Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 12 February 2024
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Fluid Teams View all 9 articles

Composition considerations for fluid teams: a review

  • 1Florida Maxima Corporation, Orlando, FL, United States
  • 2Air Force Research Laboratory, Dayton, OH, United States

The need exists to better understand how to comprise fluid teams—teams that are assembled on short notice, from members with little to no familiarity, who come together to carry out a time-limited task, and then disband. Due to the ever-increasing complexity of the modern workplace, the demand for these types of fluid teams is growing in task domains such as the military, aviation, healthcare, and industry. The aim of this paper is to review the team composition literature to shed light on composition considerations for forming fluid teams.

Introduction

Thanks to more nuanced models of team composition, novel research and analytic methods, and a plethora of meta-analytic integrations, we know a good deal about what makes teams tick. While there is still much to be learned, the question of how to best compose a team has become clearer. A cumulative body of research points to various attributes that, when combined, promote or detract from team processes and performance. Given that the weight of empirical evidence is based on traditional teams, the question remains to what degree does the current state of the science on team composition generalize to fluid teams. Consequently, the aim of this paper is to shed light on this issue. To accomplish this, we first address the concept of fluid teams and consider how they differ from traditional teams. Second, we review the team composition literature with a focus on fluid teams and summarizing the current state of the science on fluid team composition.

Fluid teams

Research on teams has traditionally focused on the study of team inputs, processes, emergent states, and outputs of intact teams, or teams with a relatively stable membership. Given this focus, much is now known about the workings of these types of teams (Driskell et al., 2018). However, research over the years has also examined the operation of ad hoc or temporary teams. Ziller (1965), in a paper titled Toward a theory of open and closed groups differentiated between open vs. closed groups. Open groups were characterized as having unstable membership (team members may be added, subtracted, or replaced in the group), reduced time perspective (tending to be short-lived), and a continuous state of disequilibrium or instability. Other research has described similar team contexts in terms of ad hoc teams (Lorge et al., 1958), temporary teams (Saunders and Ahuja, 2006; Blomqvist and Cook, 2018), swift starting action teams (McKinney et al., 2005; Wildman et al., 2012), and emergent response teams (Majchrzak et al., 2007). Driskell et al. (2023) provide a specific definition of a fluid team as comprised of four core characteristics; (1) team members are rapidly assembled to address an immediate problem, (2) members are assembled based on domain expertise and typically have no prior history or experience working together, (3) the team must begin work on a task that is immediate, time-critical, and of short duration, and (4) at completion of the task, the team disbands with little likelihood of further interaction. These types of teams, assembled temporarily from experts across various domains to accomplish critical, time-sensitive tasks, have become increasingly prevalent in various contexts such as the military, aviation, healthcare, and industry (Bell et al., 2023, this issue; Grossman et al., 2024, this issue; Linhardt and Salas, 2023, this issue; Capiola et al., 2020). Although fluid teams are typically convened to address complex and demanding tasks, little is known regarding how these teams should be assembled to support effective performance in this unique context.

Team composition

Team composition research is concerned with identifying and arranging the sets of attributes present among team members that facilitate effective team performance. The combination of team member attributes serves to moderate team performance through impacting the attitudes, behaviors, and cognitions of teams during task performance (Bell et al., 2018). Some of the more well-studied attributes that team composition research has examined include demographic diversity (e.g., Carter et al., 2019), task-related diversity (e.g., Joshi and Roh, 2009), cognitive ability (e.g., Devine and Phillips, 2001), personality (e.g., Bell, 2007; Driskell and Salas, 2013), and collective orientation (Driskell et al., 2010).

When examining individual level attributes—that is, what individual team members bring to the team—composition researchers are tasked with two primary objectives: (1) identifying the type of attributes that contribute to effective team performance, and (2) determining how these attributes should be configured within the team. Stewart (2006) noted that “One line of research examines aggregated characteristics to assess whether the inclusion of individuals with desirable dispositions and abilities improves team performance. A related but somewhat different area of research looks at how heterogeneity of individual characteristics relates to team outcomes” (p. 30). While related, these aggregation approaches represent a useful categorization method when trying to explain the relationship between selection criteria and team outcomes.

Given that fluid teams represent an understudied type of team, the collective wisdom on how to compose fluid teams is limited. The prevailing approach to assembling rapid forming fluid teams is to simply match expertise to the task with little to no thought about attributes outside of sheer capability. The following sections on team composition deal specifically with how the distribution of individual attributes among a team combine to influence team performance. That is, the extant research on group composition for both surface-level attributes and deep-level attributes will be reviewed and the relationship between these attributes and team performance identified.

Analytic approach

Figure 1 serves as an advanced organizer for this review. Because this line of research is necessarily complex, a guiding framework will assist the reader in the following exposition. For the purposes of this review, we dichotomize team member attributes as surface-level or deep-level attributes. Surface-level attributes of team members refer to overt easily distinguishable cues such as age or gender that are readily perceptible in short-term interactions. Composition researchers have further classified surface-level attributes as demographic attributes (e.g., gender, race, and age) or task-related attributes (e.g., educational level, organizational position). This dichotomization maps to the two predominant perspectives explaining the diversity-performance relationship: The social categorization/intergroup bias/similarity–attraction perspective (e.g., Byrne, 1971; Turner, 1985) and the informational diversity–cognitive resource perspective (e.g., Cox and Blake, 1991). The social categorization/intergroup bias/similarity–attraction perspective proposes that homogeneous teams (whose team members are similar in demographic characteristics such as gender, age, socioeconomic status) should outperform diverse teams due to mutual attraction among team members and social categorization processes, which can foster cooperation. The informational diversity–cognitive resource perspective proposes that diversity (especially in terms of task-related attributes such as expertise or functional background) should provide a greater corpus of knowledge, skills, abilities, and perspectives to draw upon, which could enhance team performance. As Figure 1 illustrates, we intend to examine both demographic attributes and task-related attributes that have been examined in the team composition literature, as well as relevant moderators of these effects.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of the review strategy.

We also examine deep-level attributes such as values or personality that tend to manifest themselves over time (Driskell et al., 2006; Bell, 2007). Broadly, we expect surface-level attributes to be more strongly predictive of team performance in fluid teams. This assertion is based on the notion that information regarding deep-level attributes (e.g., personality, values, and beliefs) cannot be easily conveyed in a fluid team context in which the team members are rapidly assembled, with little or no familiarity, and perform a short-term, time-limited task. As Harrison et al. (2003, p. 639) note, fluid team members have “neither initial familiarity nor a chance to develop familiarity over time.” Thus, a longer team tenure allows information to be conveyed between team members, which in turn, allows team members to unmask deep-level attributes via self-disclosure. More recent research on faultlines has also supported this claim (Zhang and Chen, 2023). However, deep-level attributes are still expected to impact performance in fluid teams, especially in instances in which opportunities exist to engage in mutual self-disclosure, when value differences are foreknown, or when surface-level attributes such as organizational affiliation may imply value differences. Moreover, some deep-level attributes such as conscientiousness may be easily construed in a task environment where proxy cues such as hard-working behavior are readily available.

