Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 April 2024
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic New Trends in Typical and Atypical Language Acquisition View all 17 articles

Exploring Spanish writing abilities of children with developmental language disorder in expository texts

  • 1NeuroDevelop eHealth Lab, eHealth Center, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya (UOC), Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Estudis de Psicologia i Ciències de l’Educació, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya (UOC), Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Departament de Cognició Desenvolupament i Psicologia de l’Educació, Secció Cognició, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
  • 4Institut de Neurociències, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain

Introduction: Numerous studies have shown that children with developmental language disorder (DLD), in addition to oral language difficulties, exhibit impaired writing abilities. Their texts contain problems in grammar, organization, cohesion, and length of written output. However, most of these studies have been conducted with English speakers. English is characterized by complex phonological structure, opaque orthography, poor morphology and strict word order. The aim of this research is to observe the writing abilities of children with DLD in a language with simple phonological structure, transparent orthography, rich morphology and flexible word order like Spanish in the production of expository texts.

Methods: Twenty-six children with DLD (mean age in months = 128.85) and 26 age-and sex-matched typically developing (TD) children (mean age in months = 124.61) wrote an expository text about their favorite animal.

Results: In order to analyze how the two groups plan and encode written texts, we looked at word frequency and sentence structure, grammatical complexity and lexical density, and omissions and errors. Compared to the TD group, the children with DLD omitted more content words; made more errors with functional words, verb conjugation and inflectional morphemes, and made a large number of spelling errors. Moreover, they wrote fewer words, fewer sentences, and less structurally and lexically complex texts.

Discussion: These results show that children with DLD who speak a transparent orthography language such as Spanish also have difficulties in most language areas when producing written texts. Our findings should be considered when planning and designing interventions.

1 Introduction

Developmental language disorder (DLD)—also known as specific language impairment (SLI)—is a neurodevelopmental disorder affecting around 7.5% of the child population (Tomblin et al., 1997; Norbury et al., 2016) with no significant difference in sex distribution (Calder et al., 2022). DLD is defined as a severe and persistent disorder in oral language acquisition and development, unassociated with a medical condition, such as hearing loss, intellectual disability, autism, or any neurological disorder or genetic syndromes (Bishop et al., 2016). Moreover, DLD may co-exist with other neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention-deficit, hyperactivity, motor, speech and behavioral problems, or dyslexia (Bishop et al., 2016).

Studies of children with DLD have found that they exhibit an heterogeneous oral language profile (Conti-Ramsden, 2008) which may, to differing degrees, involve one or several expressive and receptive language components and which affects social and/or school development (e.g., Leonard, 1998; Bishop et al., 2017). Previous studies have documented significant difficulties across the different components of oral language including phonology, morphology, syntax, vocabulary, semantics, pragmatics, discourse, and verbal learning and memory (Bishop et al., 2017). Research have found some phonological issues in productive phonology such as omissions of unstressed syllables and final consonants and changing syllabic structures (e.g., omitting final consonants and reducing consonant and vowel clusters; Aguilar-Mediavilla et al., 2002, 2007; Bishop and Clarkson, 2003; Gallon et al., 2007; Broc et al., 2013; Larkin et al., 2013). Additionally, children with DLD struggle with other phonological abilities, such as in phonological awareness abilities including complex tasks like deleting phonemes, substituting phonemes, or producing rhyming words (Thatcher, 2010; Vukovic et al., 2022; Korlaet et al., 2023). A considerable body of research has focused on studying morphological problems in children with DLD. They have deficits in the use of inflectional morphology, such as verb tense and agreement. Specifically, they omit the ending-s in the third singular person (e.g., ‘She read a book’ instead of ‘She reads a book’) and the past tense marker-ed (e.g., ‘Yesterday, I play with Rachel’ instead of ‘Yesterday, I played with Rachel; Van der Lely and Ullman, 2001; Abel et al., 2015). Research has shown that these children struggle also with functional words since they omit articles, pronouns and prepositions (Bedore and Leonard, 2001; Restrepo and Gutierrez-Clellen, 2001; Sanz-Torrent et al., 2007; Coloma et al., 2016). Children with DLD produce syntactically simpler sentences (Marinellie, 2004) and find it difficult to understand both complex syntactic structures, such as dependent clauses and passive sentences (e.g., Bishop, 1997; Leonard and Deevy, 2006; Novogrodsky and Friedmann, 2006; Montgomery and Evans, 2009; Van der Lely et al., 2011; Leonard, 2014). Different studies have analyzed vocabulary and semantics in children with DLD and have observed that they typically present smaller and less rich lexicons than their typical peers (McGregor et al., 2023) and show slower latency times and more errors in picture naming (Lahey and Edwards, 1996; Lahey and Edwards, 1999; McGregor et al., 2002). Moreover, in receptive single-word vocabulary tests, they tend to score within the average range but statistically lower than their matched TD peers (Gray et al., 1999; McGregor et al., 2002, 2013; Sheng and McGregor, 2010; Haebig et al., 2015). Furthermore, they show semantic impairments encompass problems with expressing or understanding meaning from word combinations (Katsos et al., 2011). In pragmatics, children with DLD have difficulties understanding figurative language such as metaphors, double meanings or idiomatic expressions (Norbury, 2005) and understanding communicative intentions (Andrés-Roqueta and Katsos, 2020).

Having these problems in oral language, children with DLD are very likely to also experience difficulties in literacy. Different studies have reported that they are at greater risk for reading difficulties than children with typical language development (TD) (Sanz-Torrent et al., 2010; Bishop et al., 2017; Adolf and Hogan, 2018). It has been estimated that around 50% of children with DLD present also dyslexia (Bishop et al., 2009; Ramus et al., 2013) and they may exhibit reading comprehension difficulties (Ramus et al., 2013; Gough Kenyon et al., 2018).

Regarding writing, according to Hayes and Flower (1980) model, the writing process can be divided into three stages: planning, translating, and revising. The planning stage involves generating ideas, organizing thoughts and ideas, and setting writing goals. The translation stage is where these ideas are transformed from oral language into written form, and the revising stage involves reading and editing the written material. Additionally, the Not-So-Simple View of Writing (Berninger et al., 2002; Berninger and Amtmann, 2003) which is a modification of the Hayes and Flower model that incorporates various cognitive and executive function components, including working memory, into the writing process. Thus, children with DLD are expected to face difficulties in producing written content due to their challenges both in oral language and executive functioning, particularly in working memory (Im-Bolter et al., 2006; Archibald and Joanisse, 2009; Montgomery et al., 2010; Ebert and Kohnert, 2011). Accordingly, different studies have found problems in written production in children with DLD (i.e., Broc et al., 2021; Tucci and Choi, 2023). Written productions can be evaluated accordingly through analyses of microstructure and macrostructure (Liles et al., 1995). According to Hughes et al. (1997), microstructure refers to the syntactic and lexical levels of the production, that is, the language form and content. It has been characterized in terms of productivity as well as complexity. Conversely, macrostructure denotes the hierarchical structure and coherence of the text beyond the level of a single sentence. The way the story’s episodes are arranged, how events are sequenced, and how the protagonists’ internal states drive or respond to the story’s events are all examples of macrostructure (e.g., McCabe and Peterson, 1984; McCabe and Rollins, 1994; Liles et al., 1995).

