Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 June 2024
Sec. Environmental Psychology

Nature-based experience in Venetian lagoon: Effects on craving and wellbeing in addict residential inpatients

\r\nGiulia Benvegn
Giulia Benvegnù1*Mauro SemenzatoMauro Semenzato2Alberto UrbaniAlberto Urbani2Isadora ZanlorenziIsadora Zanlorenzi2Mauro Cibin&#x;Mauro Cibin2Cristiano Chiamulera&#x;Cristiano Chiamulera1
  • 1Department of Diagnostics and Public Health, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
  • 2Centro Soranzo, Mestre, Italy

Introduction: It is known that exposure to the natural environment may positively modulate mental processes and behaviors; in particular, it can reduce stress, anxiety, and depressive symptoms. This suggests a potential integration of “nature experience” into the treatment for substance use disorder (SUD) since various types of addiction are associated with anxiety and depression. Considering that only one study has been reported to date in patients with alcohol use disorder, the effect of nature experience in SUD patients' needs to be further investigated. This study aimed to test the effects of exposure to a natural lagoon environment on craving and measures of wellbeing in SUD patients in comparison to exposure to an urban environment.

Methods: Twenty-four SUD patients were divided into three groups of eight participants and exposed to two walking sessions (interspersed with a 1-week wash-out period) in a natural environment typical of the Venetian lagoon, an Urban walk, or staying at the residential center based on a Latin-square design. Before and after each session, drug craving, mood, wellbeing, agency, openness to the future, and restorativeness were assessed.

Results: The Nature walk significantly decreased craving in participants compared to their pre-walk values, and compared to craving after the Urban walk, with the latter significantly increased vs. pre-walk values. The Nature walk significantly decreased negative mood and increased wellbeing and agency. Openness to the future and restorativeness measures showed significant improvement after the Nature walk compared to the Urban walk. On the other hand, craving scores after the Urban Walk positively correlated with negative mood and a Sense of Negative Agency values and negatively correlated with wellbeing scores.

Discussion: Our results confirm that “nature experience” may improve mood, wellbeing, attention, stress relief, openness, and sense of being active in SUD patients. Moreover, we also showed a specific effect on drug craving—a key symptom of SUD.

1 Introduction

It is widely known that the environmental context may play an important role in the modulation of mental processes and behaviors. The physical features of built and natural spaces have been shown to affect the brain mechanisms of mental health and disorders, thus potentially having a beneficial impact on behavior and health outcomes (Hartig and Kahn, 2016). Specifically, observational and experimental studies have demonstrated that contact with nature (“nature experience”) may induce acute, and sometimes persistent (Bratman et al., 2012, Supplementary material), effects on several physiological and psychological measures such as improvement of mood, attention, and reduced arousal (Hartig et al., 2011; Bratman et al., 2012; Kuo, 2015).

The biophilia hypothesis suggests that the human brain is evolutionarily wired to recognize and adapt to the natural (or nature related) environments. Nature-related features of the environment signal positive reinforcement and positive emotional effects. This phenotype owned an evident survival advantage in the past, and it is supposed to be still present in modern Homo sapiens (Wilson, 1984). This hypothesis is best supported by the vast evidence of mental benefits and improved wellbeing experienced after exposure to natural environments, plants, and nature views. Among the different theories supporting the effect of nature experience, the “Attention Restoration Theory” suggests that time spent in green areas restores cognitive processes through the practice of effortless attention (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1995; Berto, 2005; Ohly et al., 2016). The “Stress Reduction Theory” or “Psychoevolutionary Theory” proposes that contact with nature reduces stress and normalizes cardiovascular parameters controlled by the sympathetic system through an ‘automatic positive affective response” (Ulrich et al., 1991). More recent studies showed that exposure to nature may facilitate the regulation of emotions by activating the parasympathetic system (Richardson et al., 2016).

“Forest bathing” (aka Shinrin-Yoku) is an exemplary practice of contact with nature consisting of (guided or not) forest walking that promotes mental and physical wellbeing by acting on physiological parameters (e.g., heart rate, blood pressure), cognition, and affective and mood states, as well as on healthy behaviors (Hansen et al., 2017; Kobayashi et al., 2018; Twohig-Bennett and Jones, 2018; Corazon et al., 2019; Kotera et al., 2021). The beneficial effects of “forest bathing” on the reduction of stress and depression suggest a rationale for potential integration into therapeutic interventions for addiction (Kotera and Rhodes, 2020; Kotera et al., 2022; Yi et al., 2022) based on the relevant component of affective and mood disturbances in substance use (Smith and Book, 2008) and non-pathological gambling (Medeiros et al., 2016) disorders. To our knowledge, only one study assessed the effects of nature experience in people with a diagnosis of alcohol use disorders, showing significant improvement in depressive symptoms (Shin et al., 2012). Similarly, a study on patients with gambling disorder found that even just listening to high-definition forest sounds on headphones decreased negative mood compared to city sounds (Ochiai et al., 2020).

Therefore, it is important to understand whether the proven efficacy of nature-based intervention for different mental disorders may be of benefit for addiction treatment. In a study by Tesler et al., a sample of at-risk adolescents (but with no SUD diagnosis) after three activity sessions in a forest showed an increase in perceived wellbeing and a decrease in psychosomatic symptoms and tobacco/alcohol use (Tesler et al., 2018). Despite the lack of studies that have tested the effects of direct exposure to natural environments in SUD patients, some other studies suggest an effect on craving—the emotionally charged mental state characterized by the desire to take the substance(s) of abuse (May et al., 2015). For example, some studies has used imagery-based tasks involving natural settings to help reduce cravings for nicotine and food (e.g., Versland and Rosenberg, 2007; Hamilton et al., 2013). The online cross-sectional survey by Martin et al. found that access to a garden or allotment was associated with a reduction in the frequency and strength of craving (for alcohol, nicotine, and caffeine). Residential neighborhoods incorporating more than 25% green space were correlated with similar reductions in craving. Interestingly, these effects were mediated by a decrease in negative mood (Martin et al., 2019).

