Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 19 June 2024
Sec. Psychology of Aging
This article is part of the Research Topic Social Determinants of Alcohol and Substance Use and Mental Health: Implications for Population Health for Groups from Historically Marginalized Communities View all 6 articles

Loneliness and global cognitive functioning in racially and ethnically diverse US midlife and older adults

Updated
David Camacho\n\n
\nDavid Camacho1 * Kelly Pacheco\nKelly Pacheco2 Jerad Moxley\nJerad Moxley2 Maria P. Aranda,Maria P. Aranda3,4M. Carrington Reid\nM. Carrington Reid2 Elaine Wethington\nElaine Wethington2
  • 1Department of Disability and Human Development, University of Illinois Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 2Weill Cornell Medicine, Cornell University, New York, NY, United States
  • 3USC Suzanne Dworak-Peck School of Social Work, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 4Edward R. Roybal Institute on Aging, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Introduction: Few studies have examined the association of loneliness and cognitive functioning in the US. We used two common measures of loneliness and examined their association in a large sample of US Black, Latino, and White adults (ages ≥ 50).

Methods: We analyzed Wave 3 of the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (N = 2,757). We examined loneliness using one item from the CES-D and the Felt Loneliness Measure (NFLM); cognitive functioning was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) tool, where higher scores indicated better functioning. We used weighted ordinary least squares regressions to examine the effects of loneliness (CES-D loneliness and NFLM in separate models) on MoCA scores. In exploratory analyses, we examined if these relationships varied by race and ethnicity. We adjusted all models for sociodemographic and other salient factors (e.g., chronic disease, depressive symptoms, living alone).

Results: Mean age was 63.49 years, 52% were female, and 9% were Black and 6% Latino persons. Approximately 54% endorsed feeling lonely on at least one measure; 31% (CES-D) and 46% (NFLM). The relationship between loneliness measures was positive and significant, X2 (1, N = 2,757) = 435.493 p < 0.001. However, only 40% of lonely individuals were identified as lonely on both assessments. CES-D loneliness was inversely (βˆ = −0.274, p = 0.032) associated with MoCA scores and this association did not vary by race and ethnicity. Greater NFLM loneliness was positively associated (βˆ = 0.445, p < 0.001) with higher MoCA scores for Latino participants only.

Discussion: Loneliness appears to be an important predictor of cognitive functioning. However, the association of loneliness and cognitive functioning varied when using the CES-D loneliness item or the NFLM. Future work is needed to understand how loneliness and its clinically relevant dimensions (social, emotional, existential, chronicity) relate to global and individual cognitive domains. Research is needed with racially and ethnically diverse midlife and older adults, particularly to understand our counterintuitive finding for Latino participants. Finally, findings also support the need for research on interventions to prevent cognitive decline targeting loneliness.

Introduction

Cognitive impairment disproportionately affects older adults, burdens systems of care, impairs quality of life, and constitutes an important research priority (World Health Organization, 2021). By 2060, the US population is projected to include 95 million older adults (Administration of Community Living, 2021). Of these, one third of individuals will be Black or Latino (Administration of Community Living, 2020a,b). Further, Blacks and Latino individuals are more likely to experience cognitive impairment compared to their White counterparts (Alzheimer’s Association, 2016). Therefore, there is a critical need to elucidate determinants and modifiable factors (e.g., loneliness) that impact cognitive health (Peterson et al., 2020; Aranda et al., 2023). However, these examinations should consider common contextual challenges experienced by Black and Latino persons in the US.

Loneliness refers to the distressing feeling that occurs when individuals perceive their social needs are not met by the quantity and particularly the quality of their social relationships (Perlman and Peplau, 1981; Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010). Loneliness is different from but may coincide with social isolation, a depressive episode, or both. Social isolation is the absence or limited number of social relationships (de Jong-Gierveld et al., 2006; Perissinotto and Covinsky, 2014), while the primary symptoms of clinical depression are low mood (e.g., sadness) and lack of interest or pleasure in activities (Uher et al., 2014).

Loneliness may occur at any age. However, limitations in later life can limit opportunities to engage in socialization activities, but they may also shield against loneliness (Carstensen, 1992; Baltes and Carstensen, 2003). For example, older adulthood increases the likelihood of health challenges (e.g., chronic diseases; Vetrano et al., 2018), and frailty (Bandeen-Roche et al., 2015) that may diminish physical functioning and restrict social integration (Hoogendijk et al., 2016). Older adulthood also increases the likelihood of loss of significant others, reducing the pool of people they may count on. However, older adults’ expectations of chronic disease, lowered physical functioning, and social loss may promote compensatory mechanisms, including focusing on fewer but higher quality relationships (Carstensen, 1992; Baltes and Carstensen, 2003).

Loneliness is an important predictor of poor health outcomes (e.g., Ong et al., 2016), including poorer cognitive functioning around the world (Boss et al., 2015; Lara et al., 2019). However, the pathways between loneliness and cognitive functioning are not well understood (Boss et al., 2015). Loneliness may impact cognitive functioning via prolonged activation of the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, which has been associated with hypercortisolism as well as chronic psychological stress and loneliness (Dallman et al., 2004; Steptoe et al., 2004; Adam et al., 2006). Compared to those without loneliness, individuals experiencing loneliness report more chronic stressors (Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2007) and are more likely to perceive daily events as stressful (Cacioppo, 1994). Prolonged hypercortisolism may lead to cortical cellular damage and altered cognitive function in the form of dementia (Epel, 2009).

Despite growing interest, there are multiple gaps in understanding the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning. First, experiences and management of loneliness and cognitive health may be shaped by context, including variations in healthcare resources and cultural perspectives (e.g., Rokach, 2018; Kerwin et al., 2022). To date, only a limited number of studies have focused on US samples (Boss et al., 2015; Lara et al., 2019).

Five US-based studies have examined data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS). Two HRS studies explored loneliness using a one-item measure of loneliness from the CES-D. Donovan et al. (2017) examined individuals 65 years and older and found that loneliness accelerated cognitive decline over a 12-year period even after accounting for relevant covariates including depressive symptoms. Similarly, Yu et al. (2023) examined participants 50 years and older using the 1996–2016 HRS data and found that “cumulative loneliness” (increasing number of waves a participant acknowledged loneliness) was negatively associated with memory function.

Two additional HRS studies used the 3-item UCLA loneliness scale. Griffin et al. (2020) examined HRS participants 65 years and older. Their cross-sectional analyses indicated that loneliness was inversely associated with total global cognitive functioning based on the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS). However, their 6-year longitudinal analyses indicated no significant relationship between loneliness and cognitive decline. Sutin et al. (2020) examined participants 50 years and older over a period of 10 years, where dementia (TICS score of less than six) was the outcome of interest. Their results indicated that for every one-point increase in loneliness, the risk of developing dementia increased by 40%. The authors noted similar results when using the CES-D loneliness measure (“Much of the time during the past week you felt lonely”). Further, their results did not vary by race and ethnicity (i.e., African American, Hispanic, White individuals).

Poey et al. (2017) examined data from the Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study, which randomly enrolled individuals who participated in the 2000 and 2002 HRS waves. They also used the one-item measure of loneliness from the CES-D. Findings indicated that loneliness moderated the relationship between APOE e4 allele status and cognitive impairment based on nurse and neuropsychology technician assessments. Compared to individuals without APOE e4 allele or loneliness, those with the APOE e4 allele only were three times more likely to experience cognitive impairment. However, those with both the APOE e4 allele and loneliness were five times more likely to experience cognitive impairment.

Two studies by Wilson et al. (2007, 2015) used the De Jong-Giervald Loneliness Scale and analyzed data from the Rush Memory and Aging Project in a sample with a mean baseline age of 80.3 years. Wilson et al. (2007) found that at baseline, loneliness was associated with worse cognitive functioning (based on their composite measure of 19 different cognitive tests). After 4 years, loneliness was positively associated with more rapid cognitive decline. Wilson et al. (2015) found that loneliness did not moderate the relationship between negative social interactions and mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Pluim et al. (2023) studied the relationship of loneliness, purpose in life, and subjective cognitive decline (SCD) from a subsample of data collected online during the COVID-19 pandemic with US and Latin American adults. Their subsample focused on US Asian, Black, Latino, and White adults with a mean age of 67 years and high levels of education (mean: 17.1 years of education, SD = 3.2). Loneliness was measured by the De Jong-Giervald Loneliness Scale. In adjusted models (e.g., controlling for sociodemographic factors, living arrangement, occupation status, etc.), they found that loneliness increased the likelihood of reporting SCD in White participants only.