As far as we know, previous studies about writing in children with DLD have focused on analyzing the microstructure. Tucci and Choi (2023) performed a scoping review of literature focused on the effects of DLD on writing skills across the lifespan. Results showed that spelling may be the most vulnerable area for individuals with DLD. In this sense, previously studies show that children with DLD make a significantly higher percentage of spelling errors when producing written texts compared to TD children of the same age (Mackie et al., 2013; Williams et al., 2013; Dockrell et al., 2014; Reilly et al., 2014; Joye et al., 2019). For the most part, these spelling errors are due to phonological and morphological errors, which involve substituting, inserting or eliminating letters within a word (Mackie and Dockrell, 2004). Broc et al. (2021) conducted a scoping review about the nature of spelling errors in children with DLD across different orthographies. They divided the 18 reviewed studies into those based on dictation tasks and those containing written narratives because these two types of tasks involve different writing processes. In addition, they separately analyzed those two types of studies regarding the typology of the orthography of language in which they were carried out (opaque or transparent orthographic system). On dictation tasks, children with DLD produced more phonologically unacceptable spelling errors. These errors varied by age and by the nature of the words dictated. Moreover, children with DLD produced less phonologically unacceptable spelling errors when the spelling could be derived by applying one-to-one sound-letter correspondences (transparent orthographic system) than when the phoneme-grapheme correspondences were irregular (opaque orthographic system). On written narratives, they found that most of the studies to assess the spelling skills of children with DLD had been conducted in opaque orthographies and only identified one study conducted in a transparent orthography language, Spanish (Soriano-Ferrer and Contreras-González, 2012). Overall, on written narrative tasks, results were contradictory about phonologically unacceptable spelling errors. Some studies found more difficulties than the control groups (Mackie and Dockrell, 2004) but others did not replicate those results (Broc et al., 2013; Dockrell and Connelly, 2015). In the only study conducted in a transparent orthography (Soriano-Ferrer and Contreras-González, 2012) children with DLD produced more spelling errors that were phonologically unacceptable compared to their peers of the same age. However, both groups made four times as many errors when the phoneme correspondence was irregular, as opposed to when it was regular, resulting in observable errors. Finally, on written narratives, only studies conducted in opaque orthographies were reported to examine errors in inflectional morphological spelling. Children with DLD overall showed problems in their ability to accurately use inflections in their spelling. Error patterns in children with DLD were similar to younger language matched peers but more frequent than their age-matched peers.

Moreover, children and adolescents with DLD also may have difficulties with grammar, organization, cohesion, and length of written output (Tucci and Choi, 2023). They make more errors and omissions when writing nominal inflectional morphemes and using derivational morphemes (prefixes and suffixes) than age-matched children (Connelly et al., 2011; Mackie et al., 2013). For example, they produce more errors in the use of plural forms and past simple verb tenses (Windsor et al., 2000; Mackie and Dockrell, 2004; Larkin et al., 2013). Additionally, they use fewer words that contain prefixes, such as im/patient or dis/agree and suffixes, such as teach/er or profession/al (Mackie et al., 2013). Interestingly, these differences are still significant when children with DLD are matched with children possessing the same language skills. The percentage of omitted auxiliary verb be and content words—such as nouns as subjects—is also significantly higher than in TD children of the same age with a similar receptive vocabulary (Windsor et al., 2000; Mackie and Dockrell, 2004). Moreover, they display poorer lexical diversity (Scott and Windsor, 2000; Mackie et al., 2013; Williams et al., 2013; Levlin and Waldmann, 2020; Stuart et al., 2020). The differences are once again significant when children with DLD are compared to a younger cohort with the same language skills. Research has also shown differences in writing abilities to be significant when children with DLD are compared to younger children of a similar reading age (Mackie et al., 2013). In addition, written texts produced by children with DLD are also shorter (i.e., contain fewer written words; Mackie et al., 2013; Dockrell et al., 2014; Stuart et al., 2020; Ralli et al., 2021) and contain a lower percentage of meaningful syntactic units (T-units) and coordinated sentences than texts produced by TD children of the same age. The sentences in their texts are also significantly shorter and contain fewer words per clause (Scott and Windsor, 2000; Mackie et al., 2013).

Most of these studies about the characteristics of the written language in DLD have been conducted with English speakers, a language with an opaque orthographic system. However, English has a number of characteristics that make it very different from transparent orthographic system languages such as Spanish. First, Spanish has a simple phonological structure. It has approximately 23 phonemes and the majority of syllables follow a simple consonant–vowel (CV) structure and have limited clusters and blends (Gorman and Gillam, 2003; Goikoetxea, 2005). Therefore, in Spanish, segmentation of syllables at the level of onset–rime is often equivalent to segmentation at the phonemic level. For example, a Spanish speaker who segments the syllables of the word “casa” [house] into onset and rime (onset:“c,” rime: “a”) will also identify the phonemes that make up the word (/c/ /a/ /s/ /a/; Goswami, 2008). This is not the case for the English, where many onsets and rimes contain clusters of phonemes, as in “sport” and “cost” which must be segmented further (De Cara and Goswami, 2002). Spanish has a transparent orthography both in terms of reading and writing since practically every phoneme is represented by a single, unique letter. Thus, children need to learn fewer phoneme-to-letter conversions in Spanish than in English, where one phoneme can be represented by multiple spellings (as the phoneme /f/ in words like frog, tough, and photo). Moreover, Spanish is a morphologically rich language: it uses inflections to indicate the relationship between the elements; that is, the composition of the words changes (e.g., “casa” [house], “casas” [houses], “casita” [little house]). This implies important morphological changes in words that include a lexeme or radical morpheme, to which one or more grammatical morphemes can be added (e.g., cas-a, cas-as, cas-ita). Another feature of Spanish, as in other Romance languages, is that the order of the words within the sentence is flexible (e.g., “Juan ama a Maria” [“John loves Mary”]; “A Maria ama Juan” [“To Mary loves John”]; “Juan a Maria ama” [“John to Mary loves”]), although there is a basic order of words in the sentence (canonical order: subject > verb > object). In addition, Spanish is a pro-drop language that allows personal pronouns to be dropped in the sentence (“Juega al fútbol” [Plays football]).

Previous cross-linguistic research suggests that both the simple phonological structure of Spanish and its highly regular phoneme-to-letter correspondence facilitate the process of learning to read and write in Spanish children with TD (Müller and Brady, 2001; Seymour et al., 2003; Ellis et al., 2004; Ziegler et al., 2010; Florit and Cain, 2011; Castejón et al., 2015). In theory, this should also help Spanish-speaking children with DLD as they learn to write. However, to our knowledge only two studies have been conducted concerning written language production among the DLD population in Spanish (Soriano-Ferrer and Contreras-González, 2012; Buil-Legaz et al., 2023). Buil-Legaz et al. (2023) analyzed the difficulties of children with DLD in spelling. Participants were 18 children with DLD (aged 7;0–11;5, M = 8;4, SD = 1.25) and 18 children with DLD with TD (aged 7;0–11;6, M = 8;2, SD = 1.29) that completed a spelling-to-dictation task of words and pseudowords, where length was manipulated. They used digital tablets to collect data and obtain measures of accuracy, latencies and total writing durations. Results showed that children with DLD produced more errors, longer latencies and longer writing durations than age-matched children. Regarding accuracy, analysis of the errors showed that children in the control group produce few errors, most being substitutions, while children with DLD made more errors and of more varied types (substitutions, omissions and additions). Moreover, they were more affected by length on writing accuracy than the control group. Soriano-Ferrer and Contreras-González (2012) examined the written narrations and the influence of linguistic measures on narrative competence of children with DLD. Children did a written narrative task, where they had to recall, in writing, a story given to them previously orally twice. The story was composed of 19 propositions, with a simple grammatical structure. The children with DLD created short narratives, poorly organized and less cohesive. Also, their writings contained more syntax errors and had a higher proportion of phonologically inaccurate spelling errors in natural spelling but not in arbitrary spelling errors.

