathôt and Vilotijević, 2023). In addition, for the correct selection of participants, recording characteristics, stimulus and timing parameters, data accessory noise, measurement procedures, and presentation of results and plots, we suggest reviewing the paper by Steinhauer et al. (2022), “Publication guidelines and recommendations for pupillary measurement in psychophysiological studies.” Finally, we recommend reviewing the paper by Kret and Sjak-Shie (2019), “Preprocessing pupil size data: Guidelines and code,” for guidelines and technical suggestions for correct pupil data processing and statistical analysis.
4.5 Projections and limitations
The projections of pupillometry in the study of lexical access are invaluable in developing a better understanding of how people process information. First, it is an objective, non-invasive neurophysiological tool without voluntary response mechanisms (Haro et al., 2017), which provides information about the time course of the process being assessed (Guasch et al., 2017). Second, its high adaptability and usefulness are favorable for studying word recognition in different age groups, both in the native language and in L2 for bilinguals (Toivo and Scheepers, 2019), and it can be coupled with LDT or recognition-reading tasks. In addition, it allows working on semantic activation tasks, such as word retrieval, in neurotypical individuals or those with complex motor, cognitive, and behavioral control (Angulo-Chavira and Arias-Trejo, 2021; El Haj et al., 2024). Third, while specific methodological issues with pupillometric data analysis can be fixed, the reviewed studies demonstrate a high degree of rigor and thoroughness in their procedures, leading to results that seem sensitive.
Furthermore, the technology used to record and measure pupillary behavior is expected to advance in accuracy and dependability, given its volatility (Hepach, 2023). Enhancing these parameters will make it possible to record responses in greater detail, making it easier to interpret the anticipated effects. Moreover, a more systematic integration of lexical and pupillometry research with other neuroimaging technologies is necessary. Combining various measurement modalities can yield a more thorough understanding of the linguistic-cognitive processes under examination. This would not only help to increase knowledge in a normotypical population, but could also strengthen clinical research into the understanding and diagnosis of neuropsychiatric disorders, as well as the development of future biomarkers for the early detection of Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s, autism spectrum disorders and other neuropsychiatric disorders.
Pupillometry also has limitations that have partially impeded its advancement in cognitive neuroscience. The first and perhaps most common reason is that several factors affect pupil dilation, such as ambient lighting, age, fatigue, and the person’s general state of alertness, making it an extremely volatile measure that is difficult to capture and interpret. This limitation is compounded by non-cognitive autonomic physiological perturbations (stress response or direct light stimulation of the experimental material), where distinguishing between cognitive and non-cognitive responses is challenging. An additional constraint pertains to the inter-individual variability in pupillary response (basal pupil diameter) among participants. This variability must be considered when analyzing the data since a typical response for one participant might not be typical for another.
Finally, on a technical level, pupillometry requires a thorough knowledge and handling of the software and hardware used, which is essential for reliable measurements. Inappropriate pupil calibration or reading may result in errors in the interpretation of results. A reliable (and therefore reproducible) pupillary response requires strict and controlled experimental and methodological conditions. A “light” or loose experimental design may influence pupillary response or reflect other cognitive processes. Thus, despite some limitations, pupillometry is a valuable tool in cognitive research and, in this particular case, in the study of lexical processing. By combining pupillometry results with those obtained from other measures and techniques, researchers can gain a more complete understanding of the cognitive processes under study. Of course, it is important to recognize and address these limitations in order to generate and interpret these data reliably and accurately.
5 Conclusion
In this scoping review, we have attempted to determine the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in the study of lexical access and, for the first time, provide an updated overview of research in this specific field of study. The results demonstrate that pupillometry is a highly applicable and useful method for assessing various word recognition, retrieval, and semantic activation skills, that easily fits and complements traditional lexical access research paradigms and methods. In addition, the strong methodological rigor of its application gives it a good level of sensitivity to the results obtained. Considering that pupillometry is a non-invasive, objective procedure without conscious reactions, it could be a technique applicable to any population and age group, especially those with complex motor, behavioral, or cognitive control.