Two studies examined the relationship of loneliness and cognitive functioning using the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP). Kim et al. (2020) followed participants (mean age: 69 years) from Wave 1 across 10-year period to examine mediating factors in the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning. They used the 3-item version of the UCLA Loneliness scale and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). The study findings suggest that loneliness may indirectly affect cognitive functioning as their adjusted models revealed that functional ability, self-rated health, and depressive symptoms significantly mediated the effects?

Ishikawa et al. (2022) examined the cross-sectional relationship between perceived isolation and MCI using Wave 3 of the NSHAP. The investigators focused their examination on Black and White participants (95% of sample) and did not distinguish if individuals identified as Hispanic or Latino. Perceived isolation included questions about emotional and instrumental support from family members, friends, and spouse or partner as well as the 3-item UCLA loneliness scale. Ishikawa et al. used the MoCA cutoff of 23 to indicate possible MCI. They found that after controlling for demographic factors only, individuals with perceived isolation were more likely to experience MCI. Of note, neither Kim et al. (2020) nor Ishikawa et al. (2022), examined whether the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning varied across racial and ethnic groups.

A second critical issue in the study of loneliness and cognitive functioning is the variability in the assessment of loneliness (Boss et al., 2015; Lara et al., 2019). There are multiple measures of loneliness (Maes et al., 2022) and no consensus or gold standard scale. Extant studies (Boss et al., 2015; Lara et al., 2019) tend to use either “direct” assessments that include the term “lonely” (e.g., CES-D item “I feel lonely”) or “indirect” measures that avoid the use of the term (e.g., the 3-item UCLA scale; Shiovitz-Ezra and Ayalon, 2012). Some studies have found low concordance between the CES-D (direct assessment) and the dichotomized three-item UCLA (indirect assessment) and note that these assessments may capture different groups of individuals living with loneliness (e.g., Victor et al., 2005; Shiovitz-Ezra and Ayalon, 2012). More recently, Newmyer et al. (2021) analyzed data from 10 countries and concluded that both measures are valid measures of loneliness. They also found that the two measures function similarly across gender and age groups. However, they raised the possibility that there could be cross-cultural and contextual variation in the validity of loneliness measures (which they were unable to explore). Finally, current studies provide limited insight into possible differences in measures and their relationship with cognitive functioning measures.

Critical theoretical factors may contribute to measures capturing different groups living with loneliness. For example, stigma (Crocker and Major, 1989; Lau and Gruen, 1992) associated with the experience of loneliness may lead some individuals to not report their true feelings when using the direct assessment approach. Direct assessment (e.g., CES-D item) explores loneliness over the past week, while the 3-item UCLA (and its derivatives) examines a general feeling over an undefined timeframe (Shiovitz-Ezra and Ayalon, 2012). Further, we underscore that the impact of stigma and disclosure of loneliness may different and even be magnified in minoritized groups (Black and Latino groups). Despite these important theoretical differences, only one US study (Sutin et al., 2020) has explored the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning using both direct and indirect assessments. Sutin and colleagues found similar results using the 3-item UCLA scale and the one-item CES-D loneliness assessment. However, it is unclear if these results will be the same across other large datasets.

A third critical issue is that few US studies have examined loneliness in midlife and older Black or Latino individuals (Ojembe et al., 2022; Tibirica et al., 2022), or if the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning varies by race and ethnicity (e.g., Pluim et al., 2023). These studies have linked loneliness to health outcomes (e.g., frailty, cardiovascular disorders, self-rated health), including lower cognitive functioning (Gerst-Emerson et al., 2014; Han et al., 2017; Estrella et al., 2021). However, these studies focused on older Black adults living with HIV (Han et al., 2017), another focused on Mexican Americans over the age of 80 years, and none examined if the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning varied as a function of race and ethnicity.

Review articles that have considered the health impact of stressor exposure on physical and mental health (Ferraro and Shippee, 2009; Forrester et al., 2019) provide a strong basis for considering the cumulative and joint effect of stressful experiences such as loneliness on cognitive health. For example, low levels of education and low income are positively associated with the presence of loneliness and poorer cognitive functioning (e.g., Theeke, 2010; Chen et al., 2014; Weuve et al., 2018; Díaz-Venegas et al., 2019). Because US Black and Latino individuals are more likely to experience exposure to socioeconomic challenges that are associated with higher levels of stress exposure (e.g., low education, poverty) compared to their White counterparts (Ferraro and Shippee, 2009; Forrester et al., 2019) and because loneliness enhances stressful perceptions of daily events or challenges (Cacioppo et al., 2006; Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010), we consider that the reciprocal relationship between exposure to risks and limited opportunities and resources, and loneliness may contribute to prolonged and higher levels of stress among Black and Latino persons across the life course. The higher frequency of stress and its biological impact could potentially accelerate cellular damage in the brain and lead to poorer cognitive functioning among midlife and older Black and Latino individuals compared to White participants (Epel, 2009; Zaheed et al., 2020). Therefore, we posit that the impact of loneliness on cognition may vary as a function of race and ethnicity, with Black and Latino individuals being more vulnerable to the effects of loneliness compared to their White counterparts.

In sum, these results highlight a need for further work that examines the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning in the US. Most but not all studies have noted an inverse relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning. In this study, our primary aim was to examine the effects of loneliness on cognitive functioning in US Black, Latino, and White midlife and older adults (ages ≥ 50) from the NSHAP. Our sample included individuals who completed both the CES-D loneliness item and the 3-item NSHAP Felt Loneliness Measure (NFLM), as well as the MoCA at Wave 3 conducted in 2015 (Payne et al., 2014; Shega et al., 2014). As a secondary aim, we examined if the relationship between loneliness and cognition varies as a function of assessment type. Finally, we note that limited attention has been paid to how the relationship of loneliness and cognition varies across racial and ethnic groups. We conduct exploratory analyses that examine how the relationship of loneliness and cognition may vary by race and ethnicity. Guided by prior literature and theory, we posit that loneliness will contribute to poorer cognitive functioning. More specifically, we hypothesize that both assessments of loneliness will be inversely associated with cognitive functioning. Finally, we hypothesized that Black and Latino individuals with loneliness will experience poorer cognitive functioning compared to their White counterparts with the same levels of loneliness.

Materials and methods

Data source

The NSHAP is a nationally representative survey of midlife and older adults living in the community and is designed to assess the physical, mental, and social well-being of home-dwelling midlife and older Americans (O’Muircheartaigh et al., 2021). We analyzed data from Wave 3 (n = 4,777, collected 2015–2016), which included in-person interviews from two cohorts: (1) respondents continuing from the first rounds of interviews (born 1920–1947) and (2) newly recruited participants (born 1948–1965). Live-in partners of both cohorts were also eligible for the interviews. In addition to the in-person interviews, participants were asked to complete leave-behind, self-administered questionnaires and up to 11 biomeasures. We analyzed data from only Wave 3 for two reasons. First, the measures of cognitive functioning and loneliness in the planned analyses are present in Wave 3. The leave-behind questionnaire also included concepts of theoretical interest (e.g., community participation, perceived discrimination). Second, the inclusion of a fresh sample of midlife and older adults in Wave 3, as well as live-in partners, increased the sample size available to test study hypotheses and increased the age range of the sample to include the baby boomer cohort. Final return rates for the leave-behind questionnaire were satisfactory: 85% for the full sample, 91% for the continuing participants, and 80% for the newly recruited participants (O’Doherty et al., 2021). Data were collected in English and Spanish by the University of Chicago’s National Opinion Research Center.

Population

Our target population included home-dwelling adults aged 50 and older who completed all items of our cognitive measure and were Black, Latino, or White persons. Our final weighted sample included 2,757 individuals who were either White (n = 2,371), Black (n = 240) or Latino (n = 146).