However, in these two previous studies, written performance was measured by single-word dictation (Buil-Legaz et al., 2023) and by a narrative based on a story given orally (Soriano-Ferrer and Contreras-González, 2012). As stated Broc et al. (2021), in dictated tasks the words to be written are predetermined while on written narratives participants can choose words they know, which may result in fewer spelling errors as they may opt for words they feel to spell. However, in the study of Soriano-Ferrer and Contreras-González (2012) children had to retell in writing a story given orally. Therefore, as far as we know, there are not any studies that analyze the production of expository texts in Spanish-speaking children with DLD. An expository text or informational text is a non-fiction text that gives facts and information about a topic. It aims to inform, explain, describe, or define a particular topic or subject. Its primary purpose is to present factual information, clarify ideas, and provide insights in a clear, concise, and organized manner. This kind of text is very common in subjects such as science, history and social sciences. There are several different types of expository structures. Meyer and Ray (2011) proposed six structures: compare-and-contrast, problem-and- solution, cause-and- effect, sequence, enumeration, and description. Each text structure type represents a distinct text organization and purpose. For example, description focuses on describing a topic, person, place or thing by listing a collection of its features or examples while. Expository texts serve as valuable tools for assessing writing abilities in children due to their inherent structure, which demands clear, organized, and coherent expression of ideas. Evaluating a child’s ability to comprehend, synthesize, and communicate information effectively can be achieved through their creation of expository texts (Fisher and Frey, 2017). In this way, the aim of this study was to examine and compare text writing of children with DLD in the production of expository texts in Spanish. More specifically, by looking at how these children and their aged-matched TD counterparts plan and encode expository texts, we sought to find out what variables are most frequently impaired in children with DLD compared to the TD group. We hypothesize that, given the errors that children with DLD tend to make in oral language, we expect to find significantly more difficulties in this population compared to TD children in all areas of writing. Specifically, we expect to find more inflectional morpheme and verb conjugation errors, as well as a higher percentage of omitted functional words.

2 Materials and methods

This study was approved by the Universitat Oberta de Catalunya’s (UOC) Ethics Committee. Furthermore, it was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards laid out in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and subsequent updates (WMA. World Medical Association, 2013).

2.1 Participants

This sample of children was a subsample of the study conducted by Ahufinger et al. (2021), which included 70 children (35 children with DLD and 35 typically developing (TD) children). The subset of children in this study included 52 participants (12 girls and 40 boys): 26 children had DLD (mean age in months = 128.85 (10.73 years); SD = 25.02; range: 95 to 188 months) and 26 were age-and sex-matched TD children (mean age in months = 124.61 (10.38 years); SD = 24.25; range: 90 to 184 months). All participants met the following inclusion criteria: (a) nonverbal intellectual quotient (NVIQ) > 75; (b) typical hearing at 500, 1000, 2000 and 4,000 Hz at 20 dB, in accordance with the American National Standard Institute (1997); (c) typical or corrected vision; (d) typical oral and speech motor abilities, as confirmed by a certified speech language pathologist; and (e) native Spanish-Catalan bilingual speakers as reported by parents. Children were excluded if parents reported (a) other biomedical conditions commonly linked to genetic or neurological causes, such as autism, intellectual disability, Down syndrome or Williams syndrome (Bishop et al., 2017); (b) frank neurological signs; or (c) seizure disorders or the use of medication to control seizures.

In 2017, children with DLD were identified with the help of the Catalan Center of Resources for Language-and Hearing-Impaired People (CREDA), members of the Catalan service for school counseling and guidance (EAP), and the Catalan Association of Specific Language Impairment (ATELCA), who work in conjunction with public and private schools throughout Catalonia to identify children with DLD or children with language difficulties. Children in the DLD group had a formal diagnosis of DLD or a suspected diagnosis and were in the process of being diagnosed, or were children whose families or teachers were concerned about language difficulties and/or were receiving speech–language services at the time of the original study per parental report. The TD children were recruited from public schools within the larger Barcelona metropolitan area. The TD children were at grade level in school, had no history, or diagnosis, of language learning disability, and had never received speech and language services.

To confirm each participant’s language status, standardized tests were administered by two trained researchers at the time of the study. These were the non-verbal IQ (NVIQ) Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT; Kaufman and Kaufman, 2004) and the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals - Fourth Edition, Spanish (CELF-4 Spanish; Semel et al., 2006). In the latter case, the researchers evaluated and recorded the participants’ Core Language Score, Expressive Language Score and/or Receptive Language Score. For the children with DLD, the CELF Core, Expressive or Receptive composite scores were at least 1.5 standard deviation below age-level expectations. For the children in the TD group, the CELF composite scores were all at or above age-level expectations. Each child with DLD was matched with a TD child of the same sex and age (+/− 3 months), as shown in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Age and standardized cognition and language measurement scores for the group of children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and the group of typical developing (TD) children.

Table 1 shows no significant age or NVIQ differences between the participants with DLD and their matched TD peers when the sample was selected. However, the children with DLD obtained significantly lower scores than the TD group on the three CELF language test scales.

In 2019, these children were invited to participate in the current study to examine and compare text writing abilities in the production of expository texts. All families were asked to sign a new consent form following the IRB protocol from the Universitat Oberta de Catalunya (UOC).

2.2 Instruments and procedure

Children completed two testing sessions of approximately 60 min each. These sessions were part of a larger study examining reading and writing skills. In the first session, children completed a reading assessment. In the second session, children completed two brief oral morphological tasks first, and the writing test for the present study after. The time allocated to carry out the writing task was approximately 15/20 min per child. The evaluation sessions were carried out individually in the research laboratories of the Universitat Oberta de Catalunya and the Universitat de Barcelona by two research assistants trained for this purpose. All participants were administered the narration writing task from the Spanish Batería de Evaluación de los Procesos de Escritura (Writing Process Evaluation Battery – PROESC; Cuetos et al., 2018). The children had to write an expository text about their favorite animal. If they were unable to think of an animal, they were given suggestions such as cats, dogs or lions. The children were given unlimited time to write a one-page text explaining whatever they wanted to about the animal. The instructions on how to complete the task were given in Spanish, but the examiner explained to the children that they could write the text in the language they preferred. All the participants wrote the text in Spanish.

2.3 Coding

The children’s texts were transcribed for analysis using the Codes for the Human Analysis of Transcripts (CHAT) program and were analyzed using the Child Language Data Exchange System (CHILDES) Project’s Computerized Language Analysis (CLAN) program (MacWhinney, 2000). The following category system was created to study the transcribed data, drawing on Mackie et al. (2013) and Salas and Caravolas (2019):

2.3.1 Word frequency and sentence structure

• Total number of words (TNW): Total number of words written in the text.

• Number of different words (NDW): This index was used to score the lexical diversity of the vocabulary in the text. To prevent an effect caused by the length of the text, we calculated Guiraud’s R index: types/√tokens (Guiraud, 1954).

• Main clauses ($MC): Total number of simple sentences the child has written as a proportion of the total number of sentences. A simple sentence is defined as a meaning unit that has a noun phrase, functioning as subject, and a verb phrase, functioning as predicate. Thus, it is a sentence expressing a single action, e.g., ‘El gato come verdura’ (‘The cat eats vegetables’).

