Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 June 2024
Sec. Pediatric Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Theory of Mind in Relation to Other Cognitive Abilities - Volume II View all 9 articles

Weak central coherence in neurodevelopmental disorders: a comparative study

  • 1Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 2Pediatric Neurology Unit, Clinic University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 3Facultad de Educación, Universidad Internacional de La Rioja, Logroño, Spain

Introduction: Central coherence is the normal tendency to process and give meaning to incoming information taking into account the context or global view of that information.

Methods: We assessed the central coherence of 252 school children of normal intelligence between 6 and 11 years old. We compared the performance of two groups: (a) a control group (n = 194), and (b) a clinical group (n = 58) comprising children with NVLD+ADHD (n = 24), ADHD alone (n = 16), SCD (n = 8) and level-1ASD (n = 10) (Kluskall-Wallis H and Mann-Whitney U were calculated to make comparisons within groups and between pairs of groups). The effects of medication were studied (Student’s t test).

Results: The NVLD+ADHD, SCD and ASD1 groups showed weak central coherence. The performance of the ADHD group was normal and differed significantly from the NVLD+ADHD group.

Conclusion: Central coherence deficit was not exclusive to ASD1: it also characterizes NVLD and SCD.

1 Introduction

The world is perceived as being hierarchically organized and includes global perceptions comprising local details (D’Souza et al., 2016); a human being is able to process information at both global and local levels. This capacity, which is denominated central coherence, is implied in everyday activities such as categorization, inspection of details in our surroundings, perception of the structure of a scene and analysis of information (Nayar et al., 2017). Central coherence is, therefore, the normal tendency to process and give meaning, in a global manner, to incoming information in its context (Noens and van Berckelaer-Onnes, 2008). It is thanks to central coherence that when we receive a message, we prioritize comprehension of meaning and not just the literal form (Crespo-Eguílaz et al., 2012).

Attwood (Attwood, 2007) provides a metaphor that may be useful to understand the nature of weak central coherence: “imagine rolling up a sheet of paper to form a tube and with one eye closed bring it up to other open eye, as if it were a telescope, and look at the world through it: you see the details but do not perceive the context.” A person with difficulty in central coherence (that is, a preference for analytical processing rather than global processing) has specific difficulties in simultaneously processing information perceived and in giving coherent, integral meaning to that information. The cognitive style of such a person is, therefore, characterized by a tendency to process details (Navon, 1977; Schooler, 2002; Förster and Dannenberg, 2010; Crespo-Eguílaz and Narbona, 2011) This style of processing is slower and more demanding from the cognitive point of view (Nayar et al., 2017). An ability to carry out tasks requiring central coherence rapidly is of fundamental importance in learning and in social behavior (Crespo-Eguílaz et al., 2012). Therefore, weakness in central coherence entails difficulties in contextual comprehension of social situations and in adaption to these situations (López and Leekam, 2007). Difficulties in central coherence are also described by (Lamb and Robertson, 1989) as local bias and by (Vermeulen, 2015), in his studies on people with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), as context blindness. To date, dysfunction in central coherence has principally been studied in ASD (López and Leekam, 2007), although it has also been described in other disorders, such as, Down’s syndrome, Williams syndrome (D’Souza et al., 2016) non-verbal learning disorder (NVLD) (Gillberg, 2003, 2009; Crespo-Eguílaz and Narbona, 2011); and thus weak central coherence is apparently not specific to ASD.

1.1 Central coherence in autism spectrum disorder

It has been demonstrated that people with ASD tend to process information in a manner that is more focussed on details than on the overall meaning, that is, they have weak central coherence (Frith, 1992; Nydén et al., 2011). Theory concerning central coherence is supported by studies that confirm that, in tests of local preference, people with ASD perform significantly better than people of normotypical development (Shah and Frith, 1983; Jolliffe and Baron-Cohen, 1997; Plaisted et al., 1998; O’Riordan et al., 2001; Pellicano et al., 2006), which demonstrates that people with ASD tend to focus their attention more on parts of objects than on the objects themselves (Ornitz et al., 1977). Other research has shown that subjects with ASD perform poorly in tests of global preference (Mottron and Belleville, 1993; Rinehart et al., 2000), that is, in tests that include tasks that require detection of relatively small visual elements embedded in large fields (Caron et al., 2006), of visual searching (O’Riordan et al., 2001), of pattern discrimination (Plaisted et al., 1998; Bertone et al., 2005) or that involve design of blocks, impossible figures or embedded figures (Happé and Frith, 2006). Some studies find that in ASD subjects there is hyperfunction of brain areas generally involved in primary perception in contrast to perceptual integration, and researchers have proposed that this hyperfunction might be the explanation for the perceptual endophenotype in autism. The special abilities of the so-called autistic savant and the variability across the spectrum of ASD are possible manifestations of a tendency to use primary perceptual functions (Mottron et al., 2006). Various studies of people with “high-functioning autism” have led to conclusions compatible with this hypothesis (Bertone et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2007; Koldewyn et al., 2013; Syriopoulou Delli et al., 2016): participants with autism perform well at tasks involving perception of faces in stationary images (photos of faces) (Lahaie et al., 2006), at tasks requiring perception of movement (Bertone et al., 2004) and at the Wechsler scale Cubes test (Caron et al., 2006).

