Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 February 2024
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Research on Teaching Strategies and Skills in Different Educational Stages View all 32 articles

Analogical thinking modifiability and math processing strategy

  • 1Talpiot Academic College, Graduate Program of Learning Disabilities, Holon, Israel
  • 2Faculty of Education, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel

A sample of 48 children in Grade 2 was randomly assigned to an experimental (n = 24) and a control group (n = 24). Both groups were administered the Analogical Modifiability Puzzle Test (AMPT) and Math Accuracy and Processing Strategy (MAPS) test before and after a teaching phase of the AMPT. The MAPS test includes scores for Accuracy, Processing Strategy, and a Math-Total. The findings reveal significant treatment x time interactions for AMPT (near-transfer) and MAPS (far-transfer) scores. Hierarchical regression analysis showed that AMPT post-teaching score added significantly to Math Total. The findings indicate that Math Accuracy and Math Processing Strategies are affected by mediation for analogical thinking and that modifiability of analogical thinking significantly predicts Math-Total.

Introduction

Analogical thinking is a fundamental cognitive operation that plays a role in executing many academic and daily life skills (Gentner et al., 2003), especially skills related to STEM domains (Alexander, 2017). Analogical thinking is a cognitive operation that helps individuals to represent information and objects as the structures of relationships. These structures can be compared and united in innovative ways depending on the contextual objectives (e.g., Gentner et al., 2003; Holyoak, 2004). Learning by analogy involves flexible conceptual learning and problem-solving (Goswami, 1992), especially in mathematics (Gelman, 2000). It was reported, for example, that, when US high-school students engage in solving math problems, they are found to be retrieving procedures rather than perceiving the relationship and similarities between concepts and strategies. In other words, their processing strategies for solving math problems lack analogical thinking (Stigler et al., 2010; Givvin et al., 2011).

Analogical thinking was reported to be central to mathematical learning and thinking (e.g., Richland and McDonough, 2010; Vamvakoussi and Vosniadou, 2012; Vamvakoussi, 2017, 2019; Zhao et al., 2021). Most studies on the use of analogy in math education refer to within-domain (i.e., mathematics) transfer of concepts such as the complex relation between geometry and arithmetic (Dantzig, 2005). The objectives of this study were (a) to examine the transfer of analogical thinking in a figural modality of a cognitive non-mathematical domain to math performance and math processing strategies and (b) to find the prediction of improvement in math performance by analogical thinking modifiability. The transfer is examined after a short-term teaching of figural analogies within a dynamic assessment (DA) procedure (Tzuriel and George, 2009; Tzuriel and Shamir, 2010; Tzuriel, 2021a). It should be noted that the teaching of figural analogies involves, in addition to transformative rules of analogical thinking, mediation of executive functions such as control of impulsivity, systematic exploratory behavior, and working memory (Feuerstein et al., 2002; Baddeley, 2012).

Analogical thinking and mathematical skills

Analogical thinking and math achievements have been found to be intimately related both at theoretical and practical levels (e.g., Bassok and Holyoak, 1989; Goswami, 1992; Gelman, 2000; Resing, 2000; English, 2004; Richland and Simms, 2015). Learning by analogy enables the use of common concepts and strategies between mathematical representations and enhances the understanding of new mathematical constructs and synchronization between mathematical components (Gelman, 2000; Gonzales et al., 2001; National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008). Using analogies in a learning process involves finding a systematic correspondence or “mapping” two domains: an acquainted source analog and a novel target, which is concrete or abstract (Sternberg and Nigro, 1980). For example, in mathematics, the acquainted source could refer to solving inequalities, and the projected novel target is solving equations (Gick and Holyoak, 1987). Many researchers agree that mathematical reasoning involves projecting (transfer) the abstract mathematical relations (e.g., proportion, equality, integral) into different contexts (e.g., Gelman, 2000; Gentner et al., 2003). Several researchers suggest the concepts of conceptual metaphors (e.g., Núñez and Lakoff, 2005), schema construction (Gick and Holyoak, 1983), and bridging analogies (e.g., Clement, 1993, 2013) to refer to cross-domain mappings (i.e., transfer concept from one domain to another).