On this basis, the present research aims, as a first objective, to investigate the effects of exposure to nature compared to an urban environment on craving, mood, and wellbeing in a sample of SUD patients. Second, it aims to test the effects of such exposure on the sense of agency (i.e., the capacity to claim authorship over an action and associate specific consequences with a specific action; Hurault et al., 2020) and openness to the future (i.e., having positive expectations and a general disposition of acceptance toward the future; Colombo et al., 2020). Considering that these constructs have been shown to be protective factors for mental health and positively associated with wellbeing (Weinstein, 1980; Moore, 2016; Botella et al., 2018; Mikus et al., 2018; Colombo et al., 2020), we aimed to assess whether the effects of nature exposure on craving were correlated to the effect of these measures. We also assessed the effect of the nature vs. urban exposure sessions on restorativeness (i.e., physical, spatial, and non-spatial features of a setting that contributes to the recovery of psychological equilibrium; Kaplan, 1995).

A lagoon environment (Venice nature hinterland) was chosen as nature exposure in the present research. Considering that addiction treatment services are often located in urban areas, contact with forests or mountains might not be feasible. On the other hand, it would be important to assess the potential beneficial effects of nature experience in nearby natural environments, especially in the case of residential addiction centers located in extra-urban areas. Moreover, studies in different types of natural environments other than forests/mountains are needed to better understand the effects of a broader spectrum of nature experience.

In this study, we exposed a group of addict inpatients at the residential facility “Centro Soranzo” (Venice, Italy) to two 1-h walks in the typical natural environment of the Venetian lagoon and, after a 1-week wash-out period to an Urban walk session (in Mestre, mainland borough of the Venice municipality). The measures of craving, mood, wellbeing, openness to the future, agency, and restorativeness were collected before and after each walking session to assess the effects of nature vs. urban experiences. Particularly, we hypothesize to find a reduction in craving (H1) and negative mood (H2) and an increase in wellbeing (H3), openness to the future (H4), and a sense of agency (H5) after exposure to a natural environment compared to the urban environment. We, therefore, expect the natural environment to be perceived as more restorative than the urban environment (H6). Finally, we hypothesize an association between craving and negative mood (H7) and consequently that higher levels of craving are associated with lower levels of perceived wellbeing (H8), openness to the future (H9), and agency (H10).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

The experimental sample consisted of patients (age 18–65 years, M = 37.79, SD = 8.08) of both sexes, who are native Italian speakers, with a diagnosis of pathological addiction under treatment at the “Centro Soranzo” residential center (Venice, Italy). All participants received the Treatment-As-Usual (TAU) of the residential facility program for drug detoxification and rehabilitation, which includes interventions for addiction (relapse prevention group therapy and anti-craving pharmacotherapy) and individual and group psychotherapy by using verbal and non-verbal techniques (e.g., psychosomatic training, mindfulness, yoga, art therapy, expressive laboratory, dance-movement therapy, pet therapy, song therapy, autogenic training, and psycho-educational activities). It may also be possible for them to attend Alcoholics Anonymous (AA) group therapy. Comorbid psychiatric patients with anxiety or depression symptoms were supported by psychiatric intervention by using the appropriate pharmacotherapy if necessary. Pharmacotherapies prescribed ad hoc at the center for the treatment of anxiety/depressive center symptoms included antidepressants, anxiolytics, antipsychotics, lithium, mood stabilizers, and melatonin.

The presence of organic diseases that made prolonged walking difficult was used as an exclusion criterion. Twenty-four male patients (male = 20) took part in the study and were segregated into three groups through a semi-randomized distribution. Participants were admitted to the residential facility for an average of 1 month before the start of the study. The diagnosis of pathological addiction was made by territorial addiction services (SerDs). One-third of them have comorbidity with other disorders, especially mood disorders. They did not have a long history of addiction treatment failures (ranging from no previous residential treatment to a maximum of two previous experiences in other facilities). The demographics of the participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of the three groups.

The study was approved by the Scientific Board of the “Centro Soranzo” (18/04/2023 meeting; no internal protocol study number is available). The research complies with the guidelines for human studies and was conducted ethically by the World Medical Association's Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2 Measures

We measured craving, mood, wellbeing, openness to the future, agency, and restorativeness. The questionnaires used are given below.

- An ad-hoc questionnaire for measuring craving for the substance(s) of abuse. A single-item (“How much craving do you feel right now?”), 10-point scale based on the literature and adapted from the studies of Traylor et al. (2011) and Benvegnù et al. (2021) was adopted.

- Profile of Mood States (POMS) (McNair et al., 1971; italian adaptation: Farnè et al., 1991). The questionnaire consists of a list of 58 adjectives, with a 5-point scale response, measuring six factors: Tension/Anxiety (T; a state of intensified physical tension caused by anxiety and described by words of “Nervous” or “Restless”), Depression/Dejection (D; self-inadequacy hallmarked by sadness, guilt, loneliness, and hopelessness), Anger/Hostility (A; a feeling of anguish and hostility described by words such as “Annoyed” or “Resentful”), Vigor/Activity (V; a cheerful mood referring to high, positive energy described by words such as “Lively” or “Active”), Fatigue/Inertia (S; a state of low energy and described by words of “Exhausted” or “Weary”), and Confusion/ Bewilderment (C; a state of cognitive inefficiency described by words like “Forgetful” or “Bewildered”). The total score [total mood disturbance (TMD)] was calculated as the sum of the four negative scales (T, D, A, and S) minus the positive one (V). Cronbach's alpha of the total score is 0.95.