A modified version of the central coherence theory has been proposed in which it is hypothesised that in individuals with ASD the bias towards local processing can be overcome when doing tasks with explicit demands for global processing (Happé and Frith, 2006). According to this model, people with ASD do not necessarily have difficulties in perceiving the global form of things but rather have an over-specialized perceptual system that, depending on the requirements of a task, can interfere with higher-level cognition (Mottron and Burack, 2001; Caron et al., 2006).

Finally, despite otherwise contradictory findings (Happé, 1999), it has been established that the local-precedence style of information processing is not universally present across the whole autism spectrum (Happé, 1999).

1.2 Central coherence in non-verbal learning disorder

Children with non-verbal learning disorder (NVLD)−also denominated Deficits in Attention, Motor control and Perception (DAMP) and Procedural Learning Disorder (PLD) (Crespo-Eguílaz and Narbona, 2009)−show signs of weak central coherence. Such children can get lost in details rather than process information in an integrated and correct way (Doty, 2019). They find it difficult or are slow to arrive at a coherent comprehension of complex images or scenarios (Crespo-Eguílaz and Narbona, 2009; Magallón, 2011; Crespo-Eguílaz et al., 2012). They tend not to understand globally but rather in parts, which makes it difficult for them to carry out integration of concepts and abstraction and, therefore, to make correct adaptation of understanding to context (Díaz Lucero et al., 2011); they have difficulty perceiving globally, analyzing, organizing and summarizing information (Chow and Skuy, 1999; Molenaar-Klumper, 2002; Mammarella and Pazzaglia, 2010). In addition, it has been found that these children can perform poorly in certain visual perception tasks (for example, in perceiving the spatial location of objects), and have difficulties in recognizing what is detail, simultaneous processing, combining parts into a whole, and visual-spatial organization (Schoemaker et al., 2001; Drumond et al., 2005). Sometimes, children with NVLD make errors in spatial perception, for example, mistaking places in their surroundings or in the position of a person relative to themselves (Viñuela, 2007). Consequently, they find it difficult to cope with novel environments and to solve problems that have a visual-spatial component (González, 2017). They tend to get lost in open, unstructured situations in which conversations often overlap, there is more use of colloquial language, many gestures are used and body distances need to be managed (Foss, 2001). They evidence both deficits in the comprehension of extra-verbal information (facial expressions, gestures, mimicry, body postures, prosodic inflections and other visual aspects of their circumstances) and also difficulties in integrating and understanding such information (Humphries et al., 2004; Mammarella et al., 2009). They find it challenging to adapt to novel situations and tend to make generalisations based on specific verbal utterances, without taking into account the context in which a conversation is taking place (Worling et al., 1999).

The above-mentioned impediments have a major impact on people with NVLD when it comes to giving meaning to different contexts and interpreting discourse and affects the contextualisation of language. They find it difficult to understand figurative language, irony and jokes; they may interpret language literally and have problems adapting to novel situations of social interaction (Semrud-Clikeman and Hynd, 1990; Colomé Roura et al., 2009; Narbona et al., 2011). Consequently, people with NVLD are unable to communicate effectively in everyday situations (Colomé Roura et al., 2009) and experience difficulties in social relations.

1.3 Central coherence in attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder

Booth and Happé (Booth and Happé, 2010) compared how well participants with ADHD, with ASD and with normotypical development performed at completing sentences or phrases (for example, “Hunting with a knife and … fork”); they found that the participants with ADHD correctly performed at this task, while the ASD group performed significantly worse than the control group.

In a study by Crespo-Eguílaz et al. (2012), 200 school children−20 with NVLD, 60 with ADHD, 60 with both NVLD and ADHD, and 60 controls - were given a test involving a chimerical image and an incoherent visual scene. Of the children with ADHD, only 8% failed in rapid interpretation of the chimerical image, and only 7% performed poorly in comprehension of the visual scene. In a similar research (Magallón, 2011), found that only 13% of schoolchildren with ADHD performed badly in the chimerical image task. Similar findings are founded by different authors (Booth et al., 2003; Zhang and Adipat, 2005; Pina et al., 2013).

1.4 Central coherence in social communication disorder

Children with SCD tend to interpret language literally and not to detect irony, inference and/or metaphors (Bishop and Rosenbloom, 1987; Bishop and Adams, 1992; Leinonen and Letts, 1997; Bishop, 2000; Mulas Delgado et al., 2006; Velarde et al., 2017). They present problems in adapting their language to the needs of the listener or the situation, and they also lack flexibility when topics change during a dialogue (Rapin and Allen, 1983; McTear, 1985; Conti-Ramsden and Gunn, 1986; Bishop, 2000; Mendoza Lara and Muñoz López, 2005; González et al., 2015).

In the DSM-5 criteria for SCD (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), defining characteristics of the disorder are deficiency in the use of communication for social purposes (such as, greeting people and sharing information in a manner appropriate to the social context or to the needs of the person listening) and difficulty understanding what is not said in an explicit manner and what is non-literal or ambiguous (for example, idioms and humor) (Monfort, 2001; Baixauli-Fortea et al., 2004; Perkins, 2010; Monfort Juárez Centro Entender Hablar I, 2013; Martínez Alonso et al., 2021).