In one of the intriguing cross-cultural studies, Richland et al. (2007) compared the effects of the use of analogies in math teaching among teachers in the United States, Hong Kong (HK) and Japan. The reasons for the comparison were based on two main earlier findings: (a) students in HK and Japan consistently outperformed students from the United States on the Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) international achievement tests (e.g., Mullis et al., 2004) and (b) different classroom instructional practices were observed between the teachers of the USA and two Asian countries. The goal of Richland et al.'s (2007) study was to ascertain what cognitive and memory factors may explain why US students achieve lower scores in international math tests. The findings indicated that the US teachers provide the same number of analogies as the Asian teachers; however, they provide much less cognitive processing support that facilitates relational learning. For example, Asian teachers used far more gestures and more than twice as many prompts of mental and visual imagery (in HK) and spatial supports for comparison (in HK and Japan) than US teachers. The authors interpreted these findings by attributing diverse cultural orientations to analogical reasoning. Teachers in Japan and HK are found to be more attentive to processing demands using analogical reasoning than US teachers. The processing demands include memory search to understand the analogy source, accessibility to a visual image that supports working memory (WM) during the construction of the analogy, and reliance on cues that guide the analogy process.

Richland and McDonough (2010) showed that reducing the processing load during instructional analogies facilitated learning and relational reasoning. These authors showed, in a sample of undergraduate students, that supporting comparative reasoning with cues facilitated the students' ability to discriminate between relevant analogs at a test given later. Similarly, Rittle-Johnson and Star (2007) compared two teaching strategies: simultaneous visual–spatial representations of analogs (i.e., problems solved in two ways were presented simultaneously and an explicit requirement to compare them) and solutions presented sequentially one after another. The authors reported that simultaneous representation enhanced the students' procedural and conceptual understanding and that their retention of the procedures, were better transferred to novel analogy problems.

The dynamic assessment of cognitive modifiability

The term dynamic assessment (DA) is defined as “an assessment of thinking, perception, learning, and problem-solving by an active teaching process aimed at modifying cognitive function” (Tzuriel, 2021a, p. 69). The main goal of DA is to assess the cognitive modifiability and learning potential using mediated learning strategies. Cognitive modifiability affords an indication of learning potential. However, the learning potential may be actualized depending on the provision of appropriate intervention aimed at the actualization of the individual's abilities (Tzuriel, 2001, 2002, 2021a,b; Feuerstein et al., 2002). The idea upon which DA is based is that measures of cognitive modifiability are intimately related to mediational processes by which the child learns how to process information far more than they are to the standardized measures of intelligence. In other words, the strategies of mediation used within the DA procedure are similar much more to the learning processes in an academic domain and other life situations compared to standardized testing methods and, therefore, provide better indications about future changes in cognitive structures.

In a previous study with the Analogical Thinking Modifiability Test (AMPT, Tzuriel, 2022), the goals were (a) to investigate whether mediation with Construction analogies is more powerful than mediation with Closed analogies to improve analogical thinking. In Construction analogies, the child must construct the answer using tactile colored puzzle-like parts and a plate with four (2 × 2) “windows” in which the puzzle-like design is created. The solution is done in a sequential process that does not require the activation of WM. In the Closed analogies version, the child is presented with three parts of the analogy (A:B::C:__) and is asked to choose the correct answer from six designs presented at the bottom of the page. (b) The second goal was to assess whether the mediation of solving analogies improves spatial WM, as measured by the Children's Spatial Working Memory test (CSWM, Tzuriel, 2021c). More specifically, the question was which type of mediation—mediation of Construction or mediation of Closed analogies—transfers better to mathematics? (c) The last goal was to explore the correlation between analogical thinking and WM. The findings indicated clearly that mediation with Construction analogies was more effective in enhancing analogical thinking than mediation with Closed analogies and that both groups improved their spatial WM. The reason for the superiority of Construction analogies is that the format of Construction analogies contains imminent characteristics of self-regulation and systematic sequential and progressive activity, which reduce cognitive load and WM requirements. In Closed analogies, on the other hand, the examinee is required to maintain the solution in his/her memory sketchpad (Baddeley, 2012) during the search for the correct answer and sorting out distractors. The explanation was further supported by the differential correlation between AMPT performance time and CSWM. The correlations found for Construction analogies were 0.14 and 0.22 (p > 0.05), for pre-teaching and post-teaching, respectively. The correlations for Closed analogies were 0.47 and 0.47 (p < 0.001), for pre- and post-teaching, respectively. It seems that, in Closed analogies, a longer performance time helps individuals to cope with the task's requirement for WM. In contrast, Construction analogies do not require the activation of WM and hence its low correlation with spatial WM. The findings of the previous study raised the question of whether the improvement in analogical thinking was caused by the mediation strategies per se or the mere exposure and practice of the analogy problems. This question is explored further in the current study by contrasting a group that received the mediation of analogical thinking with a group that practiced the same teaching items without mediation. I chose to focus on Closed analogies, which are found to be more related to WM than to Construction analogies. Furthermore, in the current study, the focus was extended to include the far-transfer effects of mediation of analogical thinking on math performance and math processing strategies conceived to be related to the executive functions of WM (e.g., Richland et al., 2007).

