Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 June 2024
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Childhood Traumatic Experiences: New Clinical Perspectives and Interventions View all 9 articles

Navigating foster care: how parental drug use and caregiver attitudes shape children’s mentalization processes—an exploratory longitudinal follow-up study: study protocol

  • 1Dialog – Individuelle Suchthilfe, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Unit of Psychotraumatology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

Background: The current research concept of mentalization is used in the study to clearly identify affective and cognitive abilities of the caregiver-child dyad with the aim of compensating deficits on both sides with psychological-psychotherapeutic strategies.

Methods: The objective of this explorative, longitudinal intervention study is to provide an in-depth understanding of the psycho-social background of 30 children aged 6–12 years living in institutional or family-centered foster care. Data will be collected at three time points: before, after and 12 months after participating in the newly developed group intervention, which intends to address the particular needs of children of drug abusing parents living in foster care in the latency period. The study is conducted at the Faculty of Psychology of the University of Vienna in collaboration with the Association “Dialogue” (Verein Dialog). The treatment duration spans 5 months, during which two specifically trained psychotherapists conduct 10 group sessions for children and three group sessions for foster caregivers. All statistical analyses will consider the type of data available. Therefore, the primary outcome of the study will be assessed via the Friedman test due to the ordinal dependent variable as it is the non-parametric alternative to the one-way ANOVA for repeated measures. In addition, the Mann–Whitney U test is used to compare differences between two independent groups (children living in institutional foster care vs. family foster care). To assess potential correlations regarding the child and caregivers’ capacity to mentalize, Spearman correlations (ρ) are conducted. To examine the secondary outcome, apart from the methods previously outlined, we will also utilize qualitative thematic analysis.

Discussion: The present study uses the current research concept of mentalization to identify affective and cognitive abilities of the caregiver-child dyad with the aim of compensating deficits on both sides with psychological-psychotherapeutic strategies. There are some limitations of the study to mention: the small sample size does not allow to generalize the results. Due to the lack of a comparison group, a randomized control study (RCT) was not conducted. The authors are aware of these limitations. However, the studies’ findings, will help to deduce research questions for further studies.

1 Introduction

Foster care is government-subsidized and -regulated temporary care for children who have been removed from their families for reasons of abuse and neglect. Children can be placed either in family or residential care. While family foster care includes arrangements of children living with unrelated foster parents (nonrelative foster care), with relatives (kinship care), or with families who plan to adopt them (foster/adopt homes), residential programs encompass a type of living in out-of-home care placement in which specialized services for youth with emotional and behavioral problems or other special needs are provided in a highly structured environment. It was estimated that in 142 countries about 2.7 million children aged 0–17 could be living in institutionalized care worldwide (Petrowski et al., 2017).

In Austria, in 2021 a total of 12,871 minors were living in foster care (of which 31.5% only in Vienna): 61.3% of the children and adolescents were placed in institutional group care, while 38.7% were accommodated with foster families (Österreich, 2022). In terms of age, 44.3% of children in foster care, were between the ages of 6 and 14. Children placed into institutional care usually live together with 8–10 other children, cared for by a team of social pedagogues, each of them having one main contact person, called “caregiver.”

Although estimates vary widely, a detailed literature search suggests that parental substance use disorder plays a major role in the child welfare system (Seay, 2015).

Maltreatment, such as neglect, and placement into foster care are considered as traumatic affecting children’s immediate and future psychosocial development and mental health (Cicchetti and Toth, 1995; Bowlby, 1998; Cicchetti et al., 2006). Children placed in foster care might have experienced distressing feelings such as confusion, anxiety and sadness, due to the unfamiliar or previously experienced situation to which they are adapting as a result of their placement (Bruskas, 2008). High rates of internalizing problems (e.g., anxiety, depression), externalizing problems (e.g., aggression, impulsivity), poorer social skills, and lower adaptive functioning were also reported among foster children compared to children who did not experience replacement (Webb et al., 2010; Jones Harden et al., 2014).

Specifically, those children who experienced trauma frequently revealed underdeveloped mentalizing capacities (Ostler et al., 2010). Muller et al. (2012) specified that their ability to cope with physical, or emotional traumas highly correlated with the perceived quality of their current relationships, and that traumatic experiences had a major impact on attachment behaviors of out-of-home children toward their foster caregivers. As a result of the children’s adverse experiences with parental care, they may be inclined to avoid forming new and supportive relationships. This creates a complex situation for all individuals involved, including both the foster caregiver and the children. On the other side, foster parents and institutional caregivers know that foster care is mostly not permanent. This issue may interfere with attachment formation to the foster parents/caregivers (Åkerman et al., 2020).

The ability to mentalize plays a crucial role in individuals’ coping abilities with traumatic events (Pecora et al., 2005; Szilagyi et al., 2015). Mentalization can be understood as “the ability to understand the actions by both other people and oneself in terms of thoughts, feelings, wishes and desires” (Bateman and Fonagy, 2016, p. 3). By involving the internal regulation of emotions through thoughts, children as well as adults who are able to mentalize are more resilient and able to tolerate feelings of anger, fear, shame, and distress resulting from adversity and trauma (Allen et al., 2008; Brockmann and Kirsch, 2010; Ostler et al., 2010; Ensink et al., 2017; Giusti et al., 2021).

Research revealed a strong association between insecure attachment representations and struggles of drug addicted parents in responding to their children’s emotional cues (Grienenberger et al., 2005; Slade et al., 2005), which in turn interfere with the ability to form secure attachments and mentalizing skills in children (Madigan et al., 2007; Bammens et al., 2015). However, most studies have focused mainly on the characteristics of the child, neglecting the parents’ variables (Schmidt and Schimmelmann, 2015).