In turn, we think that word processing (or lexical access) may generate pupil dilation patterns specific (i.e., temporal dilation window, mean dilation diameter, or dilation peaks) for this function, which should be isolated and distinguished from other response components such as cognitive effort, surprise, attention or memory. However, the emerging development of this specific area of research and the methodological diversity observed in the included studies (which can be considered an advantage to some extent due to its versatility) do not yet allow for definitive and robust conclusions in this area, which in turn does not allow for meta-analyses or entirely conclusive statements about what the pupil response actually reflects when processing words. Defining and detailing standardized methods for recording and analyzing pupillary data in lexical access tasks would enable the creation of more accurate, reliable, and reproducible research designs and add more power and robustness to this line of research. Finally, lexical access research using pupillometric methods currently holds incalculable potential, so better knowledge and standardization of this procedure - and its integration with other technologies - would not only contribute to increasing knowledge about language processing in neurotypical populations across the lifespan but also could strengthen clinical research to understand linguistic-cognitive functions in various neuropsychiatric disorders.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Author contributions
CR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YV-R: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. GL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft. MC-M: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YS: Conceptualization, Investigation, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JC-A: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the National Research and Development Agency (ANID) Fondecyt Iniciación 11230984 (CR). This project was also funded by the CommuniCog Research Group (GI2309435) of the Universidad del Bío-Bío (CR), and the Communication, Societies & Cultures Research Group (GI2309834) of the Universidad del Bío-Bío (JC-A).
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to acknowledge the help and contribution of the Vice-rectorate Office for research and postgraduate studies University del Bío-Bío.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
References
Abrams, L., and Davis, D. (2016). “The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon” in Cognition, language and aging. ed. W. Harris (Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 13–54.
Abrams, L., and Farrell, M. T. (2011). “Language processing in normal aging” in The handbook of psycholinguistic and cognitive processes: Perspectives in communication disorders. eds. J. Guendouzi, F. Loncke, and M. J. Williams (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 49–73.
Anderson, C. J., and Colombo, J. (2009). Larger tonic pupil size in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Psychobiol. 51, 207–211. doi: 10.1002/dev.20352
Angulo-Chavira, A. Q., and Arias-Trejo, N. (2021). Mediated semantic priming interference in toddlers as seen through pupil dynamics. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 208:105146. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105146
Ariel, R., and Castel, A. D. (2014). Eyes wide open: enhanced pupil dilation when selectively studying important information. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 337–344. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3744-5
Balota, D., and Chumbley, J. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 340–357. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.340
Barbeiro, L. F., João, C., and Santos, S. (2011). The pathways to higher education: lexical availability in adult candidate students and in young first-year students. Int. J. of Lifelong. Educ. 30, 309–329. doi: 10.1080/02601370.2011.570870
Brisson, J., Mainville, M., Mailloux, D., Beaulieu, C., Serres, J., and Sirois, S. (2013). Pupil diameter measurement errors as a function of gaze direction in corneal reflection eyetrackers. Behav. Res. Methods 45, 1322–1331. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0327-0
Carbajal, I., Ryals, A. J., Witherby, A. E., and Tauber, S. K. (2018). Exploring the mechanisms underlying judgments of learning reactivity on actual learning using eye-tracking measures [poster presentation]. In the annual meeting of the Psychonomic society, New Orleans, LA
Chapman, L. R., and Hallowell, B. (2015). A novel pupillometric method for indexing word difficulty in individuals with and without aphasia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 58, 1508–1520. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0287
Cuetos, F., González, J., and Vega, M. (2015). Psicología del lenguaje. Madrid: Médica Panamericana.
