Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 February 2024
Sec. Cognitive Science

The formation of episodic autobiographical memory is predicted by mental imagery, self-reference, and anticipated details

  • Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau & Cognition (LMC2 UR 7536), Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France

Introduction: Despite the ecological nature of episodic memory (EM) and the importance of consolidation in its functioning, studies tackling both subjects are still scarce. Therefore, the present study aims at establishing predictions of the future of newly encoded information in EM in an ecological paradigm.

Methods: Participants recorded two personal events per day with a SenseCam portable camera, for 10 days, and characterized the events with different subjective scales (emotional valence and intensity, self-concept and self-relevance, perspective and anticipated details at a month, mental images…). They then performed a surprise free recall at 5 days and 1 month after encoding. Machine learning algorithms were used to predict the future of events (episodic or forgotten) in memory at 1 month.

Results: The best algorithm showed an accuracy of 78%, suggesting that such a prediction is reliably possible. Variables that best differentiated between episodic and forgotten memories at 1 month were mental imagery, self-reference, and prospection (anticipated details) at encoding and the first free recall.

Discussion: These results may establish the basis for the development of episodic autobiographical memory during daily experiences.

1 Introduction

Episodic memory (EM) is a long-term memory of everyday events, allowing to ground our sense of Self in a temporal continuity (Tulving, 2002). More specifically, EM allows to encode, store, and recall personally experienced events with their spatial and temporal context. Episodic memories are rich and detailed, as each of them associates several types of information: the target information (“what”), its external (“where” and “when”), and internal context (“how”), which includes the integration of sensory-motor information (e.g., visual, auditory, and tactile), phenomenological information (e.g., emotions, thoughts, self-reference), and idiosyncratic aspects of this event. Recollection of information in EM is characterized by an autonoetic state of consciousness where individuals mentally “travel” across time to reexperience a specific event (Tulving, 1985). Episodic memories of personal, real-life events are also called episodic autobiographical memories (EAM; Piolino et al., 2009; Staniloiu et al., 2020). However, autobiographical memory relates not only to episodic memories of events that have occurred during our lifetime but also to semantic memories (i.e., general personal events and facts about ourselves; Conway, 2009). Thus, EAM is a long-lasting EM mainly dependent on the Self, which processes new experiences and memories through the lens of personal relevance, emotions, attitudes, and goals (Conway, 2005). EM and EAM present some differences in their methodological approaches as the former is generally studied in laboratory settings, asking participants to recall lists of new material, and the latter examines the memory of specific events from one’s past (Roediger and Marsh, 2003). Previous studies with laboratory materials suggest that the emotional component (Yonelinas and Ritchey, 2015), self-relevance (Straube, 2012) and subsequent recalls or updates of information (Alberini, 2005; Dudai, 2012) are important determinants of whether some information will be captured in long-term EM, semantic memory, or forgotten.

Ecological and naturalistic paradigms are particularly well-suited for the study of EM, bringing it one step closer to the characteristics of EAM (Sonkusare et al., 2019). For instance, studies have used smartphones (Foudil et al., 2021), 360° videos (Serino and Repetto, 2018), and real-life photographs with a digital camera (Pathman et al., 2011). Another noteworthy example of this kind of paradigms uses wearable cameras to record features of real-life events, either during a specified walk (Jeunehomme and D'Argembeau, 2019; Khachatoorian et al., 2021) or during their day-to-day activities (Mair et al., 2017; Gelonch et al., 2020). Wearable cameras are small devices, often worn around the neck, that capture still images of the surrounding environment from the wearer’s perspective. Images can later be uploaded to a computer and be presented several times to amnesic participants to support the memory of new autobiographical events, by reviewing the pictures and taking memory tests a short time after encoding (Hodges et al., 2011), or as cues during retrieval (Chow and Rissman, 2017).

Despite some challenges regarding image quality, events’ sorting and qualifying as well as participant compliance and attention during recorded events, wearable cameras have already been successfully adapted to assess the nature of memories with the Remember/Know paradigm (Tulving (1985); see for instance Khachatoorian et al. (2021)). Furthermore, wearable cameras have already allowed to draw interesting conclusions regarding EM functioning. For example, Jeunehomme and D'Argembeau (2019) found that goal-directed events are two to three times less temporally compressed when mentally reexperienced compared to non-goal-directed events. St Jacques and Schacter (2013) studied the effect of an intermediate memory reactivation (48 h post encoding) on a subsequent memory test (48 h after memory reactivation). They found that memory performance is better when there is a higher level of matching between the retrieval cue and the encoding context. Wearable cameras could be particularly relevant to study long-term memory. For instance, studies have shown that, despite a high rate of forgotten events, an end-of-day review of the events improved memory performance at 1, 2, 3, and 8 weeks post intentional encoding (Finley et al., 2011; Mair et al., 2017, 2019). Milton et al. (2011) used a neuroimaging approach to assess recollection and familiarity at 36-h and 5-month retention intervals with pictures automatically taken by SenseCam. The medial temporal lobe (MTL; mostly the parahippocampal gyrus) activated only for familiarity-based retrievals at the 5-month delay, and neocortical regions such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) were more engaged for recollection-based retrievals. This is in line with the brain regions traditionally engaged in autobiographical memory (AM), which are for instance the mPFC (Markowitsch et al., 2003; Northoff et al., 2006; Brand and Markowitsch, 2008; Summerfield et al., 2009; Oddo et al., 2010; Martinelli et al., 2013b), in relationship with the involvement of the Self; the cuneus for mental imagery (Burgess et al., 2001; Piolino et al., 2009; Kim, 2018); and the hippocampus for the richness of specific events (Piolino et al., 2009; Moscovitch et al., 2016). More specifically, in a recent review, Daviddi et al. (2023) identified a differential temporal activation of regions within the default-mode network for EAM depending on the early phase (construction) versus late phase (elaboration) of the retrieval mode (see for instance Holland et al. (2011), McCormick et al. (2018), Audrain et al. (2022)). The involvement of these regions in the retrieval of episodic autobiographical memories can thus be differentiated between the two phases. On the one hand, along the temporo-parietal axis, the left hippocampus and the posterior cingulated cortex are activated during both construction and elaboration; however, construction selectively recruits the right hippocampus, the ventromedial PFC, the precuneus, and regions in the frontal lobe, while elaboration focuses on the inferior frontal gyrus. In line with these findings, the recruitment of the mPFC observed for the SenseCam pictures during long-term recollection-based retrievals (Milton et al., 2011) suggests that SenseCam paradigms would specifically enhance construction-based processes. Thus, it seems possible to resort to paradigms that use wearable cameras to study the formation of episodic autobiographical-like memories (Dubourg et al., 2016).