Given this review of the literature, our objective is to confirm that schoolchildren with ASD and NVLD experience difficulties with central coherence. Additionally, we aim to test whether children with SCD exhibit weak central coherence, a hypothesis suggested by our clinical observations but not yet empirically verified. Furthermore, we will examine the performance of children with ADHD concerning this construct and determine whether the central coherence deficit observed in children with NVLD can be explained by their attention difficulties and/or hyperactivity.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

The study sample was of 252 participants comprising a control group of 194 normotypical schoolchildren and 58 children with clinical disorders recruited at the neuropediatrics unit of the Clínica Universidad de Navarra hospital. The disorders were non-verbal learning disorder in conjunction with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (NVLD + ADHD; n = 24); ADHD (n = 16); level 1 autism spectrum disorder (ASD1 = 10) and social communication disorder (SCD; n = 8). All children were at primary school, between 6 and 11 years old (Table 1), and had typical intelligence as evaluated by Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test (2001) (Table 1). The children were from families of middle to middle-high socio-economic and cultural level on the Hollingshead scale (Hollingshead, 1957). All the children were Caucasian in race.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sex distribution and IQ-related statistics for the study sample.

The proportions of boys and girls and IQ-related statistics are given in Table 1. IQ data was not available for the control group; exclusion criteria for the control group included low academic performance, learning difficulty or behavioural disorders as determined, at the time of the study, by teachers and other specialist education professionals. 45.8% of the participants with NVLD+ADHD and 62.5% of those with ADHD were receiving pharmacological treatment with methylphenidate to improve attention (Table 4). The questionnaire and methodology for this study was approved by the Human Research Ethics committee of the University of Navarra (Ethics approval number: 2017.004mod1).

Written informed consent was obtained from the parents (Tables 1, 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Age distribution of the sample studied.

2.2 Tools and procedures

All children took the Central Coherence Test (CCT) (Gambra, 2020), which is an in-house development comprised of 36 items grouped in four dimensions, each of which has, in turn, different visual and verbal sub-tests or tasks (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Dimensions and subtests of the Central Coherence Test.

Previous research has demonstrated the validity and reliability of the CCT (Gambra, 2020) by means of reliability analysis (Cronbach’s alpha and Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient) and establishing construct validity (exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis and other studies), and establishing convergent and discriminant validity.

The dimensions that make up the test, as well as the subtests of each dimension, are as follows:

(a) Inference: The tasks that make up this dimension assess the ability to give meaning to different sentences and texts according to the contextual situation in which they exist, as well as the ability to understand figurative language, irony and jokes.

a. Irony: assesses the understanding of various ironic comments made in three different everyday situations.

b. Literality: assesses the ability to understand the non-literal meaning of a series of sentences on the basis of the context in which each sentence occurs.

c. Verbal story-telling: assesses ability to provide a coherent ending to incomplete stories.

(b) Verbal detail: this dimension evaluates ability to detect incoherent features in different situations presented verbally.

a. Nonsense questions: assesses ability to detect coherence or incoherence in a series of nonsense questions.

b. Nonsense sentences: assesses ability to detect inconsistency in sentences that are grammatically correct but that are inconsistent in terms of content. The nonsense sentences are mixed in with a series of sensible sentences, which serve as distractors.

(c) Simultaneity: this dimension assesses the ability and speed of the schoolchild in making sense of an inconsistent situation: a series of non-coherent images and texts.

a. Chimerical images: these evaluate simultanagnosia. After seeing each chimeric image for two seconds, the child is asked whether he/she has identified both, one or none of the animals or objects that make up the chimera. The child is also asked to describe the inconsistency between the two components.

b. Inconsistent pictures: these are used to evaluate ability to orally describe illustrated actions that are inherently inconsistent.

c. Hidden numbers and colours: this test evaluates ability to perceive details (numbers and colours) within a whole (a story). The test is done both visually (the child sees and reads the story) and aurally (the child hears the story).

(d) Context: the subtests in this dimension evaluate ability to understand and freely describe, orally, a series of images and audio recordings in which various actions are represented.

a. Inconsistent visual scenes: these evaluate ability to detect incongruities in various images.

b. The phone call: this is a series of role-play situations to assess ability to adapt to different contexts (Table 3).

We used Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro-Wilk tests to determine whether variables had a normal distribution. In addition Levene’s test was used to assess whether variances for the different groups were equal. For each clinical group, values for each test variable were converted to typified scores (for subtests and dimensions of the CCT, and for the CCT as a whole). For each group the performance profile was prepared. The non-parametric Kluskall-Wallis H and Mann-Whitney U were calculated to make comparisons within groups and between pairs of groups. Also, in order to determine whether medication influences test performance, we used Student’s t test to compare the mean performance of participants under medication with the mean performance of those who were not.

3 Results

There were no significant differences in performance between medicated and non-medicated participants. This was the case for all dimensions of the CCT and for all tasks. The finding applies to the two clinical groups in which there were patients under pharmacological treatment: the NVLD+ADHD group and the ADHD group (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Influence of medication on performance in dimensions and tasks of the Central Coherence Test.

There were no significant differences between clinical groups or the control group in performance in the Verbal detail dimension.