Pediatricians reported that children of drug-using families were often distressed by their parents’ substance use. They often had to take on parental roles, due to their parents’ incapacity. As they usually have blamed themselves for their parents’ behavior and felt responsible for their wellbeing (Smith and Wilson, 2016), they tended to keep the situation secret and not show their distress in public (Morrison et al., 1996; Hoffmann and Su, 1998). They also tended to self-stigmatize and live in fear of “failing” in the same way as their parents did (Matthews et al., 2017). Within addiction care, children of drug abusing parents are not always seen as individuals in need due to the fact, that the focus stays on the consuming adult. Nevertheless, detecting problems in children early as well as prompt implementation of prevention or treatment programs, would increase the likelihood of the child growing up safely (Van de Meer et al., 2019). Moreover, children of parents with Substance Use Disorder (SUD) experience a notably higher incidence of physical, emotional, and sexual abuse, as well as emotional or physical neglect (McGlade et al., 2009; Altshuler and Cleverly-Thomas, 2011). Therefore, foster care children of drug abusing families, place an additional strain on the healthcare system, thus more research identifying factors enabling children and adolescents to enhance their mental wellbeing and healthy development, as well as cost-effective treatment options, is needed.

Parental attitudes represent the primary social influence encountered by the child during their formative years (Zunich, 1966). As Farmakopoulou and Baltsioti (2024, p. 319–320) emphasize clearly: “Foster carers play a crucial role in this institution, and their experiences, perceptions, and relationships are vital for the wellbeing and development of the children under their care. Understanding foster carers’ views on factors that support or hinder successful fostering is essential for grasping the dynamics of foster care.”

Children’s ability to mentalize can be best observed during the latency period in child development. The latency period takes place between the age of 6 and 12 and is characterized by a number of new developmental challenges, such as finding a place in a peer group, realizing cognitive activities, and compliance to the rules of the family and the community. Individuals’ ability to cope with these challenges largely depends on the ego-function development, reflected, e.g., in emotional control, ability to deal with frustration and being able to reflect on the other’s perspective (Akhtar, 2009; Dejko et al., 2016).

Studies reported that a parent’s ability of reflective functioning (RF), as mentalization is operationalized, predicted children’s attachment security and mentalization ability (Grienenberger et al., 2005; Slade et al., 2005; Allen et al., 2008), and various aspects of a child’s short- and long-term development (Fonagy et al., 1991; Slade, 2002; Steele and Steele, 2008). Improvements in RF related to mothers with SUD were associated with improvements in caregiving quality (Åkerman et al., 2020). The studies mentioned above looked in samples of biological or foster parents. The majority of interventions that have been developed to support foster carers has been criticized for their lack of focus on improving the capacity of carers to respond to the child’s related needs while it is obvious that keeping the needs of a child in mind is the best support for every child’s development RF and social skills, such as reflexibility and social orientation, help every carer to understand behavior as a respond to emotional needs instead of just seeing “naughty” or “bad” behavior and the child itself feels understood and valued (Adkins et al., 2018; Midgley et al., 2019).

RF is defined as the ability to mentalize in the context of close, interpersonal relationships. With the words of Fonagy et al. (1998), RF helps us “to distinguish inner from outer reality, pretend from “real” modes of functioning as well as intra-personal mental and emotional processes from interpersonal communications” (Fonagy et al., 1998, p. 4). A recent study, showed that a group of brief psychoeducational parenting intervention increased RF in foster parents and improved parental sensitivity while decreasing children s’ internalizing behavior (Adkins et al., 2022).

Research on attachment and mentalization in substance-abusing families is emerging. While studies exist on mentalization in substance abusing mothers (e.g., Suchman et al., 2006; Pajulo et al., 2012; Suchman, 2016), research on mentalization or reflective functioning in children of substance-abusing parents is still lacking, and evidence suggest the specific need for interventions aimed to address experiences and feelings associated with foster care in children (Ostler et al., 2010).

Mentalization-based treatment (MBT) programs to increase ego-functions have been developed and implemented for multiple indications such as borderline personality disorders, drug addiction, and for children with parents suffering from mental health disorders (e.g., Bateman and Fonagy, 2008, Bruskas, 2008; Suchman et al., 2010). They aimed to understand and improve the individual’s ability to reflect upon their own and others’ feelings, thoughts, and desires. When applied to children and their caregivers, MBT focus on replacing destructive habitual ways of feeling and acting by improving mentalizing abilities, together with the individual’s emotional wellbeing, and interpersonal skills (Mayes, 2012).

A Narrative Systematic Review (Midgley et al., 2021) examined the range of mentalization-based interventions for children in middle childhood (6–12 years). According to this review, a relevant number of mentalization-based interventions have been conducted in the context of fostering and adoption, but the childrens’ mentalizing capacity was hardly ever assessed as an outcome and children from drug abusing families are still lacking as target group. This lack was already mentioned in previous publications (Jacobson et al., 2015; Midgley et al., 2017), but did not change so far.

2 Methods/design

2.1 Aims

The first aim of this study is to explore the development longitudinally of the children’s mentalization ability at three measure time points: before, after and 12 months after attending a newly developed group MBT-intervention. It is an adapted version of Midgley et al. (2017). Mentalization-Based Treatment for Children (MBT-C), which addresses the particular needs of children of substance using parents, who live in foster care during middle childhood, integrating the psycho-social background of children living in foster care. Second, we will explore the mentalization capacity of the caregivers, their attitudes on drugs and drug addiction, and their social skills (e.g., social orientation and reflexibility) with regard to the impact on the children’s mentalization at the beginning of the intervention to better understand whether it influence the development of mentalization over time.