Measurements

Outcome

Global cognitive functioning

The Chicago Cognitive Functions Measure (CCFM) was used to assess multiple cognitive domains, including (1) orientation, (2) naming, (3) executive functioning, (4) visuo-construction, (5) memory, (6) attention, (7) language, and (8) abstraction (Shega et al., 2014). We examined global CCFM scores with a possible range from 0 to 20, where higher scores indicate overall better cognitive function. Following Shega et al.’s (2014) approach, we converted CCFM scores into MoCA scores using the formula MoCA = (1.14 × CCFM) + 6.83. The MoCA has been used with a variety of populations and racially diverse samples (e.g., Rossetti et al., 2011; Zhou et al., 2015). We examined MoCA as a continuous score. Although prior studies have used a standard cutoff of 26 (Nasreddine et al., 2005) to indicate possible cognitive impairment, we considered that cutoff may vary as a function of race and ethnicity and education (Rossetti et al., 2011; Zhou et al., 2015). Because there is no accepted cutoff score for Black and Latino persons, we limited using cutoff score of 26 to descriptive purposes. For our primary analyses, we examined the MoCA as a continuous score.

Main predictors

We examined loneliness using two separate measures. First, we took one item (“I feel lonely”) from the CES-D that assessed the frequency of depressive symptoms in the past week (0 = rarely or none of the time, 1 = some of the time, and 2 = much or most of the time; Hawkley et al., 2006). Similar to prior research (Cornwell and Waite, 2009), we dichotomized responses by recoding all participants who responded some, much or most of the time as “lonely” (1 = Yes).

We also assessed loneliness using the NFLM. The NFLM is very similar to the 3-item UCLA loneliness scale. The NFLM assessed perceived frequency of lack of companionship, feeling left out, and feeling isolated, with possible responses of 0 = never, 1 = hardly ever, 2 = some of the time, and 3 = often. We followed scoring recommendations from Payne et al. (2014) on use of the NSHAP data. In line with their recommendations, we combined “never” and “hardly ever.” Total scores ranged from 0 to 6, with higher scores reflecting greater levels of loneliness. We also used their recommended cutoff of 1 to determine the presence of loneliness.

Covariates

In our analyses, we considered common social and health factors that contribute to poorer cognitive functioning. In our models we included available NSHAP measures indicative of individual cumulative inequality factors (education, perceived economic status, skipping healthcare due to in adequate health insurance, perceived discrimination) to capture their current direct effects on cognitive functioning (Ferraro and Shippee, 2009; Forrester et al., 2019). However, available measures do not fully capture the complexity of these experiences, including the magnitude, onset, or duration of exposure to these factors and their associated advantages and disadvantages across the life course and social systems. Therefore, we use the NSHAP-recommended categories of race and ethnicity to capture overall group differences resulting from cumulative inequality.

Race and ethnicity was assessed with two questions: “Do you consider yourself primarily White or Caucasian, Black or African American, American Indian, Asian, or something else?” and “Do you consider yourself Hispanic or Latino?” To analyze by group, we use an NSHAP-coded race and ethnicity variable that classified participants into four mutually exclusive groups: (1) non-Hispanic White, (2) Black (which included Hispanics who self-reported Black race), (3) Hispanic (of all races except “Black”), and (4) “other” (e.g., Asian, Native American, Pacific Islander).

Lower levels of education and low income have been associated with the presence of loneliness and poor cognitive functioning in US samples of midlife and older adults (Sachs-Ericsson et al., 2009; Theeke, 2010; Zhang et al., 2020). Further, access to healthcare (e.g., having adequate insurance) may contribute to better preventive care that in turn may improve health outcomes (McMaughan et al., 2020).

Educational attainment was measured as 1 = less than high school; 2 = high school or equivalent; 3 = vocational certificate, some college or associate degree, and 4 = bachelor’s degree or more.

Perceived economic position was determined by asking participants: “Compared with American families in general, would you say that your household income is 1 = far below average; 2 = below average; 3 = average; 4 = above average; or 5 = far above average?” Answers ranged from 1 to 5. We examined this variable as a continuous score, with higher scores indicating higher perceived economic position.

Regarding Inadequate health insurance: participants reported whether they had difficulty in receiving healthcare services because of a lack of adequate insurance: (1) “In the past year, has a lack of adequate health insurance kept you from getting medical care?” and (2) “In the past year, has a lack of adequate health insurance kept you from getting prescription medications?” Possible responses were yes or no. We recoded these items into one variable. If participants answered affirmatively to either original item, then they were coded as yes.

We also note that discrimination has detrimental effects on health including cognitive functioning (Pascoe and Smart Richman, 2009; Barnes et al., 2012). Perceived discrimination was measured by an adapted version of the Perceived Discrimination Scale (Williams et al., 1997; Monk et al., 2021). The two items included: “In your day-to-day life, how often have you been treated with less courtesy than other people?” and “In your day-to-day life, how often have people acted as if they are better than you are?” Response options were 0 = never, 1 = less than once a year, 2 = about once or twice a year, 3 = several times a year, 4 = about once a month, 5 = every week, and 6 = several times a week. We summed both items to create a total score (range: 0–12).

Age, biological sex, and marital status are important predictors of cognitive decline and impairment (Ferreira et al., 2014; Murman, 2015; Liu et al., 2019).

Sociodemographic variables included age (in years), sex (0 = male, 1 = female), and marital status (married or living as 1 = married, divorced, separated, or never 2 = married and 3 = widowed).

We considered that both chronic conditions (Maresova et al., 2019) and depressive symptoms (Rock et al., 2014) may contribute to poor cognitive functioning. The total number of chronic diseases was calculated by responses to separate items with a stem asking, “Has a medical doctor ever told you that you have: heart disease, arthritis, breathing problems, stroke, hypertension, diabetes, and cancer?” The total score ranged from 0 to 7.

Depressive symptoms were measured by the NSHAP Depressive Symptoms Measure (NDSM). The 11-item NDSM was derived from the well-validated CES-D depression instrument (Radloff, 1977; Payne et al., 2014). Similar to the original CES-D, the NDSM assesses the frequency of depressive symptoms in the past week. Original responses included 0 = rarely or none of the time, 1 = some of the time, 2 = occasionally, and 3 = most of the time. We followed the suggestion and recoded original responses of Payne et al. (2014) into three categories (0 = rarely or none of the time, 1 = some of the time, and 2 = much or most of the time). We did not use the item “I feel lonely” as this was already used in our study as a measure of loneliness (Hawkley et al., 2006). We summed scores for the remaining 10 items (possible range: 0–20).

We also considered that objective measures of social relations may also contribute to cognitive impairment (Evans et al., 2019). We did not have a direct measure of social isolation. However, we included in our models two related concepts that capture quantity of social relationships (Wang et al., 2017). Community participation during the past 12 months was assessed via three items examining frequency of volunteer work, attendance of social meetings, and gatherings with friends or relatives (Cornwell and Waite, 2009). Possible responses were 0 = never, 1 = less than once a year, 2 = about once or twice a year, 3 = several times a year, 4 = about once a month, 5 = every week, and 6 = several times per week. We created a sum score for these three items (possible range: 0–18).

A household roster was not available in the analysis dataset. Therefore, living alone was based on an examination of individuals’ reported social networks. Questions asked whether each person listed in individuals’ networks lived in their home. We identified individuals as living alone if they reported that nobody in their social network was living in their residence.

Analytical strategy

Our primary goal was to examine the relation between loneliness and global cognitive functioning. We first examined descriptive statistics for all participants. We also examined Pearson’s correlations between primary predictors and MoCA scores (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Weighted descriptive and bivariate statistics at wave 3 (n = 2,757).

In our primary models (Table 2), the dependent variable was MoCA score. To minimize the potential for confounding (Hawkley et al., 2006), we examined a priori main effects and entered our loneliness measures into separate models. Model 1 included CES-D loneliness (yes, no) and Model 2 included the NFLM loneliness score. All adjusted models included race and ethnicity (Black, Latino, White), sex (male, female), marital status (married or living with partner: divorced, separated, or never married; and widowed), educational attainment (bachelor’s or more; vocation certificate, some college, or associate degree; high school or equivalent; and less than high school), skipping health services (no, yes), and living alone as fixed classification factors. Covariates included NFLM loneliness score, age (in years), household income (1–5), total number of chronic medical conditions (0–7), perceived discrimination score (0–14), depressive symptoms (0–20), and community participation (0–18).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Effects of loneliness, and race/ethnicity on global cognitive functioning.