The present study aimed at predicting how personal, real-life events would be stored in long-term memory (i.e., remembered or forgotten) at short (5 days) and long (1 month) delays. The participants were asked to record two personal events a day with a wearable camera for 10 days. They characterized each event using different subjective scales (emotion, self-relevance, estimated memorability…). Two surprise free recall tests were done 5 days and 1 month after encoding, respectively. The second free recall was followed by a cued recall based on photos of the personal recordings. Based on previous studies, we expected a decline in memory recall over time and a strong substantial role for events’ self-related processes, emotional intensity, and valence in predicting the status of subsequent memories (forgotten or episodic) at a long delay.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

Twenty-five participants were recruited for the present study (recruitment period: January 1, 2018 to May 31, 2019). This number was based on previous studies on long-term memory using wearable cameras [e.g., Finley et al. (2011); Milton et al. (2011)]. Three older participants were excluded because they scored too low on the Mini-Mental State Evaluation (MMSE; ≤28; Folstein et al., 1975) or on the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; ≥10; Yesavage et al., 1982). Therefore, a total of 22 participants aged between 18 and 65 years old (M = 39.6, SD = 16.2, 59% female) took part in the study, each taking two pictures a day for 10 days, for a total maximum of 440 events. All the participants had to be free of physical diseases, neurological and psychiatric issues and be active and autonomous. The choice of this age group was motivated by plans to apply such an experimental paradigm in an aging study, that is, with different age groups. The participants provided written informed consent and freely took part in the study. The study received approval from the Paris Institute of Psychology local committee.

2.2 Material and procedure

The experiment consisted of three main phases (Figure 1). During the first phase (i.e., the encoding phase), the participants recorded and assessed daily events using a wearable camera SenseCam. The second and third phases consisted of memory recall tasks at 5 days and one-month intervals after the encoding phase. Participants were informed that the research intended to study daily life events, but no mention was made of subsequent memory retrieval. More specifically, the participants were told that for the second phase they would “talk [with the experimenter] about how the 10 days went, and make sure everything went smoothly” (translated from French), and for the third phase, they would come back to the lab for some final neuropsychological tests (control tests).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Procedure. During the encoding phase, the participants recorded two events a day for 10 days using a SenseCam wearable camera, that they rated with provided scales. They rated them again at the end of the first free recall, and after the second retrieval (free and cued recalls).

2.2.1 Encoding phase

Each participant was given one SenseCam wearable camera, alongside the experiment notebook, general guidelines and functioning instructions. SenseCam, developed by Microsoft Research, is a wearable camera with a wide-angle (fisheye) lens, ensuring a large point of view that encompasses everything the user sees. The user wears the camera around their neck, on a cord, for a first-person perspective. The pictures taken by SenseCam cameras are at VGA resolution (640×480 pixels) and are stored as.jpg on internal memory before being exported to a computer. SenseCam typically takes one picture every 30 s.

Participants had to select and record two events per day for 10 days by turning on the camera when a specific event happened, from its beginning to its end. More specifically, the participants were asked to “wear the SenseCam camera for 10 days to record two SPECIFIC events per day in your everyday life. Every day, you will choose TWO moments during which you think you will live something salient” (translated from French). Therefore, each participant had 20 memories encoded at the end of the encoding phase, providing a total of 440 memories (20 events x 22 participants).

The events had to be specific, that is, they had to target some significant activities and personal goals in everyday life. For instance, a walk in a park, cooking a recipe, a subway ride, grocery shopping, taking measures for new pieces of furniture, a family dinner, a work meeting, a visit to a museum, etc. would be considered as personal specific real-life events. At the end of each day, events were given short titles by the participants and assessed in the experiment notebook using 11 Likert scales ranging from 0 (‘no, not at all’) to 5 (‘yes, very much’) to identify the nature of the event and different aspects of recollective experience. These scales (listed in Table 1) were inspired by previous studies on EAM covering different retention delays [e.g., Piolino et al. (2004); Viard et al. (2007)]. They consisted in emotional intensity, emotional valence, self-relevance, self-concept, perspective, thinking, conversation, anticipated details, remembering, reliving, mental images. The encoding was incidental, as the subjects did not know that they would take two memory tests nor that the SenseCam images (that they could not see again) would be used for that purpose after a delay of 1 month.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List and description of the variables.

2.2.2 First recall

Four to 5 days after the encoding phase, participants had to give back all the material to the laboratory (notebook and wearable camera). They took part in a first surprise memory task (free recall). They were asked to recall aloud all the memories of the 20 events they had previously recorded with as many details as possible, such as the time and the location of the events, their perception and feelings of the events. At the end of the first recall, the participants assessed again the recalled events with the same subjective scales used at encoding (evaluation at the retrieval time). No feedback was given regarding the events that were not recalled.

2.2.3 Second recall

One month after the end of the encoding phase, participants came back to the lab under the pretense of answering some final questionnaires. They took part in a second surprise memory task. This time, the free recall was followed by a cued recall for the events that were not recalled. Participants’ own SenseCam recordings were used as cues. Pictures of the general location where the events had taken place, with some specific details regarding the event and the people involved and taking part in the action, were presented (Figure 2). All the pictures were taken from a first-person perspective so that the participants might have seen parts of their bodies in the images. The pictures had to be well-lit and clear (e.g., a picture of a table at a restaurant). After the last recall, the participants assessed the events again with the same subjective scales (evaluation at the retrieval time).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Examples of pictures taken by the participants using SenseCam.