With regard to the Simultaneity dimension, the performance levels of the NVLD+ADHD group and the SCD group were significantly poorer than that of the control group (Figure 1); the effect size is high (Table 5). Performance levels of the ADHD and ASD1 groups were typical (Figure 1). There were significant differences in this respect between the NVLD+ADHD group and the ADHD and ASD1 groups (Figure 1; Table 5).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Comparison of performance levels of the clinical groups in the dimensions of the CCT and in the CCT as a whole.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Comparisons of pairs of groups in performance in the dimensions of Inference, Simultaneity and Context and in overall CCT performance.

The performance of NVLD+ADHD and ASD1 groups in the Inference dimension was significantly lower than that of the control group, while the performance of the ADHD group was typical. The effect size of the differences was high (Table 5). The performance of the SCD group was also apparently lower, but this difference was not statistically significant (Figure 1).

In the Context dimension, the mean performance levels of the NVLD+ADHD group and the ASD1 group were significantly lower than that of the control group (Figure 1 and Table 5). Mean performance of subjects in ADHD and SCD groups was adequate. The effect size was high for all comparisons.

Finally, for the CCT as a whole, mean performance levels of the NVLD+ADHD group and of the SCD group were significantly lower (with large effect size) than that of the control group, while mean performance levels of the ADHD and ASD1 groups were average (Figure 1 and Table 5). Overall mean performance of the NVLD+ADHD group was significantly lower than that of the ADHD group (Table 5).

In total CCT score and in all dimensions except Verbal detail, the NVLD+ADHD group performed significantly worse than the control group. The performance of the ADHD group, however, was similar to that of the control group in all dimensions. Furthermore, the NVLD+ADHD and ADHD groups differed in overall CCT (Table 6). As the participants in both groups have attention deficit, we infer that the clear difficulties in central coherence in subjects with NVLD+ADHD is not to be explained by attention deficit but rather as being characteristic of NVLD.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. CCT performance profiles based on differences with respect to the control group.

NVLD+ADHD and ASD1 groups had significantly lower mean performance than the control group in the Inference and Context dimensions (Table 6). The two groups differ in mean performance in Simultaneity: children with NVLD+ADHD performing worse than those with ASD1.

CCT performance profiles for NVLD+ADHD and SCD groups were similar. In both groups, mean performance in Simultaneity was significantly lower than that of the control group. However, the performance deficit of the NVLD+ADHD group was more serious than that of the SCD group because the symptomatology was more pronounced: the mean performance of the NVLD+ADHD group was significantly lower than that of the control group in most dimensions of the CCT (Table 6).

4 Discussion

The results of our study indicate that deficit in central coherence is not an effect exclusive to ASD. While various studies have found a reduction in global processing in ASD (Rinehart et al., 2000; Pellicano et al., 2005; Seernani et al., 2020) or that people with ASD perform significantly better in tests of local preference than people with typical development (Shah and Frith, 1983; Plaisted et al., 1998; O’Riordan et al., 2001; Pellicano et al., 2006; Gambra, 2020), there are also numerous studies that contradict such findings (Brian and Bryson, 1996) and that demonstrate that ability for global processing in people with ASD is intact (Bertone et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2004; Johnson et al., 2010).

Mottron and Belleville (Mottron and Belleville, 1993) demonstrate that people with autism process information, at both the local and the global level, as well as control subjects, but that in ASD the local interferes with the global when stimuli are incongruous. Subsequent studies confirm these findings (Jolliffe and Baron-Cohen, 1997; Rinehart et al., 2001). These somewhat contradictory findings have also been obtained in studies using more than one evaluation tool (Edgin and Pennington, 2005). Syriopoulou Delli et al. (2016) suggest that disparity in results might be best explained by considering that the style of information processing in autism is personal rather than a distinct characteristic of ASD.

In our research, the general performance of the ASD1 group was lower than that of the control group, but this difference was not always significant (depending on the dimension of the CCT); this is in agreement with the results of Syriopoulou Delli et al. (2016) who found that in a global preference task the scores of children with typical development were apparently higher than the scores of children with ASD, but the difference was not statistically significant. With regard to central coherence abilities, schoolchildren with ASD1 had average performance in simultaneity tasks (which were principally visual tasks) and average-low performance in tasks involving inference, verbal detail and understanding in context. Relative to the control group, differences in performance at tasks involving inference and context were significant. (The same differences were found in the NVLD+ADHD group, but children with ASD1 performed better than children with NVLD+ADHD in simultaneity tasks.)

In contrast to our findings, Loth (Loth, 2003) concluded that only about 35% of children with autism show weak central coherence in different tasks, while 48% have mixed styles of processing, with good performance in conceptual tasks. In our study, however, 90% of the schoolchildren in the ASD1 group had difficulty with one or more abilities related to central coherence. The difference in results can be explained in various ways. First, ASD is by nature heterogeneous and symptoms vary according to the level of ASD (Santangelo and Folstein, 1999; Tager-Flusberg and Joseph, 2003); in our study, all participants with ASD were level 1. Second, within ASD there can be subgroups defined by performance in terms of central coherence (Tager-Flusberg and Joseph, 2003). Third, as already discussed, the different tasks used by different studies make different demands on subjects and therefore give different measures of performance. Finally, Mottron et al. (Mottron et al., 2006) suggest that autism is often associated with improved perceptual processing, but this is not evident in all children with ASD.