The review process included searching online databases (e.g., PsychInfo) for relevant literature using the search terms team composition and team performance, and then using an ancestry search approach to locate relevant studies from the reference lists of obtained studies. In addition, the review and analysis rely heavily on a set of meta-analytic integrations that have been conducted on team composition (e.g., Bell, 2007; Joshi and Roh, 2009; Bell et al., 2011; van Dijk et al., 2012; Carter et al., 2019; Gonzalez-Mulé et al., 2020; Niler et al., 2021; Triana et al., 2021; Byron et al., 2023; Kilcullen et al., 2023) (Summary data from these meta-analyses are provided in a Supplementary Table 1) Most of the relevant obtained studies have been published in the last two decades and published in a wide range of journals, including the Journal of Applied Psychology, Journal of Management, Personnel Psychology, Small Group Research, and Journal of Organizational Behavior, among others.

Furthermore, given that the results of these prior analyses identify a large number of predictor-performance relationships and moderators, we try to focus our discussion on those studies and results most potentially relevant to fluid teams. For example, recent research has examined how faultlines can change and evolve over time (Meister et al., 2020) but this is less relevant to fluid teams that operate and then disband within a short time frame. In many cases we seek to “read between the lines” to identify certain proxies that can be leveraged to glean information about fluid team composition. One such proxy that has been examined in the various meta-analytic integrations is study setting (i.e., laboratory studies versus field studies). As Bell et al. (2011, p. 13) note, “Study setting is…likely to be highly correlated with the length of time that a team has been together.” That is, laboratory studies are likely to be composed of ad hoc teams compared to field studies, which often are comprised of intact teams. Thus, this comparison may shed light on fluid team composition. That said, caution should be taken when trying to draw direct comparisons.

Surface-level attributes

Surface-level diversity: demographic

Demographic attributes represent fairly immutable characteristics of individuals such as age and sex. The following sections describe the overall composite demographic diversity-performance relationship, followed by the diversity-performance relationship for the most studied individual attributes.

Overall, composite demographic diversity (i.e., demographic diversity considered as a unidimensional or composite variable) has been shown to have a weak negative to near zero relationship with performance (see Webber and Donahue, 2001; Stewart, 2006; Horwitz and Horwitz, 2007; Joshi and Roh, 2009; van Dijk et al., 2012). However, looking only at the overall mean effect size of the relationship between a specific variable and the relevant outcome measure can mask important differences, such as how the effect size increases or decreases as a function of some theoretically relevant and practically important moderators. The most relevant moderating variables are outlined below.

Task characteristics

Task difficulty/complexity

Shuffler and Carter (2018) define task complexity as a context that is ambiguous, multifaceted, dynamic, and demands a sense of urgency. Meta-analyses examining the moderating effects of task difficulty/complexity suggest that demographic diversity may adversely impact teams performing simple tasks (i.e., effect sizes range from small negative to near zero) and have little effect on performance for more difficult tasks. Bowers et al. (2000) found that homogenous teams performed moderately better on low difficulty tasks and heterogenous teams performed better on high difficulty tasks. van Dijk et al. (2012) also examined the moderating effects of task complexity but did not find moderation for task complexity. Lastly, Wang et al. (2019) found that surface-level diversity was detrimental to performance, operationalized as creativity/innovation, on simple tasks, but unrelated to performance for complex tasks. Thus, to the extent that fluid teams are primarily convened to address complex, critical tasks, this suggests that negative effects of demographic diversity on fluid team performance may be weak.

Task interdependence

Demographic diversity can lead to process loss (e.g., via increased conflict) and such disruptions during task performance have the potential to be detrimental. This would particularly be the case when interdependence is high. That is, interdependent tasks rely on efficient intra-team communications and disruptions to this should adversely impact performance. Joshi and Roh (2009) found that for low interdependence tasks there was a small positive relationship to performance, moderate levels of interdependence showed a small negative relationship, and high interdependence showed a small to near zero relationship to performance. As Joshi and Roh note, these results suggest “that the interactive effects of team diversity and interdependence may be more complex than acknowledged in the past” (p. 620).

Team cognition

Team cognition refers to emergent states that determine how task and team knowledge is organized among team members (e.g., transactive memory, team mental models). Although team cognition has been shown to be a robust predictor of team performance, Niler et al. (2021) examined moderators of this overall effect. They proposed that team homogeneity would weaken the relationship between team cognition and team performance, and results indicated that the team cognition-performance relationship was significantly stronger for heterogeneous teams (varying in demographic attributes such as race, gender, or age) than homogenous teams. This moderation was not significant for teams with varying functional diversity (e.g., expertise, functional background). This suggests that team cognition is most strongly related to team performance when the team members are more demographically diverse. The authors note that demographic diversity can trigger social categorization processes that can negatively impact information processing, rendering team cognition processes more important in supporting performance.

Team characteristics

Team tenure

Team tenure, or the longevity of team membership, is of direct relevance to fluid teams. Examining short-term vs. long-term teams—a proxy for tenure or longevity, Joshi and Roh (2009) found that relations-oriented diversity (similar to demographic diversity) demonstrated a positive relationship to performance in short-term teams and a negative relationship to performance in long-term teams (defined as teams working together for longer than 2 years). Joshi and Roh suggest that the urgency surrounding short-term team task performance may lead team members to overlook differences and instead require them to leverage these differences during task performance. Moreover, these authors suggest that in long-term teams, “divisions based on diversity attributes may become more entrenched and self-reinforcing, so that conflicts and differences based on relations-oriented attributes have a more debilitating impact on team performance” (p. 611). Although these results imply that demographic diversity would have a stronger effect in long-term teams versus short-term teams, the operationalization of short versus long-term teams as either less or more than 2 years duration may yield unique results compared to other operationalizations of team tenure. The authors note that more research is needed to better understand the moderating relationship of team tenure. For example, Byron et al. (2023) found team tenure to be curvilinearly related to team outcomes of innovation and creativity, thus suggesting that this relationship may be more complex than previously thought.

Study characteristics

Diversity conceptualization

How diversity is conceptualized has an impact on the diversity-performance relationship. Harrison and Klein (2007) outlined three types of diversity including variety, disparity, and separation. These types of diversity relate to different theories and consequently predict varying outcomes. Variety represents a uniform distribution of an attribute among a team. Minimum variety would occur if every member had the same level or characteristic of that attribute. Maximum variety would occur when every individual has different levels of that attribute. Variety is related to the informational diversity–cognitive resource perspective and is predicted to result in greater creativity/innovation, better decision quality, more task conflict, and greater unit flexibility. Disparity represents an inequitable, positively skewed distribution of an attribute among a team such that one member is at the high end and the other members at the low end. Minimum disparity would occur if all members are equal on an attribute and maximum disparity occurs when one member is high and the others low. Disparity relates to status hierarchy and distributive justice (Harrison and Klein, 2007) and is predicted to result in more within-unit competition, reduced member input, withdrawal, and resentful deviance. Lastly, separation represents a bimodal distribution on an attribute such that half the team members are high and half the members low on an attribute. Minimum separation would occur if every member was the same on an attribute and maximum separation would occur when half were low and half high (or half one category and half the other; e.g., gender). Separation is related to the social categorization/intergroup bias/similarity–attraction perspective and is predicted to result in reduced cohesion, more interpersonal conflict and distrust, and decreased task performance.