2.2 Participants

In the study 30 children living in foster care (institutional or foster home) and their actual main foster caregiver will be included (N = 60).

Children’s inclusion criteria for participation are: (1) age between 6 and 12 years, (2) stable mental health status (e.g., no psychotic state), (3) living in foster care for at least 6 months. A minimum of time in a stable and persistent placement needs to be considered to assure the children’s ability to reorganize their internal world and behavioral outcomes (Cassibba et al., 2023). Caregiver’s inclusion criteria are (1) child lives in the same household and (2) 18 years of age or older. Children are excluded from the study if presenting neurological, cognitive, and/or psychiatric problems, and/or difficulties reported by their caregiver.

Children in middle childhood, aged between 6 and 12, are very common as primary target of mentalization-based treatments. However, middle childhood has received only limited attention regarding the ability to mentalize (Midgley et al., 2021).

2.3 Measures

Relevant biographical data are collected at baseline within a clinical interview. They included the children’s age at the first out-of-home placement, substance use dependency syndrome of biological parents and the number of caregivers since out-of-home placement. Data Research Topic is conducted by specifically trained clinical psychologists and psychotherapists.

2.3.1 Primary outcome

Caregiver and child Reflective Functioning (RF) are assessed by a licensed coder using the Reflective Functioning Scale (RFS) (Fonagy et al., 1998; Shmueli-Goetz et al., 2011) to interpret predefined sections of the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) (George et al., 1985), and using the Child Reflective Functioning Scale (CRFS) (Target et al., 2001) to interpret the Child Attachment Interview (CAI) (Shmueli-Goetz et al., 2008) transcripts. The AAI and the CAI, semi- structured clinical interviews focus on the subject’s attachment experiences with their parents during childhood. Some questions in the AAI (e.g., “Why did your parents behave as they did during your childhood?,” “Do you think your childhood experiences have an influence on who you are today?”) and some questions in the CAI (e.g., “Do your parents sometimes argue? How do they feel? Why do you think they do that?” or “What happens when your mum gets cross with you or tells you off? How do you feel?”) require reflective functioning (RF), while others allow it. According to Fonagy et al. (1998), RF occurs when the interviewee shows (1) awareness of the nature of mental states, (2) an explicit effort to tease out the mental states underlying one’s own and others’ behavior and (3) the tendency to recognize developmental aspects of mental states over time or (4) mental states in relation to the interviewer. After rating each identified passage of the AAI, an overall score is assigned to each interview ranging from −1 (negative RF) to 9 (exceptional RF) (Taubner et al., 2013). The same procedure will be conducted for the CAI.

After rating each identified passage of the CAI, an overall classification is assigned to the interview, using a hierarchical approach, and distinguishing between 10 levels of RF, ranging from −1 (negative RF) to 9 (exceptional RF) (Ensink et al., 2013).

Validation studies of the RFS (Fonagy et al., 1998; Ensink et al., 2015) proved discriminant and predictive validity and good interrater reliability. Temporal stability of children’s RF was shown to be high over a 3-month period and adequate over 12 months (Ensink, 2004).

2.3.2 Secondary outcomes

To investigate the children’s mental health, their clear-thinking ability, and their dynamic personality structure, as well as the caregiver’s attitudes on drugs and drug addiction and their social skills (e.g., social orientation and reflexibility) the following measures are chosen:

• The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/6-18R) is a parent report form to screen for emotional, behavioral, and social problems in children, based on the DSM-IV (Esser and Hänsch-Oelgart, 2018). A DSM-5 oriented version is not yet available. For the aim of the present study the presence of the following psychiatric disorders from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) were assessed: anxiety, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct problems, somatic problems, affective problems, and attention deficit disorder. The profiles show raw scale scores derived by the sum of each item. The scales are quantitatively scored in terms of gender- and age-specific t-scores with cut points for normal (t-scores from 50 to 64), borderline (t-scores from 65 to 69), and clinical (t-scores from 70 to 100) ranges (Döpfner et al., 2014). The CBCL is widely used in clinical research and diagnostical practice and shows good results regarding reliability and validity (Nakamura et al., 2009).

• Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM) measures clear-thinking ability and is designed for young children aged 5–10 years and older adolescents. The RCPM is internationally recognized as a culture-fair test of nonverbal (fluid) intelligence. The test consists of 36 items clustered in 3 sets (A, Ab, B), with 12 items per set. Most items are presented on a colored background to make the test visually stimulating for participants. The RCPM produces a single raw score that can be converted to a percentile based on normative data, designating the following categories: Performance Level 1 “well above average” (>95 percentile); Performance Level 2 “above average”(percentiles 75–95); Performance Level 3 “average” (percentiles 25–75); Performance Level 4 “well below average” (percentiles 5–25); Performance Level 5 “intellectual disability” (percentiles <5) (Bulheller and Häcker, 2002). In daily clinical practice, RCPM test is used as one of the best general intelligence measures (Smirni, 2020).

• The social orientation and Reflexibility subscales of the 33 items- Inventory of Social Competences (ISK; Inventar sozialer Kompetenzen) by Kanning (2009) is used to assess the caregiver’s social skills. The raw-data analysis is standardized by means of an evaluation form. The 17 raw scores of the primary scales and the four total raw scores of the secondary scales are transformed into stanine scores with the help of standard tables (Kanning, 2009). The reliability of the instrument can be rated as satisfactory to good. Numerous results from several individual studies on convergent and discriminant validity are reported (Scherp, 2010). For this study, we focus on the two subscales social orientation and reflexibility.