To support interpretation, we examined differences between loneliness groups. We used a cutoff score of 1 (Payne et al., 2014) to identify individuals who were lonely based on the NFLM. First, we conducted chi-square test between our dichotomized loneliness measures. Second, we created four groups (no loneliness on either measure, CES-D lonely only, NFLM lonely only, lonely on both measures). We used logistic regression to examine the differences across the groups of CES-D lonely only and NFLM lonely only. These groups represented individuals who were inconsistently classified as lonely across assessments.

We considered that disproportionate exposure to risks, opportunities, and resources (Ferraro and Shippee, 2009) across the life course may contribute to varying impact of loneliness on stress and cognitive functioning. We conceptualized racial and ethnic groups as categories that encapsulate the overall differences in exposure to risk or opportunity. In our models, we controled for multiple factors that may contribute to cumulative inequality in health outcomes, including educational attainment, perceived economic position, skipping health services, and perceived discrimination. However, these available NSHAP measures do not fully capture all factors and experiences that may contribute to disproportionate exposure to risk or opportunities (e.g., quality of education, wealth, quality of healthcare, etc.). To test differences in the impact of loneliness on cognitive functioning across race and ethnicity, we systematically examined the interactions of each loneliness measure (CES-D and NFLM) and race and ethnicity. In these models, we controlled for exposure to risk or opportunity, sociodemographics, and health factors. We demonstrate significant findings in Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effects of NFLM scores on MoCA by race and ethnicity.

We used ordinary least squares regressions. We applied NSHAP-generated person-level weights that accounted for non response to all statistical analyses (detailed descriptions of weighting approach is found in O’Muircheartaigh et al., 2021). We used the recommended Wave 3 NSHAP variable (weight_adj) that assigns each case different weights (by simulated replication) and is designed to provide unbiased estimates of population parameters IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 29) (O’Muircheartaigh et al., 2021). Assigned weights indicated the number of observations that each case represented. Cases with missing values were excluded from our analyses. We also conducted analyses using linear mixed models to control for nesting among subjects from the same household. We performed analyses with SPSS version 27 survey procedures.

Results

We present results for our original non nested models. Our nested analyses supported the robustness of our primary and exploratory findings. Most of our results were the same, with one exception. We describe this difference in its appropriate section. Nested models are available upon request.

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 demonstrates the descriptive information of our weighted sample of 2,757 NSHAP participants. The mean MoCA score was 24.80 (SD = 3.18). Approximately 70% scored 26 or above, indicating no cognitive impairment (Nasreddine et al., 2005). In terms of loneliness, 30% were lonely as assessed by the CES-D item, while 46% were identified as lonely based on the NFLM (score ≥ 1). Most participants were White persons, with approximately 9% being Black and close to 6% being Latino. The overall mean sample age was 63.49 years (SD = 9.2) and most participants were female (52%). Overall, 72% of participants were married or living with a partner. Mean perceived economic position was 3.03 (SD = 0.97), with close to half of participants feeling that their economic position was “average” relative to other Americans. Overall, 14% reported missing medical care or medication due to inadequate insurance. The perceived discrimination score was 2.87 (SD = 2.73), indicating that on average, participants felt they were treated with less courtesy or that others acted as if they were better than them less than once a year. Perceived discrimination was higher for Black individuals, with a mean of 3.28 (SD = 2.88) indicating that they felt discriminated close to several times a year. On average, participants reported 1.33 chronic diseases (SD = 1.21) and a mean depressive symptoms score of 7.59, (SD = 3.08). Community participation was 9.21 (SD = 4.22), indicating that on average participants engaged in volunteer work, attended of social meetings, and gathered with friends or relatives several times a year.

In bivariate analyses, CES-D loneliness, NFLM loneliness score, being Black, being Latino, separated, divorced, never married, widowed, skipping healthcare services, having one or more chronic diseases, and having more depressive symptoms were all inversely associated with cognitive functioning. Being female, greater perceived economic position, more community participation, and living alone were all positively associated with higher cognitive scores. Lower educational attainment was also associated with lower MoCA scores.

Relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning measures

Table 2 includes two separate adjusted ordinary least squares models estimating the main effects of being lonely based on the CES-D (Model 1) and NFLM loneliness score (Model 2) on MoCA scores. In Model 1, compared to those who reported no loneliness during the past week, CES-D loneliness was inversely associated with global cognitive functioning scores. In Model 2, NFLM loneliness was not associated with MoCA scores. In both models, being Black or Latino, older age, and more depressive symptoms were inversely associated with MoCA scores. Being female and greater community participation were positively associated with higher MoCA scores. Lower educational attainment contributed to worse MoCA scores. Finally, widowhood was inversely associated with MoCA scores in Model 2 only.

Differences across lonely only groups

The relationship between loneliness measures was significant, X2(1, N = 2,757) = 435.493, p < 0.001. Only 40% of lonely individuals were identified as lonely on both measures. In original and nested analyses, there were consistent significant differences in the categories of “CES-D lonely only” and “NFLM lonely only.” Compared to those who were NFLM lonely only, individuals who were CES-D lonely only evidenced significantly lower MoCA and perceived discrimination scores. The CES-D lonely only group also had a greater proportion of Latino participants widows and greater depressive symptoms. We note that in our original analyses (non nested), there were marginally significant differences in chronic disease across groups. In nested analyses, these differences were no longer significant.

Exploratory analyses: relationship between loneliness and MoCA scores by race and ethnicity

Our exploratory analyses indicated that the association of CES-D loneliness with MoCA score did not vary by race and ethnicity. Figure 1 demonstrates that NFLM loneliness score was not significantly related to cognitive functioning scores for White or Black participants. However, NFLM loneliness was positively associated with better MoCA scores for Latino participants only (βˆ = 0.445, SD = 0.128, p < 0.001).

Discussion

Our study examined the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning in a US sample of midlife and older adults community-dwelling Black, Latino, and White adults. After adjusting for multiple salient variables, our results indicated that loneliness identified with the CES-D loneliness item was inversely associated with MoCA scores. However, there was no significant relationship between NFLM loneliness and MoCA scores among White or Black individuals. Counterintuitively, NFLM loneliness was positively associated with better MoCA scores among Latino participants only. These findings contribute to the literature examining effects of loneliness on cognitive functioning. Given the subjective nature of loneliness and racial and ethnic diversity in the US context, our findings spark multiple questions for future work.

We hypothesized that both of our measures of loneliness (CES-D and NFLM) would be associated with poorer cognitive functioning. Our findings provided partial support for these hypotheses. Our findings documenting an inverse relationship between CES-D loneliness and global cognitive functioning are consistent with prior research (Boss et al., 2015; Donovan et al., 2017; Poey et al., 2017; Yu et al., 2023). Our final models indicated that NFLM loneliness was not associated with cognitive functioning among Black and White participants. These results are different from cross-sectional examinations by Griffin et al. (2020) and Ishikawa et al. (2022) using the UCLA loneliness scale with predominantly White and Black persons in analyses of the HRS and NSHAP (Griffin et al., 2020; Ishikawa et al., 2022). We consider multiple factors that may explain our findings.

First, our findings suggest that “direct assessment” (e.g., CES-D loneliness) and “indirect assessment” of loneliness (NFLM) capture varying impacts of loneliness on cognitive functioning (Shiovitz-Ezra and Ayalon, 2012). It is possible that the CES-D loneliness item, compared to the NFLM, is a better method for detecting distressing feelings of loneliness and their impact on cognitive functioning among younger (e.g., midlife) and healthier (e.g., no significant cognitive impairment) samples. Although our loneliness measures were significantly correlated, only 40% of lonely individuals were identified as lonely by both measures. We posit that our loneliness measures captured lonely groups with different characteristics that may contribute to health status, levels of stress, and cognitive functioning. Specifically, compared to individuals in the NFLM lonely group, the CES-D lonely group was more likely be widowed and have depressive symptoms? Each of these factors may contribute to overall poorer health (e.g., Kung, 2020) higher stress levels (e.g., Cohen et al., 2016) and may moderate the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning.