Wei et al. (2021) investigated the moderating effect of diversity type on the relation-oriented (demographic) diversity-innovation relationship. Their findings demonstrate that relation-oriented variety had a negative, yet non-significant impact on corporate innovation. Relation-oriented disparity did not show a relationship with innovation. And relation-oriented faultline strength (i.e., equivalent to separation) showed a negative relationship with innovation.

Study setting

Examining studies that were carried out in laboratory or field settings, Bell et al. (2011) found that the harmful effects of demographic diversity were only demonstrated in field settings. Given that laboratory studies may be a broad proxy for ad hoc fluid teams, further research is needed to examine this relationship.

Contextual characteristics

Industry setting

Two meta-analyses examined the moderating effect of industry setting. Joshi and Roh (2009) found that diversity has the most detrimental impact on teams from high-technology industries, a small negative relationship for manufacturing teams, and a beneficial effect for service industry teams (see also Carter et al., 2019). To the extent that high-technology settings may represent more complex and interdependent task contexts, these results may also apply to fluid teams.

Overall, the findings from this literature suggest that demographic diversity can lead to process-loss in teams via increased levels of conflict, reduction in cohesion, and impaired information processing (e.g., Stahl et al., 2010). For example, Mesmer-Magnus and DeChurch (2009) found that information sharing (operationalized as uniqueness and openness) was greater for teams with greater surface-level similarity. However, whether an apparent process-loss impacts team performance depends on a variety of variables.

Individual attributes: age, gender, and race

Shifting from the examination of composite diversity measurement to more specific individual demographic variables, researchers have examined age, gender, and racial diversity.

Age diversity reflects chronological age differences between team members. Meta-analytic results show weak to near zero relationships between age diversity and performance [Bell et al., 2011 (ρ = −0.03); Byron et al., 2023 (ρ = 0.01); Carter et al., 2019 (r = 0.04); Joshi and Roh, 2009 (r = −0.06); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.03)]. Despite these insignificant findings, moderator analyses have shown significant variability among various moderating variables. For example, Carter et al. (2019) found that the age diversity benefits service industry teams and disadvantages high-technology teams.

Examining gender diversity, meta-analytic results show a small to near zero negative relationship between gender diversity and performance [Bell et al., 2011 [−0.06]; Bowers et al., 2000 (z = −0.38, p > 0.05); Byron et al., 2023 (ρ = −0.04); Carter et al., 2019 (r = −0.03); Joshi and Roh, 2009 (r = −0.02); Schneid et al., 2015 (r = −0.01); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.01)]. Moderator analyses have identified significant moderating variables. For instance, Bowers et al. (2000) found that gender homogenous teams performed better on simple tasks, while gender heterogenous teams performed better on difficult tasks. Schneid et al. (2015) examined the moderating effect of societal gender egalitarianism and found that gender diversity adversely impacts performance in low gender egalitarianism societies that do not promote gender equality (see also Feitosa et al., 2023). Lastly, examining the moderating effect of study setting, Bell et al. (2011) found sex diversity to be unrelated to performance in laboratory settings, yet negatively related to performance in field settings.

The race/ethnicity diversity-performance relationship has been shown to be negative to near zero [Bell et al., 2011 (ρ = −0.11); Byron et al., 2023 (ρ = 0.03); Carter et al., 2019 (r = −0.06); Joshi and Roh, 2009 (r = −0.01); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.05)], however, again, moderating factors are informative. Joshi and Roh (2009), examining occupational demography, found that race/ethnicity diversity in majority white occupations negatively impacts performance, while diversity promotes performance in balanced occupations. Stahl et al. (2010) investigated the relationship between cultural diversity and team processes, finding a positive relationship between cultural diversity (e.g., race/ethnicity) and conflict and a negative relationship between cultural diversity and communication effectiveness and social integration (e.g., cohesion). Bell et al. (2011) found that race diversity was more damaging to performance in field settings as opposed to laboratory settings. As mentioned previously, study setting may be viewed as a proxy for fluid teams. As Bell et al. note, this would lead one to predict a weaker relationship between demographic diversity and performance in field settings (i.e., they have been together longer and thus surface-level diversity should give way to deep-level diversity) and a stronger relationship in ad hoc teams, although further research is needed to apply these findings to fluid teams.

Future research may look beyond single attributes and examine the interaction of multiple attributes. That is, age alone, or any demographic attribute, is likely to be insufficient to explain the diversity-process-performance relationship. Research on faultlines—hypothetical dividing lines that may separate team members based on one or more attributes—has made progress in investigating the impact of multiple attributes on performance. Leveraging theories of social categorization, social identity and the similarity–attraction perspective, stronger faultlines (i.e., the alignment of multiple attributes) are predicted to have a greater impact on team process and performance. Several meta-analytic integrations have been able to examine this proposition. Results from meta-analytic integrations show a negative effect of demographic faultlines on performance [Carter et al., 2019 (r = −0.05); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.06); Wei et al., 2021 (ρ = −0.17)]. Moreover, Zhang and Chen (2023) found that surface-level social faultlines activate subgroup formation, which in turn adversely impacts team interaction quality and team performance.

Surface-level diversity: task-related

Task or job-related attributes reflect team member characteristics expected to relate more directly to task performance (e.g., functional background). Leveraging the informational diversity–cognitive resource perspective (e.g., Cox and Blake, 1991), task-related diversity is expected to enhance performance in that task-related diversity should provide a greater corpus of KSAs to draw upon. In instances where a diverse range of expertise is needed for task performance, a diverse functional background should benefit performance. More specifically, diverse functional backgrounds should benefit performance when (a) a task requires varied functional backgrounds, (b) the varied functional backgrounds on the team are task-relevant, and (c) when teams have the resources and abilities to draw upon these backgrounds. Overall, task-related diversity has been shown to have a small positive relationship with performance (see Webber and Donahue, 2001; Horwitz and Horwitz, 2007; Joshi and Roh, 2009; van Dijk et al., 2012; Bui et al., 2019). Again, the overall findings have called attention to the importance of the following moderating variables.

Task characteristics

Task difficulty/complexity

van Dijk et al. (2012) was the only meta-analysis to directly examine task complexity as a moderating variable. Their findings show that task-related diversity is most beneficial when teams are executing complex tasks, in comparison to low or medium complexity tasks.

Interdependence

Joshi and Roh (2009) found some evidence for the beneficial effects of task-oriented diversity under moderate and high levels of task interdependence, as opposed to low interdependence.