2.2.4 Objective scoring of the episodic nature of the memories

The researchers used a standard scale of EAM at the first and second recall to rate the episodicity of memories which takes into account the specificity of the event: unicity, spatio-temporal context, and richness of details [adapted from the TEMPau task, Piolino et al. (2006); for a review see Piolino et al. (2009)]. The 7-point scoring grid assessed different elements: the unicity of the event (“what”), the temporal context of the event (“when”: general time, with the duration and broad dating, and more precise temporal details, with a sequential localization in time), its spatial context (“where”: general localization context, then more detailed position in the setting), contextual non-temporal and non-spatial details (who was there, how did the event happen, diverse anecdotes…), and phenomenological context (thoughts, emotions, perceptions). The main details of each event could be controlled and checked using the SenseCam recordings. Each event was scored on a maximum of 7 points (global EAM score). Events were further differentiated in three categories based on their global EAM score: scores from 4 to 7 classified them as specific (EPI), a score of 0 classified the event as not recalled (NR). Memories scored from 1 to 3 were considered too vague and generic to be classified as specific and were therefore classified as semantic (SEM). Because there were not enough events in this category, it was not used in the analysis.

2.3 Data analyses

The memories constituted the observations. As some participants did not record every event as the instructions required, the total dataset comprised 414 observations. We used two dependent variables in separate analyses. The first set of dependent variables was the global EAM score and subjective assessments; the second was the events’ memory status (i.e., NR, EPI). Missing data in the subjective assessments was replaced using a principal component analysis (PCA) to preserve the trends in the dataset.

To test how performance and assessments would evolve with time, we first conducted a series of analyses of variance (ANCOVAs) on all the measures, with the time of the session as the within-subject factor while keeping the age of the participants constant. To determine the direction of the differences in the interactions between phases, we ran out post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected tests. The effect sizes were reported with partial eta squared (η2). In line with Guéguen (2009), we considered effect sizes as small for η2 < 0.06, medium for 0.06 < η2 < 0.14 and large for η2 ≥ 0.14.

The machine learning (ML) algorithm took all encoding and first recall variables, the age and the global EAM score for the first free recall, to predict the category of the second free recall (episodic or EPI versus not recalled or NR). Given that this accounts for a considerable number of variables, and to avoid any risk of overfitting, they were clustered using a principal component analysis (PCA), and only the corresponding “mean variable” of each cluster was considered in the training and testing of the classifiers. The PCA provided a total of 5 stable clusters (Table 2). The dataset was then randomly split into two parts: the training set, which would allow the ML algorithms to learn the patterns in the data to make their predictions, and the validation or test set, which would enable checking how well the ML algorithms worked. 80% of the dataset went into the training set, and 20% into the validation set. This partition was done randomly but preserved the repartition of the classes (EPI and NR) in each set. The algorithms that were tested were linear discriminant analysis, classification and regression trees, k-nearest neighbors, and random forest. The exact performance depended on the algorithm, but the conclusions presented here remain the same regardless of the nature of the classifier.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Clusters for the ML algorithm as extracted with a PCA.

We used STATISTICA 12 (StatSoft, I, 2014) for the ANCOVAs and R (R-Core-Team, 2019) with the caret (Kuhn, 2008) and clustofvar (Chavent et al., 2012) packages for the rest of the analyses.

3 Results

After the one-month free recall, 144 of the 414 memories were recalled as episodic, that is, 34.8% of the events. 114 events were recalled only after the presentation of the visual cue. This number accounts for about a third (33.6%) of the episodic memories after the cued recall. In total, 339 events were classified as episodic memories for the cued recall, that is, 81.9% of the events. Table 3 summarizes the main results regarding the categories of the memories for the two free recalls. All the raw data are presented in Table 4 where the results of within-factor analyses are reported, controlling for the age of the participants. Overall, subjects had better memory scores (global EAM score) during the first free recall session (M = 3.64, SD = 2.26) than during the second one at a month (M = 3.02, SD = 2.37; r = 0.67, p < 0.05). However, when controlling for their age, the decrement was not significant anymore [F(1,412) = 0.22, p = 0.64]. Cued recall significantly improved the EAM score at 1 month [F(1,413) = 57.84, p < 0.001] and became a trend when controlling for the participants’ age [F(1,412) = 3.13, p = 0.077]. By contrast, most of the different scales used throughout the study were significantly impacted by time, independently of the age of the participants. Repeated measures ANCOVAs showed a significant effect of time (p < 0.05) for remembering, perspective, self-concept, thinking, conversation, anticipated details, reliving, and mental images. The scores declined from encoding to second recall regarding remembering, perspective, self-concept, thinking, reliving and mental images (vividness). Scores concerning the frequency of rehearsal through conversation were significantly inferior at the second recall compared to those of the encoding and first recall. Finally, scores of anticipated details at the one-month retrieval were higher at encoding than the first and second recalls. There was no time effect regarding emotion intensity, emotion valence, and self-relevance.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of the main results for the two recall phases.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. EAM and characteristics of memories from encoding to delayed retrieval and results of ANCOVAs controlling for the age of the participants.

Exploratory analyses based on ML were carried out to determine whether the memory status at a month could be predicted. The main goal was to assess how well a trained classifier algorithm could predict the nature of an event in memory at the long-term free recall (either well remembered, EPI, or not remembered, NR, see Methods: 144 episodic memories EPI at the one-month free recall versus 145 not recalled NR, for a total of 289 events). It was not possible to train classifiers on the cued recall as the difference between the numbers of episodic memories (339) and not recalled events (32) was too high. Overall, for the one-month free recall, the accuracy fell around 75% as a mean for all ML algorithms, and kappa around 0.50. The best algorithm was random forest (rf), with a classification’s mean accuracy of 78% (Table 5) and a Cohen’s kappa value of 0.56 (Table 6). The accuracy is satisfactory as the chance level here is about 50%. The kappa value, measuring the inter-trial reliability, can be considered a fair one, which is confirmed by the confusion matrix of the predictions on the validation dataset (Table 7). On the validation or test dataset, the model correctly predicted 25 of 28 episodic memories, and 17 of 29 not-recalled events, reaching a correct accuracy for both categories. The receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve (Figure 3) showed an area under the curve (AUC) of 0.772. Table 8 gives the respective weights of the clusters in the model: the best predictors among the phenomenal variables concerned anticipated long-term details, reliving details, mental images, and the self-reference at encoding and at the first free recall.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Accuracy of the ML model on the training set for the best algorithm (rf).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Cohen’s kappa values of the ML model on the training set for the best algorithm (rf).