• The Patte-Noire (“black paw”) -Test is a French thematic-projective test for children from 6 to 12 years: a storytelling test through the character of the little pig Patte Noire, used in children’s clinical assessment, to elicit themes related to the child’s perceptions of the relationships between parents and children, and siblings and to obtain unconscious material. The test is administered according to Corman (1972) criteria in two main parts: telling a story and identifications. The child is asked to look at 17 different panels that show pigs in social situations, to choose one or more of them, to tell a story about “Patte Noire” and also to tell with which character on the panels he/she identifies—if possible. While working on the story, the child has to put him/herself in the situation described by the panels. In each panel one little pig is marked with a black paw. Every panel stimulates one of the following themes: orality, anality, oedipal issues, aggression, dependency-independence, guilt, black spot, inverted sexes, nurturing father, ideal mother. For the purposes of this study, 7 sheets out of the total 17 comprising Corman’s Patte Noire Test have been selected to take a look at the identifications in terms of attachment relations as it was already conducted and published by Ballus et al. (2019). The 7 selected pictures express clearly attachment relations in terms of attachment theory (Corman, 1977; Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1982; Yarnoz, 1993) and were selected in order to elicit information about the children’s attachment experiences, for example panel N°4: “the Cart.” Patte Noire (PN) dreams that a farmer is taking some piglets away in a cart. PN’s parents and two other piglets watch the scene. Theme of separation and loss. The childrens’ identifications on panel N°4 (“the cart”) in relation to the foster care situation has our special interest looking at the children’s identifications with the little pig (“Patte Noire”) or with “No One” or with somebody outside the scenery on the panel. This test is highly recommended by child psychoanalysts and is frequently used for clinical psychological diagnosis in addition to clinical psychological tests.

• Attitudes to Drug Use (ADU) (Harmon, 1993). A German version, translated and retranslated by expert native speakers, of a non-standardized 12-items paper-pencil-questionnaire is used to assess the caregiver’s attitudes to drug use on a 5-point-rating scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The questions are formulated as statements such as “using illegal drugs can be pleasant” or “trying drugs is losing control of your life.” Computation of Indices: Items (a), (d), (e), (i), (k), were scored “5” for “strongly agree” to “1” for “strongly disagree,” while the other items (−) were scored in the opposite way (1 for “strongly agree” to “5” for “strongly disagree”). To obtain an attitude score for each individual, the scores for each item were summed and divided by the number of items answered. A score of 5.00 indicates a completely positive attitude toward drugs while a score of 1.00 indicates a completely negative attitude. In accordance with Hulin et al. (2001), the internal reliability of this scale is within a very good range (α = 0.89).

2.4 Procedure

The study is conducted by the Faculty of Psychology of the University of Vienna in collaboration with the NGO Dialog.1 The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Vienna (#00295). All procedures performed in the study follow the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and are in agreement with the Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Written informed consent is obtained from the foster parents or legal guardians, and assent from children is gained as well.

Officially registered with the DRKS (German Clinical Trials Register) #DRKS00027868, the trial is displayed on the public website. It has been submitted to the WHO and is searchable through its meta-register.2 Caregiver and child Reflective Functioning (RF) are assessed by a licensed coder using the Reflective Functioning Scale (RFS) (Fonagy et al., 1998; Shmueli-Goetz et al., 2011) to interpret predefined sections of the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) (George et al., 1985), and using the Child Reflective Functioning Scale (CRFS) (Target et al., 2001) to interpret the Child Attachment Interview (CAI) (Shmueli-Goetz et al., 2008) transcripts.

The trial site is the outpatient center of the NGO Dialog in Vienna (A), where the mentalization-based group intervention was developed and takes place. This NGO specializes in prevention and treatment of substance use disorders for more than 40 years.

Participants follow a 20-week mentalization-based group intervention aiming at increasing knowledge about drug addiction and at enhancing social and affective skills in children (6–12 years) of drug abusing families living in foster care. The participation in the study is voluntary. Data are collected at baseline (T1), within 1 month after treatment termination (T2) and at 12 months follow-up (T3) (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study flow chart. Sequence of steps from T1 to T3.

2.5 Mentalization-based group intervention

Findings from a cross-European study conducted in 2007 on Domestic Violence and Abuse among Adolescents from Alcohol-affected Families indicated that adolescents expressed the value of being in relation with others who have undergone similar experiences or have faced similar challenges within their families. They perceived these interactions beneficial specifically in realizing that they were not alone in these circumstances (Velleman et al., 2008). The mentalization-centered group model offers a secure and contained environment, akin to a relational laboratory, in which children can safely investigate their thoughts and emotions regarding their real-life situations and emotional challenges (Malberg, 2012).

The Mentalized-Based Group Intervention (MBGI) was adapted from the MBT-C by Midgley et al. (2017) to the particular needs of children of drug abusing parents living in foster care in middle childhood. In accordance with other short-term interventions, such as “SMART” (Fearon et al., 2006) or “Trampolin” (Wiedow et al., 2011), ten group sessions for children and three group sessions for the foster caregivers are provided by specifically trained psychotherapists in a period of 5 months (school semester). The facilitators have attended trainings at the Anna Freud Centre in London (UK) on “Mentalizing and Mentalization Based Treatments with Children, Young People and Families (MBT CYP)” as well as “Reflective Parenting (MBT-RP).” Each session lasts about 90 min. The first encounter with the caregivers takes place after the first children’s group session to get to know each other, to obtain a first impression of the group by the psychotherapists, and to introduce mentalization processes to the caregivers in order to make them part of the children’s support. The second caregiver session is, scheduled halfway through the intervention. It is used to reflect on the ongoing treatment and its impact on the children’s daily lives. The final group session with the caregivers serves as a review of the experience and a preview of future intervention opportunities. The three caregiver sessions include information on parental licit/illicit drug abuse background focusing on the caregiver-child-interaction, along the process described by Fonagy and Allison (2014): (1) development of a therapeutic context in which the children/caregivers feel understood to reduce the sense of epistemic trust; (2) reemergence of the participants’ capacity to mentalize, as they “find their mind in the mind of the therapist”; (3) being aware of and interested in the content of the psychoeducational work of the therapists and to benefit from the group as a place to share experiences.