Team characteristics

Team type (short vs. long-term)

Joshi and Roh (2009) found that the relationship between task-related diversity and performance was positive, yet very small to near zero, for both short and long-term teams.

Study characteristics

Diversity conceptualization

A single meta-analysis examined the moderating effect of diversity operationalization (e.g., variety, disparity, and faultline strength) on the general task-related-performance relationship (Wei et al., 2021). These authors found that each diversity operationalization was positively related to corporate innovation. Examining faultline strength, Zhang and Chen (2023) found surface-level task faultlines to benefit team interaction quality.

Contextual characteristics

Industry setting

Joshi and Roh (2009) found that task-related diversity was positively related to performance for high-technology teams and unrelated to performance for service industry and manufacturing teams.

Functional background

Functional background diversity refers to the “distribution of work history across the different functional specializations that exist within an organization” (Bell et al., 2011, p. 8). Meta-analytic results show a small positive relationship between functional background diversity and performance [Bell et al., 2011 (ρ = 0.10); Byron et al., 2023 (ρ = 0.06); Carter et al., 2019 (r = 0.06); Joshi and Roh, 2009 (r = 0.13); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = 0.07)]. Moreover, moderator analyses have shown significant variability among variables. Examining team type, Bell et al. (2011) found functional background diversity to be most beneficial to design/cross-functional teams. Carter et al. (2019) found that similar functional backgrounds were most beneficial to manufacturing teams, whereas high-technology teams benefited the most from functional background diversity.

Educational background

Educational background refers to the content area of one’s education. The various meta-analyses examining educational background have shown no direct relation to performance [Bell et al., 2011 (ρ = 0.01); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = 0.00)]. However, when the outcome of interest is creativity or innovation, a small positive effect is demonstrated [Byron et al., 2023 (ρ = 0.09); Bell et al., 2011 (ρ = 0.23)]. One explanation for the null results is that information about individual educational background is not often shared between team members. If these differences are not readily known by team members, as is fluid teams, the potential process gains from diverse educational backgrounds may not be realized.

Team tenure

Team tenure reflects how long team members have been on a team. Both Bell et al. and Carter et al. showed small, positive effects of team tenure on performance (ρ = 0.09, r = 0.09, respectively).

Team tenure diversity reflects the distribution of tenure among team members. Meta-analytic findings suggest the team tenure diversity-performance relationship is at or near zero [Bell et al., 2011 (ρ = −0.04); Byron et al., 2023 (ρ = 0.06); Carter et al., 2019 (r = −0.00); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.02)]. Thus, the variability of tenure among team members does not seem to impact performance to a significant degree.

A recent meta-analysis examining the team tenure-process-performance relationship deserves special attention (Gonzalez-Mulé et al., 2020). Gonzalez-Mulé et al. (2020) examined the relationship between (a) three types of tenure (i.e., additive, collective, and dispersion), (b) team processes (i.e., cognition, motivational-affective states, and behavioral processes), and (c) performance. Additive team tenure was defined as the average amount of time team members have spent in a given job, organizational role, or team. Collective team tenure was defined as the amount of time team members have spent together as members of the same team (e.g., time since last joining of a new member). And dispersion team tenure was defined as the variance of time in a job, organizational role, or team, equivalent to Harrison and Klein’s (2007) variety diversity. Gonzalez-Mulé et al. found that additive team tenure was positively related to performance, and that this relationship was mediated by team cognition. Collective team tenure was positively related to performance, and this relationship was mediated by motivational-affective states. Team tenure dispersion was also positively related to performance, and this relationship was mediated by team behavioral processes. Overall, they found additive team tenure to be most strongly related to team performance, followed by collective tenure, and then tenure dispersion. Moderator analyses found that the additive tenure-performance relationship was moderated by task interdependence such that additive team tenure is more strongly related to performance for highly interdependent tasks relative to less interdependent tasks, reflecting the importance of team cognition for interdependent tasks.

Team member familiarity

Carter et al. (2019) investigated whether the level of familiarity among team members relates to performance. While related to team tenure, familiarity captures instances whereby, for example, a newly added team member is known by the rest, or some, of the incumbent members. In instances where familiarity includes knowledge about work style, or member KSAs, familiarity can aid in, for example, team cognition (Bedwell, 2019). Results from Carter et al. showed that, overall, aggregated familiarity was not related to performance. Further research is warranted.

Deep-level attributes

Deep-level attributes represent more deep-seated, difficult to discern characteristics about a person such as personality, values, attitudes, preferences, and beliefs (Harrison et al., 2002). Compared to surface-level attributes, deep-level attributes have been suggested to be more directly related to process and can have a stronger impact on performance (Bell, 2007; Triana et al., 2021), although, as we previously noted, deep-level attributes may not have the time to fully express themselves in short-term fluid teams. For deep-level attributes, higher average levels of desirable attributes among team members are generally viewed as desirable. For example, higher team average cognitive ability would be expected to be positively correlated with team performance. Thus, Stewart (2006) found that higher-levels of deep-level attributes (i.e., personality, cognitive ability, expertise) benefit team performance (r = 0.20). Diversity, or differing levels of deep-level attributes would reflect members with high and low scores on various attributes. The following sections review the overall composite deep-level diversity-performance relationship (including composite deep-level diversity, personality, values, and culture), followed by examination of the diversity-performance relationship for the most studied individual attributes.

Deep-level diversity

The overall deep-level diversity-performance relationship has shown to be insignificant [Stewart, 2006 (r = −0.04); Triana et al., 2021 (r = −0.01); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.01)]. However, Triana et al. (2021) examined the impact of specific types of deep-level diversity (i.e., personality diversity, cultural diversity, and values diversity) on team processes and found that deep-level diversity showed a significant negative relationship with positive team emergent states, a significant negative relationship with team process, and a significant positive relationship with team conflict. Moreover, mediation analysis showed that the relationship between deep-level diversity and team performance was mediated by positive emergent states, positive team processes, and team conflict. Regarding faultline strength, Zhang and Chen (2023) found deep-level faultlines to be detrimental to team performance. Moreover, this research showed that this effect was serially meditated by subgroup formation and team interaction quality, respectively.

Task difficulty/complexity

van Dijk et al.’s (2012) meta-analysis examining the moderating effects of task difficulty/complexity suggests that deep-level diversity is detrimental to team performance in more complex tasks. This overall finding is in line with Triana et al. (2021) who found deep-level diversity to be more detrimental to team positive emergent states and team processes in tasks of high complexity. This suggests that on one hand, deep-level diversity may be detrimental to performance in complex fluid team tasks, but on the other hand, we have argued that deep-level attributes may be less impactful in such short-term time-compressed team contexts. Further research is needed.

Composite personality diversity

Considering composite personality (aggregate scores of the Big Five personality traits), the overall personality diversity-performance relationship has been shown to be insignificant [Bowers et al., 2000 (ns); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = 0.04)]. A recent meta-analysis by Triana et al. (2021) found that personality diversity exhibited a significant negative relationship with positive emergent states.