Dale, P. S., and Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behav. Res. Meth. Instrum. Comput. 28, 125–127. doi: 10.3758/BF03203646
Davis, B. (2020). “Language, aging, and dementia” in The Cambridge introduction to applied linguistics. eds. S. Conrad, A. Harting, and L. Santelmann (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press)
De Winter, J. C. F., Petermeijer, S. M., Kooijman, L., and Dodou, D. (2021). Replicating five pupillometry studies of Eckhard Hess. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 165, 145–205. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.03.003
Dromi, E. (1999). “Early lexical development” in The development of language. ed. M. Barrett (Hove: Psychology Press)
Eckstein, M. K., Guerra-Carrillo, B., Miller, A. T., and Bunge, S. A. (2017). Beyond eye gaze: what else can eyetracking reveal about cognition and cognitive development? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 69–91. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.001
Egan, C., Cristino, F., Payne, J. S., Thierry, G., and Jones, M. W. (2020). How alliteration enhances conceptual–attentional interactions in reading. Cortex 124, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.005
Egan, C., Payne, J. S., and Jones, M. W. (2023). The impact of phonological relatedness on semantic congruency judgements in readers with dyslexia: evidence from behavioural judgements, event related potentials and pupillometry. Neuropsychologia 184:108548. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108548
El Haj, M., Kapogiannis, D., and Boutoleau-Bretonnière, C. (2024). Pupil size shows diminished increases on verbal fluency tasks in patients with behavioral-variant-frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurolinguist. 69:101164. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2023.101164
El Haj, M., Lenoble, Q., and Allain, P. (2020). Pupil dilation as an index of verbal fluency. Am. J. Psychol. 133, 501–507. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.133.4.0501
El Haj, M., Moustafa, A. A., Allain, P., and Chapelet, G. (2022). The talking eyes: pupillometry to index verbal fluency in normal aging. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 223:107525. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2022.107525
Geller, J., Landrigan, J. F., and Mirman, D. (2019). A pupillometric examination of cognitive control in taxonomic and thematic semantic memory. J. Cogn. 2:6. doi: 10.5334/joc.56
Goldinger, S. D., and Papesh, M. H. (2012). Pupil dilation reflects the creation and retrieval of memories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 90–95. doi: 10.1177/0963721412436811
Gomes, C. A., Montaldi, D., and Mayes, A. (2015). The pupil as an indicator of unconscious memory: introducing the pupil priming effect. Psychophysiology 52, 754–769. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12412
Goral, M., Spiro, A., Albert, M., Obler, L., and Connor, L. (2007). Change in lexical retrieval skills in adulthood. Ment. Lex. 2, 215–238. doi: 10.1075/ml.2.2.05gor
Granholm, E., Chock, D., and Morris, S. (1998). Pupillary responses evoked during verbal fluency tasks indicate semantic network dysfunction in schizophrenia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 20, 856–872. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.6.856.1107
Guasch, M., Ferre, P., and Haro, J. (2017). Pupil dilation is sensitive to the cognate status of words: further evidence for non-selectivity in bilingual lexical access. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 20, 49–54. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000651
Haro, J., Guasch, M., Vallès, B., and Ferré, P. (2017). Is pupillary response a reliable index of word recognition? Evidence from a delayed lexical decision task. Behav. Res. Methods 49, 1930–1938. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0835-9
Haro, J., López-Cortés, N., and Ferré, P. (2023). Pupillometric and behavioural evidence shows no differences between polyseme and homonym processing. Acta Psychol. 238:103985. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2023.103985
Henderson, S. K., Peterson, K. A., Patterson, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2023). Verbal fluency tests assess global cognitive status but have limited diagnostic differentiation: evidence from a large-scale examination of six neurodegenerative diseases. Brain Commun. 5:fcad042. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcad042
Hepach, R. (2023). “Pupillometry in developmental psychology. pupillometry: cognition, neuroscience, and practical applications” in Modern Pupillometry: Cognition, neuroscience, and practical applications. eds. M. Papesh and S. Goldinger (USA: Springer)
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., Müller, K., and Tomasello, M. (2019). Toddlers’ intrinsic motivation to return help to their benefactor. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 188:104658. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.011
Hyönä, J., Tommola, J., and Alaja, A. M. (1995). Pupil dilation as a measure of processing load in simultaneous interpretation and other language tasks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 48, 598–612. doi: 10.1080/14640749508401407
Igoa, J. (2009). El procesamiento del léxico, In Panorama de la Lexicología, Ed E. MiguelDe Barcelona: Ariel.