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Confusion matrix of the prediction on the testing set of the ML model for the best algorithm (rf).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. ROC curve for the prediction in Table 7. AUC = 0.772.

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Scaled importance of the clusters in the final model.

4 Discussion

In daily life, the encoding of unique events relies on variables related to the idiosyncratic values of these events for a person. Determining whether the subsequent episodic memories are predictable, depending on the subjective variables at encoding, some being better predictors than others, could become an asset in memory research and therapy. The main aim of this study was to assess the predictability of real-life episodic memories (i.e., episodic autobiographical memories) based on a given dataset acquired using SenseCam technology. Different subjective characteristics of events were recorded during the encoding phase and two subsequent recalls to provide potential inputs to predict the final mnesic state of the—remembered or not—events at a delay of 1 month. The memory scores tended to decline depending on how much time had passed since encoding. Some phenomenological variables proved to be excellent predictors, such as the mental images, the reliving of details, the Self and the amount of anticipated details in memory at encoding and first recall; moreover, the age of the subjects had a strong influence on the dataset, which points to a role of age in highly ecological memories. Likewise, the memory performance at first recall impacted the prediction of the one-month recall, revealing the role of the consolidation process in this experiment. The final exploratory classifier model (random forest) had a 78% mean accuracy, providing some evidence that such predictions based mainly on subjective assessment could be possible.

In the present study, the decline of the memory score over the one-month time interval depended on the age of the participants. The influence of age is consistent with previous findings concerning EAM [see notably Faßbender et al. (2022), Mair et al. (2021), Sander et al. (2021), Armson et al. (2017), Martinelli et al. (2013a), Piolino et al. (2006)] as well as for the memory of new naturalistic events (Plancher et al., 2010; Abichou et al., 2019). Nevertheless, independently of the participants’ age, we found a decline of the subjective self-rated scales over time (e.g., remembering, first-person perspective, reliving details, vividness), which is consistent with previous literature, in particular regarding the recency effect of the EAM retention curve as first described by Rubin (1982) and further experimentally confirmed by Piolino et al. (2002) for instance. In previous SenseCam studies, Finley et al. (2011) also found a decreased picture-cued recall over time. By contrast, the emotion and the self-relevance remained stable over time and were limited predictors of long-term recall compared to other phenomenological measures. In general, item-emotion binding has a well-established role in memory encoding and consolidation [see for instance Allen et al. (2008), Sheldon et al. (2020), Talmi et al. (2019), Yonelinas and Ritchey (2015)], and so does self-relevance (Straube, 2012; Penaud et al., 2022). A possible explanation for this result lies in the very nature of the events recorded via SenseCam by the participants. The events were indeed part of their daily lives, and they thus might not have been emotional and salient enough to allow a more discriminant consolidation role of these variables from encoding to predict long-term retrieval.

The first recall memory score and the subjective assessments regarding anticipated long-term memory details, the reliving of details, and the mental images at both encoding and the first recall were the best predictors of long-term recall, proving their fundamental importance in building long-lasting EAM. This finding is in keeping with previous literature that subsequent recalls or updates of information (Alberini, 2005; Dudai, 2012) are important determinants of whether some information will be captured in long-term EM or forgotten. Moreover, it is noteworthy that mental images have a substantial role in the properties of EM (recollection of events with spatial–temporal context and perceptual details, see Tulving (1985)). In the same line, visual mental imagery provides rich contextual, sensorial and perceptual information that enhances access to specific autobiographical memories (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Aydin, 2018) and EM (Hussey et al., 2012; Barry et al., 2019) and the sense of Self (Lin, 2018). With the importance of the anticipated long-term details at encoding for long-term recall, the subjects also seemed to anticipate how the experienced event would be later remembered. This is a direct translation of the prospective brain and the ability of EM to project into the future (Buckner, 2007; La Corte et al., 2021). Prospection and future-oriented cognition describe the capacity to envision the possible future which encompasses episodic simulation, prediction, intention, and planning. The present result extends the importance of episodic prediction, which concerns the estimation of the likelihood of a specific episodic autobiographical future event, to the estimation of the nature of subsequent long-term EM. Other factors about metacognition assessments also play a role in this prediction, notably belief, which is distinct from recollection (Fitzgerald and Broadbridge, 2013), with a double dissociation including different factors predicting autobiographical belief and autobiographical recollection (Scoboria et al., 2014).