Composite values diversity

Values diversity was examined in two meta-analyses. The direct values diversity-performance relationship was examined by van Dijk et al. (2012) and did not show a significant relationship between values diversity and performance (r = −0.07). More interestingly, Triana et al. (2021) found that values diversity showed a significant negative relationship with positive emergent states, a significant negative relationship with positive team process, and a significant positive relationship with team conflict. Thus, while more research is needed to examine the impact of values diversity on performance, values diversity seems to disrupt important team processes.

Composite cultural diversity

Examining team processes, Triana et al. (2021) found cultural diversity to be unrelated to positive emergent states, unrelated to positive team processes, and positively related to team conflict (ρ = 0.24). Stahl et al. (2010) reported that overall cultural diversity was positively related to both task and relationship conflict, but unrelated to process conflict. Although all types of conflict have been shown to negatively impact team process and performance, this relationship has been shown to be complex (De Dreu and Weingart, 2003; de Wit et al., 2012; O'Neill et al., 2013). That is, some types of conflict may benefit certain teams in certain circumstances (e.g., creative problem-solving tasks). This relationship, however, has been shown in past research to be curvilinear, such that medium levels of task conflict have been shown to increase team innovation, while low and high levels can be detrimental (De Dreu, 2006). Potential positive effects of conflict aside, conflict should be viewed as a negative process. The research examining the conflict-trust relationship shows that all types of conflict degrade trust, and significantly so (relationship conflict, ρ = −0.45; task conflict, ρ = −0.53; process conflict ρ = −0.59; de Wit et al., 2012). Moreover, research suggests that the possible positive effects of conflict (specifically task conflict) may only be present when high levels of trust are already present among teams (De Dreu and Weingart, 2003). Balliet and Van Lange (2013) concur, concluding that “Trust matters most in situations that contain greater amounts of conflicting interests” (p. 1102). Cultural diversity may be particularly detrimental to fluid teams, given that initial levels of trust among unfamiliar team members may be low (Driskell et al., 2023).

Personality

The personality-team performance relationship has focused on the distribution of the big-five personality traits (i.e., conscientiousness, agreeableness, extraversion, emotional stability, and openness to experience) among team members. The overall findings from meta-analyses that examined individual personality traits, as opposed to those collapsing personality traits into a single variable, have found elevated average trait levels to be either positively related or unrelated to performance and for variability in trait levels to be either negatively related or unrelated to performance (Peeters et al., 2006; Bell, 2007; van Dijk et al., 2012; Carter et al., 2019). These relationships, as well as significant moderators, are described in more detail below.

Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness has been described by Costa and McCrae (1992) as a dimension that contrasts organized, scrupulous, diligent people against lax, unorganized, and lackadaisical people. In team task settings, high mean levels of conscientiousness are desirable, whereas high variability or varying levels of conscientiousness may impede performance due to (a) tension, or conflict, arising from personality conflicts on this dimension, (b) from lackluster performance from low conscientious members, and/or (c) the requirement of other members to make up for potential process-loss attributed to low conscientious members.

In separate meta-analyses, Bell (2007) found average conscientiousness to be positively related to performance (ρ = 0.14), Peeters et al. (2006) found a positive relationship between conscientiousness and performance (ρ = 0.20), and Carter et al. (2019) also found aggregated conscientiousness to relate positively to performance (r = 0.08). Bell further reported that this relationship was only present in field studies as opposed to laboratory studies, whereas Peeters et al. (2006) found this relationship to be notably larger for professional teams versus student teams.

Meta-analytic efforts show the relationship between conscientiousness diversity and performance to be negative, yet effect sizes range from small to near-zero [Bell, 2007 (ρ = −0.03); Carter et al., 2019 (r = −12); Peeters et al., 2006 (ρ = −0.24); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.09)]. Bell further found detrimental effects of conscientiousness diversity only for field studies.

Taken as a whole, the findings suggest that team performance benefits from greater levels of overall conscientiousness and suffers from conscientiousness diversity. Although current data do not directly address this, these effects may be somewhat weaker in ad hoc fluid teams that operate over short time periods.

Agreeableness

Agreeableness has been described as a dimension of interpersonal behavior that contrasts persons who are trusting, sympathetic, and cooperative against persons who are callous, cynical and antagonistic (Costa and McCrae, 1992). Similar to conscientiousness, agreeableness is deemed to be a desirable trait to have among team members. Overall, aggregated levels of agreeableness are shown to be positively related to performance [Bell, 2007 (ρ = 0.12); Carter et al., 2019 (r = 0.14); Peeters et al., 2006 (ρ = 0.24)]. In regard to moderators, Bell found that agreeableness was most strongly related to performance when aggregation was operationalized as average (i.e., mean team agreeableness) and when operationalized as team minimum agreeableness (i.e., the low team member on agreeableness) suggesting that one disagreeable team member can impact the entire team. Similar to their findings regarding conscientiousness, the agreeableness-performance relationship was found to be only significant in field settings. Paralleling Bell’s findings, Peeters et al. showed elevated agreeableness to be only related to performance in professional teams.

Meta-analytic integrations examining the agreeableness diversity-performance relationship show a negative to near zero association [Bell, 2007 (ρ = −0.04); Carter et al., 2019 (r = −0.14); Peeters et al., 2006 (ρ = −0.12); van Dijk et al., 2012 (r = −0.03)]. Moderator analyses from these studies are non-significant.

The overall findings suggest that teams benefit from members who score high on agreeableness. Moreover, the results also highlight the importance of the member with the minimum level of agreeableness, suggesting an antagonistic, uncooperative individual can degrade the team. Variability on agreeableness seems to have a weak negative relationship with performance. Results may be weaker for ad hoc vs. field teams.

Extraversion

According to Costa and McCrae (1992), extraversion includes traits such as sociability, activity, and the tendency to experience positive emotions. It is worth noting that these lower-level traits that comprise the broader Extraversion factor may show differential effects. Some researchers differentiate between the sociability/affiliation sub-trait of Extraversion and the outgoing/assertiveness sub-trait (Driskell and Salas, 2013).

Concerning aggregated levels of extraversion, Bell (2007) found a small, yet positive relationship between extraversion and team performance (ρ = 0.10); Carter et al. (2019) replicated this finding (r = 0.10), but Peeters et al. (2006) found no relationship (ρ = 0.03). Bell’s findings show this relationship to only be significant for teams in field settings. Regarding variability, or diversity, no significant extraversion diversity-performance was found. The results of the meta-analyses reviewed suggest a small benefit to having team members with higher levels of extraversion, albeit perhaps weaker in ad hoc vs. field team settings. Moreover, diversity in extraversion has shown to be unrelated to team performance.

Emotional stability

Emotional stability reflects a person’s tendency to not experience psychological distress (Costa and McCrae, 1992). With lower emotional stability reflecting neuroticism, emotional stability would be deemed a desirable trait among team members. This may be particularly true when teams must perform under high demand or stress.