In the current study, the measurement/research version of DA was applied (Tzuriel, 1997, 2001, 2020, 2021a). This version includes the preliminary, pre-teaching, teaching, and post-teaching phases; the last three phases consist of parallel items. In the preliminary phase, the child is acquainted with the dimensions of the tasks and cognitive strategies to solve the problems (e.g., referring to task dimensions). The focus of the teaching phase is on processing the analogical rules of transformation, efficient strategies (e.g., use of one dimension at a time), applying verbal anticipation of a solution, and making efficient use of executive functions such as self-regulation and WM. Cognitive modifiability is measured quantitatively and is indicated by the pre- to post-teaching improvement. Previous DA findings revealed that children in kindergarten and first years of primary school could solve complex analogies after a teaching phase than what might have been expected developmentally of children their age (Tzuriel and Klein, 1985; Tzuriel, 2000, 2001, 2020, 2021a). Children in Grade 2 were randomly assigned to the experimental (EX) and control (CN) groups. Both groups received the AMPT and the Math Accuracy and Processing Strategy (MAPS) Test (see Measures). Children in the EX group received a teaching phase of analogies using mediation strategies by trained experimenters. Children in the CN group solved the same analogies without teaching. Both groups were evaluated again after the intervention with the AMPT and MAPS test.

Hypotheses

1. Children in the EX group who received the mediation of analogical thinking will demonstrate higher improvement on AMPT analogies from pre- to post-teaching than children in the CN group.

2. Children in the EX group who received the mediation of analogical thinking will demonstrate higher improvement in the MAPS test than children in the CN group.

3. Cognitive modifiability of analogical thinking will significantly predict the modifiability in MAPS scores.

Method

Participants

The participants were 48 children (25 boys and 23 girls) in Grade 2 who were randomly selected from six classes from six different schools in the central region of Israel. Students with learning problems (e.g., learning disability, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder) were excluded from the sample. The children were randomly assigned to the EX (n =24) and CN (n= 24) groups. The mean age of children in the EX group was 87.96 months (SD = 2.90) and in the CN group was 89.33 months (SD = 3.17). Both groups were similar on variables of age, t(46) = −1.56 2, p > 0.05, gender distribution, χ2 = 0.08 p > 0.05, occupation level of fathers, χ2 = 0.22 p > 0.05, and of mothers χ2 = 0.93 p > 0.05 (on a 1–5 scale). The trainers were graduate students in a Learning Disabilities Program of a college for teachers with 4–15 years of teaching experience. All trainers received 8 h of training prior to the administration of tests and mediation strategies. The training included proficiency in test administration, specific mediation strategies of the analogies in the teaching phase, practice of administration with two children prior to the start of the study, and accurate recording and scoring.

Measures

The analogical modifiability puzzle test

The AMPT (Tzuriel, 2021a,b, 2022) is a DA measure of cognitive modifiability in the domain of analogical thinking. The AMPT contains three parallel sets of problems, each containing 12 items. The AMPT can be administered either in the Construction analogies or Closed analogies version (Tzuriel, 2022). In the current study, the Closed analogies version was applied. Before the start of the test, children are presented with two introductory problems aimed at familiarizing the children with the dimensions of AMPT (i.e., color, shape of puzzle parts) and the rules of problem-solving. The items are arranged progressively from easy to difficult problems. In the Closed analogies version, which is used in the current study, the child is presented at the top of the page with three sections of the analogy (see Figure 1), and the child must choose the fourth section, from six answers, of which five are distractors. To solve the problem and complete the puzzle, the child must consider the changes from the left to the right section and from the top to the bottom section. The correct solution requires considering changes in the color and position of the panels and shapes from left to right and from top to bottom.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example of an easy item from the Analogical Modifiability Puzzle Test (AMPT) (by permission of the author).