James, L., and Goring, S. (2018). Language and cognitive aging. Oxford: Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology
Joshi, S. (2021). Pupillometry: arousal state or state of mind? Curr. Biol. 31, R32–R34. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.11.001
Joshi, S., and Gold, J. (2020). Pupil size as a window on neural substrates of cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 466–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.03.005
Kafkas, A., and Montaldi, D. (2015). The pupillary response discriminates between subjective and objective familiarity and novelty. Psychophysiology 52, 1305–1316. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12471
Kret, M. E., and Sjak-Shie, E. E. (2019). Preprocessing pupil size data: guidelines and code. Behav. Res. Methods 51, 1336–1342. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1075-y
Kuchinke, L., Võ, M. L., Hofmann, M., and Jacobs, A. M. (2007). Pupillary responses during lexical decisions vary with word frequency but not emotional valence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 65, 132–140. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.004
Kuchinsky, S. E., Ahlstrom, J. B., Vaden, K., Cute, S. L., Humes, L. E., Dubno, J. R., et al. (2013). Pupil size varies with word listening and response selection difficulty in older adults with hearing loss. Psychophysiology 50, 23–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01477.x
Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron 67, 713–727. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.038
Laeng, B., Sirois, S., and Gredebäck, G. (2012). Pupillometry: a window to the preconscious? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 18–27. doi: 10.1177/1745691611427305
Larsen, R. S., and Waters, J. (2018). Neuromodulatory correlates of pupil dilation. Front. Neural. Circuits. 12, 12–21. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00021
Levelt, W.J.. (1989). Speaking: from intention to articulation. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) Press.
Liu, Y., Rodenkirch, C., Moskowitz, N., Schriver, B., and Wang, Q. (2017). Dynamic lateralization of pupil dilation evoked by locus coeruleus activation results from sympathetic, not parasympathetic, contributions. Cell Rep. 20, 3099–3112. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.08.094
Luce, P. A., and Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: the neighborhood activation model. Ear Hear. 19, 1–36. doi: 10.1097/00003446-199802000-00001
Ludewig, U., Hübner, N., and Schroeder, S. (2022). Vocabulary, text coverage, word frequency and the lexical threshold in elementary school reading comprehension. Read. Writ. 36, 2409–2431. doi: 10.1007/s11145-022-10385-0
Marini, A., and Andreetta, S. (2016). “Age-related effects on language production: a combined psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic perspective” in Cognition, language and aging. ed. W. Harris (Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 55–79.
Mathôt, S., Grainger, J., and Strijkers, K. (2017). Pupillary responses to words that convey a sense of brightness or darkness. Psychol. Sci. 28, 1116–1124. doi: 10.1177/0956797617702699
Mathôt, S., and Vilotijević, A. (2023). Methods in cognitive pupillometry: design, preprocessing, and statistical analysis. Behav. Res. Methods 55, 3055–3077. doi: 10.3758/s13428-022-01957-7
Mckee, C. L., Tirado, P., Litvin, J. M., Carbajal, I., and Ryals, A. J. (2018). Evidence for individual differences in pupillary dilation to aversive stimuli in the general population [Poster presentation at the annual meeting of the Society for Cognitive Neuroscience]. Boston, MA.
McLaughlin, D. J., Zink, M. E., Gaunt, L., Spehar, B., Van Engen, K. J., Sommers, M. S., et al. (2022). Pupillometry reveals cognitive demands of lexical competition during spoken word recognition in young and older adults. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 29, 268–280. doi: 10.3758/s13423-021-01991-0
Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 4, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1
Munn, Z., Peters, M. D., Stern, C., Tufanaru, C., McArthur, A., and Aromataris, E. (2018). Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 18, 1–7. doi: 10.1186/s12874-018-0611-x
Ouzzani, M., Hammady, H., Fedorowicz, Z., and Elmagarmid, A. (2016). Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 5:210. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4
Partala, T., and Surakka, V. (2003). Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud. 59, 185–198. doi: 10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00017-X
Pätzold, W., and Liszkowski, U. (2019). Pupillometry reveals communication-induced object expectations in 12-but not 8-month-old infants. Dev. Sci. 22:e12832. doi: 10.1111/desc.12832
Peelle, J.. (2019). Language and aging, In The Oxford Handbook of Neurolinguistic, Eds.
Perea, M., and Rosa, E. (1999). Psychology of reading and visual lexical processing: a review of experimental techniques and analysis procedures. Psicol. J. 20, 65–90.