Second, we consider that the CES-D or NFLM measures of loneliness do not capture the complexity of the experience of loneliness (Kidambi and Lee, 2020). For example, stigma (Lau and Gruen, 1992; Rokach, 2018) associated with the experience of loneliness may lead some individuals to not report their true feelings in response to the CES-D loneliness item. Further, individuals who are assessed as lonely on scales like the NFLM may not believe themselves to be lonely. For example, in a qualitative study of loneliness with older Latino adults (Camacho et al., 2021), some participants cited feeling lonely due to missing loved ones who live outside of the US or deceased loved ones. At the same time, these participants reported being surrounded by close others. Some participants did not feel “left out,” a “lack of companionship,” or “isolated” (Payne et al., 2014) or used these terms to describe their feelings of loneliness. We note that the CES-D lonely group only had higher proportion of Latino participants. Therefore, it is possible that the ability of different assessments to identify lonely individuals may vary across racial and ethnic groups.

Experiences of loneliness likely vary across individuals as a function of clinically relevant dimensions (e.g., frequency of symptoms, chronicity, intensity), but these are not captured by either loneliness assessment employed in the current study. Recently, Yu et al. (2023) noted the significant cumulative effects of loneliness on memory by summing the number of waves in which participants acknowledged feeling lonely on the CES-D item (i.e., during the past week). On the one hand, it is possible that individuals who reported feeling lonely across waves may approximate individuals with chronic loneliness. However, their approach may also include individuals who experienced multiple bouts of situational loneliness across waves. Measures that capture the chronicity of loneliness are necessary to enhance understanding of how they relate to stress and cognitive functioning (Kidambi and Lee, 2020). Further, different types of loneliness (social, emotional, existential; Weiss, 1973) may have differing impact on stress and cognitive functioning on US midlife and older adults. Interestingly, our CES-D lonely only group had a greater proportion of widowed individuals, while those found to be lonely via the NFLM measure reported significantly greater perceived discrimination scores. We posit that perceived causes of loneliness may vary across and within CES-D and NFLM lonely groups, producing varied types of loneliness, chronicity, impact on stress and ultimately cognitive functioning.

We note that multiple factors may explain why our results indicated that NFLM loneliness was not associated with cognitive functioning for Black and White participants.

We consider that the elements of study design and participants’ ages may explain this finding. For example, Griffin et al.’s (2020) cross-sectional analyses yielded a significant relationship between loneliness (UCLA loneliness scale) and cognitive functioning. However, their sample was composed of individuals 65 years and older. Further, our sample included individuals who completed the full MoCA battery and therefore, individuals who were more severely impaired (i.e., unable to answer questions with possible dementia) were not included. As a result, our sample of Black and White participants may have been healthier than prior studies, therefore making it less likely for an effect of NFLM loneliness on cognitive functioning to be detected. Additionally, our study was cross-sectional. Prior studies indicating loneliness (UCLA loneliness scale; De Jong-Giervald Loneliness Scale) is inversely associated with cognitive functioning followed samples over 6 to 10 years on average (Wilson et al., 2007, 2015; Griffin et al., 2020; Sutin et al., 2020). Further, samples from Wilson et al. (2007, 2015) had mean ages in the 80s. Our sample ages ranged from 50 to 97 years old (mean: 63 years). It is possible that the association of loneliness assessed with the NFLM and similar measures is most noticeable in Black and White participants with advanced ages using longitudinal designs.

To date, most US research on loneliness and its impact on health has focused on White adult majority groups. Our inclusion and examination of Black and Latino persons extends the limited literature on loneliness and cognitive health in these important groups (Ojembe et al., 2022; Tibirica et al., 2022). Our exploratory analyses indicated that the effects of CES-D loneliness on cognitive functioning did not vary by race and ethnicity. We acknowledge that our sample of Black and Latino participants lacked the statistical power to capture the moderating effects of race and ethnicity on CES-D loneliness and cognitive functioning (e.g., overall Latino participants with loneliness [direct measure] made up only 2% of the entire sample). However, these same statistical concerns do not apply to our interactions of NFLM scores by race and ethnicity. We found that NFLM loneliness was not associated with cognitive functioning for Black participants. However, NFLM loneliness contributed to better cognitive functioning for Latino individuals in fully adjusted models. Considering these exploratory analyses and findings, we note multiple factors that may explain these findings.

Subjective perceptions, and personal resources such as coping strategies may vary across individuals, particularly across racial and ethnic groups, and partially explain our findings (Pearlin and Schooler, 1978; Lazarus and Folkman, 1984, p. 142; Ferraro and Shippee, 2009). For example, as with other historically oppressed groups, midlife and older Black persons may be better equipped to accept aging challenges as a result of “crisis competence” (Friend, 1991). Years of successful management of one or multiple stigmatized minority statuses earlier in life may influence subjective perceptions of loneliness that enhance their ability to cope with (or suppress) personalized psychological challenges. It will be critical to examine how midlife and older Black individuals perceive and cope with loneliness to better understand the relationship between loneliness, stress, and cognitive functioning.

Interestingly, our exploratory analyses indicated that NFLM loneliness was positively associated with cognitive functioning for Latino participants only. These results contrast with findings from Pluim et al. (2023) and Sutin et al. (2020). Sutin et al. (2020) noted that UCLA loneliness scale scores significantly increased prior to the onset of dementia over a 6-year period for Hispanics from the HRS. However, Pluim et al. (2023) found that loneliness was not associated with subjective cognitive decline for Latino individuals. We underscore that Latino persons are not well represented in studies of loneliness and cognition (Boss et al., 2015; Lara et al., 2019; Tibirica et al., 2022). We considered the possibility that our fully adjusted models may have blurred the true effects of NFLM loneliness on cognitive functioning in Latino participants. However, we note that simplified models (i.e., controlling for race and ethnicity, NFLM loneliness, and interaction) indicated a positive and stronger relationship between NFLM loneliness and cognitive functioning for Latino individuals (βˆ =0.584, p < 0.001).

We consider that multiple individual and interacting factors not explored in the NSHAP data may explain our counterintuitive results with Latinos. First, nativity may influence the experience of loneliness and cognitive impairment (Angel et al., 2022; Quiroz et al., 2022). Most Latino participants (63.9%) in our sample were foreign born. We were unable to determine country of origin, years living in the US, and citizenship. We consider that Latinos from different countries or regions of origin, immigration trajectories, and acculturation may have differing exposure to risk and opportunities across the life course (e.g., Puerto Ricans are US citizens) that may contribute to important impacts on loneliness, stress, and cognitive functioning. Nevertheless, migration experiences commonly include feelings of loneliness as they leave behind loved ones and their homeland and join a socially excluded and minoritized group in the US (e.g., Negi et al., 2021). On the one hand, loneliness may be a chronic problem for some participants. On the other hand, some Latino participants in our sample may have found efficient ways of coping or overcoming stress resulting from loneliness. Further, loneliness may even enhance problem solving (e.g., executive functioning), as perceiving limited social resources may push immigrants to identify solutions to everyday challenges with limited or no social support (e.g., lack of family, friends, social services).

Second, we consider that varied availability of contextual resources may influence both the experience and impact of loneliness on cognitive health. For example, living in a Latino enclave can facilitate linguistic interactions and socialization opportunities and healthcare that may protect against loneliness and its effects, particularly for monolingual Spanish speakers (Aranda et al., 2023). Living in ethnic enclaves may also facilitate “active,” “regulative,” and religious or spiritual coping approaches to manage loneliness (Schoenmakers et al., 2012). On the other hand, living in areas where Latinos are a minority may enhance the likelihood of experiencing discrimination and limit opportunities to obtain social support and culturally sensitive health services. These factors may have detrimental effects on feelings of isolation and cognitive health (Viruell-Fuentes and Schulz, 2009).

Third, cultural perspectives are important in the study of loneliness as these may shape the nature and extent of closeness in relationships and social connectedness (Perlman and Peplau, 1981; Van Staden and Coetzee, 2010). Differences between individualist and communal cultures may partially contribute to varying rates, perceptions, experiences, and diverse effects of loneliness on cognitive health among different groups of midlife and older Latinos (Van Staden and Coetzee, 2010). For example, in line with cultural values of familism, as Latinos age in the US, they may wish and expect to maintain close relationships (e.g., live in multigenerational households; Viruell-Fuentes and Schulz, 2009). However, varying levels of acculturation (e.g., acceptance of individualist perspectives) may influence midlife and older Latino adults’ expectations and perceived reality of their relationships with US-born children and extended family, and resulting stress (Garcia Diaz et al., 2019) may impact cognitive functioning.