In Figure 1, the colors of the background panels change from left to right (white and blue to blue and white). The hearts on the left sections that are empty are filled up with red hearts on the right, and the empty triangles on the top are filled up with red triangles on the bottom. The correct solution is number 3 (blue and white panels and red hearts and triangles). Cronbach's alpha reliability coefficients for the pre- and post-teaching phases of the AMPT based on a sample of kindergarten children (n = 96) were0.94 and 0.95, respectively (Tzuriel, 2022). Cronbach's alpha coefficients for the Pre- and Post-Teaching phases in the current sample were 0.94 and 0.73, respectively. Previous findings showed that a short-term intervention within a DA procedure using the AMPT improved spatial working memory (SWM) and that the improvement in SWM was even more powerful (ηp2 = 61) than the effect on analogical thinking (ηp2 = 0.40) per se, which was the targeted mediation activity (Tzuriel, 2022).

Math accuracy and processing strategy test (MAPS)

The MAPS test (Tzuriel, 2024), designed specifically for the current study, is aimed at the examination of both performance accuracy and processing strategy. In each item, the children are presented a math problem and asked to solve it and to write how they solved the problem (processing strategy). The test is composed of 10 items and is aimed at children in the second grade. In each item, the child is asked first to solve a mathematical problem and then to explain the strategy used to reach a solution. The explanation was aimed at verifying that the answer is based on a strategy and not on a technical performance. Separate scores are given to accurate answers and to explanations revealing an efficient strategy. A separate score of 1 was given in each item to an accurate answer and to an efficient processing strategy. Examples of problems are shown in Table 1. The Cronbach's alpha coefficients based on the current sample, for pre- and post-intervention, respectively, were Math Accuracy, 0.82 and 0.77; Math Processing Strategy, 0.84 and 0.83; Math Total, 0.77 and 0.77.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Examples of problems from the Math Accuracy and Processing Strategy (MAPS) Test.

Procedure

All children were administered the AMPT individually using the measurement/research format and the MAPS test before and after the intervention phase. Children in the EX group received the mediation with 12 analogy teaching items for 1 h. Mediation was focused on the systematic exploration of task dimensions, the deletion of impossible alternatives, the delay of rapid answer and inhibition of behavior, the comparison of the different options, and the verbal anticipation of the answer. Children in the CN group practiced the teaching items with no mediation for the same amount of time. The test was administered in a quiet room in the school during regular school time. The pre-intervention phase was administered to all participants for 1 h followed by the intervention and post-intervention phases that were given between 1 and 5 days later; both tests were administered on the same day with a small break of 10 min in-between.

Results

The effects of teaching analogies on analogical thinking and mathematics performance

Table 2 presents the means and standard deviations of the study's variables. To examine the effect of teaching on analogical thinking modifiability (near transfer), a repeated measures ANOVA of Treatment × Time (2 × 2) for the AMPT scores was conducted. Similarly, repeated measures ANOVAs for the variables (far-transfer) of math were conducted. The ANOVA findings for the AMPT are shown in Table 3 and the ANOVA findings for the math variables are shown in Table 4. Finally, we examined the prediction of Math-Total scores by analogical modifiability using the hierarchical regression procedure. The findings (Tables 3, 4) reveal significant Treatment × Time interactions for all the dependent variables. The interactions are portrayed in Figures 25. A summary of the simple effects analyses of the interactions is shown in Table 5.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means and standard deviations of pre- and post-teaching scores of AMPT, math accuracy, math processing strategy, and math total in the EX and CN groups.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Repeated measures ANOVA's of AMPT scores by treatment and time.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Repeated measures ANOVAs of math variables: math accuracy, math processing strategy, and math total by treatment and time.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Interaction of treatment × time on AMPT scores.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Simple effects analyses of treatment × time interactions of AMPT, math accuracy, math processing strategy, and math total variables.