For the children, the 10 group sessions follow a ritualized procedure: there are predefined contents, such as drug addiction in general, the children’s situation in foster care, their feelings toward their biological parents and their foster caregivers. Since group dynamics in the sessions are never predictable, there is always time for “open topics,” brought up by the children. Group sessions are always conducted in the same room, which does not contain too many distracting elements but is large enough to allow for physical games like throwing and catching a ball or play “Emotion-Charade,” where children try to nonverbally pantomime a pre-selected emotion to each other. The material provided to the children involved painting, drawing and sketching tools. Children are invited to talk or to express themselves and their feelings by using available materials. Drawing is particularly useful, feels to be less intrusive and threatening than the request to talk about one’s experiences (Gardner and Harper, 1997).

The aims of the group sessions are (1) restoring the frame to think and talk about highly affective feelings such as anger, fear and shame, (2) building up a small and safe mentalizing community to explore mental states and giving sense to the behavior of oneself and others, (3) discussing the individual meaning and consequences of the experience of parental substance use or parents being “high” on licit/illicit drugs, and (4) providing new ego-strengthening skills.

The ritualized procedure contains a welcome circle that allows space for current events or feelings, planned psychoeducational inputs, and a special feedback method to recap what was discussed in each session and gather the children’s opinions on it.

The psychoeducational input always contains an interactive play or method.

As outcome of this Mentalization-Based Group Intervention, we expect a decrease in children’s psychiatric symptoms as there is evidence from previous studies on the effects of Mentalization-Based Interventions (Åkerman et al., 2020; Dalgaard et al., 2023). Furthermore, we expect an increase in the RF Score (capacity to mentalize). It has been shown that the RF Score can change as a result of psychotherapy, especially in psychodynamic oriented psychotherapy. These results have been observed, using the AAI to gain the RF Score (Katznelson, 2014).

2.6 Statistical plan and data analysis

Sociodemographic data will be used to characterize the sample and to provide an in-depth look into the participant’s psycho-social background. Frequencies and means are derived for demographic variables.

Thresholds for the measures will be used to describe each case, and to provide descriptive statistics, such as frequencies, mean and standard variation (SPSS 27.0), for each group: caregivers, children living in foster families and children living in institutional foster care.

Analyses on quantitative data will be conducted using the statistical software SPSS (IBM, Version 27.0).

All statistical analyses will consider the type of data available. Therefore, the primary outcome of the study will be assessed via the Friedman test as it is the non-parametric alternative to the one-way ANOVA with repeated measures, to compare the changes in reflective functioning scores between the pre-intervention, post-intervention, and one-year follow-up assessments, with the group (family foster care vs. residential programs) as a between-subjects factor and the time of assessment (pre, post, follow-up) as a within-subjects factor. In addition, the Mann–Whitney U test is used to compare differences between two independent groups (foster family and institutional foster care). To assess potential correlations regarding the child and caregivers’ capacity to mentalize, Spearman correlations (ρ) are conducted.

For the secondary outcome variables we choose a mixed-method approach for data analysis. Qualitative data from the “Patte-noir” will be processed via qualitative thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke, 2006). To provide an in-depth understanding of the psycho-social background of foster children and their foster caregiver, taking into account the caregivers’ social competences, their attitudes to drugs and drug addiction and the children’s mental state, quantitative methods, equal to the ones for the primary outcome variables, will be used.

This mixed-method approach was chosen to obtain different and multiple perspectives to validate the results and to build a comprehensive understanding.

3 Discussion

This study uses the concept of mentalization to identify affective and cognitive abilities of the caregiver-child dyad with the aim of compensating deficits on both sides with psychological-psychotherapeutic strategy. On the foster caregivers’ side, social structures and their anchoring in the social environment are addressed. Their prejudices are pointed out and questioned with the aim of providing suitable measures for change. Cassidy (2018) highlights that a foster caregiver of a traumatized child should be able to consistently empathize with the child, understanding that the child’s challenging behaviors may be a result from the child’s past experiences of abuse and neglect. As a further consequence, the child may be able to develop more secure and less disorganized attachment representations. On the children’s side, the emotional and affective background is illuminated, which at the same time reveals the risks for their future emotional, cognitive and social development. According to Suchman et al. (2020), cited in Paris et al. (2023), to the implementation of mentalization based approaches in research and practice of SUD patients treatment is still missing.

Initial findings from our research indicate that certain institutional foster caregivers exhibit low levels of mentalization capacity. Consequently, in collaboration with the Institute for Drug Prevention of the Office of Addiction and Drug Policy of Vienna, we will start in the near future the development for a mentalization-focused training program for institutional foster caregivers. The primary aims will be the enhancement of their reflective functioning, defined as the ability to mentalize, and to heighten sensitivity in interpersonal communication with children experiencing complex PTSD. The comprehensive training regimen will span several days of training sessions, supplemented by at least one mandatory reflection meeting within the initial year. The evaluation of the program’s effectiveness will rely on pre- and post-training measures of participants’ reflective functioning, utilizing an adapted version of the Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (RFQ-8). The detailed methodology and further findings will be disseminated in a forthcoming publication. The continued availability of this training program is an example for the successful transfer of results from a scientific study into practice to actually provide support to the sensitive target group.