Our study found that children with NVLD had difficulties with central coherence, confirming that this difficulty is a characteristic in the cognitive profile of NVLD. This finding is in line with different studies (Crespo-Eguílaz and Narbona, 2009; Magallón, 2011). Other authors (Chow and Skuy, 1999; Schoemaker et al., 2001; Molenaar-Klumper, 2002; Viñuela, 2007; Mammarella and Pazzaglia, 2010; Mammarella et al., 2019) do not refer explicitly to the construct of central coherence but nonetheless affirm that children with NVLD have difficulty with global perception and with analysing, organizing and synthesizing information.

General performance of the NVLD+ADHD group in central coherence tasks was lower than that of the control group (p < 0.001). However, performance was average for abilities related to verbal details. That is, for children with NVLD+ADHD, performance in tasks that involve verbal aspects of central coherence seems to stand out against the background of their overall performance. This concurs with the more general observation that the verbal abilities of children with NVLD+ADHD are better than their manipulative abilities. In our study, performance was average-low for abilities to understand in a simultaneous manner, to make inferences and to understand in a context (all of which were statistically significant differences relative to the control group). These results are coherent with those of Crespo-Eguílaz et al. (2012), who reported that rapid interpretation of a chimerical image - to make sense of which it was necessary to perceive and integrate both parts of the chimera - posed difficulty for 85% of schoolchildren with NVLD+ADHD but for only 5% of controls. Magallón (2011) found that children with NVLD+ADHD had difficulty in noticing incongruities in chimerical images (62.1% carried out this task badly in comparison to 8% of controls) and in visual scenes (about 60% performed poorly at this task). These results are along the same lines (Díaz Lucero et al., 2011), who evaluated the neuropsychological profile of a group of 22 children with DAMP and concluded that they did not understand globally but rather by parts, which made it difficult for them to integrate concepts, to carry out abstraction and, therefore, to adapt correctly to the context. Another research group, (Drumond et al., 2005), also found that children with NVLD had difficulty in tasks that involved construction of a whole from parts. Semrud-Clikeman et al. (2010) reported findings similar to ours for a group of children with NVLD (with difficulties in central coherence) relative to other groups of children with level-1 ASD and ADHD (without difficulties in central coherence).

In our study, the children with NVLD also had ADHD. However, as in the study of Makris et al. (2021), the difficulties in central coherence that we have discussed were not observed with a separate ADHD group. Therefore, these difficulties cannot be explained by attention deficit and appear to be characteristic of NVLD. In contrast to these results, (Cardillo, 2018) found that the central coherence profile of a group of children with ADHD was heterogeneous: the children had difficulty with visuoconstruction abilities when they had to battle with global configurations but performed visual-perception tasks correctly. In this study, the SCD group was characterized by average-low performance for all central coherence abilities studied. Thus, as a group, schoolchildren with a deficit in social communication skills have below-average performance, which indicates that their neuropsychological profile is also characterized by weak central coherence.

The study’s limitations include that all children in the NVLD sample have ADHD. It would have been preferable to identify another pure NVLD group, although this is challenging due to the frequent presence of comorbid disorders. Additionally, the sample size of the ASD group could be larger to draw definitive conclusions. Environmental or socio-economic factors not accounted for in the study could influence central coherence abilities. These factors might limit the generalizability of findings to different populations.

5 Conclusion

The current study, through the profiles of central coherence for the clinical groups described, is consistent with a deficit in central coherence is not exclusive to autism spectrum disorders. This study evidence that that children with other neurodevelopmental and learning disorders, such as, non-verbal learning disorder and social communication disorder, experience difficulties related with this cognitive function. In addition, it was found that schoolchildren with ADHD did not have difficulty with central coherence. Finally, we establish that the Central Coherence Test provides complementary information that is useful for differential diagnosis between neurodevelopmental disorders involving weak central coherence.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics committee of the University of Navarra (Ethics approval number: 2017.004mod1). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the minor(s)’ legal guardian/next of kin for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

LG: Writing–original draft, Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing–review and editing. SM: Formal analysis, Writing–original draft, Writing–review and editing. NC-E: Formal analysis, Writing–review and editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of the article. This study was funded by SM Grant PID2020-119328GA-I00 funded by MCIN/AEI/ 10.13039/501100011033.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association [APA]. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Attwood, T. (2007). Guía del síndrome de Asperger. Barcelona: Paidoìs.

Google Scholar

Baixauli-Fortea, I., Roselló, B., and Miranda-Casas (2004). Evaluación de las dificultades pragmáticas. Estudio de casos. Rev. Neurol. 38, 69–79. doi: 10.33588/rn.38S1.2004063

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertone, A., Mottron, L., and Faubert, J. (2004). Autism and schizophrenia: Similar perceptual consequence, different neurobiological etiology? Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 592–593. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04260137

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., and Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: A “complex” Issue. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 218–225. doi: 10.1162/089892903321208150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., and Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain 128, 2430–2441. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. M. (2000). How does the brain learn language? Insights from the study of children with and without language impairment. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 42, 133–142. doi: 10.1017/S0012162200000244

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. M., and Adams, C. (1992). Comprehension problems in children with specific language impairment: Literal and inferential meaning. J. Speech Hear. Res. 35, 119–129. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3501.119

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V., and Rosenbloom, L. (1987). Classification of childhood language disorders. Lang. Dev. Disord. 22, 61–81.