- Flourishing Scale (FS) (Diener et al., 2010; italian validation: Giuntoli et al., 2017): An 8-item scale with a 5-point Likert scale response provides a single psychological wellbeing score given by the sum of all items. The construct measured by the FS can be called “eudaimonic wellbeing” and summarizes dimensions that describe individuals' optimal psychological functioning (e.g., meaning in life, positive relationships, and self-acceptance measured by items such as “My social relationships are supportive and rewarding”). Cronbach's alpha is 0.85.

- Openness to the Future Scale (OFS) (Botella et al., 2018): OFS is a 10-item scale with a 5-point Likert scale response. “Openness to the future” refers to the active process of accepting future scenarios and trusting in one's personal ability to make plans to achieve desired outcomes and cope with adversity. It is measured with items such as “When I make plans, I am sure I will be able to carry them out.” Total openness to the future score is calculated as the sum of all items. Cronbach's alpha is 0.82.

- Sense of Agency Scale (SoAS) (Tapal et al., 2017): This scale is a French adaptation (Hurault et al., 2020) of the original SoAS involving 7 items with a 7-point Likert scale response. It measures two constructs: Sense of Positive Agency (SoPA, which represents the level of control over the body, mind, and environment felt by an individual and is measured by items such as “I am in full control of what I do”) and Sense of Negative Agency (SoNA, which represents the lack of control over the body, mind, and environment and the perception of being existentially helpless. It is described by items such as “My actions just happen without my intention”). Cronbach's alpha value is 0.82 for SoPA and 0.53 for SoNA.

- Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS-11) (Hartig et al., 1997; italian validation of the short-form: Pasini et al., 2014): The Italian validation includes 11 items (e.g., “To stop thinking about the things that I must get done I like to go to places like this”) with an 11-point response scale. It measures four factors: Fascination (referring to the effortless and involuntary attention to the natural setting that is the key process to restoring from mental fatigue), Being away (referring to escaping from unwanted distractions in the surroundings and distancing oneself from one's usual work), Coherence (referring to a physically coherent environment that sustains exploration; the perception of the environment as a whole with a larger organizational structure), and Scope (referring to the environmental characteristics that extend in time and space so that the environment is perceived to be possible to enter and spend time in). Cronbach's alpha of the total score is 0.82.

2.3 Procedure

All participants, after signing an informed consent and checking their adherence to the inclusion/exclusion criteria, were asked to complete a short demographic questionnaire. They were then semi-randomly allocated into three eight-participant groups to be exposed according to a Latin square design to two walking sessions in nature or staying at the center (interspersed with a 1-week wash-out period). All participants underwent the three conditions (two walking sessions and one “staying session”). The two walking sessions took place (i) in an urban environment (in Mestre, a mainland borough of the Venice municipality; henceforth “City condition”), and (ii) in a natural environment typical of the Venice lagoon (natural hinterland of Venice; henceforth “Nature condition”).

The lagoon environment includes wetlands and salt marshes characterized by the presence of trees, shrubs, reeds, rushes, sedges, and salt-tolerant coastal vegetation, such as seagrass and glasswort. A typical feature is the “barena” (mudflat)—sandy areas covered and uncovered by the tides.

In contrast, the urban environment includes tree-lined boulevards with moderate car traffic, bordered on both sides by residential buildings averaging 4–5 stores high, and areas with predominantly commercial establishments, offices, restaurants, and parking lots. The Urban walk took place along a loop route in the city's most architecturally valuable areas but to avoid both the most degraded and drug-associated areas.

During the “staying session,” and during the entire period of the study, participants performed unstructured activities at the residential facility and received the TAU (Figure 1). No data were collected during the session and time at the center.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the phases of the procedure based on a Latin-square design. POMS, Profile of Mood States; SoAS, Sense of Agency Scale; OFS, Openness to the Future Scale; FS, Flourishing Scale; and PRS-11, Perceived Restorativeness Scale.

During both City and Nature conditions, participants took an easy, no-slope 1-h walk (tourist difficulty) led by a practitioner and were free to interact with each other and contemplate the view. Before and after each walking session, participants completed the ad-hoc craving measurement questionnaire, POMS, SoAS, OFS, and FS. At the end of each session, the PRS-11 was completed.

2.4 Data analysis

All analyses were performed with GraphPad Prism 9.1.0 and Microsoft Excel (Version 2404 Build 16.0.17531.20140). To verify H1, a two-way ANOVA (mixed-effects analysis) with Greenhouse– Geisser correction was performed on craving scores, with “CONDITION” as the between factor (two levels: “City” and “Nature”) and “TIME” as the within factor (two levels: “pre-” and “post-” session). In case of significance, Post-hoc comparisons were conducted (Fisher's LSD test). To test hypotheses H2–H5, the same analyses were conducted for POMS scores (H2) – for both Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) and subscales –, FS scores (H3), OFS scores (H4), and SoAS scores (H5)—for both Sense of Positive Agency (SoPA) and Sense of Negative Agency (SoNA). The subscales of PRS-11 and the total scores were analyzed by paired t-test (or Wilcoxon signed-rank tests in case of non-normal data) to test H6.

To verify hypotheses H7–H10, correlations between craving scores and POMS scores (H7), FS scores (H8), OFS scores (H9), and SoAS scores (H10) were tested with Spearman's rank correlation coefficient ρ.

Descriptive statistics of the instruments used in the three groups for each condition and time point are reported in Supplementary Table S1.

3 Results

3.1 Craving

There was a significant CONDITION [F(1, 23) = 17.45, p < 0.001] main effect and CONDITION × TIME interaction [F(1, 17) = 18.43, p < 0.001] for craving (Figure 2). Post-hoc tests showed a significant increase in craving from pre- to post-session in the City condition (p < 0.001) and an opposite effect in the Nature condition (p = 0.039). When the two conditions were directly compared, significantly higher craving was reported after the City than Nature condition at the post-session time point (p < 0.001).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effects of natural and urban environments on craving scores. The bars represent the mean score (± SEM) of craving measured at the beginning of the session (pre) and after the walking session (post) in the lagoon (Nature; green markers) and in the urban environment (City; red markers). *p < 0.05 and ***p < 0.001, Fisher's LSD Post-hoc test.