Limitations

We recognize several limitations in our study. First, our study was cross-sectional and therefore, we could not establish directionality in the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning. Second, our sample included disparate samples across Black, Latino, and White groups. Further work with comparable group sample sizes may further elucidate and confirm possible racial and ethnic differences in the relationship between loneliness and cognitive health. Third, other mental health conditions (e.g., anxiety; Gulpers et al., 2016), life experiences (e.g., grief and loss; Pérez et al., 2018), and treatment (e.g., Puustinen et al., 2011) may influence loneliness, stress processes, and cognitive function. Fourth, our analyses provided an initial glimpse into possible differences in identifying individuals living with loneliness using two common measures of loneliness. Conducting a formal examination of direct and indirect measures of loneliness was beyond the scope of our study. Nevertheless, our findings support the need for formal examinations of psychometric properties of loneliness assessment tools in diverse groups of midlife and older adults. We affirm that midlife and older adult communities include sexual and gender minorities, which was not addressed here. Future research is needed to elucidate how these factors influence the relationship between loneliness, stress, and cognitive health outcomes (Kimberly, 1989; Puustinen et al., 2011; Richardson and King, 2017; Camacho et al., 2018).

Conclusion: implications for research and practice

Our findings have important implications for research and clinical practice. Our findings suggest that loneliness may lead to lower cognitive functioning, particularly among individuals who are willing to disclose or recognize their feelings of loneliness (i.e., CES-D loneliness). It will be important to focus further attention on understanding how different dimensions of loneliness (e.g., onset, chronicity, frequency) impact cognitive health (global and individual domains). Future research should also test models that can help to explain whether objective and subjective measures of stress mediate or moderate the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning. Among diverse groups of midlife and older Black and Latino adults, research is particularly needed to identify coping strategies (e.g., Lazarus and Folkman, 1984; Forrester et al., 2019) and how intra group differences (e.g., immigrant trajectories) influence the relationship of loneliness and cognitive functioning.

Finally, 54% of our sample reported loneliness on at least one measure. Considering that loneliness is associated with multiple negative health outcomes, we underscore a need for loneliness prevention and treatment programs that can be delivered in real-world settings such as primary care, geriatric medicine clinics, and outpatient mental health practices. Most recently, psychological interventions have been shown to be effective in treating loneliness (Hickin et al., 2021). However, given our counterintuitive results with Latinos, future research should explore how cultural and contextual factors may influence loneliness assessment across midlife and older adults Latinos in the US. These examinations may then support further work that examines if and how extant mental health interventions need be adapted to address loneliness across racially and ethnically diverse midlife and older adults (Hickin et al., 2021).

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article or supplementary materials. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

This study is a secondary analysis of de-identified data. The data were acquired for analysis under a data use agreement from National Archive of Computerized Data on Aging that precluded the investigators from performing analyses that might re-identify participants in the study. Use of the de-identified data for this study was reviewed by the Weill Cornell Medicine Institutional Review Board. The original data were collected by the National Opinion Research Center which obtained informed consent from the participants.

Author contributions

DC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KP: Writing – review & editing. JM: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing. MA: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MR: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. EW: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The authors declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, or publication of this article. This manuscript was supported by a fellowship from National Institutes of Health (T32 AG049666) and grant from the Columbia Center for Interdisciplinary Research on Alzheimer’s Disease Disparities (P30 AG059303; MPIs Manly/Brickman). Funding for this work to Dr. Aranda was provided by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under award numbers P30AG066530, U54AG063546, and P30AG043073 (NIA/NIH). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The authors declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer-review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors, and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103, 17058–17063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Administration of Community Living . (2020a). 2020 Profile of African Americans age 65 and older. Available at: https://acl.gov/sites/default/files/Profile%20of%20OA/AAProfileReport2021.pdf

Google Scholar

Administration of Community Living . (2020b). 2020 Profile of Hispanic Americans age 65 and older. Available at: https://acl.gov/sites/default/files/Profile%20of%20OA/HispanicProfileReport2021.pdf

Google Scholar

Administration of Community Living . (2021). Profile of older Americans. Available at: https://acl.gov/aging-and-disability-in-america/data-and-research/profile-older-americans

Google Scholar

Alzheimer’s Association (2016). 2016 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 12, 459–509. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Angel, J. L., Rote, S., Vega, W. A., Kim, J., Chiu, C. T., Aranda, M., et al. (2022). Duration of dementia and social service use in the US-born and foreign-born Mexican-American population. J. Aging Health 34, 1291–1301. doi: 10.1177/08982643221125845

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aranda, M. P., Marquez, D. X., Gallagher-Thompson, D., Pérez, A., Rojas, J. C., Hill, C. V., et al. (2023). A call to address structural barriers to Hispanic/Latino representation in clinical trials on Alzheimer's disease and related dementias: a micro-meso-macro perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 9:e12389. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12389

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltes, M. M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). “The process of successful aging: selection, optimization, and compensation” in Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology. eds. U. M. Staudinger and U. Lindenberger (Boston, MA: Springer US), 81–104.

Google Scholar

Bandeen-Roche, K., Seplaki, C. L., Huang, J., Buta, B., Kalyani, R. R., Varadhan, R., et al. (2015). Frailty in older adults: a nationally representative profile in the United States. J Gerontol Series A Biomed Sci Med Sci 70, 1427–1434. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, L. L., Lewis, T. T., Begeny, C. T., Yu, L., Bennett, D. A., and Wilson, R. S. (2012). Perceived discrimination and cognition in older African Americans. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 18, 856–865. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000628

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boss, L., Kang, D. H., and Branson, S. (2015). Loneliness and cognitive function in the older adult: a systematic review. Int. Psychogeriatr. 27, 541–553. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002749

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). Social neuroscience: Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune responses to stress. Psychophysiology 31, 113–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01032.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M., Berntson, G. G., Nouriani, B., et al. (2006). Loneliness within a nomological net: an evolutionary perspective. J. Res. Pers. 40, 1054–1085. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camacho, D., Estrada, E., Lagomasino, I. T., Aranda, M. P., and Green, J. (2018). Descriptions of depression and depression treatment in older Hispanic immigrants in a geriatric collaborative care program. Aging Ment. Health 22, 1056–1062. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1332159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camacho, D., Rodriguez, C. V., Moore, K. L., and Lukens, E. P. (2021). Older immigrant Latino gay men and childhood sexual abuse: findings from the palabras Fuertes project. Qual. Soc. Work. 21, 932–955. doi: 10.1177/14733250211027644

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychol. Aging 7:331. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Hicks, A., and While, A. E. (2014). Loneliness and social support of older people in China: a systematic literature review. Health Soc. Care Community 22, 113–123. doi: 10.1111/hsc.12051

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Gianaros, P. J., and Manuck, S. B. (2016). A stage model of stress and disease. Perspec. Psychol. Sci. 11, 456–463.

Google Scholar

Cornwell, E. Y., and Waite, L. J. (2009). Measuring social isolation among older adults using multiple indicators from the NSHAP study. J Gerontol Series B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 64, i38–i46. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocker, J., and Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: the self-protective properties of stigma. Psychol. Rev. 96:608. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.608

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallman, M. F., la Fleur, S. E., Pecoraro, N. C., Gomez, F., Houshyar, H., and Akana, S. F. (2004). Minireview: glucocorticoids—food intake, abdominal obesity, and wealthy nations in 2004. Endocrinology 145, 2633–2638. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-0037

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jong-Gierveld, J., van Tilburg, T. G., and Dykstra, P. A. (2006). “Loneliness and social isolation” in The Cambridge handbook of personal relationships. eds. A. L. Vangelisti and D. Perlman (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press), 485–500.

Google Scholar

Díaz-Venegas, C., Downer, B., Langa, K. M., and Wong, R. (2019). “Cognitive functioning of US adults by race and Hispanic origin” in Contextualizing health and aging in the Americas: Effects of space, time and place. eds. W. A. Vega, J. L. Angel, L. M. F. G. Robledo, and K. S. Markides (Cham, Switzerland: Springer), 85–107.