• Coordinate clauses ($CC): Total number of coordinate sentences the child has written as a proportion of the total number of sentences. A coordinate sentence consists of two simple clauses, with equal syntactic importance, linked by a conjunction, e.g., ‘El perro ladra y el gato maúlla’ (‘The dog barks and the cat meows’).

• Subordinate clauses ($SC): Total number of subordinate sentences the child has written as a proportion of the total number of sentences. A subordinate sentence consists of a simple independent clause and at least one simple dependent clause. In subordinate sentences, dependent clauses do not make sense on their own; they need to be embedded in the independent clause to convey their meaning, e.g., ‘El perro, que es mi animal favorito, come comida de perro’ (‘Dogs, which are my favorite animal, eat dog food’).

• Total number of clauses (TNC): Total number of main, coordinate and subordinate clauses.

• Words per clause (WpC): This index is calculated by dividing the total number of words the child produces by the total number of clauses in the text.

2.3.2 Grammatical complexity and lexical density

• Number of adjectives ($NAj): Number of adjectives the child uses in the text.

• Number of adverbs ($NAv): Number of adverbs the child uses in the text.

• Number of connectors ($CO): Number of connectors the child uses in the text. Connectors include conjunctions, e.g., ‘y’, ‘o’ and ‘también’ (‘and’, ‘or’ and ‘also’), and discourse markers, e.g., ‘Primero de todo’ and ‘finalmente’ (‘first of all’ and ‘finally’). The purpose of linguistic connectors is to provide contextual meaning and clarity to the text by combining sentences and paragraphs.

2.3.3 Errors

• Spelling errors ($SE): Spelling errors in the children’s texts are defined using the categories established by Matute et al. (2010).

• Omission errors ($SEo): Omitting a letter, syllable or segment from the word, e.g., writing ‘hose’ instead of ‘horse’.

• Joining words ($SEw): Omitting the space between words, e.g., writing ‘elcaballo’ (‘thehorse’) instead of ‘el caballo’ (‘the horse’).

• Segmentation errors ($SEs): Dividing words incorrectly, e.g., writing ‘con migo’ (‘to gether’) instead of ‘conmigo’ (‘together’).

• Translocation errors ($SEt): Changing the letter or syllable order in a word, e.g., writing ‘fuetne’ (‘soucre’) instead of ‘fuente (‘source’).

• Addition errors ($SEa): Adding a letter or syllable to a word, e.g., writing ‘cominida’ (‘dininer’) instead of ‘comida’ (‘dinner’).

• Phoneme substitution errors ($SEp): Substituting an unvoiced sound for a voiced sound, e.g., ‘peso’ (‘weight’) instead of ‘beso’ (‘kiss’). An English example would be ‘pear’ instead of ‘bear’.

• Articulatory substitution errors ($SEas): Natural spelling errors caused by substituting a consonant for another that has a close production point, e.g., ‘cato’ instead of ‘gato’ (‘cat’), and/or a similar mode of articulation, e.g., ‘mida’ instead of ‘mira’ (‘look’). An English example would be ‘coal’ instead of ‘goal’ for the first case and ‘deal’ instead of ‘real’ for the second.

• Arbitrary spelling errors ($SEar): Spelling errors related to spelling rules. In Spanish, these manifest as substitution errors between the letters /v,b/, /c,s,z/, /g,j/, /y,ll/ and /h,∅/., e.g., ‘cantava’ instead of ‘cantaba’ (‘sang’). An English example would be ‘liv’ instead of ‘live’ or ‘werked’ instead of ‘worked’.

• Accent errors ($SEc): Errors such as ‘tenia’ instead of ‘tenía’ (had).

• Code-switching ($CSE): Words written in Catalan instead of Spanish, e.g., ‘gos’ (‘dog’ in Catalan) instead of ‘perro’ (‘dog’ in Spanish).

• Word omissions ($WOM): Omission of nouns, verbs, articles, prepositions or pronouns that are required to understand the context of the expository text (including argument omissions and subject elisions), e.g., writing “es alto” (‘is tall’) instead of ‘el caballo es alto’ (‘the horse is tall’).

• Functional words errors ($WE): Errors in the use of articles, prepositions or pronouns.

• Errors in nominal inflectional morphemes ($EIM): Changing or omitting a word’s gender inflection (feminine and masculine), e.g., writing ‘el niña’ (‘girl’ with masculine article ‘el’) instead of ‘la niña’ (‘girl’ with feminine article ‘la’); or changing or omitting the nominal number inflection (singular and plural), e.g., ‘los perro’ (singular ‘dog’ with plural article ‘los’) instead of ‘los perros’ (plural ‘dogs’ with plural article ‘los’).

• Verb conjugation errors ($VCE): Verbal inflection errors made when conjugating regular and irregular verbs (errors of number, person or mode). This category also includes errors in gerund and participle use.

• Semantic errors ($SEE): These occur when the child writes one word instead of another, i.e., the child tries to activate a given concept but activates another in the same semantic category (González et al., 2008), e.g., writing ‘gato’ (‘cat’) instead of ‘perro’ (‘dog’), or replacing a word with another semantically unrelated word, e.g., writing ‘yo he abierto la puerta con la bolsa’ (‘I unlocked the door with the bag’) instead of ‘con la llave’ (‘with the key’).

• Pragmatic errors ($PrE): This error is counted when the child uses literal expressions, writes oral expressions or uses a set phrase incorrectly, e.g., ‘El animal te muerde y estás acabado’ (‘The animal bites you and you are done’).

2.3.4 Other

• Language switch ($LS): An occasional use of Catalan to write the text. This category includes switching language for whole sentences, in which case the code is $LSS.

• Colloquialisms ($CW): Slang words, e.g., ‘guay’ (‘cool’) instead of “bueno” (‘good’), ‘mega’ instead of ‘muy’ (‘very’) or ‘chicha’ (colloquial way to refer to ‘meat’ in Spanish) instead of ‘carne’ (meat).

2.3.5 Reliability

Approximately 30% of the written texts (n = 16) were randomly selected from the sample to test the reliability using Cohen’s Kappa. Errors were coded by two independent reviewers. The reliability estimates for each writing measure are as follows: MC, 1; CC, 0.93; SC, 0.95; NAj, 0.89; NAv, 0.91; CO, 0.88; SE, 0.97; WOM, 0.92; WE, 0.70; EIM, 1; VCE, 0.82; SEE, 1; PrE, 1; LS, 1; LSS, 1; and CW, 1. If the two evaluators disagreed, they discussed the discrepancy until they reached an agreement. In the exceptional cases that no agreement was reached, the scores of the first author was used in the main analyses.

3 Results

3.1 Data analysis

Starting with the coding of the expository texts using the CHAT system and the subsequent analysis using CHILDES, we obtained the values of each category for each subject. To assess the differences between the groups, descriptive data for each variable were used, and a non-parametric analysis, specifically the Mann–Whitney U test, was conducted. This choice was made due to the sample size, as it does not follow a normal distribution (as determined by the Shapiro–Wilk test) and the heterogeneity of variances (as determined by Levene’s test). The data is available online in https://n9.cl/0er91.