Google Scholar

Booth, R., and Happé, F. (2010). “Hunting with a knife and fork”: Examining central coherence in autism, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical development with a linguistic task. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 107, 377–393. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, R., Charlton, R., Hughes, C., and Happé, F. (2003). Disentangling weak coherence and executive dysfunction: Planning drawing in autism and attention–deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 387–392. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1204

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brian, J. A., and Bryson, S. E. (1996). Disembedding performance and recognition memory in autism/PDD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 865–872. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01482.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardillo, R. (2018). Local-global visuospatial processing in autism spectrum disorders and nonverbal learning disabilities: A cross-task and cross-disorder comparison. Padua: University of Padova.

Google Scholar

Caron, M. J., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., and Dawson, M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain 129, 1789–1802. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl072

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, D., and Skuy, M. (1999). Simultaneous and successive cognitive processing in children with nonverbal learning disabilities. Sch. Psychol. Int. 20, 219–231. doi: 10.1177/0143034399202005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colomé Roura, R., Sans Fitó, A., López Sala, A., and Boix Lluch, C. (2009). Trastorno de aprendizaje no verbal: Características cognitivo-conductuales y aspectos neuropsicológicos. Rev. Neurol. 48:77. doi: 10.33588/rn.48S02.2009009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conti-Ramsden, G., and Gunn, M. (1986). The development of conversational disability: A case study. Br. J. Disord. Commun. 21, 339–351. doi: 10.3109/13682828609019846

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespo-Eguílaz, N., and Narbona, J. (2009). Trastorno de aprendizaje procedimental: Características neuropsicológicas. Rev. Neurol. 49:409. doi: 10.33588/rn.4908.2009079

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespo-Eguílaz, N., and Narbona, J. (2011). Dificultades en la percatación raìpida de incongruencias en el trastorno de aprendizaje procedimental: Posible disfunción de la coherencia central. Rev. Neurol. 52(Suppl. 1), 39–41. doi: 10.33588/rn.52S01.2010807

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespo-Eguílaz, N., Narbona, J., and Magallón, S. (2012). Disfunción de la coherencia central en niños con trastorno de aprendizaje procedimental. Rev. Neurol. 55:513. doi: 10.33588/rn.5509.2012334

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Souza, D., Booth, R., Connolly, M., Happé, F., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2016). Rethinking the concepts of ‘local or global processors’: Evidence from Williams syndrome, down syndrome, and autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Sci. 19, 452–468. doi: 10.1111/desc.12312

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz Lucero, A. H., Melano, C. A., and Etchepareborda Simonini, M. C. (2011). Síndrome de déficit de atención, del control motor y de la percepción (DAMP): Perfil neuropsicológico. Rev. Neurol. 52:S071. doi: 10.33588/rn.52S01.2010797

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doty, N. (2019). “The Massachusetts general hospital guide to learning disabilities,” in Current clinical psychiatry, eds H. K. Wilson and E. B. Braaten (Cham: Humana Press), doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-98643-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Drumond, C., Ahmad, S., and Rourke, B. (2005). Rules for the classification of younger children with nonverbal learning disabilities and basic phonological processing disabilities. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 171–182. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edgin, J. O., and Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Superior, impaired, or just intact? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 729–745. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0020-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Förster, J., and Dannenberg, L. (2010). GLOMO sys: A systems account of global versus local processing. Psychol. Inq. 21, 175–197. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2010.487849

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foss, J. M. (2001). “Nonverbal learning disability: How to recognize it and minimize its effects,” in Proceedings of the ERIC clearinghouse on disabilities and gifted education, council for exceptional children, (Arlington, VA).

Google Scholar

Frith, U. (1992). Autismo: Hacia una explicación del enigma. madrid: Alianza Editorial.

Google Scholar

Gambra, L. (2020). Central coherence: Design and validation of an assessment and study test in procedural learning disorder. Pamplona: University of Navarra.

Google Scholar

Gillberg, C. (2003). Deficits in attention, motor control, and perception: A brief review. Arch. Dis. Child. 88, 904–910. doi: 10.1136/adc.88.10.904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillberg, C. (2009). “Developmental and neuropsychiatric disorders of childhood,” in Diseases of the nervous system in childhood, 3rd Edn, eds J. Aicardi, M. Bax, and C. Gillberg (London: Mac Keith Press), 889–901.