3.2 Mood

Analysis of POMS (Figure 3) showed significant main effects of TIME [F(1, 23) = 4.98, p = 0.036] and CONDITION [F(1, 23) = 6.07, p = 0.022] and a significant CONDITION × TIME interaction [F(1, 21) = 10.13, p = 0.004]. Post-hoc tests showed a decrease in TMD values between the start and end of the session in Nature condition only [p < 0.001]. When the two conditions were directly compared, a significantly higher TMD in the City condition than in the Nature condition was observed after the post-session time point (p = 0.004).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effects of natural and urban environments on Profile of Mood States (POMS) scores. The bars represent the mean score (± SEM) of Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) measured at the beginning of the session (pre) and after the walking session (post) in the lagoon (Nature; green markers) and in the urban environment (City; red markers). **p < 0.01 and ***p < 0.001, Fisher's LSD Post-hoc test.

3.3 Wellbeing

Regarding wellbeing (Figure 4), the FS showed significant main effects of TIME [F(1, 23) = 4.66, p = 0.041] and CONDITION [F(1, 23) = 5.02, p = 0.035]. Post-hoc tests showed a significant increase in FS scores from pre-session to post-session in the Nature condition only (p = 0.011). When the two conditions were directly compared, there were significantly higher FS scores in the Nature condition compared to the City condition at the post-session time point (p = 0.011).

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effects of natural and urban environments on Flourishing Scale (FS) scores. The bars represent the mean score (± S.E.M.) of FS measured at the beginning of the session (pre) and after the walking session (post) in the lagoon (nature; green markers) and in the urban environment (city; red markers). *p < 0.05, Fisher's LSD Post-hoc test.

3.4 Openness to the future

Analysis of the Openness to the Future scale (Figure 5) highlighted a significant CONDITION × TIME interaction [F(1, 21) = 5.97, p = 0.023]. Post-hoc tests showed a significant decrease in OFS scores from pre-session to post-session in the City condition (p = 0.044). When the two conditions were directly compared, there was a significantly higher OFS score in the Nature condition compared to the City condition at the post-session time point (p = 0.007).

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effects of natural and urban environments on Sense of Positive Agency (SoPA) scores. The bars represent the mean score (± S.E.M.) of SoPA measured at the beginning of the session (pre) and after the walking session (post) in the lagoon (Nature; green markers) and in the urban environment (City; red markers). *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01, Fisher's LSD Post-hoc test.

3.5 Sense of agency

Regarding Sense of Agency (Figure 6), the SoPA subscale showed a significant CONDITION × TIME interaction [F (1, 20) = 4.69, p = 0.042]. Post-hoc tests showed a significant increase in SoPA scores from pre-session to post-session in the Nature condition only (p = 0.042). When the two conditions were directly compared, there was a significantly higher SoPA score in the Nature compared to the City condition at the post-session timepoint (p = 0.046). Regarding the SoNA subscale, although the CONDITION × TIME interaction was significant [F(1, 20) = 5.41, p = 0.031], Post-hoc tests did not reveal any significant differences.

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Effects of natural and urban environments on Openness to the Future Scale (OFS) scores. The bars represent the mean score (± S.E.M.) of OFS measured at the beginning of the session (pre) and after the walking session (post) in the lagoon (Nature; green markers) and in the urban environment (City; red markers). *p < 0.05, Fisher's LSD Post-hoc test.

3.6 Restorativeness

All PRS-11 factors and the total score (Figure 7) were found to score significantly higher in the Nature condition than in the City condition: Fascination [t(22) = 4.81, p < 0.001]; Being Away [t(22) = 7.76, p < 0.001]; Coherence [t(22) = 4.20, p < 0.001], Scope [t(22) = 4.50, p < 0.001], and total score [t(22) = 6.51, p < 0.001].

Figure 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Effects of natural and urban environments on Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS-11) total score and factors. The bars represent the mean score (± S.E.M.) of Fascination (FAS), Being Away (B-A), Coherence (COH), Scope (SCO), and total score (TOT) measured after the walking session in the lagoon (Nature; green markers) and in the urban environment (City; red markers). ***p < 0.001, Fisher's LSD Post-hoc test.

3.7 Association between craving and mood

The correlational analysis shows a series of significant positive correlations between craving scores measured at the post-session time point in the City condition and the POMS global score (Figure 8) and all subscales (except Vigor): Total Mood Disturbance: ρ = 0.52, p = 0.009; Tension: ρ = 0.72, p < 0.001; Depression: ρ = 0.44, p = 0.032; Anger: ρ = 0.49, p = 0.017; Fatigue: ρ = 0.60, p = 0.002; and Confusion: ρ = 0.48, p = 0.02. On the other hand, no significant correlation emerged between craving and POMS scores in the Nature condition.

Figure 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Correlational analysis between the craving scores measured after the walking session in the urban environment (abscissa) and the POMS global score (Total Mood Disturbance; ordinates).

3.8 Association between craving and wellbeing

The correlation between the craving scores measured at the post-session time point in the City condition and wellbeing was significant (Figure 9). In particular, there was a negative correlation between craving and the FS scores (ρ = −0.46, p = 0.025). No significant correlations emerged in the Nature condition.

Figure 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Correlational analysis between the craving scores measured after the walking session in the urban environment (abscissa) and the Flourishing Scale (FS; ordinates).

3.9 Association between craving and openness to the future

No significant correlations emerged between craving and OFS scores in both the Nature and City conditions.