The meta-analyses examining aggregated levels of emotional stability generally support this claim. Bell (2007) showed a small significant relationship between average emotional stability and performance (ρ = 0.13), Carter et al. (2019) reported similar findings (r = 0.13), yet Peeters et al. (2006) found no significant relationship (ρ = 0.03). Of the three meta-analyses examining the aggregated emotional stability-performance relationship, the moderator analyses report mixed results, suggesting that the relationship between aggregated emotional stability and team performance is likely moderated by other variables (e.g., task difficulty, intrateam conflict, etc.). The findings regarding the emotional stability diversity-performance relationship demonstrate no significant relationship to performance.

Overall, the findings suggest a small performance gain for elevated aggregate levels of emotional stability on team performance. Variability in emotional stability does not seem to impact performance.

Openness to experience

Openness to experience refers to individuals who are imaginative and have a rich and complex emotional life, and who are intellectually curious, behaviorally flexible and nondogmatic in their attitudes and values (Costa and McCrae, 1992).

The findings regarding aggregated openness to experience are similar to those of extraversion and emotional stability. Bell (2007) and Carter et al. (2019) found a significant positive relationship between mean openness to experience and team performance (ρ = 0.11 and r = 0.13, respectively). When broken down by type of setting, Bell only found a significant positive relationship between openness to experience and performance for field studies. The meta-analytic integrations find the overall openness to experience diversity-performance relationship to be unrelated to team performance; although both Bell and Peeters et al. showed that diversity negatively impacted performance for teams in field settings.

Overall, the results suggest that teams benefit from individuals with greater openness to experience. Diversity may be detrimental to performance in field settings versus laboratory settings. As noted with other personality traits, openness to experience may not have opportunity to express itself in time-compressed, short-duration tasks.

Cognitive ability

Cognitive ability, also referred to as general mental ability, reflects an individual’s ability to process information and learn. In regard to team performance, one would expect that teams with higher levels of cognitive ability would outperform teams of lower levels of cognitive ability. Meta-analyses support this relationship [Bell, 2007 (ρ = 0.30); Carter et al., 2019 (r = 0.07; ns); Devine and Phillips, 2001 (r = 0.29); Stewart, 2006 (r = 0.30)]. Both Devine and Phillips as well Bell examined study setting as a potential moderator. Devine and Phillips found the cognitive ability-performance relationship to be stronger in laboratory settings than in field settings, whereas Bell’s results did not show significant subgroup variation.

Diversity in cognitive ability would reflect team members with varying levels of cognitive ability. The meta-analyses examining the cognitive ability diversity-performance relationship have shown diversity to be unrelated to performance (Devine and Phillips, 2001; Bell, 2007; van Dijk et al., 2012; Carter et al., 2019). The overall findings suggest that selecting team members high on cognitive ability benefits team performance.

Collective orientation

Several concepts, including collective orientation, team orientation, and preference for teamwork, collectively reflect an individual’s propensity to work in a collective manner in team settings (Driskell et al., 2010). A related concept, collectivism, originally viewed at the national-level (Hofstede, 1980), but since applied to the individual-level (Triandis, 1995), reflects an emphasis on the collective over the individual. Taken as a whole, a focus on the team over the self should benefit teamwork and team performance.

Bell’s (2007) meta-analysis showed a significant positive aggregated preference for teamwork (ρ = 0.23) and collectivism (ρ = 0.31) team performance relationship. In both instances, the relationship to performance was only significant for teams in field settings. A recent more focused meta-analysis by Kilcullen et al. (2023) showed a positive impact of team orientation on team process and performance. Specifically, they found team orientation to be positively related to communication, coordination, cooperation, trust, shared mental models, backup behaviors, cohesion, innovation, satisfaction, leadership, and team performance and negatively related to conflict. Collectivism and preference for teamwork operationalized as diversity was not shown to be related to performance (Bell, 2007). Consequently, the findings suggest, unsurprisingly, that teams benefit from individuals who have a predilection for teamwork.

Emotional intelligence

Emotional intelligence (EI) is described as the capacity to perceive, use, regulate, and understand both one’s own emotions and the emotions of others (Joseph and Newman, 2010). Bell (2007) found average team emotional intelligence to be positively related to team performance (ρ = 0.18), and this relationship was stronger in lab settings than in field settings, somewhat paradoxically. Joseph and Newman (2010, p. 54) concluded that “EI positively predicts performance for high emotional labor jobs and negatively predicts performance for low emotional labor jobs.” Thus, the impact of EI on team performance may be dependent on the task environment; when task settings do not allow for much emotional expression, EI should have less impact.

Implications for forming fluid teams

As previously noted, fluid teams are unique in that they are rapidly assembled to execute critical, time-limited tasks and are composed of members who typically have no prior familiarity or experience working together, and who disband upon task completion. The temporary nature of these teams leads to several important implications. First, given their short nature, surface-level attributes should have a greater impact on fluid teams than deep-level attributes. That said, deep-level attributes should still impact fluid team process and performance, especially when opportunities exist for team members to engage in mutual self-disclosure, when member attributes are foreknown (e.g., military values), and when deep-level attributes are discernable (e.g., conscientiousness). Second, it is conceivable, and some of the data reviewed suggests this as a possibility, that diversity may have a lesser impact on fluid teams. That is, for example, the requirement to “hit the ground running” may override adverse diversity effects by reducing the time to “categorize” teammates and increasing the motivation to execute near-term tasks. Moreover, the knowledge that team membership is temporary may lead members to overlook diversity (e.g., there is no need to be concerned about future interactions). Furthermore, members of fluid teams are expected to have had more experience working on multiple teams. Exposure to different types of team members may also lessen diversity effects. The diversity in knowledge and skills gained from working on multiple teams can be imported to newly formed groups conferring an advantage to fluid teams. Third, fluid teams are faced with the obstacles presented to all new teams with the added difficulty of carrying out critical, time-limited tasks often characterized by high levels of complexity. That is, new teams lack the “know-how” to be an effective team, whereas longer-term or intact teams develop this knowledge over time. Fluid teams are unlikely to have the time to develop these team knowledge structures and as such are at a significant disadvantage.

In addition to the implications noted above, we review the most important implications drawn from the review in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary implications: universal.

In addition to these universal implications, we highlight some of the more relevant discrete implications drawn from the findings in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary implications: discrete.

Research gaps

The relationships between team composition and process/performance are extremely complex, however this literature is robust and comprehensive. Although ad hoc or temporary teams have been of interest to researchers for some time, the type of fluid teams that we conceptualize are largely a product of modern demands, in that many task contexts require immediate attention to near-term problems for which domain experts must be drawn from various disciplines, departments, or organizations. To an extent, the utilization of fluid teams in modern contexts such as industry, healthcare, and the military has outstripped our understanding of how to best support fluid team performance. Therefore, the majority of the implications that we can derive from the broad literature on team composition must be elaborated and examined in a fluid team context. Most of this work has yet to be done. In a separate article in this issue, Driskell et al. (2024) provide an overview of the critical research gaps and opportunities to support selection, training, and workplace design for fluid teams.