3.2 Word frequency and sentence structure

Table 2 shows the mean, standard deviation and differences between the two groups with respect to word frequency, lexical diversity and sentence structure. The difference between the DLD and TD groups was significant for four out of the seven variables (total number of words, number of different words, total number of clauses and subordinate clauses). Children with DLD wrote significantly fewer words and sentences than TD children. Also, children with DLD wrote texts with less lexical diversity and used a significantly lower proportion of subordinate clauses compared to TD children.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation (SD) of the word frequency and sentence structure variables for the Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) and Typically Developing (TD) groups.

3.3 Grammatical complexity and lexical density

Table 3 shows the mean, standard deviation and differences between the two groups with regard to grammatical complexity and lexical density. Using the nonparametric Mann–Whitney U test, significant differences between the DLD and TD groups were identified in all three variables. Children with DLD used significantly fewer adjectives, adverbs and connectors compared to TD children of the same age.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean and standard deviation (SD) of the grammatical complexity and lexical density variables for the Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) and Typically Developing (TD) groups.

3.4 Errors

Table 4 shows the mean, standard deviation and differences between the two groups with regard to omissions and errors in their expository texts. Significant differences between the two groups were found in 5 out of the 7 variables. The DLD group made significantly more functional words, verb conjugation, nominal inflectional morpheme and spelling errors than the TD group. Children with DLD also omitted more words needed to understand the context of the text. A more detailed analysis was carried out for spelling errors. When the different categories of spelling errors were observed more closely, significant differences appeared between the two groups with respect to arbitrary spelling errors and articulatory spelling errors (z[2.309], p < 0.05 and z[3.105], p < 0.01, respectively). The children with DLD made significantly more arbitrary spelling errors (mean = 1.02, SD = 0.88) compared to the TD group (mean = 0.5, SD = 0.7). They also made significantly more articulatory spelling errors (mean = 0.81, SD = 1.34) compared to their TD peers (mean = 0.11, SD = 0.21; see Figure 1).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mean and standard deviation (SD) of the omissions and errors variables for the Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) and Typically Developing (TD) groups.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean number of arbitrary spelling errors (arbitrary sp. err.), accent mark spelling errors (accent sp. err.) and articulatory spelling errors (natural sp. err) in children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) and Typically Developing (TD) children.

With regard to switching languages, the results revealed significant differences between the two groups (z[2.194], p < 0.05). The ratio of language switch per word is significantly higher in children with DLD (mean = 0.013, SD = 0.026) compared to TD children (mean = 0.001, SD = 0.003). No significant differences were observed between children with DLD and TD children (z[1.42], p = 0.15) in terms of language switches applied to whole sentences. There were also no significant differences between the two groups in the use of colloquialisms (z[0.487], p = 0.648).

4 Discussion

In this study we explored the Spanish writing abilities of a group of children with DLD in comparison to a group of sex-and age-matched TD children using a written expository text. We were particularly interested in analyzing the writing abilities of this oral-language-impaired population in Spanish. This language is characterized by simple phonological structure, transparent orthography, rich morphology and flexible word order like Spanish.

Building on previous research on English-speaking children with DLD, in this study we analyzed word omissions, inflectional morpheme errors and verb conjugation errors. The analysis showed that the ratio of word omissions, errors in inflectional morphemes marking gender or number, and verb conjugation errors were significantly higher in the texts written by children with DLD compared to TD children of the same chronological age. These results are similar to other studies, such as Mackie et al. (2013), Connelly et al. (2011) and Mackie and Dockrell (2004), who also found that children with DLD had trouble using gender inflectional morphemes, as well as plural (−s), past (−ed) and gerund (−ing) markers, in their writing. Another morphological measurement not reported in previous writing research but key among Spanish-speaking children with DLD are errors using functional words such as articles, prepositions and pronouns that are very frequent in oral language (Bedore and Leonard, 2001; Restrepo and Gutierrez-Clellen, 2001; Sanz-Torrent et al., 2008; Morgan et al., 2013; Coloma et al., 2016). In our study, the children with DLD produced texts with significantly more functional words errors compared to the TD group. All these errors are similar to those made by children with DLD when they express themselves orally, for example difficulties with verb morphology (i.e., No me gusta [:gustan] las avejas [:abejas] / I do not like [number error in Spanish] sheep [: bees]) and in the use of functional words, such as articles (i.e., …la [:las] leonas… / …the [sing.]: the [pl.] lioness…), prepositions (i.e., …pueden oir [:oír] [:a] distansias [:distancias]…/..They can hear [:from] distance) and pronouns (i.e., Mi [:la] raza de mi perro…/ my [:the] breed of my dog.). This clearly shows that such difficulties in oral language production also affect children with DLD’s writing.

Spelling is one of the most impaired aspects of writing among English-speaking children with DLD when compared to TD children (Williams et al., 2013; Dockrell et al., 2014; Reilly et al., 2014). Our results showed that in Spanish, children with DLD also made significantly more spelling errors than their age-matched peers. A more specific analysis showed that they make significantly more articulatory and arbitrary spelling errors. These results are in line with previous research by Mackie and Dockrell (2004), who concluded that most spelling errors are due to letter substitution, insertion or elimination and letter combinations that do not comply with spelling rules. Although the research sample in Mackie and Dockrell (2004) were English-speaking children, their results are similar to ours. This means that children with DLD exhibit difficulties with phonological awareness whether they speak a language with shallow orthography or one with deep orthography.

Children with DLD performed worse in most of the other writing variables compared to their peers in the TD group. These results suggest that children with DLD had more difficulty writing longer texts, i.e., they wrote significantly fewer words and significantly fewer sentences than TD children. Texts by children with DLD were also structurally simpler, contained a significantly lower percentage of subordinate clauses and were not as lexically rich as those written by the TD group. These results track with previous studies done on English-speaking samples (Scott and Windsor, 2000; Puranik et al., 2006; Dockrell et al., 2014; Reilly et al., 2014), where children with DLD produced texts with significantly fewer words and a lower percentage of syntactic units. Our research found no significant differences between the two groups regarding mean sentence length. These results are consistent with Puranik et al. (2006), who found that, despite significant differences between children with DLD and children with TD in sentence production, mean sentence length does not vary significantly between the groups. However, these results contradict research by Mackie et al. (2013) and Scott and Windsor (2000), who found significant differences between the groups, with children with DLD producing fewer words per clause than their TD peers. Regarding clause types, no significant differences were found between the groups in terms of the percentage of simple and coordinate clauses used. There was, however, a difference when comparing the two groups for percentage of subordinate clauses. Proportionally, children with DLD used significantly fewer subordinate clauses than TD children, which indicates that their texts were simpler and less structurally complex. These results were also reported by Mackie and Dockrell (2004).

Along with structural characteristics, this study also evaluated the grammatical richness of the texts. The texts written by the children with DLD contained significantly fewer adjectives, adverbs and connectors (i.e., they were characterized by poor lexical density compared to the same-aged TD children). Another characteristic not analyzed in earlier research related to children with DLD’s writing abilities is the role of pragmatic errors and semantic errors in written texts. We explored these characteristics and did not find significant differences between the DLD and TD groups in these areas.

Finally, we looked for code-switching in the written texts. This measurement was included because the children in our sample were bilingual, speaking both Catalan and Spanish. The results show that the ratio of language switch per word is significantly higher in children with DLD than in TD children, indicating a lack of consistency in language used while writing and supporting the idea that bilingual children with DLD code-switch more than TD children (Pert et al., 2004). This could also be explained as a difficulty in thinking of a word and using the same word in another language as a compensation mechanism. However, as regards oral language, both Gutiérrez-Clellen et al. (2009) and Sanz-Torrent et al. (2007) found that Spanish-English and Spanish-Catalan bilingual children with DLD, respectively, did not differ from age-matched control children in terms of code-switching. Future research might look more closely at code-switching in written texts by bilingual children to analyze the differences between oral and written language.