3.10 Association between craving and sense of agency

In the City condition, a positive correlation (ρ = 0.60, p = 0.002) was found between craving and SoNA scores at the post-session time point (Figure 10). No significant correlations were found in the Nature condition.

Figure 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Correlational analysis between the craving scores measured after the walking session in the urban environment (abscissa) and Sense of Negative Agency (SoNA; ordinates).

4 Discussion

In our study, a 1-h Nature walk in the Venetian lagoon environment significantly decreased cravings compared to pre-walk values and compared to cravings after a 1-h City walk, with the latter significantly increased when compared to pre-walk values. These results confirm our hypothesis H1 and suggest that even brief exposure to natural environments may decrease craving not only in SUD patients but also in the general population, as investigated by previous studies (Versland and Rosenberg, 2007; Hamilton et al., 2013; Tesler et al., 2018; Martin et al., 2019). The same type of comparisons showed a significant decrease in total mood disturbances (POMS), and increased wellbeing (Flourishing Scale) and Sense of Positive Agency, confirming our hypotheses (H2, H3, and H5) and extending the results of previous studies (Shin et al., 2012; Tesler et al., 2018; Ochiai et al., 2020). Openness to the Future and Restorativeness measures showed a significant improvement after the Nature vs. City walk. This last point verifies our hypothesis H6: The lagoon environment chosen for the present study has been shown to exert a greater restorative effect than the urban environment in all subdimensions investigated; attracting attention and distracting participants from daily worries (subscales Fascination and Being away), being perceived as a space with meaning and being pleasantly explorable (subscales Coherence and Scope). Craving scores after a City walk positively correlated with POMS and a Sense of Negative Agency values and negatively correlated with the Flourishing scale values.

Participants were admitted to the residential facility at an average of 1 month before the start of the study. They were, therefore, already receiving the TAU for drug detoxification and rehabilitation, which included interventions for addiction and individual and group psychotherapy. These therapies could explain the low levels of craving at the pre-sessions time points (both for City and Nature conditions). Nevertheless, the Nature walk was able to further decrease craving. Our findings confirm that “nature experience”—such as a guided walk in a natural environment that includes elements (such as water, plants, and trees) known to exert effects on mental health—may improve mood, wellbeing, attention, stress relief, openness, and a sense of being active. Moreover, we also showed a specific effect on craving—a key symptom of SUD.

The correlational analysis did not show any association between craving and the other psychological measures (H7 to H10), despite the working hypothesis that the effects on mental functions might restore the affective, emotional, and cognitive unbalances underlying drug addiction. Therefore, we may hypothesize that the Nature walk under our conditions induced beneficial effects on both psychological and addictive measures but probably through different mechanisms. Although we did not investigate such underlying mechanisms, we could however hypothesize the involvement of processes that are affected by nature experience, such as those reported by mountain and forest studies (Kuo, 2015). In particular, a phenomenon that has been frequently observed is restoration. Restoration is the relaxed recovery of attention and cardiovascular parameters, that is, an effortless cognitive load, a positive initial affective response, or both. Bottom-up visual information (e.g., Hägerhäll et al., 2015; Kardan et al., 2015) activates affective and emotional processing, aligning with expectations and involves prefrontal cognitive functions (Joung et al., 2015), focusing attention and triggering endogenous reward expectations (Valtchanov and Ellard, 2015). The high restorativeness scores mentioned earlier (in particular, the Fascination subscale refers precisely to “the effortless and involuntary attention to the natural setting that is the key process to restore from mental fatigue”) suggest this possible interpretation.

We planned our study to show that the benefits of a natural environment experience were better than the daily experiences in the residence center. The residential center “Centro Soranzo” is in a former military facility that was recently redesigned with evidence-based and neuroarchitecture design features. These features of the space might presumably contribute to the mental health of the subjects and possibly help to lower craving values as assessed at the center before the walking sessions. Furthermore, craving values after the Nature walk were significantly lower, suggesting an additional benefit to the TAU effect on craving and the psychological measures.

As far as the comparison design is concerned, previous studies have investigated the effects of mountain hiking (indoor vs. outdoor activity) on the affective state (Niedermeier et al., 2017), or of forest therapy vs. mountain hiking on mental health (Huber et al., 2023). We also included as a comparator a walk in an urban area (Mestre city), which is not only characterized by contemporary architectural renovation but also by twenteeth-century buildings and degraded social housing. The comparison was not based on a specific hypothesis since the literature on the effects of exposure to urban context, already largely investigated, reported mixed results depending on specific features of the urban space (Hartig et al., 2003; Roe et al., 2013; White et al., 2013; Bratman et al., 2015). In fact, several reports showed the beneficial effects of nature vs. urban experience (e.g., Hartig et al., 2003), whereas other studies showed that green exposure in an urban setting may reduce stress (Roe et al., 2013), improve wellbeing (White et al., 2013), and enhance cognition (Bratman et al., 2015). In our study, the decrease in craving after the Nature walk was significantly different from the Urban after-walk measure, evidently due to the large craving increase in the latter group compared to the pre-walk assessment. Moreover, the increase in craving after the Urban walk was correlated to the impairment of mood, wellbeing, sense of autonomy, and optimism for the future. Similar data were obtained for emotions, mood, restorativeness, and subjective vitality when comparing a nature forest environment to a “building” control, with improvement and impairment in the indices of mental health, respectively (Bielinis et al., 2021). “Cue Reactivity” is the phenomenon that could underlie the observed effect in our participants. Cue reactivity is the response to salient information associated with rewards (Niaura et al., 1988; Rohsenow et al., 1991; Chiamulera, 2005). Not only discrete stimuli (i.e., cues) may be determinant factors of craving, but also the living space (the spatial context) may play a relevant role as a determinant factor (Conklin and Tiffany, 2002; Conklin, 2006; Conklin et al., 2008; Chiamulera et al., 2017). Our data, collected after the participants walked in an urban area where most of them used to live, that is an environment rich in conditioned stimuli and context associated with the substance, suggest that cue and context reactivity should be taken into consideration when evaluating residential program outcomes after patients' discharge. “Re-naturing” of urban spaces is a process that is recommended by several policies: integration of nature exposure into the planning and design of urban, architectural, and public spaces may change the value of the urban environment to a better, or at least less negative, place for mental health (Milgram, 1970; Bauduceau et al., 2015; Hartig and Kahn, 2016; Leloup et al., 2016; Astell-Burt et al., 2022) and possibly, as we suggest, for reducing relapse risk in former addicts. Future studies could also investigate more in depth about the role of urban environment features by testing the effect of different architectural and urban interventions and by identifying the elements that may induce a restorative response (and possibly, reduce cue reactivity). In this regard, a useful tool could be virtual reality (VR). Although the complexity of real-world investigations cannot be easily modeled in the laboratory, technologies such as VR may offer the possibility of ecologically and safely mimicking the effects of changing environmental context while maintaining rigorous control of experimental parameters (García-Rodríguez et al., 2012; Chiamulera et al., 2017, 2024; Spano et al., 2023; Theodorou et al., 2023).