Treatment × time interaction on AMPT scores

The significant Treatment × Time interaction (Figure 2) revealed that children in the EX group improved their scores from pre- to post-teaching more than children in the CN group. Simple effects analyses (Table 5) revealed no significant group differences in the pre-teaching phase. However, in the post-teaching phase, children in the EX group outperformed children in the CN group. Within-group analyses revealed a significant improvement from pre- to post-teaching only in the EX group.

Treatment × time interaction on math accuracy scores

The significant interaction of Treatment × Time (Figure 3) indicates that children in the EX group showed higher pre- to post-teaching improvement than children in the CN group. Simple effects analyses (Table 5) revealed no significant group differences in the pre- and post-teaching phases. However, within-group analyses showed that only the EX group showed significant pre- to post-teaching improvement.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Interaction of treatment × time on math accuracy scores.

Treatment × time interaction on math processing strategy scores

The significant interaction of Treatment × Time (Figure 4) indicates that children in the EX group showed higher pre- to post-teaching improvement than children in the CN group. The simple effects of between-group analyses (Table 5) revealed no significant group differences in either pre- or post-teaching phases. Within-group analyses showed significant pre- to post-teaching improvement in both the EX and CN groups, though the slope of change was steeper in the EX group.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interaction of treatment × time on math processing strategy scores.

Treatment × time interaction on math total scores

As can be seen in Figure 5, children in the EX group demonstrated higher pre- to post-teaching improvement than children in the CN group. Simple effects between-group analyses (Table 5) revealed no significant group differences in the pre-teaching phase, but in the post-teaching phase, children in the EX group outperformed children in the CN group. Simple effects of within-group analyses showed that both groups revealed pre- to post-teaching improvement, though the EX group demonstrated a much higher improvement.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Interaction of treatment × time on math total scores.

Hierarchical regression analysis: prediction of post-teaching MAPS-total by AMPT scores

An intriguing objective of the current study was to assess whether analogical thinking modifiability predicts the modifiability of math performance. According to Hypothesis 3, the modifiability of Total-Math will be predicted by analogical thinking modifiability. In other words, the hypothesis is that a short-term intervention of analogical thinking within a DA procedure will affect not only analogical thinking but will be transferred to math thinking. This hypothesis was evaluated by a three-step hierarchical regression analysis where the predicted variable (criterion) was post-intervention MAPS-Total. In Step 1, the variable of age (in months) was introduced. In Step 2, the variables of pre-teaching variables of MAPS-Total and AMPT were introduced, and in Step 3, the post-teaching variables of MAPS-Total and AMPT were introduced. The modifiability of MAPS-Total score was indicated by the post-teaching score after statistically “washing out” the variance contributed by the MAPS-Total pre-teaching score. Similarly, the modifiability of AMPT score was demonstrated by the post-teaching AMPT score after statistically “washing out” for the variance contributed by the AMPT pre-teaching score. The results, as shown in Table 6, indicate that Age was not a significant predictor of the post-intervention math performance. The variance of Age was negligible, F(1, 46) = 0.01, p < 0.92, and R2 = 0.00. In Step 2, however, as expected, the pre-intervention math score was a significant predictor of the post-intervention math score (β = 0.86). The pre-intervention AMPT score was not found as a significant predictor (β = 0.04). The significant prediction of Step 2 added 75% to the prediction of Math-Total score, F(3, 44) = 45.63, p < 0.0001, ΔR2 = 0.75, p < 0.0001.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Hierarchical regression analyses of post-teaching math-total score by pre- and post-intervention AMPT scores.

In Step 3, after controlling for the variance of the pre-intervention variables, the post-intervention AMPT variable has emerged as a significant predictor (β = 0.24) of post-teaching MAPS-Total (criterion variable), thus confirming Hypothesis 3. The overall prediction of post-intervention MAPS score was significant, F(4, 43) = 41.46, p < 0001, R2 = 0.79. The AMPT post-intervention score, as an indicator of analogical modifiability, added significantly to the prediction of post-intervention Math-Total, ΔR2 = 0.04 p < 0.008.