Other variables from the subjective assessments stood out as decent predictors, though less important than the previous ones, and seemed to point at the idiosyncratic criteria of selection in daily life by the subjects and at self-reference processes. Besides the role of actor-perspective and the sense of remembering via autonoetic consciousness at encoding and first recall, which are closely related to mental imagery and the reliving of details and the first-person perspective, the implication of the self-concept in predicting long-term recall highlighted the role of the Self in long-lasting EM or EAM (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Conway, 2005; Piolino et al., 2009; Staniloiu et al., 2020). In particular, Conway introduced the notion of working self which corresponds to a complex, dynamic and executive set that contains active goals and personal aspirations in the short and long term. Its central role is to maintain coherence between goals and memories. The link with the Self is a constant in long-term, autobiographical-like memories, as the core of the self-memory system (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Martinelli et al., 2013b). For 40 years, many experimental studies have shown the role of the Self in EM encoding using the manipulation of self-reference (relating a new piece of information in some way to oneself) in a laboratory context (Symons and Johnson, 1997; Klein, 2012) as an experimental simulation of the formation of EAM (Lalanne et al., 2013). In the present study using SenseCam, which can be considered as an ecological self-referential paradigm, the participants had to decide by themselves to record personal moments, even the most mundane ones, which were, thus, more or less explicitly linked to their self-images and current self-concepts (goals, values, desires…). Moreover, each recorded event involved the Self as an agent (‘I’) that was reexperiencing previous real-life personal events in a first-person perspective, a prerequisite for rich EM (Prebble et al., 2013; Bergouignan et al., 2014; Iriye and St. Jacques, 2021). The subjective assessment of the “perspective” element (actor vs. observer) was linked with the free recall score, in line with these elements. A recent study by Baldwin et al. (2021) investigated the effect on memory of the self-choice, that is, freely chosen events to remember versus assigned ones: self-choice improves memory, but prior actions reflecting self-control (for instance, consuming self-regulatory resources with a Stroop task), that is, ego depletion, reduce this influence. Interestingly, self-relevance was a limited predictor compared to the connection of events with self-concept, which may indicate a difference between those two notions related to the Self (Brechet et al., 2020; Morin and Racy, 2021; Tanguay et al., 2022). Another explanation could be that the events were not fully self-defining memories, as attested by the effect of the age found in our sample, which generally disappears in the case of memories highly related to the Self (Martinelli et al., 2013a).

Finally, event memory rehearsal at encoding and first recall appeared as a very limited predictor. Memory repetition, that is, thinking about the event and talking about it with others, is well known to be linked with the adaptative process of (re)consolidation (Nadel and Moscovitch, 1997; McGaugh, 2000; Wichert et al., 2011; Dudai, 2012; Squire et al., 2015; van Ekert et al., 2017; Yonelinas et al., 2019; Cowan et al., 2021); this link has notably been explored in relationship with mind-wandering (Girardeau et al., 2023). However, in our results, both the internal and external repetition were among the least strong predictors of the free recall memory score at a month. These predictors appeared in the same cluster as emotional intensity and self-relevance, hinting at the links between these measures for long-term EM [see, for instance, D'Argembeau et al. (2005), Kalenzaga et al. (2013), Durbin et al. (2017), Moses-Payne et al. (2022)]. With longer delays, internal and external repetitions could be more decisive in predicting long-term EM or its generalization or semanticization (Cermak, 1984).

To our knowledge, no other study using SenseCam tested different recall times with a focus on different subjective factors characterizing real-life events. The present study thus provides a first approach in this field, with a promising prediction of potential long-term memory episodicity of encoded events in ML. This naturalistic approach leads to consider long-lasting EM with its encoding variables in the most ecological setting possible. Indeed, using SenseCam allowed an experimental paradigm that was as ecological as possible in a real-life situation, as the participants kept on doing their daily activities, and freely chose the events they recorded. The naturalistic, ecological aspect of SenseCam paradigms has been checked by studies indicating similar neural activation as during EAM retrieval [see for instance Milton et al. (2011), Cabeza et al. (2004)]. Nonetheless, this study presents some limitations that should be considered in future studies. First, we addressed mundane real-life events and a long-term delay recall of a month. The predictors could differ with more remarkable events and/or longer delays. Second, distinct age cohorts would help disentangle the exact role of age in this context. Besides, adding other experimental groups with only one recall time would provide a comparison controlling for forced re-consolidation through testing. The further investigation of a longer delay for the cued recall could lead to decreased performance for the participants (Tulving et al., 1982), and thus allow the comparison of classifiers trained on the free recall, versus trained on the cued recall. All things considered, this dataset was best described by measures related to mental imagery (e.g., number of images, reliving details), self-reference (self-concept, autonoetic consciousness, first-person perspective), and prospection (anticipated details). The critical role of these factors for EAM consolidation suggest their potential role in enhancing the construction phase of retrieval over time, as regions associated with mental imagery, self-reference, integration of perceptual details, multisensory features, and social processes are selectively activated during this early phase (Daviddi et al., 2023). Investigating this finding using fMRI during the early and late phases of EAM retrieval at different retention delays from encoding would shed further light on this issue. Furthermore, it would be quite interesting to test the same algorithms with different datasets or, better yet, with datasets testing the ecological validity of the encoding environment, that is, comparing SenseCam with other ecological paradigms, such as virtual reality. Such a naturalistic approach would increase our understanding of the formation of autobiographical memory and the joint influences of the encoding variables on later recollection. From a clinical perspective, the results of this study suggest that instructing patients to pay particular attention to certain features of their real-life events as they occur (taking ‘mental pictures’, reflecting on how they relate to themselves, what they might remember in the future…) and then rehearsing them both mentally and verbally with other people could potentially lead to better memory performance. Rehabilitation and training using SenseCam usually focus on repeated review of recorded events to strengthen consolidation and retrieval, rather than better encoding (Woodberry et al., 2015; Mair et al., 2019). EAM rehabilitation could thus benefit from taking into account both the influence of the encoding and retrieval variables, to design protocols aiming at maximum efficiency with adapted procedures, and the importance of an ecological design to apply the protocols more easily to real-life situations (Dubourg et al., 2016; Silva et al., 2018).

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Paris Institute of Psychology local committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DL: Data curation, Formal analysis, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BF: Formal analysis, Software, Writing – review & editing. PP: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and publication of this article, with funding received from Faculté Sciences et Humanités de Université Paris Cité, and a support from an ANR Research Fund Grant (the Interplay between Memory and Temporality ANR-17-CE36-0009-01) awarded to PP.