Our study has some limitations. First, the participants were almost exclusively men with an average age of 40 years. Our data from a population of middle-aged male addicts cannot be inferred to a similar female sample, and, in general, to a younger population (Astell-Burt et al., 2013). Second, nature exposure limited to a single 1-h walk, without following sessions, does not allow us to make predictions about the relationship to exposure time or frequency dependency of the effect (White et al., 2019). Finally, some of the measures used do not have high internal consistency (especially SoAS).

Further studies are needed to assess the effects of changing Nature walk time durations to assess the relationship to response. Similarly, more frequent walks may help in assessing whether Nature walk effects persist or are transient and, maybe, whether a devaluation of the negative effects by Urban walk may take place after repetitive Urban walks (i.e., exposure to therapy-like effect).

5 Conclusion

In conclusion, our findings showed that experience in nature can reduce drug craving in SUD patients, and improve mood, wellbeing, openness to the future, and a sense of agency. In addition, an opposite pattern emerged after exposure to an urban environment, with craving scores associated with worsening mood, agency, and wellbeing. These findings suggest, on the one hand, the importance of considering the context when evaluating the outcomes of residential programs after patients are discharged and, on the other hand, when applying more efforts in evidence-based design of the urban environment, with both actions informed by multidisciplinary experimental and ecological research.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of Centro Soranzo. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

GB: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. MS: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AU: Writing – review & editing. IZ: Writing – review & editing, Investigation. MC: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. CC: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1356446/full#supplementary-material

References

Astell-Burt, T., Feng, X., and Kolt, G. S. (2013). Mental health benefits of neighbourhood green space are stronger among physically active adults in middle-to-older age: evidence from 260,061 Australians. Prev. Med. 57, 601–606. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.08.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Astell-Burt, T., Hartig, T., Eckermann, S., Nieuwenhuijsen, M., McMunn, A., Frumkin, H., et al. (2022). More green, less lonely? A longitudinal cohort study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 51, 99–110. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyab089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauduceau, N., Berry, P., Cecchi, C., Elmqvist, T., Fernandez, M., Hartig, T., et al. (2015). Towards an EU research and innovation policy agenda for nature-based solutions and re-naturing cities: final report of the Horizon 2020 expert group on ‘Nature-based solutions and re-naturing cities.'

Google Scholar

Benvegnù, G., Tommasi, F., Ferraro, S., Libener, E., Di Chio, M., Bosi, S., et al. (2021). Smokers “context reactivity” in virtual domestic environments. Eur. Addict. Res. 27, 439–446. doi: 10.1159/000515301

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 249–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bielinis, E., Janeczko, E., Takayama, N., Zawadzka, A., Słupska, A., Pietka, S., et al. (2021). The effects of viewing a winter forest landscape with the ground and trees covered in snow on the psychological relaxation of young finnish adults: a pilot study. PLoS ONE 16, 1–16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244799

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Botella, C., Molinari, G., Fernández-Álvarez, J., Guillén, V., García-Palacios, A., Baños, R. M., et al. (2018). Development and validation of the openness to the future scale: a prospective protective factor. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 16, 1–16. doi: 10.1186/s12955-018-0889-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bratman, G. N., Daily, G. C., Levy, B. J., and Gross, J. J. (2015). The benefits of nature experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landsc. Urban Plan. 138, 41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.02.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bratman, G. N., Hamilton, J. P., and Daily, G. C. (2012). The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1249, 118–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06400.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiamulera, C. (2005). Cue reactivity in nicotine and tobacco dependence: a “multiple- action” model of nicotine as a primary reinforcement and as an enhancer of the effects of smoking-associated stimuli. Brain Res. Rev. 48, 74–97. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiamulera, C., Benvegn,ù, G., Piva, A., and Paolone, G. (2024). Ecocebo: how the interaction between environment and drug effects may improve pharmacotherapy outcomes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 161:105648. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiamulera, C., Ferrandi, E., Benvegn,ù, G., Ferraro, S., Tommasi, F., Maris, B., et al. (2017). Virtual reality for neuroarchitecture: cue reactivity in built spaces. Front. Psychol. 8, 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, D., Fernández-Álvarez, J., Suso-Ribera, C., Cipresso, P., García-Palacios, A., Riva, G., et al. (2020). Biased affective forecasting: a potential mechanism that enhances resilience and well-being. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conklin, C. A. (2006). Environments as cues to smoke: Implications for human extinction-based research and treatment. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 14, 12–19. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.1.12