In summary, writing abilities of children with DLD in Spanish showed more morphology-related, spelling and other writing errors compared to their age-matched TD peers. These results highlight the limitations that children with DLD may face in school when instruction is based on written language, and how these can affect their academic performance.

5 Limitations and future directions

This is the first study to explore the characteristics of expository text production in Spanish by children with DLD. However, there are a few areas for future improvement. Although we aimed to recruit as many participants as possible, the final sample consisted of 52 children (26 with DLD and 26 TD children). This sample size aligns with prior studies on English (e.g., Mackie et al., 2013; Dockrell et al., 2014; Andreou and Aslanoglou, 2022; Brimo et al., 2023) and Spanish (Soriano-Ferrer and Contreras-González, 2012; Buil-Legaz et al., 2023) writing abilities in children with DLD. However, our study included participants ranging in age from 7;11 to 15;8 years, representing a diverse range of ages. In order to further enhance the generalizability and reliability of our findings, it is recommended for future studies to expand the sample size and include a more specific age range. By increasing the number of participants and narrowing down the age range, researchers can obtain a more comprehensive understanding of the topic at hand.

Our study centered on the production of expository texts, a common school activity that requires children to plan, translate, and revise. Although we primarily focused on microstructure, the task also allows for macrostructure analysis (global structure and coherence), a crucial factor in gaging the quality of children’s written texts. This aspect could be examined in future studies. The expository text task is less controlled than other tasks like dictation, where evaluators can choose words with different spelling characteristics. When writing expository texts, children can use words they are familiar with, potentially resulting in fewer spelling errors as they may prefer words they can spell correctly. Additionally, even though we allowed unlimited time for children to write a one-page text, the length of their texts significantly varied. Future research should examine the microstructure abilities in children with DLD, and attempt to control the text length to yield a similar amount of information.

In conclusion, the findings from our study should be considered when planning and conducting activities with these children. We emphasize the value of using expository text writing in assessing children with DLD. It is a simple, quick method that yields substantial information about their language and writing skills. Additionally, it would be insightful to examine the effectiveness of interventions targeting oral language issues on improving writing impairments, and vice versa.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://n9.cl/0er91.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Universitat Oberta de Catalunya’s (UOC) Ethics Committee. Furthermore, it was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards laid out in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and subsequent updates (WMA. World Medical Association, 2013). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

RB-C: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. NA: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MS-T: Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LA: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación del Gobierno de España [2016EDU2016-75368-P and PID2020-114690RB-I00] and the Agència de Gestió d’Ajuts Universitaris i de Recerca (AGAUR) de la Generalitat de Catalunya [2021SGR01102].

Acknowledgments

We thank the Associació del Trastorn Específic del Llenguatge de Catalunya (ATELCA) and all the schools that participated in the study. Especially, we thank the participation of the Escola Santa Anna (Premià de Dalt), the Centres de Recursos per a Deficients Auditius de Catalunya (CREDA) and the Equips d’Assessorament Psicopedagògic de Catalunya (EAP). We would like to extend our gratitude to the professors, speech and language pathologists, and other experts who participated in the study. Finally, we thank Coral Mayo and Joan Tarrida for their assistance with the data collection.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abel, A. D., Rice, M. L., and Bontempo, D. E. (2015). Effects of verb familiarity on finiteness marking in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 58, 360–372. doi: 10.1044/2015_jslhr-l-14-0003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Adolf, S. M., and Hogan, T. P. (2018). Understanding dyslexia in the context of developmental language disorders. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 49, 762–773. doi: 10.1044/2018_LSHSS-DYSLC-18-0049

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguilar-Mediavilla, E. M., Sanz-Torrent, M., and Serra-Raventos, M. (2002). A comparative study of the phonology of pre-school children with specific language impairment (SLI), language delay (LD) and normal acquisition. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 16, 573–596. doi: 10.1080/02699200210148394

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., Sanz-Torrent, M., and Serra-Raventós, M. (2007). Influence of phonology on morpho-syntax in romance languages in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 42, 325–347. doi: 10.1080/13682820600881527

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahufinger, N., Berglund-Barraza, A., Cruz-Santos, A., Ferinu, L., Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., et al. (2021). Consistency of a nonword repetition task to discriminate children with and without developmental language disorder in Catalan–Spanish and European Portuguese speaking children. Child. Aust. 8:85. doi: 10.3390/children8020085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

American National Standard Institute (1997). American National Standard: Methods for calculation of the speech intelligibility index. New York, NY, US: Acoustical Society of America.

Google Scholar

Andreou, G., and Aslanoglou, V. (2022). Written language production in children with developmental language disorder. Front. Psychol. 13:833429. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.833429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrés-Roqueta, C., and Katsos, N. (2020). A distinction between linguistic and social pragmatics helps the precise characterization of pragmatic challenges in children with autism Spectrum disorders and developmental language disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 63, 1494–1508. doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00263

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Archibald, L. M., and Joanisse, M. F. (2009). On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52, 899–914. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0099)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedore, L. M., and Leonard, L. B. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 44, 905–924. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Graham, S., and Richards, T. (2002). Writing and reading: connections between language by hand and language by eye. J. Learn. Disabil. 35, 39–56. doi: 10.1177/002221940203500104

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., and Amtmann, D. (2003). “Preventing written expression disabilities through early and continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: research into practice” in Handbook of learning disabilities. eds. H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, and S. Graham (New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press), 345–363.

Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Comprehension in specific language impairment. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

BISHOP, D., and CLARKSON, B. (2003). Written language as a window into residual language deficits: a study of children with persistent and residual speech and language impairments. Cortex 39, 215–237. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70106-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V., McDonald, D., Bird, S., and Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. (2009). Children who read words accurately despite language impairment: who are they and how do they do it? Child Dev. 80, 593–605. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01281.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., and Greenhalgh, T.Catalise Consortium (2016). CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study. Identifying language impairments in children. PLoS One 11:e0158753. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., the CATALISE‐2 consortiumArchibald, L., et al. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: terminology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 1068–1080. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12721

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brimo, D., Nallamala, K., and Werfel, K. L. (2023). Writing errors of children with developmental language disorder. Top. Lang. Disord. 43, 302–316. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000326

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Broc, L., Bernicot, J., Olive, T., Favart, M., Reilly, J., Quémart, P., et al. (2013). Lexical spelling in children and adolescents with specific language impairment: variations with the writing situation. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 3253–3266. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Broc, L., Joye, N., Dockrell, J. E., and Olive, T. (2021). Capturing the nature of the spelling errors in developmental language disorder: a scoping review. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 52, 1127–1140. doi: 10.1044/2021_LSHSS-20-00086

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buil-Legaz, L., Suárez-Coalla, P., Santamarina-Rabanal, L., Martínez-García, C., and Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J. (2023). Spelling problems after early oral language difficulties. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 58, 756–764. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12819

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Calder, S. D., Brennan-Jones, C. G., Robinson, M., Whitehouse, A., and Hill, E. (2022). The prevalence of and potential risk factors for developmental language disorder at 10 years in the Raine study. J. Paediatr. Child Health 58, 2044–2050. doi: 10.1111/jpc.16149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castejón, L., González-Pumariega, S., and Cuetos, F. (2015) Development of word reading fluency along primary education: A six-year follow-up / El desarrollo de la fluidez en la lectura de palabras en educación primaria: Un seguimiento longitudinal de seis años. Infanc. Aprend. Available at: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02103702.2015.1076266

Google Scholar

Coloma, C. J., Araya, C., Quezada, C., Pavez, M. M., and Maggiolo, M. (2016). Grammaticality and complexity of sentences in monolingual Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 30, 649–662. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2016.1163420

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Connelly, V., Critten, S., Walter, K., and Dockerll, J. (2011). The relationship between spelling and writing development in children with language difficulties. In: The Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders (SRCLD), Society for Res. Children with Lang. Disord. Madison, Wisconsin doi: 10.13140/2.1.1074.5604

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conti-Ramsden, G. (2008). “Heterogeneity of specific language impairment in adolescent outcomes” in Understanding developmental language disorders: From theory to practice. eds. C. F. Norbury, J. B. Tomblin, and D. V. M. Bishop (London: Psychology Press), 115–129.