Discussion

The results of this study support the hypothesis that the mediation of analogical thinking within a DA procedure will significantly improve both analogical thinking (Hypothesis 1, near transfer) and math performance (Hypothesis 2, far transfer). These findings agree with the previous findings that showed an intimate relationship between analogical thinking and math thinking, especially with math processing strategies (Núñez and Lakoff, 2005; Richland et al., 2007; Vamvakoussi, 2017, 2019). However, the results of this study go beyond previous studies by indicating that a short-term intervention in analogical thinking can be transferred to math performance, especially to math processing strategies. Math problem-solving has been conceptualized as a transfer challenge requiring students to apply strategies, skills, and knowledge to novel problems (Fuchs and Fuchs, 2005). This idea is congruent with the schema construction theory according to which students' development of schemas occurs when students group problems into types that require the same solution (e.g., Gick and Holyoak, 1983; Chen, 1999). It should be emphasized that the intervention for solving the AMPT analogies was not limited only to specific strategies of solving analogies per se but included mediation for the control of impulsivity, considering two or more sources of information simultaneously, activation of WM, systematic exploratory behavior, and verbal anticipation of a solution. There was no way of separating these cognitive functions from the content aspects of the visual analogies of the AMPT.

It is important to note that math processing has much in common with analogical thinking. For example, the question of “In how much the sum of 64 + 8 is smaller than 64 + 28?” requires a simple analogy—if 8 is smaller than 28 in 20 and 64 is a constant, then 64 + 8 is smaller in 20 than 64 + 28. The analogical processing is a much more efficient way to solve the problem than the meticulous procedure of adding up the sums, calculating each segment, and subtracting one from the other.

The teaching of AMPT items within the DA procedure was found in an earlier study on Grade 1 children to be powerful in improving not only their analogical thinking but was also transferred to significant improvement of spatial WM (Tzuriel, 2022). The improvement in spatial working memory was impressive, as indicated by a more powerful effect (ηp2 = 0.61) than that found on analogical thinking (ηp2 = 0.40), which was the targeted domain of mediation strategies. Moreover, the correlations of AMPT analogies (both pre- and post-teaching) with spatial WM was.47 (p < 0.001). It should be emphasized that the contents of the MAPS tasks as well as the spatial WM task were different from the contents of the intervention contents, as represented by the AMPT analogies; thus, the improvement would be attributed to the mediation characteristics of the AMPT. Our findings might pave the way for long-term intervention studies aimed at establishing the effects of analogical thinking intervention on the development of math skills. The question is, of course, to what extent is this transfer durable, especially since the intervention was short within a DA procedure and the well-known phenomenon of fading-out of intervention effects (e.g., Bailey et al., 2020).

The results of the hierarchical regression analyses confirm Hypothesis 3. The results show that the variable of post-teaching AMPT significantly contributed to the prediction of math ability after controlling for the variables of pre-teaching scores of analogical thinking and math. These intriguing findings imply that children who demonstrate analogical thinking modifiability in a DA procedure are also those who show pre- to post-intervention modifiability in mathematical thinking. The meaning of this finding is that a short-term mediation within a DA procedure was transferred to math domain enhancing both math accuracy and math processing strategies. This conclusion, however, should be studied further with different age groups, varying the duration of the intervention and employing different analogy tests. It should be emphasized that the finding of a far transfer after a short-term intervention does not mean that the change is durable. One should bear in mind that the intervention in the current study was within a DA procedure, which provides an important indication for the transfer of intervention of analogical thinking to mathematical processing. For a durable long-lasting effect of the intervention, a more comprehensive intervention and a follow-up assessment should be performed. The finding of a far-transfer of analogical thinking to math processing strategies is of special importance for teaching math to students who experience difficulties in processing math problems. Teaching analogical problem-solving might facilitate math processing even more than the direct teaching of math.

Limitations

The study has several limitations. First, the study was conducted on a limited sample of 48 Grade 2 students. Despite the impressive findings of far-transfer effects from analogical thinking training to math performance, more research involving bigger samples is required. Second, the MAPS test is composed of only 10 items, which limits the validity of findings. Although there is a separate score for accuracy and for processing strategy (a total of 20) for each item, a much larger scale with more items would help to establish the findings. In fact, in a further unpublished study (Tzuriel, 2024), steps were made to enlarge the number of items in MAPS. The third limitation is related to the lack of recording of the specific mediation strategies used in the intervention phase of the analogies. The assumption is that all examiners use the same mediation strategies. A higher resolution of the specific strategies used and their effect on the transfer to math domain would facilitate not only theoretical construction but also practical implications for teachers.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The study involving human participants was reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of Talpiot Academic College. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the parents of children prior to start of data gathering and based on Israel Ministry of Education requirements.