Conclusion

As Bell et al. (2018) note, some combinations of team members work better than others. This is perhaps even more the case for unique contexts in which certain teams may operate. This review examines the weight of cumulative evidence on team composition that can be leveraged to better understand the determinants of fluid team effectiveness. We hope this discussion provides a foundation for continuing research on this topic.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

TD: Writing – original draft. GF: Writing – review & editing. MT: Writing – review & editing. AC: Writing – review & editing. JD: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by an Air Force Research Laboratory grant (FA8650-20-D-6207).

Conflict of interest

TD and JD were employed by Florida Maxima Corporation.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author (s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the U.S. Air Force.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1302022/full#supplementary-material

References

Balliet, D., and Van Lange, P. A. (2013). Trust, conflict, and cooperation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 139, 1090–1112. doi: 10.1037/a0030939

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedwell, W. L. (2019). Adaptive team performance: the influence of membership fluidity on shared team cognition. Front. Psychol. 10:2266. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02266

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, S. T. (2007). Deep-level composition variables as predictors of team performance: a meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 595–615. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.595

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, S. T., Brown, S. G., Colaneri, A., and Outland, N. (2018). Team composition and the ABCs of teamwork. Am. Psychol. 73, 349–362. doi: 10.1037/amp0000305

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, E. A., Rufrano, G. A., Traylor, A. M., Ohning, B. L., and Salas, E. (2023). Enhancing team success in the neonatal intensive care unit: challenges and opportunities for fluid teams. Front. Psychol. 14:1284606. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1284606

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, S. T., Villado, A. J., Lukasik, M. A., Belau, L., and Briggs, A. L. (2011). Getting specific about demographic diversity variable and team performance relationships: a meta-analysis. J. Manag. 37, 709–743. doi: 10.1177/0149206310365001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomqvist, K., and Cook, K. S. (2018). “Swift trust: state-of-the-art and future research directions” in The Routledge Companion to Trust. eds. R. H. Searle, A. I. Nienaber, and S. B. Sitkin (New York: Routledge), 29–49.

Google Scholar

Bowers, C. A., Pharmer, J. A., and Salas, E. (2000). When member homogeneity is needed in work teams: a meta-analysis. Small Group Res. 31, 305–327. doi: 10.1177/104649640003100303

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bui, H., Chau, V. S., Degl'Innocenti, M., Leone, L., and Vicentini, F. (2019). The resilient organisation: a meta-analysis of the effect of communication on team diversity and team performance. Appl. Psychol. 68, 621–657. doi: 10.1111/apps.12203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, D. (1971). The Attraction Paradigm. New York: Academic Press

Google Scholar

Byron, K., Keem, S., Darden, T., Shalley, C. E., and Zhou, J. (2023). Building blocks of idea generation and implementation in teams: a meta-analysis of team design and team creativity and innovation. Pers. Psychol. 76, 249–278. doi: 10.1111/peps.12501

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Capiola, A., Baxter, H. C., Pfahler, M. D., Calhoun, C. S., and Bobko, P. (2020). Swift Trust in ad hoc Teams: a cognitive task analysis of intelligence operators in multi-domain command and control contexts. J. Cogn. Eng. Decis. Making 14, 218–241. doi: 10.1177/1555343420943460

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, K. M., Mead, B. A., Stewart, G. L., Nielsen, J. D., and Solimeo, S. L. (2019). Reviewing work team design characteristics across industries: combining Meta-analysis and comprehensive synthesis. Small Group Res. 50, 138–188. doi: 10.1177/1046496418797431

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: the NEO personality inventory. Psychol. Assess. 4, 5–13. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, T., and Blake, S. (1991). Managing cultural diversity: implications for organizational competitiveness. Acad. Manag. Exec. 5, 45–46.

Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K. (2006). When too little or too much hurts: evidence for a curvilinear relationship between task conflict and innovation in teams. J. Manag. 32, 83–107. doi: 10.1177/0149206305277795

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K., and Weingart, L. R. (2003). Task versus relationship conflict, team performance, and team member satisfaction: a meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 741–749. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.741

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wit, F. R., Greer, L. L., and Jehn, K. A. (2012). The paradox of intragroup conflict: a meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 97, 360–390. doi: 10.1037/a0024844

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine, D. J., and Phillips, J. L. (2001). Do smarter teams do better? A meta-analysis of cognitive ability and team performance. Small Group Res. 32, 507–532. doi: 10.1177/104649640103200501

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driskell, T., Funke, G., Tolston, M., Capiola, A., and Driskell, J. (2023). Fluid and virtual teams. Small Group Res. doi: 10.1177/10464964231216367 (Epub ahead of print).

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driskell, T., Funke, G., Toltson, M., Capiola, A., and Driskell, J. (2024). Supporting fluid teams: A research agenda. Front. Psychol. 14:1327885. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1327885

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driskell, J. E., Goodwin, G. F., Salas, E., and O’Shea, P. G. (2006). What makes a good team player? Personality and team effectiveness. Group Dynam. 10, 249–271. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.10.4.249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driskell, J. E., and Salas, E. (2013). “Personality and work teams” in Handbook of Psychology at Work. eds. N. Christiansen and R. Tett (New York: Routledge), 744–771.

Google Scholar

Driskell, J. E., Salas, E., and Driskell, T. (2018). Foundations of teamwork and collaboration. Am. Psychol. 73, 334–348. doi: 10.1037/amp0000241

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driskell, J. E., Salas, E., and Hughes, S. (2010). Collective orientation and team performance: development of an individual differences measure. Hum. Factors 52, 316–328. doi: 10.1177/0018720809359522

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feitosa, J., Davis, A. S., Romain, R., and Delice, F. (2023). Team membership change events: processes that support gender diverse teams. Small Group Res. doi: 10.1177/10464964231209924 (Epub ahead or print).