Google Scholar

González, D. (2017). Un trastorno desconocido: Trastorno de aprendizaje no verbal. Padres Maestros 369, 17–19. doi: 10.14422/pym.i369.y2017.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

González, M., Rivas, R. M., and López, S. (2015). Caracterización y delimitación del trastorno de la comunicación social (pragmático). Rev. Estud. Invest. Psicol. Educ. 24, 005–8. doi: 10.17979/reipe.2015.0.09.132

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 216–222. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01318-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollingshead, A. B. (1957). Two factors index of social position. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

Google Scholar

Humphries, T., Cardy, J. O., Worling, D. E., and Peets, K. (2004). Narrative comprehension and retelling abilities of children with nonverbal learning disabilities. Brain Cogn. 56, 77–88. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.06.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. A., Blaha, L. M., Houpt, J. W., and Townsend, J. T. (2010). Systems factorial technology provides new insights on global–local information processing in autism spectrum disorders. J. Math. Psychol. 54, 53–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.06.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolliffe, T., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 527–534. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01539.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., and Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 2329–2340. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1777-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahaie, A., Mottron, L., Arguin, M., Berthiaume, C., Jemel, B., and Saumier, D. (2006). Face perception in high-functioning autistic adults: Evidence for superior processing of face parts, not for a configural face-processing deficit. Neuropsychology 20, 30–41. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.30

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamb, M. R., and Robertson, L. C. (1989). Do response time advantage and interference reflect the order of processing of global- and local-level information? Percept. Psychophys. 46, 254–258. doi: 10.3758/BF03208087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leinonen, E., and Letts, C. (1997). Referential communication tasks: Performance by normal and pragmatically impaired children. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 32, 53–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.1997.tb01624.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

López, B., and Leekam, S. R. (2007). Teoría de la coherencia central: Una revisión de los supuestos teóricos. Inf. Aprend. 30, 439–457. doi: 10.1174/021037007781787462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loth, E. (2003). On social, cultural and cognitive aspects of theory of mind in practice (ProQuest No. 10166930). St Andrews: University of St Andrews.

Google Scholar

Magallón, S. (2011). Memoria procedimental en escolares típicos y en escolares con trastornos de aprendizaje. Pamplona: Universidad de Navarra.

Google Scholar

Makris, G., Pervanidou, P., Chouliaras, G., Stachtea, X., Valavani, E., Bastaki, D., et al. (2021). Diverse patterns of vulnerability to visual illusions in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Cogn. Process. 22, 659–673. doi: 10.1007/s10339-021-01041-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, I. C., and Pazzaglia, F. (2010). Visual perception and memory impairments in children at risk of nonverbal learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychol. 16, 564–576. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.485125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, I. C., Cardillo, R., and Zoccante, L. (2019). Differences in visuospatial processing in individuals with nonverbal learning disability or autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability. Neuropsychology 33, 123–134. doi: 10.1037/neu0000492

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, I. C., Meneghetti, C., Pazzaglia, F., Gitti, F., Gomez, C., and Cornoldi, C. (2009). Representation of survey and route spatial descriptions in children with nonverbal (visuospatial) learning disabilities. Brain Cogn. 71, 173–179. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.05.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez Alonso, M., Fernández Rodríguez, M., Pérez Moleiro, L., and Belén Martínez Alonso, M. (2021). Trastorno de la comunicación social (pragmático), nueva categoría diagnóstica DSM-5, consideraciones clínicas y diagnóstico diferencial a propósito de un caso. Rev. Psiquiatría Infanto Juvenil 32, 155–160.

Google Scholar

McTear, M. F. (1985). Pragmatic disorders: A case study of conversational disability. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 20, 129–142. doi: 10.3109/13682828509012255

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendoza Lara, E., and Muñoz López, J. (2005). Del trastorno específico del lenguaje al autismo. Rev. Neurol. 41:S091. doi: 10.33588/rn.41S01.2005316

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Molenaar-Klumper, M. (2002). Non-verbal learning disabilities. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Google Scholar

Monfort Juárez Centro Entender Hablar I (2013). TRASTORNOS DEL LENGUAJE. Rev. Neurol. 56, S141–S146. doi: 10.33588/rn.56S01.2012667

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Monfort, M. (2001). Niños con un déficit semántico-pragmático1. Rev. Logop. Foniatría Audiol. 21, 188–194. doi: 10.1016/S0214-4603(01)76208-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mottron, L., and Belleville, S. A. (1993). Study of perceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject with exceptional graphic abilities. Brain Cogn. 23, 279–309. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1993.1060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mottron, L., and Burack, J. A. (2001). “Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism,” in The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research, eds J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, and R. Philip (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 131–148.

Google Scholar

Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., and Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 27–43. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulas Delgado, F., Etchepareborda Simonini, M. C., Díaz Lucero, A. H., and Ruiz Andrés, R. (2006). El lenguaje y los trastornos del neurodesarrollo. Revisión de las características clínicas. Rev. Neurol. 42:S103. doi: 10.33588/rn.42S02.2005828

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Narbona, J., Crespo-Eguílaz, N., and Magallón, S. (2011). “Trastorno de aprendizaje procedimental,” in Trastornos del neurodesarrollo, eds J. Artigas-Pallarés and J. Narbona (Barcelona: Viguera Editores), 428–449.