Author contributions

DT: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing.

Funding

The author declares that financial support for this publication was received from Bar-Ilan University and Talpiot Academic College.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alexander, A. (2017). Relational reasoning in STEM domains: a foundation for academic development. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 29, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s10648-016-9383-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, H., Duncan, C. J., Cunha, F., Foorman, B. R., and Yeager, S. (2020). Persistence and fade-out of educational intervention effects: mechanisms and potential solutions. Psychol. Sci. Public Int. 21, 55–97. doi: 10.1177/1529100620915848

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassok, M., and Holyoak, K. J. (1989). Interdomain transfer between isomorphic topics in algebra and physics. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 15, 153–166. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.15.1.153

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Z. (1999). Schema induction in children's analogical problem-solving. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 703–715. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.703

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clement, J. (1993). Using bridging analogies and anchoring intuitions to deal with students' preconceptions in physics. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 30, 1241–1257. doi: 10.1002/tea.3660301007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clement, J. (2013). “Roles for explanatory models and analogies in conceptual change,” in International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change, 2nd Edn, ed S. Vosniadou (Abingdon-on-Thames, Oxfordshire: Routledge), 412–446.

Google Scholar

Dantzig, T. (2005). Number: The Language of Science, 4th Edn. London: Pi Press.

Google Scholar

English, L. D. (2004). “Mathematical and analogical reasoning in early childhood,” in Mathematical and Analogical Reasoning of Young Learners, ed L. D. English (Abingdon-on-Thames, Oxfordshire: Routledge), 13–34.

Google Scholar

Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein, R. S., Falik, L. H., and Rand, Y. (2002). The Dynamic Assessment of Cognitive Modifiability: The Learning Propensity Assessment Device. Jerusalem: ICELP Press.

Google Scholar

Fuchs, L., and Fuchs, D. (2005). Enhancing mathematical problem solving for students with disabilities. J. Spec. Educ. 39, 45–57. doi: 10.1177/00224669050390010501

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, R. (2000). The epigenesis of mathematical thinking. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 21, 27–37. doi: 10.1016/S0193-3973(99)00048-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., and Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and transfer: a general role for analogical encoding. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 393–408. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.393

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gick, M. L., and Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 1–38. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90002-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gick, M. L., and Holyoak, K. J. (1987). “The cognitive basis of knowledge transfer,” in Transfer of Learning: Contemporary Research and Application, eds S. M. Cormier, and J. D. Hagman (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 9–45.

Google Scholar

Givvin, K. B., Stigler, J. W., and Thompson, B. J. (2011). What community college developmental mathematics students understand about mathematics. Part II: the interviews. MathAMATYC Educ. 2, 4–18.

Google Scholar

Gonzales, P., Calsyn, C., Jocelyn, L. J., Mak, K., Kastberg A.rafeh, S., Williams, T., et al. (2001). Pursuing Excellence: Comparisons of International Eighth-Grade Mathematics and Science Achievement from a U.S. Perspective, 1995-1999. Initial Findings from the Third International Mathematics and Science Study–Repeat. Statistical Analysis Report. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, US Department of Education.

Google Scholar

Goswami, U. (1992). Analogical Reasoning in Children. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Holyoak, K. J. (2004). “Analogy,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning, eds K. J. Holayoak, and R. J. Forrison (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 117–142.

Google Scholar

Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzales, E. J., and Chrostowski, S. J. (2004). TIMSS 2003 International Mathematics Report: Findings From IEA's Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study at the Fourth and Eighth Grades. Washington, DC: TIMSS and PIRLS International Study Center.

Google Scholar

National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008). Foundations for Success: The Final Report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.

Google Scholar

Núñez, R., and Lakoff, G. (2005). “The cognitive foundations of mathematics: the role of conceptual metaphor,” in Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, ed J. I. D. Campbell (Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press), 109–124.

Google Scholar

Resing, W. C. M. (2000). “Assessing the learning potential for inductive reasoning (LIR) in young children,” in Dynamic Assessment: Prevailing Models and Applications, eds C. S. Lidz, and J. G. Elliott (JAI/Elsevier Science), 229–262.