Acknowledgments

We thank the students of the Université Paris Cité for their contribution in collecting the data of this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abichou, K., La Corte, V., Hubert, N., Orriols, E., Gaston-Bellegarde, A., Nicolas, S., et al. (2019). Young and older adults benefit from sleep, but not from active wakefulness for memory consolidation of what-where-when naturalistic events. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alberini, C. M. (2005). Mechanisms of memory stabilization: are consolidation and reconsolidation similar or distinct processes? Trends Neurosci. 28, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.11.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, P. A., Kaut, K. P., and Lord, R. R. (2008). “Chapter 1.8 emotion and episodic memory” in Handbook of behavioral neuroscience. eds. E. Dere, A. Easton, L. Nadel, and J. P. Huston (United States of America: Elsevier), 115–132.

Google Scholar

Armson, M. J., Abdi, H., and Levine, B. (2017). Bridging naturalistic and laboratory assessment of memory: the Baycrest mask fit test. Memory 25, 999–1008. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1241281

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Audrain, S., Gilmore, A. W., Wilson, J. M., Schacter, D. L., and Martin, A. (2022). A role for the anterior Hippocampus in autobiographical memory construction regardless of temporal distance. J. Neurosci. 42, 6445–6452. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0832-22.2022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aydin, C. (2018). The differential contributions of visual imagery constructs on autobiographical thinking. Memory 26, 189–200. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1340483

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, C., Garrison, K. E., Baumeister, R. F., and Schmeichel, B. J. (2021). Making memorable choices: cognitive control and the self-choice effect in memory. Self Identity 21, 363–386. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2021.1888787

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, D. N., Barnes, G. R., Clark, I. A., and Maguire, E. A. (2019). The neural dynamics of novel scene imagery. J. Neurosci. 39, 4375–4386. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2497-18.2019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergouignan, L., Nyberg, L., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). Out-of-body–induced hippocampal amnesia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 4421–4426. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318801111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, M., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2008). “Chapter 3.5 the role of the prefrontal cortex in episodic memory” in Handbook of behavioral neuroscience. eds. E. Dere, A. Easton, L. Nadel, and J. P. Huston, (United States of America: Elsevier), 317–619.

Google Scholar

Brechet, L., Hausmann, S. B., Mange, R., Herbelin, B., Blanke, O., and Serino, A. (2020). Subjective feeling of re-experiencing past events using immersive virtual reality prevents a loss of episodic memory. Brain Behav. 10:e01571. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1571

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2007). Prospection and the brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 30, 318–319. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07002038

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., Spiers, H. J., and O'Keefe, J. (2001). A Temporoparietal and prefrontal network for retrieving the spatial context of lifelike events. NeuroImage 14, 439–453. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0806

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Prince, S. E., Daselaar, S. M., Greenberg, D. L., Budde, M., Dolcos, F., et al. (2004). Brain activity during episodic retrieval of autobiographical and laboratory events: an fMRI study using a novel photo paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1583–1594. doi: 10.1162/0898929042568578

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cermak, L.S. (1984). "the episodic-semantic distinction in amnesia," in neuropsychology of memory, eds. L.R. Squire & N. Butters. (New York: Guilford Press).

Google Scholar

Chavent, M., Kuentz-Simonet, V., Liquet, B., and Saracco, J. (2012). ClustOfVar: an R package for the clustering of variables. J. Stat. Softw. 50, 1–16. doi: 10.18637/jss.v050.i13

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, T. E., and Rissman, J. (2017). Neurocognitive mechanisms of real-world autobiographical memory retrieval: insights from studies using wearable camera technology. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1396, 202–221. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13353

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53, 594–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A. (2009). Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia 47, 2305–2313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A., and Pleydell-Pearce, C. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol. Rev. 107, 261–288. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.107.2.261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, E. T., Schapiro, A. C., Dunsmoor, J. E., and Murty, V. P. (2021). Memory consolidation as an adaptive process. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 28, 1796–1810. doi: 10.3758/s13423-021-01978-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Argembeau, A., Comblain, C., and Van der Linden, M. (2005). Affective valence and the self-reference effect: influence of retrieval conditions. Br. J. Psychol. 96, 457–466. doi: 10.1348/000712605x53218

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Daviddi, S., Pedale, T., St Jacques, P. L., Schacter, D. L., and Santangelo, V. (2023). Common and distinct correlates of construction and elaboration of episodic-autobiographical memory: an ALE meta-analysis. Cortex 163, 123–138. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2023.03.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubourg, L., Silva, A. R., Fitamen, C., Moulin, C. J., and Souchay, C. (2016). SenseCam: a new tool for memory rehabilitation? Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 172, 735–747. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2016.03.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudai, Y. (2012). The restless engram: consolidations never end. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 227–247. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Durbin, K. A., Mitchell, K. J., and Johnson, M. K. (2017). Source memory that encoding was self-referential: the influence of stimulus characteristics. Memory 25, 1191–1200. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1282517

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faßbender, R. V., Risius, O. J., Dronse, J., Richter, N., Gramespacher, H., Befahr, Q., et al. (2022). Decreased efficiency of between-network dynamics during early memory consolidation with aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:780630. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.780630

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Finley, J. R., Brewer, W. F., and Benjamin, A. S. (2011). The effects of end-of-day picture review and a sensor-based picture capture procedure on autobiographical memory using SenseCam. Memory (Hove, England) 19, 796–807. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2010.532807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, J. M., and Broadbridge, C. L. (2013). Latent constructs of the autobiographical memory questionnaire: a recollection-belief model of autobiographical experience. Memory 21, 230–248. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.725736

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foudil, S. A., Pleche, C., and Macaluso, E. (2021). Memory for spatio-temporal contextual details during the retrieval of naturalistic episodes. Sci. Rep. 11:14577. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93960-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelonch, O., Cano, N., Vancells, M., Bolaños, M., Farràs-Permanyer, L., and Garolera, M. (2020). The effects of exposure to recent autobiographical events on declarative memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a preliminary pilot study. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 17, 158–167. doi: 10.2174/1567205017666200317093341

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Girardeau, J. C., Ledru, R., Gaston-Bellegarde, A., Blondé, P., Sperduti, M., and Piolino, P. (2023). The benefits of mind wandering on a naturalistic prospective memory task. Sci. Rep. 13:11432. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-37996-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guéguen, N. (2009). The importance of an effect: simple methodology for determining and evaluating the "effect size" [L'importance d'un effet: Méthodologie simple de détermination et d'évaluation de « l'effect size »]. Eur. J. Sci. Res. 38, 20–25. doi: 10.1684/san.2011.0240