Google Scholar

Cuetos, F., Rodriguez Ramos, J.L, and Ruano, E. (2018). PROESC. Batería de Evaluación de los procesos escritores. TEA Ediciones. Spain.

Google Scholar

De Cara, B., and Goswami, U. (2002). Similarity relations among spoken words: the special status of rimes in English. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 34, 416–423. doi: 10.3758/BF03195470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dockrell, J. E., and Connelly, V. (2015). The role of oral language in underpinning the text generation difficulties in children with specific language impairment. J. Res. Read. 38, 18–34.

Google Scholar

Dockrell, J. E., Ricketts, J., Charman, T., and Lindsay, G. (2014). Exploring writing products in students with language impairments and autism spectrum disorders. Learn. Instr. 32, 81–90. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.01.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebert, K. D., and Kohnert, K. (2011). Sustained attention in children with primary language impairment: a meta-analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 54, 1372–1384. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0231)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, N. C., Natsume, M., Stavropoulou, K., Hoxhallari, L., Van Daal, V. H., Polyzoe, N., et al. (2004). The effects of orthographic depth on learning to read alphabetic, syllabic, and logographic scripts. Read. Res. Q. 39, 438–468. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.39.4.5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, D., and Frey, N. (2017). Teaching students to write informational texts. Read. Teach. 71, 239–242. doi: 10.1002/trtr.1606

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Florit, E., and Cain, K. (2011). The simple view of Reading: is it valid for different types of alphabetic orthographies? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23, 553–576. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9175-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallon, N., Harris, J., and van der Lely, H. (2007). Non-word repetition: an investigation of phonological complexity in children with grammatical SLI. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 21, 435–455. doi: 10.1080/02699200701299982

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goikoetxea, E. (2005). Levels of phonological awareness in preliterate and literate Spanish-speaking children. Read. Writ. 18, 51–79. doi: 10.1007/s11145-004-1955-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

González, M., Rodriguez, J., and Cuetos, F. (2008). Variabilidad en los errores semánticos producidos por pacientes con dano cerebral. Psicothema 20, 795–800.

Google Scholar

Gorman, B. K., and Gillam, R. B. (2003). Phonological awareness in Spanish: a tutorial for speech-language pathologists. Commun. Disord. Q. 25, 13–22. doi: 10.1177/15257401030250010301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U. (2008). The development of reading across languages. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1145, 1–12. doi: 10.1196/annals.1416.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gough Kenyon, S. M., Palikara, O., and Lucas, R. M. (2018). Explaining reading comprehension in children with developmental language disorder: the importance of elaborative inferencing. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 61, 2517–2531. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0416

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, S., Plante, E., Vance, R., and Henrichsen, M. (1999). The diagnostic accuracy of four vocabulary tests administered to preschool-age children. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 30, 196–206. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461.3002.196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guiraud, P. (1954) Les Charactères Statistiques du Vocabulaire. Essai de méthodologie. Spain. Presses Universitaires de France

Google Scholar

Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Simon-Cereijido, G., and Erickson Leone, A. (2009). Code-switching in bilingual children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Biling. 13, 91–109. doi: 10.1177/1367006909103530

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haebig, E., Kaushanskaya, M., and Ellis Weismer, S. (2015). Lexical processing in school-age children with autism Spectrum disorder and children with specific language impairment: the role of semantics. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 4109–4123. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2534-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, J. R., and Flower, L. (1980). “Identifying the Organization of Writing Processes” in Cognitive processes in writing: An interdisciplinary approach. eds. L. W. Gregg and E. R. Steinberg (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 3–30.

Google Scholar

Hughes, D., McGillvray, L., and Schmidek, M. (1997). Guide to narrative language: Procedures for assessments. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.

Google Scholar

Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., and Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: the role of executive function. Child Dev. 77, 1822–1841. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00976.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joye, N., Broc, L., Olive, T., and Dockrell, J. (2019). Spelling performance in children with developmental language disorder: a meta-analysis across European languages. Sci. Stud. Read. 23, 129–160. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2018.1491584

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katsos, N., Roqueta, C. A., Estevan, R. A. C., and Cummins, C. (2011). Are children with specific language impairment competent with the pragmatics and logic of quantification? Cognition 119, 43–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A. S., and Kaufman, N. L. (2004) Test breve de Inteligencia Kaufman (K-BIT). (Madrid: Pearson. Adaptación española: Agustín Cordero Pando y Isabel Calonge Romano).

Google Scholar

Korlaet, M., Vuković, M., and Tešić, J. (2023). Pre-skills of reading and writing in children with developmental language disorder and children with typical language development. Biomed Istraž 14, 64–76. doi: 10.59137/BII202301302K

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahey, M., and Edwards, J. (1996). Why do children with specific language impairment name pictures more slowly than their peers? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 39, 1081–1098. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3905.1081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahey, M., and Edwards, J. (1999). Naming errors of children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 42, 195–205. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4201.195

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Larkin, R. F., Williams, G. J., and Blaggan, S. (2013). Delay or deficit? Spelling processes in children with specific language impairment. J. Commun. Disord. 46, 401–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2013.07.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B. (1998). Language, speech, and communication. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B., and Deevy, P. (2006). “Cognitive and linguistic issues in the study of children with specific language impairment” in Handbook of psycholinguistics. eds. M. Traxler and M. Gernsbacher (New York, NY, US: Elsevier), 1143–1171.

Google Scholar

Levlin, M., and Waldmann, C. (2020). Written language in children with weak reading and spelling skills: the role of oral language, phonological processing, verbal working memory and reading. L1-Educ. Stud. Lang. Lit. 20, 1–25. doi: 10.17239/L1ESLL2020.20.01.02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liles, B. Z., Duffy, R. J., Merritt, D. D., and Purcell, S. L. (1995). Measurement of narrative discourse ability in children with language disorders. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 38, 415–425. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3802.415

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackie, C., and Dockrell, J. E. (2004). The nature of written language deficits in children with SLI. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 47, 1469–1483. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/109)

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackie, C. J., Dockrell, J., and Lindsay, G. (2013). An evaluation of the written texts of children with SLI: the contributions of oral language, reading and phonological short-term memory. Read. Writ. 26, 865–888. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9396-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacWhinney, B. (2000) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Marinellie, S. A. (2004). Complex syntax used by school-age children with specific language impairment (SLI) in child–adult conversation. J. Commun. Disord. 37, 517–533. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matute, E., Rosselli, M., and Ardila, A. (eds.). (2010). “Trastorno de la expresión escrita” in Neuropsicología del desarrollo infantil. (México: Manual moderno), 161–180.