Google Scholar

Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nayar, K., Voyles, A. C., Kiorpes, L., and di Martino, A. (2017). Global and local visual processing in autism: An objective assessment approach. Autism Res. 10, 1392–1404. doi: 10.1002/aur.1782

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noens, I. L. J., and van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A. (2008). The central coherence account of autism revisited: Evidence from the ComFor study. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2, 209–222. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2007.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nydén, A., Hagberg, B., Goussé, V., and Rastam, M. A. (2011). cognitive endophenotype of autism in families with multiple incidence. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 5, 191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.03.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 27, 719–730. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.719

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ornitz, E. M., Guthrie, D., and Farley, A. H. (1977). The early development of autistic children. J. Autism Child Schizophr. 7, 207–229. doi: 10.1007/BF01538999

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., and Durkin, K. (2005). Central coherence in typically developing preschoolers: Does it cohere and does it relate to mindreading and executive control? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 533–547. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00380.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., and Maley, A. (2006). Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism spectrum disorder: “Weak” central coherence and its relationship to theory of mind and executive control. Dev. Psychopathol. 18, 77–98. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perkins, M. (2010). “Pragmatic impairment,” in The handbook of language and speech disorders, eds S. Jack, Damico, Nicole Müller J. Martin, and Ball (Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell), 227–246. doi: 10.1002/9781444318975.ch10

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pina, F., Flavia, M., and Patrizia, O. (2013). Relationship between weak central coherence and mental states understanding in children with autism and in children with ADHD. Mediter. J. Clin. Psychol. 1:1.

Google Scholar

Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39:S0021963098002613. doi: 10.1017/S0021963098002613

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapin, I., and Allen, D. (1983). Developmental language disorders: Nosologic considerations. Neuropsychol. Lang. Read. Spell. 55, 155–184. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-409680-6.50014-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., and Tonge, B. J. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and asperger’s disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 769–778. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00664

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., and Tonge, B. J. (2001). A deficit in shifting attention present in high-functioning autism but not Asperger’s disorder. Autism 5, 67–80. doi: 10.1177/1362361301005001007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Santangelo, S. L., and Folstein, S. E. (1999). “Autism: A genetic perspective,” in Neurodevelopmental disorders, ed. H. Tager-Flusberg (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 431–447. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/4945.003.0024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoemaker, M. M., van der Wees, M., Flapper, B., Verheij-Jansen, N., Scholten-Jaegers, S., and Geuze, R. H. (2001). Perceptual skills of children with developmental coordination disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 20, 111–133. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(01)00031-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schooler, J. W. (2002). Verbalization produces a transfer inappropriate processing shift. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 16, 989–997. doi: 10.1002/acp.930

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seernani, D., Ioannou, C., Damania, K., Spindler, K., Hill, H., Foulsham, T., et al. (2020). Studying global processing in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with gaze movements: The example of a copying task. PLoS One 15:e0224186. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224186

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrud-Clikeman, M., and Hynd, G. W. (1990). Right hemisphere dysfunction in nonverbal learning disabilities: Social, academic, and adaptive functioning in adults and children. Psychol. Bull. 107, 196–209. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., and Christopher, G. (2010). Neuropsychological differences among children with asperger syndrome, nonverbal learning disabilities, attention deficit disorder, and controls. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 582–600. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.494747

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, A., and Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 24, 613–620. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1983.tb00137.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Syriopoulou Delli, C. K., Varveris, A., and Geronta, A. (2016). Application of the theory of mind, theory of executive functions and weak central coherence theory to individuals with ASD. J. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 7:102. doi: 10.5539/jedp.v7n1p102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tager-Flusberg, H., and Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 303–314. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1198

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Velarde, M., Echevarría, J. A. V., and Gómez Velarde, M. (2017). Trastorno de la comunicación social (pragmático) (TCS f80.82). Pediátr. Panamá 46, 99–104.

Google Scholar

Vermeulen, P. (2015). Context blindness in autism spectrum disorder. Focus Autism Other Dev. Disabl. 30, 182–192. doi: 10.1177/1088357614528799

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Viñuela, F. (2007). “Trastornos de las funciones visuoespacial y constructiva,” in Neurología de la conducta y neuropsicología, ed. Peña-Casanova (Madrid: Médica Panamericana), 233–242.

Google Scholar

Wang, A. T., Dapretto, M., Hariri, A. R., Sigman, M., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2004). Neural correlates of facial affect processing in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 43, 481–490. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200404000-00015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C., and Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 24, 550–574. doi: 10.1080/13546800701417096

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Worling, D. E., Humphries, T., and Tannock, R. (1999). Spatial and emotional aspects of language inferencing in nonverbal learning disabilities. Brain Lang. 70, 220–239. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2156

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., and Adipat, B. (2005). Challenges, methodologies, and issues in the usability testing of mobile applications. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 18, 293–308. doi: 10.1207/s15327590ijhc1803_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: central coherence, non-verbal learning disorder, attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder, social communication disorder, autism

Citation: Gambra L, Magallon S and Crespo-Eguílaz N (2024) Weak central coherence in neurodevelopmental disorders: a comparative study. Front. Psychol. 15:1348074. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1348074

Received: 04 December 2023; Accepted: 23 May 2024;
Published: 12 June 2024.

Edited by:

Douglas Frye, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Reviewed by:

James Hugo Smith-Spark, London South Bank University, United Kingdom
Munis Dundar, Erciyes University, Türkiye

Copyright © 2024 Gambra, Magallon and Crespo-Eguílaz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Leyre Gambra, bGdhbWJyYUB1bmF2LmVz; Nerea-Crespo-Eguílaz, bmVyZWEuY3Jlc3BvQHVuaXIubmV0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.