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conklin, C. A., Robin, N., Perkins, K. A., Salkeld, R. P., and McClernon, F. J. (2008). Proximal versus distal cues to smoke: the effects of environments on smokers' cue-reactivity. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 16, 207–214. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.16.3.207

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conklin, C. A., and Tiffany, S. T. (2002). Applying extinction research and theory to cue-exposure addiction treatments. Addiction 97, 155–167. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00014.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corazon, S. S., Sidenius, U., Poulsen, D. V., Gramkow, M. C., and Stigsdotter, U. K. (2019). Psycho-physiological stress recovery in outdoor nature-based interventions: A systematic review of the past eight years of research. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16101711

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D., won Oishi, S., et al. (2010). New well-being measures: short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Soc. Indic. Res. 97, 143–156. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farnè, M., Sebellico, A., Gnugnoli, D., and Corallo, A. (1991). Profile of Moods States. Un Questionario per lo studio Delle Emozioni. Firenze: Giunti.

Google Scholar

García-Rodríguez, O., Pericot-Valverde, I., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., Ferrer-García, M., and Secades-Villa, R. (2012). Validation of smoking-related virtual environments for cue exposure therapy. Addict. Behav. 37, 703–708. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.02.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuntoli, L., Ceccarini, F., Sica, C., and Caudek, C. (2017). Validation of the Italian versions of the flourishing scale and of the scale of positive and negative experience. SAGE Open 7:293. doi: 10.1177/2158244016682293

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hägerhäll, C. M., Laike, T., Küller, M., Marcheschi, E., Boydston, C., and Taylor, R. P. (2015). Human physiological benefits of viewing nature: EEG responses to exact and statistical fractal patterns. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 19, 1–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hamilton, J., Fawson, S., May, J., Andrade, J., and Kavanagh, D. J. (2013). Brief guided imagery and body scanning interventions reduce food cravings. Appetite 71, 158–162. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, M. M., Jones, R., and Tocchini, K. (2017). Shinrin-yoku (Forest bathing) and nature therapy: A state-of-the-art review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 14:851. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14080851

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T., Evans, G. W., Jamner, L. D., Davis, D. S., and Gärling, T. (2003). Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 109–123. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00109-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T., and Kahn, P. (2016). Living in cities, naturally. Science 352, 938–990. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf3759

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartig, T., Kaiser, F. G., and Bowler, P. A. (1997). Further Development of a Measure of Perceived Environmental Restorativeness. Working Paper No. 5, Uppsala University, Institute for Housing Research, Gavle, Sweden.

Google Scholar

Hartig, T., Van Den Berg, A. E., Hagerhall, C. M., Tomalak, M., Bauer, N., Hansmann, R., et al. (2011). “Health benefits of nature experience: Psychological,” in Forests, Trees, and Human Health, eds. K. Nilsson, M. Sangster, C. Gallis, T. Hartig, S. de Vries, K. Seeland (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Huber, D., Freidl, J., Pichler, C., Bischof, M., Kiem, M., Weisböck-Erdheim, R., et al. (2023). Long-term effects of mountain hiking vs. forest therapy on physical and mental health of couples: a randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:469. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20021469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurault, J. C., Broc, G., Crône, L., Tedesco, A., and Brunel, L. (2020). Measuring the sense of agency: a French adaptation and validation of the sense of agency scale (F-SoAS). Front. Psychol. 11:584145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.584145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joung, D., Kim, G., Choi, Y., Lim, H., Park, S., Woo, J. M., et al. (2015). The prefrontal cortex activity and psychological effects of viewing forest landscapes in Autumn season. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 12, 7235–7243. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120707235

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, R., and Kaplan, S. (1989). The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 15, 169–182. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kardan, O., Demiralp, E., Hout, M. C., Hunter, M. C. R., Karimi, H., Hanayik, T., et al. (2015). Is the preference of natural versus man-made scenes driven by bottom-up processing of the visual features of nature? Front. Psychol. 6, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00471

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobayashi, H., Song, C., Ikei, H., Park, B. J., Lee, J., Kagawa, T., et al. (2018). Forest walking affects autonomic nervous activity: a population-based study. Front. Public Heal. 6, 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00278

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotera, Y., Lyons, M., Vione, K. C., and Norton, B. (2021). Effect of nature walks on depression and anxiety: a systematic review. Sustainability 13, 1–17. doi: 10.3390/su13074015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotera, Y., and Rhodes, C. (2020). Commentary: Suggesting Shinrin-yoku (forest bathing) for treating addiction. Addict. Behav. 111:106556. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotera, Y., Richardson, M., and Sheffield, D. (2022). Effects of Shinrin-Yoku (Forest Bathing) and nature therapy on mental health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 20, 337–361. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00363-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, M. (2015). How might contact with nature promote human health? Promising mechanisms and a possible central pathway. Front. Psychol. 6, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leloup, L., Dongo Miletich, D., Andriet, G., Vandermeeren, Y., and Samson, D. (2016). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation on the right temporo-parietal junction modulates the use of mitigating circumstances during moral judgments. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:355. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L., Pahl, S., White, M. P., and May, J. (2019). Natural environments and craving: The mediating role of negative affect. Heal. Place 58:102160. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2019.102160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

May, J., Kavanagh, D. J., and Andrade, J. (2015). The elaborated intrusion theory of desire: a 10-year retrospective and implications for addiction treatments. Addict. Behav. 44, 29–34. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.09.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., and Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

Google Scholar

Medeiros, G. C., Sampaio, D. G., Leppink, E. W., Chamberlain, S. R., and Grant, J. E. (2016). Anxiety, gambling activity, and neurocognition: A dimensional approach to a non-treatment-seeking sample. J. Behav. Addict. 5, 261–270. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikus, A., Hoogendoorn, M., Rocha, A., Gama, J., Ruwaard, J., and Riper, H. (2018). Predicting short term mood developments among depressed patients using adherence and ecological momentary assessment data. Internet Interv. 12, 105–110. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milgram, S. (1970). The experience of living in cities. Science 167, 1461–1468. doi: 10.1126/science.167.3924.1461