Google Scholar

Richland, E. A., and Simms, N. (2015). Analogy, higher order thinking, and education, WIREs Cogn. Sci. 6, 177–192. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1336

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richland, L. E., and McDonough, I. M. (2010). Learning by analogy: discriminating between potential analogs. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 35, 28–43. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richland, L. E., Zur, O., and Holyoak, K. J. (2007). Cognitive supports analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science 316, 1128–1129. doi: 10.1126/science.1142103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rittle-Johnson, B., and Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve equations. J. Educ. Psychol. 99, 561–574. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.561

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., and Nigro, G. (1980). Developmental patterns in the solution of verbal analogies. Child Dev. 51, 27–38. doi: 10.2307/1129586

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stigler, J. W., Givvin, K. B., and Thompson, B. (2010). What community college developmental mathematics students understand about mathematics. MathAMATYC Educ. 10, 4–16.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (1997). A novel dynamic assessment approach for young children: major dimensions and current research. Educ. Child Psychol. 14, 83–108.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2000). Dynamic assessment of young children: educational and intervention perspectives. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 12, 385–435.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2001). Dynamic Assessment of Young Children. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2002). “Dynamic assessment of learning potential,” in Encyclopedia of Education, 2nd edn. (McMillan Press), 127–131.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2020). “Dynamic cognitive assessment for preschool age children,” in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education, ed L. F. Zhang (Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190264093.013.942

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2021a). Mediated Learning Experience and Cognitive Modifiability. Springer Nature.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2021b). Analogical Modifiability Puzzle Test-Instruction Manual. Bar-Ilan University.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2021c). The Childrens' Spatial Working Memory (CSWM) test. Instruction Manual. Faculty of Education, Bar-Ilan University, Israel.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2022). Construction versus closed analogies: Effects of mediation and relation to working memory. Formazione Insegnamento. 20, 34–49. doi: 10.7346/-feis-XX-02-22_04

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D. (2024). Math Accuracy and Processing Strategy (Maps) Test (Unpublished Test). Talpiot Academic College, Israel.

Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D., and George, T. (2009). Improvement of analogical reasoning and academic achievement by the Analogical Reasoning Programme (ARP). Educ. Child Psychol. 26, 71–94. doi: 10.53841/bpsecp.2009.26.3.71

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D., and Klein, P. S. (1985). Analogical thinking modifiability in disadvantaged, regular, special education, and mentally retarded children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 13, 539–552. doi: 10.1007/BF00923140

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzuriel, D., and Shamir, A. (2010). Mediation strategies and cognitive modifiability in young children as a function of peer mediation with young children program and training in analogies versus math tasks. J. Cogn. Educ. Psychol. 9, 48–72. doi: 10.1891/1945-8959.9.1.48

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vamvakoussi, X. (2017). Using analogies to facilitate conceptual change in mathematics learning. ZDM 49, 497–507. doi: 10.1007/s11858-017-0857-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vamvakoussi, X. (2019). The use of analogies in mathematics instruction: affordances and challenges. Math. Cognit. Learn. 5, 247–268. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-815952-1.00010-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vamvakoussi, X., and Vosniadou, S. (2012). Bridging the gap between the dense and the discrete: the number line and the “rubber line” bridging analogy. Math. Think. Learn. 14, 265–284. doi: 10.1080/10986065.2012.717378

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, H., Alexander, P. A., and Sun, Y. (2021). Relational reasoning's contributions to mathematical thinking and performance in Chinese elementary and middle-school students. J. Educ. Psychol. 113, 279–303. doi: 10.1037/edu0000595

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: analogical thinking, dynamic assessment, mathematical performance, mediated learning, executive functions, mathematical processing strategy

Citation: Tzuriel D (2024) Analogical thinking modifiability and math processing strategy. Front. Psychol. 15:1339591. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1339591

Received: 16 November 2023; Accepted: 31 January 2024;
Published: 28 February 2024.

Edited by:

Linda Saraiva, Polytechnic Institute of Viana do Castelo, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Zhuoying Wang, The University of Texas at Austin, United States
Elizabeth Jane Newton, London South Bank University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Tzuriel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David Tzuriel, ZGF2aWQudHp1cmllbCYjeDAwMDQwO2JpdS5hYy5pbA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.