The aim of publishing this study protocol is to inform the scientific community about the ongoing research and its transfer to clinical practice, and further to reach out to the community for exchanging ideas and to increase the coordination of research efforts.

However, the authors are very aware of some limitations of the study to mention. The small sample size will not allow do draw any gentrifications out of the results. Taken into account that the study is no RCT, historical control data will be used for comparison (Bizzi et al., 2022). One of the critical aspects of RCT is that “Intervention fidelity refers to the reliability and validity of the clinical interventions that are used in the randomized trial” (Cook and Thigpen, 2019, p. 64). Even though each group intervention of this study follows a strong ritualized procedure, its intervention fidelity cannot be comparable with an RCT on dosage of medication or manual based interventions. Still, the amount of the studies’ findings, due to the mixed-method design that is used, helps to deduce research questions for further studies. The study is carried out in a naturalistic setting. This can be considered as a strength as it increases its ecological validity.

As we are aware of the need for long-term support for this particularly sensitive group of subjects, all children have the option to attend the group intervention program more than once. Due to the third testing point after 12 months, the children who participate at the study will have to take a break until they attended the third measure time point before they can continue to participate at the group intervention.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Vienna (#00295). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

NS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft. BL-S: Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Adkins, T., Luyten, P., and Fonagy, P. (2018). Development and preliminary evaluation of family in minds: a mentalization-based psychoeducation program for Foster parents. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27, 2519–2532. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1080-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Adkins, T., Reisz, S., Hasdemir, D., and Fonagy, P. (2022). Family minds: a randomized controlled trial of a group intervention to improve foster parents’ reflective functioning. Dev. Psychopathol. 34, 1177–1191. doi: 10.1017/S095457942000214X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Åkerman, A.-K. E., Holmqvist, R., Frostell, A., and Falkenström, F. (2020). Effects of Mentalization-based interventions on mental health of youths in Foster Care. Child Care Pract. 28, 536–549. doi: 10.1080/13575279.2020.1812531

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Akhtar, S. (2009). Comprehensive dictionary of psychoanalysis. London: Karnac Books.

Google Scholar

Allen, J., Fonagy, P., and Bateman, A. (2008). Mentalizing in clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Google Scholar

Altshuler, S. J., and Cleverly-Thomas, A. (2011). What do we know about drug- endangered children when they are first placed into care? Child Welfare 90, 45–68

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th Edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

Google Scholar

Ballus, E., Urrutia, E., Casas, M., and Loizaga, F. (2019). Attachment picture story (APS): adaptation and validation of the projective Patte noire test to evaluate attachment in adolescence. Rorschach. J. Int. Soc. Rorschach 40, 73–94. doi: 10.1027/1192-5604/a000113

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bammens, A.-S., Adkins, T., and Badger, J. (2015). Psycho-educational intervention increases reflective functioning in foster and adoptive parents. Adopt. Foster. 39, 38–50. doi: 10.1177/0308575914565069

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateman, A., and Fonagy, P. (2008). 8-year follow-up of patients treated for borderline personality disorder: mentalization-based treatment versus treatment as usual. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 631–638. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040636

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateman, A., and Fonagy, P. (2016). Mentalization-based treatment for personality disorders: a practical guide : Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Bizzi, F., Charpentier-Mora, S., Cavanna, D., Borelli, J. L., and Ensink, K. (2022). Testing children’s mentalizing in middle childhood: adopting the child and adolescent reflective functioning scale with clinical and community children. J. Child Fam. Stud. 31, 114–127. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-02057-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: vol. 1. attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1998). Attachment and loss: loss: sadness and depression. London: Pimlico.

Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brockmann, J., and Kirsch, H. (2010). Konzept der Mentalisierung, Relevanz für die psychotherapeutische Behandlung. Psychotherapeut 55, 279–290. doi: 10.1007/s00278-010-0751-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruskas, D. (2008). Children in foster care: a vulnerable population at risk. J. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Nurs. 21, 70–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6171.2008.00134.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulheller, S., and Häcker, H. O. (2002) in Coloured progressive matrices (CPM). eds. D. Bearbeitung and N. N. J. C. Raven (Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment).

Google Scholar

Cassibba, R., Balenzano, C., Silletti, F., Coppola, G., Costantini, A., Giorgio, S., et al. (2023). The Placement of Children in Need of Out-of-Home Care: Forms of Care and Differences in Attachment Security and Behavioral Problems in the Italian Context. International journal of environmental research and public health, 20:7111. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20237111

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, J. (2018). “The nature of the child’s ties” in Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. eds. J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York: Guilford Press), 3–24.

Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D., Ragosch, F. A., and Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through preventive interventions. Dev. Psychopathol. 18, 623–649. doi: 10.1017/s0954579406060329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D., and Toth, S. L. (1995). A developmental psychopathology perspective on child abuse and neglect. J. Ame. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. 34, 541–565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199505000-00008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, C. E., and Thigpen, C. A. (2019). Five good reasons to be disappointed with randomized trials. J. Man. Manip. Ther. 27, 63–65. doi: 10.1080/10669817.2019.1589697

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corman, L. (1972). Le test PN.: Manuel. France: Presses universitaires de France.