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, S., Berry, E., and Wood, K. (2011). SenseCam: a wearable camera that stimulates and rehabilitates autobiographical memory. Memory (Hove, England) 19, 685–696. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.605591

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holland, A. C., Addis, D. R., and Kensinger, E. A. (2011). The neural correlates of specific versus general autobiographical memory construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 49, 3164–3177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hussey, E. P., Smolinsky, J. G., Piryatinsky, I., Budson, A. E., and Ally, B. A. (2012). Using mental imagery to improve memory in patients with Alzheimer disease: trouble generating or remembering the mind's eye? Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 26, 124–134. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31822e0f73

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iriye, H., and St Jacques, P. L. (2021). Memories for third-person experiences in immersive virtual reality. Sci. Rep. 11:4667. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84047-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeunehomme, O., and D'Argembeau, A. (2019). The time to remember: temporal compression and duration judgements in memory for real-life events. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 72, 930–942. doi: 10.1177/1747021818773082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalenzaga, S., Bugaïska, A., and Clarys, D. (2013). Self-reference effect and autonoetic consciousness in Alzheimer disease: evidence for a persistent affective self in dementia patients. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 27, 116–122. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318257dc31

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khachatoorian, N., Loveday, C., Dima, D., Mair, A., Illingworth, S., Conway, M. A., et al. (2021). A behavioural and ERP investigation of the wearable camera photo review in autobiographical memory. Memory 29, 224–233. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2021.1880601

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. (2018). Parietal control network activation during memory tasks may be associated with the co-occurrence of externally and internally directed cognition: a cross-function meta-analysis. Brain Res. 1683, 55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.01.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, S. B. (2012). Self, memory, and the self-reference effect: an examination of conceptual and methodological issues. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 283–300. doi: 10.1177/1088868311434214

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, M. (2008). Building predictive models in R using the caret package. J. Stat. Softw. 28, 1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v028.i05

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

La Corte, V., Ferrieux, S., Abram, M., Bertrand, A., Dubois, B., Teichmann, M., et al. (2021). The role of semantic memory in prospective memory and episodic future thinking: new insights from a case of semantic dementia. Memory 29, 943–962. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2021.1936069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lalanne, J., Rozenberg, J., Grolleau, P., and Piolino, P. (2013). The self-reference effect on episodic memory recollection in young and older adults and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Alzhei. Res. 10, 1107–1117. doi: 10.2174/15672050113106660175

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y. T. (2018). Visual perspectives in episodic memory and the sense of self. Front. Psychol. 9:2196. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mair, A., Poirier, M., and Conway, M. A. (2017). Supporting older and younger adults' memory for recent everyday events: a prospective sampling study using SenseCam. Conscious. Cogn. 49, 190–202. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mair, A., Poirier, M., and Conway, M. A. (2019). Memory for staged events: supporting older and younger adults' memory with SenseCam. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 72, 717–728. doi: 10.1177/1747021818765038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mair, A., Poirier, M., and Conway, M. A. (2021). Age effects in autobiographical memory depend on the measure. PLoS One 16:e0259279. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259279

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Markowitsch, H. J., Vandekerckhove, M. M., Lanfermann, H., and Russ, M. O. (2003). Engagement of lateral and medial prefrontal areas in the ecphory of sad and happy autobiographical memories. Cortex 39, 643–665. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70858-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinelli, P., Anssens, A., Sperduti, M., and Piolino, P. (2013a). The influence of normal aging and Alzheimer's disease in autobiographical memory highly related to the self. Neuropsychology 27, 69–78. doi: 10.1037/a0030453

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinelli, P., Sperduti, M., and Piolino, P. (2013b). Neural substrates of the self-memory system: new insights from a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1515–1529. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, C., Moscovitch, M., Valiante, T. A., Cohn, M., and McAndrews, M. P. (2018). Different neural routes to autobiographical memory recall in healthy people and individuals with left medial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 110, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.08.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory--a century of consolidation. Science 287, 248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milton, F., Muhlert, N., Butler, C. R., Smith, A., Benattayallah, A., and Zeman, A. Z. (2011). An fMRI study of long-term everyday memory using SenseCam. Memory 19, 733–744. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.552185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, A., and Racy, F. (2021). “Chapter 15 - dynamic self-processes” in The handbook of personality dynamics and processes. ed. J. F. Rauthmann (US: Academic Press), 365–386.

Google Scholar

Moscovitch, M., Cabeza, R., Winocur, G., and Nadel, L. (2016). Episodic memory and beyond: the Hippocampus and neocortex in transformation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 105–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143733

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moses-Payne, M. E., Chierchia, G., and Blakemore, S. J. (2022). Age-related changes in the impact of valence on self-referential processing in female adolescents and young adults. Cogn. Dev. 61:101128. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101128

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadel, L., and Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 217–227. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80010-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., and Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain--a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage 31, 440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oddo, S., Lux, S., Weiss, P. H., Schwab, A., Welzer, H., Markowitsch, H. J., et al. (2010). Specific role of medial prefrontal cortex in retrieving recent autobiographical memories: an fMRI study of young female subjects. Cortex 46, 29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pathman, T., Samson, Z., Dugas, K., Cabeza, R., and Bauer, P. J. (2011). A "snapshot" of declarative memory: differing developmental trajectories in episodic and autobiographical memory. Memory 19, 825–835. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.613839

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Penaud, S., Jebara, N., Zaoui, M., Orriols, E., Berthoz, A., and Piolino, P. (2022). Episodic memory and self-reference in a naturalistic context: new insights based on a virtual walk in the Latin quarter of Paris. J. Environ. Psychol. 81:101801. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101801

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Benali, K., and Eustache, F. (2002). Episodic and semantic remote autobiographical memory in ageing. Memory 10, 239–257. doi: 10.1080/09658210143000353