Google Scholar

McCabe, A., and Peterson, C. (1984). What makes a good story. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 13, 457–480. doi: 10.1007/BF01068179

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, A., and Rollins, P. (1994). Assessment of preschool narrative skills. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 3, 45–56. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360.0301.45

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., and Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 45, 998–1014. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/081)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGregor, K. K., Ohlmann, N., Eden, N., Arbisi-Kelm, T., and Young, A. (2023). Abilities and disabilities among children with developmental language disorder. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 54, 927–951. doi: 10.1044/2023_LSHSS-22-00070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGregor, K. K., Oleson, J., Bahnsen, A., and Duff, D. (2013). Children with developmental language impairment have vocabulary deficits characterized by limited breadth and depth. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 48, 307–319. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, B. J., and Ray, M. N. (2011). Structure strategy interventions: increasing Reading comprehension of expository text. Int. Electron. J. Elementary Educ. 4, 127–152. Available at: https://www.iejee.com/index.php/IEJEE/article/view/217

Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. W., and Evans, J. L. (2009). Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52, 269–288. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0116)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., and Finney, M. C. (2010). Working memory and specific language impairment: an update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 19, 78–94. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/09-0028)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, G. P., Restrepo, M. A., and Auza, A. (2013). Comparison of Spanish morphology in monolingual and Spanish–English bilingual children with and without language impairment. Bilingualism 16, 578–596. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000697

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, K., and Brady, S. (2001). Correlates of early reading performance in a transparent orthography. Read. Writ. 14, 757–799. doi: 10.1023/A:1012217704834

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 90, 142–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.11.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Norbury, C. F., Gooch, D., Wray, C., Baird, G., Charman, T., Simonoff, E., et al. (2016). The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: evidence from a population study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 1247–1257. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12573

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Novogrodsky, R., and Friedmann, N. (2006). The production of relative clauses in syntactic SLI: a window to the nature of the impairment. Adv. Speech Lang. Pathol. 8, 364–375. doi: 10.1080/14417040600919496

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pert, S., Stow, C., and Letts, C. (2004). The identification of specific language impairment in bilingual children: how useful is the matrix language frame model? Retrieved July 31, 2008, from http://www.speechtherapy.co.uk/PDF/2004SPertSS15-170.pdf

Google Scholar

Puranik, C. S., Lombardino, L. J., and Altmann, L. J. (2006). Writing through retellings: an exploratory study of language-impaired and dyslexic populations. Read. Writ. 20, 251–272. doi: 10.1007/s11145-006-9030-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ralli, A. M., Chrysochoou, E., Giannitsa, A., and Angelaki, S. (2021). Written text production in Greek-speaking children with developmental language disorder and typically developing peers, in relation to their oral language, cognitive, visual-motor coordination, and handwriting skills. Read. Writ. 33, 1–29. doi: 10.1007/s11145-021-10196-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., and Van der Lely, H. K. J. (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: towards a multidimensional model. Brain 136, 630–645. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws356

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McKean, C., Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., et al. (2014). Specific language impairment: a convenient label for whom? Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 416–451. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Restrepo, M. A., and Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F. (2001). Article use in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. J. Child Lang. 28, 433–452. doi: 10.1017/S0305000901004706

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salas, N., and Caravolas, M. (2019). Dimensionality of early writing in English and Spanish. J. Lit. Res. 51, 272–292. doi: 10.1177/1086296X19858146

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanz-Torrent, M., Andreu, L., Badia, I., and Serra, M. (2010). El proceso lector en niños con antecedentes de retraso y trastorno específico del lenguaje. Rev. Logop. Foniatr. Audiol. 30, 23–33. doi: 10.1016/S0214-4603(10)70005-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanz-Torrent, M., Badia, I., and Serra, M. (2007). “Contributions from bilingual specific language impairment in Catalan and Spanish to the understanding of typical and pathological language acquisition” in A portrait of the Young in the new multilingual Spain. eds. C. Pérez-Vidal, M. Juan-Garau, and A. Bel (Clevedon: Multilingual matters), 135–158.

Google Scholar

Sanz-Torrent, M., Serrat, E., Andreu, L., and Serra, M. (2008). Verb morphology in Catalan and Spanish in children with specific language impairment: a developmental study. J. Multiling. Commun. Disord. 22, 459–474. doi: 10.1080/02699200801892959

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, C. M., and Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school-age children with language learning disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 324–339. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4302.324

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., and Secord, W. A. (2006) Spanish clinical evaluation of language Fundamentals-4 (CELF-4). San Antonio: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., and Erskine, J. M.Network, collaboration with C.A.A (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Brit. J. Psychol. 94, 143–174. doi: 10.1348/000712603321661859

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheng, L., and McGregor, K. K. (2010). Lexical-semantic organization in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 53, 146–159. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0160)

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soriano-Ferrer, M., and Contreras-González, M. C. (2012). Narraciones escritas en niños con Trastorno Específico del Lenguaje (TEL). Univ. Psychol. 11, 1341–1351. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy11-4.nent

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, N. J., Connelly, V., and Dockrell, J. E. (2020). Written verb use and diversity in children with developmental language disorder: stepping stones to academic writing. Read. Writ. 33, 67–96. doi: 10.1007/s11145-019-09978-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thatcher, K. L. (2010). The development of phonological awareness with specific language-impaired and typical children. Psychol. Sch. 47, 467–480. doi: 10.1002/pits.20483

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., and O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 40, 1245–1260. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucci, A., and Choi, E. (2023). Developmental language disorder and writing: a scoping review from childhood to adulthood. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 66, 2900–2920. doi: 10.1044/2023_JSLHR-22-00583

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Lely, H. K., Jones, M., and Marshall, C. R. (2011). Who did buzz see someone? Grammaticality judgement of wh-questions in typically developing children and children with grammatical-SLI. Lingua 121, 408–422. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Lely, H. K. J., and Ullman, M. T. (2001). Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Lang. Cogn. Process. 16, 177–217. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000076

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vukovic, M., Jovanovska, M., and Rajic, L. J. (2022). Phonological awareness in children with developmental language disorder. Arch. Public Health 14, 135–144. doi: 10.3889/aph.2022.6046

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, G. J., Larkin, R. F., and Blaggan, S. (2013). Written language skills in children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 48, 160–171. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Windsor, J., Scott, C. M., and Street, C. K. (2000). Verb and noun morphology in the spoken and written language of children with language learning disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43:1322. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

WMA. World Medical Association (2013). World medical association declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of Reading: a cross-language investigation. Psychol. Sci. 21, 551–559. doi: 10.1177/0956797610363406

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: developmental language disorder (DLD), specific language impairment (SLI), writing abilities, shallow language, expository text

Citation: Balboa-Castells R, Ahufinger N, Sanz-Torrent M and Andreu L (2024) Exploring Spanish writing abilities of children with developmental language disorder in expository texts. Front. Psychol. 15:1360245. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1360245

Received: 22 December 2023; Accepted: 20 March 2024;
Published: 11 April 2024.

Edited by:

Alejandra Auza B., Hospital General Dr. Manuel Gea Gonzalez, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Paz Suárez-Coalla, University of Oviedo, Spain
Ioannis Dimakos, University of Patras, Greece

Copyright © 2024 Balboa-Castells, Ahufinger, Sanz-Torrent and Andreu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Raquel Balboa-Castells, cmJhbGJvYUB1b2MuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.