Google Scholar

Corman, L. (1977). Der Schwarzfuß-Test: Grundlagen, Durchführung, Deutung und Auswertung, vol. Band 1. München: Reinhardt.

Google Scholar

Dalgaard, N. T., Villumsen, A. M. A., Sørensen, K. M., Midgley, N., Væver, M. S., Almlund, M., et al. (2023). Holding a foster child’s mind in mind: study protocol for a cluster-randomized controlled trial of mentalization-based therapy (MBT) for foster families. BMC Psychol. 11:62. doi: 10.1186/s40359-023-01103-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dejko, K., Janusz, B., and Treger, B. (2016). Characteristics of mentalization patterns in parents of children with difficulties in realizing develop- mental objectives of the latency stage—qualitative analysis results. Psychiatr. Pol. 50, 597–606. doi: 10.12740/PP/OnlineFirst/38904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Döpfner, M., Plück, J., and Kinnen, C. (2014). CBCL/6-18R, TRF/6-18R, YSR/11-18R. Deutsche Schulalter-Formen der Child Behavior Checklist von Thomas M. Achenbach. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Ensink, K. (2004). Assessing theory of mind, affective understanding and reflective functioning in primary school age children. Dissertation. London: University College London.

Google Scholar

Ensink, K., Bégin, M., Normandin, L., Godbout, N., and Fonagy, P. (2017). Mentalization and dissociation in the context of trauma: implications for child psychopathology. J. Trauma Dissociat. 18, 11–30. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2016.1172536

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ensink, K., Normandin, L., Target, M., Fonagy, P., Sabourin, S., and Berthelot, N. (2015). Mentalization in children and mothers in the context of trauma: an initial study of validity of the child reflective functioning scale. Dev. Psychol. 33, 203–217. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12074

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ensink, K., Target, M., and Oandasan, C. (2013). Child reflective functioning scale scoring manual: For application to the child attachment interview. (Unpublished manuscript). UK: Anna Freud Centre – University College London.

Google Scholar

Esser, G., and Hänsch-Oelgart, S. (2018). CBCL/6-18R, TRF/6-18R, YSR/11-18R. Deutsche Schulalter-Formen der Child Behavior Checklist von Thomas M. Achenbach. Elternfragebogen über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen (CBCL/6-18R), Lehrerfragebogen über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen (TRF/6-18R), Fragebogen für Jugendliche (YSR/11-18R). Psychol. Rundsch. 69, 109–148. doi: 10.1026/0033-3042/a000394

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmakopoulou, I., and Baltsioti, V. (2024). Revealing the Profile of Foster Parents, Biological Parents, Foster Children and the Triadic Relationship amongst them. Technium Social Sciences Journal 55, 319–341. doi: 10.47577/tssj.v55i1.10728

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fearon, P., Target, M., Sargent, J., Williams, L. L., McGregor, J., Bleiberg, E., et al. (2006). “Short-term mentalization and relational therapy (SMART): an integrative family therapy for children and adolescents” in The handbook of mentalization-based treatment. eds. J. G. Allen and P. Fonagy (West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 201–222.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., and Higgitt, A. C. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Ment. Health J. 12, 201–218. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(199123)12:3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Allison, E. (2014). The role of mentalizing and epistemic trust in the therapeutic relationship. Psychotherapy 51, 372–380. doi: 10.1037/a0036505

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., and Steele, M. (1998). Reflective functioning manual. Version 5.0 for application to the adult attachment interviews. London: University College.

Google Scholar

Gardner, D., and Harper, P. (1997). “The use of metaphor and impression” in The therapeutic use of stories. ed. K. N. Davidi (London: Routledge), 129–141.

Google Scholar

George, C., Kaplan, N., and Main, M. (1985). Adult Attachment Interview. Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Google Scholar

Giusti, E. M., Manna, C., Scolari, A., Mestre, J. M., Prevendar, T., Castelnuovo, G., et al. (2021). The relationship between emotional intelligence, obesity and eating disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic mapping review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:2054. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18042054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grienenberger, J., Kelly, K., and Slade, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, mother–infant affective communication and infant attachment: exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behaviour in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attach Hum. Dev. 7, 299–311. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245963

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon, M. A. (1993). Reducing the risk of drug involvement among early adolescents. Eval. Rev. 17, 221–239. doi: 10.1177/0193841x9301700206

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, J. P., and Su, S. S. (1998). Parental substance use disorder, mediating variables and adolescent drug use: a non-recursive model. Addiction 93, 1351–1364. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.93913516.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulin, C., Netemeyer, R., and Cudeck, R. (2001). Can a reliability coefficient be too high? J. Consum. Psychol. 10, 55–58. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1001&2_05

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, M. N., Ha, C., and Sharp, C. (2015). A mentalization-based treatment approach to caring for youth in foster care. J. Infant Child Adolesc. Psychother. 14, 440–454. doi: 10.1080/15289168.2015.1093921

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones Harden, B., Morrison, C., and Clyman, R. (2014). Emotion labelling among preschool children in foster care. Early Educ. Dev. 25, 1180–1197. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2014.907694

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanning, U. P. (2009). Inventar sozialer Kompetenzen. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Katznelson, H. (2014). Reflective functioning: a review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 34, 107–117. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.12.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Madigan, S., Moran, G., Schuengel, C., Pederson, D. R., and Otten, R. (2007). Unresolved maternal attachment representations, disrupt maternal behavior and disorganized attachment in infancy: links to toddler behavior problems. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 1042–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01805.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malberg, N. T. (2012). “Thinking and feeling in the context of chronic illness. A mentalization-based group intervention with adolescents” in Minding the child: mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families. eds. N. Midgley and I. Vrouva (London: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Matthews, S., Dwyer, R., and Snoek, A. (2017). Stigma and self-stigma in addiction. Bioeth. Inq. 14, 275–286. doi: 10.1007/s11673-017-9784-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayes, L. C. (2012). “Foreword” in Minding the child: mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families. eds. N. Midgley and I. Vrouva, vol. 74 (London: Routledge), 1.