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Clarys, D., Guillery-Girard, B., Taconnat, L., Isingrini, M., et al. (2006). Autobiographical memory, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective in aging. Psychol. Aging 21, 510–525. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., and Eustache, F. (2009). Episodic autobiographical memories over the course of time: cognitive, neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. Neuropsychologia 47, 2314–2329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piolino, P., Giffard-Quillon, G., Desgranges, B., Chételat, G., Baron, J. C., and Eustache, F. (2004). Re-experiencing old memories via hippocampus: a PET study of autobiographical memory. NeuroImage 22, 1371–1383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plancher, G., Gyselinck, V., Nicolas, S., and Piolino, P. (2010). Age effect on components of episodic memory and feature binding: a virtual reality study. Neuropsychology 24, 379–390. doi: 10.1037/a0018680

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prebble, S. C., Addis, D. R., and Tippett, L. J. (2013). Autobiographical memory and sense of self. Psychol. Bull. 139, 815–840. doi: 10.1037/a0030146

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

R-Core-Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.

Google Scholar

Roediger, H. L., and Marsh, E. J. (2003). “Episodic and autobiographical memory” in Handbook of psychology: Experimental psychology, vol. 4 eds. A. F. Healy and R. W. Proctor (Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc), 475–497.

Google Scholar

Rubin, D. C. (1982). On the retention function for autobiographical memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 21, 21–38. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90423-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, M. C., Fandakova, Y., and Werkle-Bergner, M. (2021). Effects of age differences in memory formation on neural mechanisms of consolidation and retrieval. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 116, 135–145. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2021.02.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scoboria, A., Jackson, D. L., Talarico, J., Hanczakowski, M., Wysman, L., and Mazzoni, G. (2014). The role of belief in occurrence within autobiographical memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 1242–1258. doi: 10.1037/a0034110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Serino, S., and Repetto, C. (2018). New trends in episodic memory assessment: immersive 360° ecological videos. Front. Psychol. 9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, S., Williams, K., Harrington, S., and Otto, A. R. (2020). Emotional cue effects on accessing and elaborating upon autobiographical memories. Cognition 198:104217. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104217

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, A. R., Pinho, M. S., Macedo, L., and Moulin, C. J. A. (2018). A critical review of the effects of wearable cameras on memory. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 28, 117–141. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2015.1128450

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonkusare, S., Breakspear, M., and Guo, C. (2019). Naturalistic stimuli in neuroscience: critically acclaimed. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 699–714. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., Genzel, L., Wixted, J. T., and Morris, R. G. (2015). Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:a021766. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021766

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

St Jacques, P. L., and Schacter, D. L. (2013). Modifying memory: selectively enhancing and updating personal memories for a museum tour by reactivating them. Psychol. Sci. 24, 537–543. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457377

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Staniloiu, A., Kordon, A., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2020). Quo vadis 'episodic memory'? - past, present, and perspective. Neuropsychologia 141:107362. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

StatSoft, I. (2014). Statistica (data analysis software system), version 12. Tulsa: United States.

Google Scholar

Straube, B. (2012). An overview of the neuro-cognitive processes involved in the encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of true and false memories. Behav. Brain Funct. 8:35. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-35

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Summerfield, J. J., Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2009). Cortical midline involvement in autobiographical memory. NeuroImage 44, 1188–1200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Symons, C. S., and Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 121, 371–394. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Talmi, D., Lohnas, L. J., and Daw, N. D. (2019). A retrieved context model of the emotional modulation of memory. Psychol. Rev. 126, 455–485. doi: 10.1037/rev0000132

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanguay, A. F. N., Johnen, A. K., Markostamou, I., Lambert, R., Rudrum, M., Davidson, P. S. R., et al. (2022). The ERP correlates of self-knowledge in ageing. Mem. Cogn. 50, 564–585. doi: 10.3758/s13421-021-01225-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychol. Psychologie Canadienne 26, 1–12. doi: 10.1037/h0080017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E., Schacter, D. L., and Stark, H. A. (1982). Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 8, 336–342. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.8.4.336

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ekert, J., Wegman, J., Jansen, C., Takashima, A., and Janzen, G. (2017). The dynamics of memory consolidation of landmarks. Hippocampus 27, 393–404. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Viard, A., Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Chételat, G., Lebreton, K., Landeau, B., et al. (2007). Hippocampal activation for autobiographical memories over the entire lifetime in healthy aged subjects: an fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2453–2467. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichert, S., Wolf, O. T., and Schwabe, L. (2011). Reactivation, interference, and reconsolidation: are recent and remote memories likewise susceptible? Behav. Neurosci. 125, 699–704. doi: 10.1037/a0025235

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodberry, E., Browne, G., Hodges, S., Watson, P., Kapur, N., and Woodberry, K. (2015). The use of a wearable camera improves autobiographical memory in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Memory 23, 340–349. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.886703

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 17, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonelinas, A. P., Ranganath, C., Ekstrom, A. D., and Wiltgen, B. J. (2019). A contextual binding theory of episodic memory: systems consolidation reconsidered. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 364–375. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0150-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonelinas, A. P., and Ritchey, M. (2015). The slow forgetting of emotional episodic memories: an emotional binding account. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 259–267. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: episodic memory, SenseCam, consolidation, episodic autobiographical memory, sense of self, machine learning

Citation: Lenormand D, Fauvel B and Piolino P (2024) The formation of episodic autobiographical memory is predicted by mental imagery, self-reference, and anticipated details. Front. Psychol. 15:1355343. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1355343

Received: 13 December 2023; Accepted: 08 February 2024;
Published: 27 February 2024.

Edited by:

Hui Chen, Zhejiang University, China

Reviewed by:

Francis Eustache, Université Paris Sciences et Lettres, France
Valerio Santangelo, University of Perugia, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Lenormand, Fauvel and Piolino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Diane Lenormand, diane.lenormand@etu.u-paris.fr; Pascale Piolino, pascale.piolino@u-paris.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.