Google Scholar

Donovan, N. J., Wu, Q., Rentz, D. M., Sperling, R. A., Marshall, G. A., and Glymour, M. M. (2017). Loneliness, depression and cognitive function in older US adults. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 32, 564–573. doi: 10.1002/gps.4495

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epel, E. S. (2009). Psychological and metabolic stress: a recipe for accelerated cellular aging? Hormones 8, 7–22. doi: 10.14310/horm.2002.1217

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Estrella, M. L., Durazo-Arvizu, R. A., Gallo, L. C., Tarraf, W., Isasi, C. R., Perreira, K. M., et al. (2021). Psychosocial factors associated with cognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos: the Hispanic community health study/study of Latinos and its sociocultural ancillary study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 79, 433–449. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200612

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, I. E., Martyr, A., Collins, R., Brayne, C., and Clare, L. (2019). Social isolation and cognitive function in later life: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 70, S119–S144. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferraro, K. F., and Shippee, T. P. (2009). Aging and cumulative inequality: how does inequality get under the skin? Gerontologist 49, 333–343. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnp034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, L., Ferreira Santos-Galduróz, R., Ferri, C. P., and Fernandes Galduroz, J. C. (2014). Rate of cognitive decline in relation to sex after 60 years-of-age: a systematic review. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 14, 23–31. doi: 10.1111/ggi.12093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Forrester, S. N., Gallo, J. J., Whitfield, K. E., and Thorpe, R. J. (2019). A framework of minority stress: from physiological manifestations to cognitive outcomes. Gerontologist 59, 1017–1023. doi: 10.1093/geront/gny104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friend, R. A. (1991). Older lesbian and gay people: a theory of successful aging. J. Homosex. 20, 99–118. doi: 10.1300/J082v20n03_07

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia Diaz, L., Savundranayagam, M. Y., Kloseck, M., and Fitzsimmons, D. (2019). The role of cultural and family values on social connectedness and loneliness among ethnic minority elders. Clin. Gerontol. 42, 114–126. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2017.1395377

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerst-Emerson, K., Shovali, T. E., and Markides, K. S. (2014). Loneliness among very old Mexican Americans: findings from the Hispanic established populations epidemiologic studies of the elderly. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 59, 145–149. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2014.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffin, S. C., Mezuk, B., Williams, A. B., Perrin, P. B., and Rybarczyk, B. D. (2020). Isolation, not loneliness or cynical hostility, predicts cognitive decline in older Americans. J. Aging Health 32, 52–60. doi: 10.1177/0898264318800587

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gulpers, B., Ramakers, I., Hamel, R., Köhler, S., Voshaar, R. O., and Verhey, F. (2016). Anxiety as a predictor for cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 823–842. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.05.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, S. D., Adeyemi, O., Wilson, R. S., Leurgans, S., Jimenez, A., Oullet, L., et al. (2017). Loneliness in older black adults with human immunodeficiency virus is associated with poorer cognition. Gerontology 63, 253–262. doi: 10.1159/000455253

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Aging and loneliness: downhill quickly? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 187–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00501.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann. Behav. Med. 40, 218–227. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood pressure. Psychol. Aging 21:152. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickin, N., Käll, A., Shafran, R., Sutcliffe, S., Manzotti, G., and Langan, D. (2021). The effectiveness of psychological interventions for loneliness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 88:102066. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogendijk, E. O., Suanet, B., Dent, E., Deeg, D. J., and Aartsen, M. J. (2016). Adverse effects of frailty on social functioning in older adults: results from the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam. Maturitas 83, 45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2015.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, K. M., Davis, J., Chen, J. J., and Lim, E. (2022). The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by aspects of social isolation. PLoS One 17:e0269795. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0269795

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerwin, D., Abdelnour, C., Caramelli, P., Ogunniyi, A., Shi, J., Zetterberg, H., et al. (2022). Alzheimer's disease diagnosis and management: perspectives from around the world. Alzheimers Dement. 14:e12334. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12334

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidambi, N., and Lee, E. E. (2020). Insight into potential mechanisms linking loneliness and cognitive decline: commentary on “health factors as potential mediator the longitudinal effect of loneliness on general cognitive ability”. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 1284–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.08.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, A. J., Beam, C. R., Greenberg, N. E., and Burke, S. L. (2020). Health factors as potential mediators of the longitudinal effect of loneliness on general cognitive ability. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 1272–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.07.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimberly, C. (1989). Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: a black feminist critique of anti‐discrimination doctrine, feminist theory and anti racist politics. University Chicago Legal Forum. 140:139.

Google Scholar

Kung, C. S. (2020). Health in widowhood: the roles of social capital and economic resources. Soc. Sci. Med. 253:112965. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.112965

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lara, E., Martín-María, N., De la Torre-Luque, A., Koyanagi, A., Vancampfort, D., Izquierdo, A., et al. (2019). Does loneliness contribute to mild cognitive impairment and dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 52, 7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.03.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, S., and Gruen, G. E. (1992). The social stigma of loneliness: effect of target person's and perceiver's sex. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 18, 182–189. doi: 10.1177/0146167292182009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). “Stress, appraisal, and coping” in Encyclopedia of behavioral medicine. eds. M. D. Gellman and J. R. Turner (New York, New York: Springer), 1913–1915.

Google Scholar

Liu, H., Zhang, Y., Burgard, S. A., and Needham, B. L. (2019). Marital status and cognitive impairment in the United States: evidence from the National Health and aging trends study. Ann. Epidemiol. 38, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2019.08.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maes, M., Qualter, P., Lodder, G. M., and Mund, M. (2022). How (not) to measure loneliness: a review of the eight most commonly used scales. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:10816. doi: 10.3390/ijerph191710816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maresova, P., Javanmardi, E., Barakovic, S., Barakovic Husic, J., Tomsone, S., Krejcar, O., et al. (2019). Consequences of chronic diseases and other limitations associated with old age–a scoping review. BMC Public Health 19, 1–17. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7762-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McMaughan, D. J., Oloruntoba, O., and Smith, M. L. (2020). Socioeconomic status and access to healthcare: interrelated drivers for healthy aging. Front. Public Health 8:231. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00231

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Monk, E. P., Kaufman, J., and Montoya, Y. (2021). Skin tone and perceived discrimination: health and aging beyond the binary in NSHAP 2015. J Gerontol Series B 76, S313–S321. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murman, D. L. (2015). The impact of age on cognition. Semin. Hear. 36, 111–121. doi: 10.1055/s-0035-1555115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Negi, N. J., Siegel, J. L., Sharma, P. B., and Fiallos, G. (2021). “The solitude absorbs and it oppresses”: ‘illegality’ and its implications on Latino immigrant day laborers' social isolation, loneliness and health. Soc. Sci. Med. 273:113737. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113737

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Newmyer, L., Verdery, A. M., Margolis, R., and Pessin, L. (2021). Measuring older adult loneliness across countries. J Gerontol Series B 76, 1408–1414. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Doherty, K., Lawrence, D., Wiencrot, A., Walsh, S., Satorius, J., Burgess, E., et al. (2021). Ongoing refinement and innovation in the data collection protocols of the third round of the National Social Life, health, and aging project. J Gerontol Series B 76, S215–S225. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab179

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Muircheartaigh, C., English, N., Pedlow, S., and Schumm, L. P. (2021). Sample design and estimation in the National Social Life, health, and aging project: round 3 (2015–2016). J Gerontol Series B 76, S207–S214. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab182

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojembe, B. U., Kalu, M. E., Donatus Ezulike, C., Iwuagwu, A. O., Ekoh, P. C., Oyinlola, O., et al. (2022). Understanding social and emotional loneliness among black older adults: a scoping review. J. Appl. Gerontol. 41, 2594–2608. doi: 10.1177/07334648221118357

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ong, A. D., Uchino, B. N., and Wethington, E. (2016). Loneliness and health in older adults: a mini-review and synthesis. Gerontology 62, 443–449. doi: 10.1159/000441651

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascoe, E. A., and Smart Richman, L. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 135:531. doi: 10.1037/a0016059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, C., Hedberg, E. C., Kozloski, M., Dale, W., and McClintock, M. K. (2014). Using and interpreting mental health measures in the National Social Life, health, and aging project. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 69, S99–S116. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu100

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearlin, L. I., and Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. J. Health Soc. Behav. 19, 2–21. doi: 10.2307/2136319

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez, H. C. S., Ikram, M. A., Direk, N., and Tiemeier, H. (2018). Prolonged grief and cognitive decline: a prospective population-based study in middle-aged and older persons. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26, 451–460. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2017.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perissinotto, C. M., and Covinsky, K. E. (2014). Living alone, socially isolated or lonely—what are we measuring? J. Gen. Intern. Med. 29, 1429–1431. doi: 10.1007/s11606-014-2977-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlman, D., and Peplau, L. A. (1981). “Toward a social psychology of loneliness” in Personal relationships. eds. R. Duck and R. Gilmour (London: Academic Press), 31–56.