Pluchino, P., Gamberini, L., Barral, O., and Minelle, F. (2014). “How semantic processing of words evokes changes in pupil” in Symbiotic interaction: third international workshop, symbiotic 2014, Helsinki, Finland, October 30–31, 2014. eds. G. Jacucci, L. Gamberini, J. Freeman, and A. Spagnolli, vol. 8820 (Helsinki: Springer Lecture Notes in Computer Science), 99–112.
Reilly, J., Zuckerman, B., Kelly, A., Flurie, M., and Rao, S. (2020). Neuromodulation of cursing in American English: A combined tDCS and pupillometry study. Brain Lang. 206:104791. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2020.104791
Richter, T., Isberner, M. B., Naumann, J., and Neeb, Y. (2013). Lexical quality and reading comprehension in primary school children. Sci. Stud. Read. 17, 415–434. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2013.764879
Rojas, C., Riffo, B., and San Martín, M. (2022). Language skills in an aging brain: review of empirical studies in cognitively healthy older adults of third and fourth age. Estudios Filosóficos. 69, 213–233. doi: 10.4067/s0071-17132022000100213
Rojas, C., Riffo, B., San Martín, M., and Guerra, E. (2023). Lexical access restrictions after the age of 80. Brain Sci. 13:1343. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13091343
Ryals, A. J., Kelly, M. E., and Cleary, A. M. (2021). Increased pupil dilation during tip-of-the-tongue states. Conscious. Cogn. 92:103152. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103152
Samuels, E. R., and Szabadi, E. (2008). Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part I: principles of functional organisation. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 6, 235–253. doi: 10.2174/157015908785777229
Sara, S. J. (2009). The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 211–223. doi: 10.1038/nrn2573
Schmidtke, J. (2018). Pupillometry in linguistic research: an introduction and review for second language researchers. Stud. Second. Lang. Acquis. 40, 529–549. doi: 10.1017/S0272263117000195
Shafto, M. A., Stamatakis, E. A., Tam, P. P., and Tyler, L. K. (2010). Word retrieval failures in old age: the relationship between structure and function. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1530–1540. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21321
Shechter, A., Hershman, R., and Share, D. L. (2022). A pupillometric study of developmental and individual differences in cognitive effort in visual word recognition. Sci. Rep. 12:10764. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-14536-9
Shechter, A., and Share, D. L. (2021). Keeping an eye on effort: a pupillometric investigation of effort and effortlessness in visual word recognition. Psychol. Sci. 32, 80–95. doi: 10.1177/0956797620958638
Steinhauer, S. R., Bradley, M. M., Siegle, G. J., Roecklein, K. A., and Dix, A. (2022). Publication guidelines and recommendations for pupillary measurement in psychophysiological studies. Psychophysiology 59:e14035. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14035
Steinhauer, S. R., Siegle, G. J., Condray, R., and Pless, M. (2004). Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 52, 77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.005
Strauch, C., Wang, C. A., Einhäuser, W., Van der Stigchel, S., and Naber, M. (2022). Pupillometry as an integrated readout of distinct attentional networks. Trends Neurosci. 45, 635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.05.003
Toivo, W., and Scheepers, C. (2019). Pupillary responses to affective words in bilinguals’ first versus second language. PLoS One 14:e021045:e0210450. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210450
Tricco, A. C., Lillie, E., Zarin, W., O'Brien, K. K., Colquhoun, H., Levac, D., et al. (2018). PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann. Intern. Med. 169, 467–473. doi: 10.7326/M18-0850
Keywords: pupillometry, lexical access, word recognition, word retrieval, semantic processing
Citation: Rojas C, Vega-Rodríguez YE, Lagos G, Cabrera-Miguieles MG, Sandoval Y and Crisosto-Alarcón J (2024) Applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in the study of lexical access. A scoping review of primary research. Front. Psychol. 15:1372912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1372912
Edited by:
Andriy Myachykov, Northumbria University, United KingdomReviewed by:
Natalia López-Cortés, University of Zaragoza, SpainJack Silcox, The University of Utah, United States
Copyright © 2024 Rojas, Vega-Rodríguez, Lagos, Cabrera-Miguieles, Sandoval and Crisosto-Alarcón. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jaime Crisosto-Alarcón, amNyaXNvc3RvQHViaW9iaW8uY2w=