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Mulé, E., Cockburn, B. S., McCormick, B. W., and Zhao, P. (2020). Team tenure and team performance: a meta-analysis and process model. Pers. Psychol. 73, 151–198. doi: 10.1111/peps.12319

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, R., Billotti, B. M., Ha, J. J., and Cassara, M. (2024). Should the existing science of teams be applied to fluid teams? An exploration of fluid team effectiveness within the context of healthcare simulation. Front. Psychol. 14:1323469. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1323469

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, D. A., and Klein, K. J. (2007). What’s the difference? Diversity constructs as separation, variety, and disparity in organization. Acad. Manag. Rev. 32, 1199–1228. doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.26586096

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, D. A., Mohammed, S., McGrath, J. E., Florey, A. T., and Vanderstoep, S. W. (2003). Time matters in team performance: effects of member familiarity, entrainment, and task discontinuity on speed and quality. Pers. Psychol. 56, 633–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2003.tb00753.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, D. A., Price, K. H., Gavin, J. H., and Florey, A. T. (2002). Time, teams, and task performance: changing effects of surface-and deep-level diversity on group functioning. Acad. Manag. J. 45, 1029–1045. doi: 10.2307/3069328

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Newbury Park, CA: Sage

Google Scholar

Horwitz, S. K., and Horwitz, I. B. (2007). The effects of team diversity on team outcomes: a meta-analytic review of team demography. J. Manag. 33, 987–1015. doi: 10.1177/0149206307308587

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, D. L., and Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: an integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. J. Appl. Psychol. 95, 54–78. doi: 10.1037/a0017286

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, A., and Roh, H. (2009). The role of context in work team diversity research: a meta-analytic review. Acad. Manag. J. 52, 599–627. doi: 10.5465/amj.2009.41331491

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilcullen, M., Bisbey, T. M., Rosen, M., and Salas, E. (2023). Does team orientation matter? A state-of-the-science review, meta-analysis, and multilevel framework. J. Organ. Behav. 44, 355–375. doi: 10.1002/job.2622

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Linhardt, R. M., and Salas, E. (2023). Examining the fluidity of innovation teams: a conceptual framework. Front. Psychol. 14:1296651. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1296651

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorge, I., Fox, D., Davitz, J., and Brenner, M. (1958). A survey of studies contrasting the quality of group performance and individual performance, 1920–1957. Psychol. Bull. 55:337.

Google Scholar

Majchrzak, A., Jarvenpaa, S. L., and Hollingshead, A. B. (2007). Coordinating expertise among emergent groups responding to disasters. Organ. Sci. 18, 147–161. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1060.0228

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McKinney, E. H., Barker, J. R., Davis, K. J., and Smith, D. (2005). How swift starting action teams get off the ground: what united flight 232 and airline flight crews can tell us about team communication. Manag. Commun. Q. 19, 198–237. doi: 10.1177/0893318905278539

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meister, A., Thatcher, S. M., Park, J., and Maltarich, M. (2020). Toward a temporal theory of faultlines and subgroup entrenchment. J. Manag. Stud. 57, 1473–1501. doi: 10.1111/joms.12538

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., and DeChurch, L. A. (2009). Information sharing and team performance: a meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 94, 535–546. doi: 10.1037/a0013773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niler, A. A., Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., Larson, L. E., Plummer, G., DeChurch, L. A., and Contractor, N. S. (2021). Conditioning team cognition: a meta-analysis. Organ. Psychol. Rev. 11, 144–174. doi: 10.1177/2041386620972112

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Neill, T. A., Allen, N. J., and Hastings, S. E. (2013). Examining the “pros” and “cons” of team conflict: a team-level meta-analysis of task, relationship, and process conflict. Hum. Perform. 26, 236–260. doi: 10.1080/08959285.2013.795573

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, M. A., Van Tuijl, H. F., Rutte, C. G., and Reymen, I. M. (2006). Personality and team performance: a meta-analysis. Eur. J. Personal. 20, 377–396. doi: 10.1002/per.588

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, C. S., and Ahuja, M. K. (2006). Are all distributed teams the same? Differentiating between temporary and ongoing distributed teams. Small Group Res. 37, 662–700. doi: 10.1177/1046496406294323

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneid, M., Isidor, R., Li, C., and Kabst, R. (2015). The influence of cultural context on the relationship between gender diversity and team performance: a meta-analysis. Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manag. 26, 733–756. doi: 10.1080/09585192.2014.957712

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuffler, M. L., and Carter, D. R. (2018). Teamwork situated in multiteam systems: key lessons learned and future opportunities. Am. Psychol. 73, 390–406. doi: 10.1037/amp0000322

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stahl, G. K., Maznevski, M. L., Voigt, A., and Jonsen, K. (2010). Unraveling the effects of cultural diversity in teams: a meta-analysis of research on multicultural work groups. J. Int. Bus. Stud. 41, 690–709. doi: 10.1057/jibs.2009.85

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, G. L. (2006). A meta-analytic review of relationships between team design features and team performance. J. Manag. 32, 29–55. doi: 10.1177/0149206305277792

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Triana, M. D. C., Kim, K., Byun, S. Y., Delgado, D. M., and Arthur, W. Jr. (2021). The relationship between team deep-level diversity and team performance: a meta-analysis of the main effect, moderators, and mediating mechanisms. J. Manag. Stud. 58, 2137–2179. doi: 10.1111/joms.12670

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and Collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview

Google Scholar

Turner, J. C. (1985). “Social categorization and the self-concept: a social cognitive theory of group behavior” in Advances in Group Processes: Theory and Research. ed. E. J. Lawler, vol. 2 (Greenwich, CT: JAI Press), 77–121.

Google Scholar

van Dijk, H., Van Engen, M. L., and Van Knippenberg, D. (2012). Defying conventional wisdom: a meta-analytical examination of the differences between demographic and job-related diversity relationships with performance. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 119, 38–53. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.06.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Cheng, G. H. L., Chen, T., and Leung, K. (2019). Team creativity/innovation in culturally diverse teams: a meta-analysis. J. Organ. Behav. 40, 693–708. doi: 10.1002/job.2362

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Webber, S. S., and Donahue, L. M. (2001). Impact of highly and less job-related diversity on work group cohesion and performance: a meta-analysis. J. Manag. 27, 141–162. doi: 10.1177/014920630102700202

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, X., Yang, H., and Han, S. (2021). A meta-analysis of top management team compositional characteristics and corporate innovation in China. Asia Pac. Bus. Rev. 27, 53–76. doi: 10.1080/13602381.2021.1846952

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wildman, J. L., Shuffler, M. L., Lazzara, E. H., Fiore, S. M., Burke, C. S., Salas, E., et al. (2012). Trust development in swift starting action teams: a multilevel framework. Group Org. Manag. 37, 137–170. doi: 10.1177/1059601111434202

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., and Chen, H. (2023). How surface-level and deep-level faultlines influence team performance through subgroup formation and team interaction quality: a meta-analytic review. Manag. Organ. Rev. 19, 876–909. doi: 10.1017/mor.2023.13

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziller, R. C. (1965). Toward a theory of open and closed groups. Psychol. Bull. 64, 164–182. doi: 10.1037/h0022390

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fluid teams, teams, composition, team performance, team composition

Citation: Driskell T, Funke G, Tolston M, Capiola A and Driskell J (2024) Composition considerations for fluid teams: a review. Front. Psychol. 15:1302022. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1302022

Received: 25 September 2023; Accepted: 29 January 2024;
Published: 12 February 2024.

Edited by:

Eduardo Salas, Rice University, United States

Reviewed by:

Chester S. Spell, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, United States
Marissa Shuffler, Clemson University, United States

Copyright © 2024 Driskell, Funke, Tolston, Capiola and Driskell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tripp Driskell, Tripp@floridamaxima.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.