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. W. (2016). What is the sense of agency and why does it matter? Front. Psychol. 7, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01272

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niaura, R. S., Rohsenow, D. J., Binkoff, J. A., Monti, P. M., Pedraza, M., and Abrams, D. B. (1988). Relevance of Cue reactivity to understanding alcohol and smoking relapse. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 97, 133–152. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.2.133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niedermeier, M., Einwanger, J., Hartl, A., and Kopp, M. (2017). Affective responses in mountain hiking - A randomized crossover trial focusing on differences between indoor and outdoor activity. PLoS ONE 12, 1–17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177719

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochiai, H., Song, C., Jo, H., Oishi, M., Imai, M., and Miyazaki, Y. (2020). Relaxing effect induced by forest sound in patients with gambling disorder. Sustainability 12:969. doi: 10.3390/su12155969

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohly, H., White, M. P., Wheeler, B. W., Bethel, A., Ukoumunne, O. C., Nikolaou, V., et al. (2016). Attention restoration theory: a systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Heal. - Part B Crit. Rev. 19, 305–343. doi: 10.1080/10937404.2016.1196155

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasini, M., Berto, R., Brondino, M., Hall, R., and Ortner, C. (2014). How to measure the restorative quality of environments: the PRS-11. Procedia - Soc. Behav. Sci. 159, 293–297. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.12.375

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., McEwan, K., Maratos, F., and Sheffield, D. (2016). Joy and Calm: how an evolutionary functional model of affect regulation informs positive emotions in nature. Evol. Psychol. Sci. 2, 308–320. doi: 10.1007/s40806-016-0065-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roe, J. J., Ward Thompson, C., Aspinall, P. A., Brewer, M. J., Duff, E. I., Miller, D., et al. (2013). Green space and stress: evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 10, 4086–4103. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10094086

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohsenow, D. J., Childress, A. R., Monti, P. M., Niaura, R. S., and Abrams, D. B. (1991). Cue reactivity in addictive behaviors: theoretical and treatment implications. Int. J. Addict. 25, 957–993. doi: 10.3109/10826089109071030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, W. S., Shin, C. S., and Yeoun, P. S. (2012). The influence of forest therapy camp on depression in alcoholics. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 17, 73–76. doi: 10.1007/s12199-011-0215-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. P., and Book, S. W. (2008). Anxiety and substance use disorders: a review prevalence of comorbid anxiety and substance use disorders. Psychiatr Times 25, 19–23.

Google Scholar

Spano, G., Theodorou, A., Reese, G., Carrus, G., Sanesi, G., and Panno, A. (2023). Virtual nature and psychological and psychophysiological outcomes: a systematic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 89:102044. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapal, A., Oren, E., Dar, R., and Eitam, B. (2017). The sense of agency scale: a measure of consciously perceived control over one's mind, body, and the immediate environment. Front. Psychol. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01552

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tesler, R., Plaut, P., and Endvelt, R. (2018). The effects of an urban forest health intervention program on physical activity, substance abuse, psychosomatic symptoms, and life satisfaction among adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:2134. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Theodorou, A., Romano, L., Bratman, G. N., Carbone, G. A., Rodelli, R., Casagrande, G., et al. (2023). Different types of virtual natural environments enhance subjective vitality through restorativeness. J. Environ. Psychol. 87:101981. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.101981

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Traylor, A. C., Parrish, D. E., Copp, H. L., and Bordnick, P. S. (2011). Using virtual reality to investigate complex and contextual cue reactivity in nicotine dependent problem drinkers. Addict. Behav. 36, 1068–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.06.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Twohig-Bennett, C., and Jones, A. (2018). The health benefits of the great outdoors: a systematic review and meta-analysis of greenspace exposure and health outcomes. Environ. Res. 166, 628–637. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.06.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valtchanov, D., and Ellard, C. G. (2015). Cognitive and affective responses to natural scenes: Effects of low level visual properties on preference, cognitive load and eye-movements. J. Environ. Psychol. 43, 184–195. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Versland, A., and Rosenberg, H. (2007). Effect of brief imagery interventions on craving in college student smokers. Addict. Res. Theory 15, 177–187. doi: 10.1080/16066350701200582

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 806–820. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

White, M. P., Alcock, I., Grellier, J., Wheeler, B. W., Hartig, T., Warber, S. L., et al. (2019). Spending at least 120 minutes a week in nature is associated with good health and wellbeing. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

White, M. P., Alcock, I., Wheeler, B. W., and Depledge, M. H. (2013). Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychol. Sci. 24, 920–928. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464659

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Yi, Y., Seo, E., and An, J. (2022). Does forest therapy have physio-psychological benefits? A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:10512. doi: 10.3390/ijerph191710512

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: nature experience, craving, substance use disorders, residential programs, wellbeing

Citation: Benvegnù G, Semenzato M, Urbani A, Zanlorenzi I, Cibin M and Chiamulera C (2024) Nature-based experience in Venetian lagoon: Effects on craving and wellbeing in addict residential inpatients. Front. Psychol. 15:1356446. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1356446

Received: 22 January 2024; Accepted: 20 May 2024;
Published: 12 June 2024.

Edited by:

Angelo Panno, European University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Glenda Garza-Teran, University of Sonora, Mexico
Giulia Ballarotto, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Benvegnù, Semenzato, Urbani, Zanlorenzi, Cibin and Chiamulera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giulia Benvegnù, Z2l1bGlhLmJlbnZlZ251JiN4MDAwNDA7dW5pdnIuaXQ=

These authors share senior authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.