Google Scholar

Peterson, R. L., Fain, M. J., Butler, A., Ehiri, J. E., and Carvajal, S. C. (2020). The role of social and behavioral risk factors in explaining racial disparities in age-related cognitive impairment: a structured narrative review. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 27, 173–196. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2019.1598539

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluim, C. F., Anzai, J. A., Martinez, J. E., Munera, D., Garza-Naveda, A. P., Vila-Castelar, C., et al. (2023). Associations among loneliness, purpose in life and subjective cognitive decline in ethnoracially diverse older adults living in the United States. J. Appl. Gerontol. 42, 376–386. doi: 10.1177/07334648221139479

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poey, J. L., Burr, J. A., and Roberts, J. S. (2017). Social connectedness, perceived isolation, and dementia: does the social environment moderate the relationship between genetic risk and cognitive well-being? Gerontologist 57, 1031–1040. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Puustinen, J., Nurminen, J., Löppönen, M., Vahlberg, T., Isoaho, R., Räihä, I., et al. (2011). Use of CNS medications and cognitive decline in the aged: Alongitudinal population-based study. BMC Geriatr. 11, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-70

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quiroz, Y. T., Solis, M., Aranda, M. P., Arbaje, A. I., Arroyo-Miranda, M., Cabrera, L. Y., et al. (2022). Addressing the disparities in dementia risk, early detection and care in Latino populations: highlights from the second Latinos & Alzheimer's symposium. Alzheimers Dement. 18, 1677–1686. doi: 10.1002/alz.12589

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Measur. 1, 385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, V. E., and King, S. D. (2017). Mental health for older LGBT adults. Annu. Rev. Gerontol. Geriatr. 37, 59–75. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0697-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., and Blackwell, A. (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 2029–2040. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002535

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rokach, A. (2018). The effect of gender and culture on loneliness: a mini review. Emerg Sci J 2, 59–64. doi: 10.28991/esj-2018-01128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossetti, H. C., Lacritz, L. H., Cullum, C. M., and Weiner, M. F. (2011). Normative data for the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) in a population-based sample. Neurology 77, 1272–1275. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318230208a

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs-Ericsson, N., Corsentino, E., and Cougle, J. R. (2009). Problems meeting basic needs predict cognitive decline in community-dwelling Hispanic older adults. J. Aging Health 21, 848–863. doi: 10.1177/0898264309340689

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoenmakers, E. C., van Tilburg, T. G., and Fokkema, T. (2012). Coping with loneliness: What do older adults suggest? Aging & Mental Health. 16, 353–360.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shega, J. W., Sunkara, P. D., Kotwal, A., Kern, D. W., Henning, S. L., McClintock, M. K., et al. (2014). Measuring cognition: the Chicago cognitive function measure in the national social life, health and aging project, wave 2. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 69, S166–S176. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu106

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiovitz-Ezra, S., and Ayalon, L. (2012). Use of direct versus indirect approaches to measure loneliness in later life. Res. Aging 34, 572–591. doi: 10.1177/0164027511423258

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Owen, N., Kunz-Ebrecht, S. R., and Brydon, L. (2004). Loneliness and neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory stress responses in middle-aged men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 593–611. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00086-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M., and Terracciano, A. (2020). Loneliness and risk of dementia. J Gerontol Series B 75, 1414–1422. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Theeke, L. A. (2010). Sociodemographic and health-related risks for loneliness and outcome differences by loneliness status in a sample of US older adults. Res. Gerontol. Nurs. 3, 113–125. doi: 10.3928/19404921-20091103-99

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tibirica, L., Jester, D. J., and Jeste, D. V. (2022). A systematic review of loneliness and social isolation among Hispanic/Latinx older adults in the United States. Psychiatry Res. 313:114568. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114568

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uher, R., Payne, J. L., Pavlova, B., and Perlis, R. H. (2014). Major depressive disorder in DSM-5: implications for clinical practice and research of changes from DSM-IV. Depress. Anxiety 31, 459–471. doi: 10.1002/da.22217

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Staden, W. C., and Coetzee, K. (2010). Conceptual relations between loneliness and culture. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 23, 524–529. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32833f2ff9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vetrano, D. L., Calderón-Larrañaga, A., Marengoni, A., Onder, G., Bauer, J. M., Cesari, M., et al. (2018). An international perspective on chronic multimorbidity: approaching the elephant in the room. J Gerontol Series A 73, 1350–1356. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx178

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Victor, C., Grenade, L., and Boldy, D. (2005). Measuring loneliness in later life: a comparison of differing measures. Rev. Clin. Gerontol. 15, 63–70. doi: 10.1017/S0959259805001723

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Viruell-Fuentes, E. A., and Schulz, A. J. (2009). Toward a dynamic conceptualization of social ties and context: implications for understanding immigrant and Latino health. Am. J. Public Health 99, 2167–2175. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.158956

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Lloyd-Evans, B., Giacco, D., Forsyth, R., Nebo, C., Mann, F., et al. (2017). Social isolation in mental health: a conceptual and methodological review. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 52, 1451–1461. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1446-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, R. S. (1973). Loneliness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Weuve, J., Barnes, L. L., de Leon, C. F. M., Rajan, K. B., Beck, T., Aggarwal, N. T., et al. (2018). Cognitive aging in black and white Americans: cognition, cognitive decline, and incidence of Alzheimer disease dementia. Epidemiology 29:151. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000747

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. R., Yu, Y., Jackson, J. S., and Anderson, N. B. (1997). Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J. Health Psychol. 2, 335–351. doi: 10.1177/135910539700200305

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., James, B. D., Leurgans, S. E., Buchman, A. S., and Bennett, D. A. (2015). Negative social interactions and risk of mild cognitive impairment in old age. Neuropsychology 29:561. doi: 10.1037/neu0000154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly, J. F., Barnes, L. L., et al. (2007). Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 234–240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization . (2021). Global status report on the public health response to dementia. Available at: https://iris.who.int/bitstream/handle/10665/344701/9789240033245-eng.pdf

Google Scholar

Yu, X., Westrick, A. C., and Kobayashi, L. C. (2023). Cumulative loneliness and subsequent memory function and rate of decline among adults aged≥ 50 in the United States, 1996 to 2016: cumulative loneliness and memory aging in the US. Alzheimers Dement. 19, 578–588. doi: 10.1002/alz.12734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaheed, A. B., Sharifian, N., Kraal, A. Z., Sol, K., Manly, J. J., Schupf, N., et al. (2020). Mediators and moderators of the association between perceived stress and episodic memory in diverse older adults. J. Inter. Neuropsychol. Soc. 27, 883–895.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Liu, H., and Choi, S. W. (2020). Early-life socioeconomic status, adolescent cognitive ability, and cognition in late midlife: evidence from the Wisconsin longitudinal study. Soc. Sci. Med. 244:112575. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112575

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., Ortiz, F., Nuñez, C., Elashoff, D., Woo, E., Apostolova, L. G., et al. (2015). Use of the MoCA in detecting early Alzheimer's disease in a Spanish-speaking population with varied levels of education. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord Extra 5, 85–95. doi: 10.1159/000365506

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer’s Disease and related dementias (ADRD), cognitive impairment (CI), perceived social isolation, minority aging, African American (AA), Hispanic and Latinx

Citation: Camacho D, Pacheco K, Moxley J, Aranda MP, Reid MC and Wethington E (2024) Loneliness and global cognitive functioning in racially and ethnically diverse US midlife and older adults. Front. Psychol. 15:1344044. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1344044

Received: 22 January 2024; Accepted: 15 April 2024;
Published: 19 June 2024.

Edited by:

Catalina Lopez-Quintero, University of Florida, United States

Reviewed by:

Denee Thomas Mwendwa, Howard University, United States
Dijuan Meng, Nanjing University of Chinese Medicine, China

Copyright © 2024 Camacho, Pacheco, Moxley, Aranda, Reid and Wethington. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David Camacho, ZGMxOTgxQHVpYy5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.