Google Scholar

McGlade, A., Ware, R., and Crawford, M. (2009). Child protection outcomes for infants of substance-using mothers: a matched-cohort study. Pediatrics 124, 285–293. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0576

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Midgley, N., Cirasola, A., Austerberry, C., Ranzato, E., West, G., Martin, P., et al. (2019). Supporting foster carers to meet the needs of looking after children: a feasibility and pilot evaluation of the reflective fostering program. Dev. Child Welfare 1, 41–60. doi: 10.1177/2516103218817550

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Midgley, N., Ensink, K., Lindquist, K., Malberg, N., and Muller, N. (2017). Mentalization-based treatment for children. A time-limited approach. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Midgley, N., Sprecher, E. A., and Sleed, M. (2021). Mentalization-based interventions for children aged 6-12 and their carers: a narrative systematic review. J. Infant Child Adolesc. Psychother. 20, 169–189. doi: 10.1080/15289168.2021.1915654

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, M. D., Kauffman, E., Nelson-Zlupko, L., and Granfort, E. (1996). Psychosocial functioning and treatment needs of latency-age children from drug-involved families. Famil. Soc. 77, 595–604.

Google Scholar

Muller, N., Gerits, L., and Siecker, I. (2012). “Mentalization-based therapies with adopted children and their families” in Minding the child: mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families. eds. N. Midgley and I. Vrouva (London: Routledge), 113–130.

Google Scholar

Nakamura, B. J., Ebesutani, C., Bernstein, A., and Chorpita, B. F. (2009). A psychometric analysis of the child behavior checklist DSM-oriented scales. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 31, 178–189. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9119-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Österreich, B. (2022). Kinder- und Jugendhilfestatistik 2021. Available at: https://www.statistik.at/fileadmin/publications/Kinder-und-Jugendhilfestatistik-2021.pdf (Accessed June 29, 2023).

Google Scholar

Ostler, T., Bahar, O. S., and Jessee, A. (2010). Mentalization in children exposed to parental methamphetamine abuse: relations to children’s mental health and behavioural outcomes. Attach Hum. Dev. 12, 193–207. doi: 10.1080/14616731003759666

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pajulo, M., Pyykkönen, N., Kalland, N., Sinkkonen, J., Helenius, H., Punamäki, R. L., et al. (2012). Substance-abusing mothers in resident treatment with their babies: importance of pre- and postnatal maternal reflective functioning. Infant Ment. Health J. 33, 70–81. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paris, R., Herriott, A. L., and Holt, M. (2023). Parenting stress and competence among mothers of young children with substance use disorders: the roles of trauma and reflective functioning. Infant Ment. Health J. 44, 228–239. doi: 10.1002/imhj.22040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pecora, P. J., Kessler, R. C., Williams, J., O’Brien, K., Downs, A. C., English, D., et al. (2005). Improving family foster care: findings from the northwest foster care alumni study. Available at: https://www.casey.org/northwest-alumni-study/

Google Scholar

Petrowski, N., Cappa, C., and Gross, P. (2017). Estimating the number of children in formal alternative care: challenges and results. Child Abuse Negl. 70, 388–398. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherp, E. (2010). ISK – Inventar sozialer Kompetenzen. Z. Arbeits Organisationspsychol. 54, 193–200. doi: 10.1026/0932-4089/a000030

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, S. J., and Schimmelmann, B. G. (2015). Mechanisms of change in psychotherapy for children and adolescents: current state, clinical implications, and methodological and conceptual recommendations for mediation analysis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 24, 249–253. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0698-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seay, K. (2015). How many families in child welfare services are affected by parental substance use disorders? A common question that remains unanswered. Child Welfare 94, 19–51

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shmueli-Goetz, Y., Target, M., Fonagy, P., and Datta, A. (2008). The child attachment interview: a psychometric study of reliability and discriminant validity. Dev. Psychol. 44, 939–956. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.939

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shmueli-Goetz, Y., Target, M., Datta, A., and Fonagy, P. (2011). Child Attachment Interview (CAI) Coding and Classification Manual. Version VI. London: The Sub-Department of Clinical Health Psychology, University College London.

Google Scholar

Slade, A. (2002). Keeping the baby in mind: a critical factor in perinatal mental health. Zero Three 2002, 10–16.

Google Scholar

Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., and Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment and the transmission gap: a preliminary study. Attach Hum. Dev. 7, 283–298. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245880

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smirni, D. (2020). The Raven’s Coloured progressive matrices in healthy children: a qualitative approach. Brain Sci. 10:877. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10110877

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, V. C., and Wilson, C. R. (2016). AAP committee on substance use and prevention. Families affected by parental substance use. Pediatrics 138:e20161575. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1575

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, H., and Steele, M. (2008). “On the origins of reflective functioning” in Mentalization: theoretical considerations, research findings, and clinical implications. ed. F. Busch, vol. 29 (New York, NY: The Analytic Press), 133–158). Psychoanalytic Inquiry Book Series,.

Google Scholar

Suchman, N. (2016). Mothering from the inside out: a mentalization-based therapy for mothers in treatment for drug addiction. Int. J. Birth Parent Educ. 3, 19–24

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Suchman, N. E., Borelli, J. L., and DeCoste, C. L. (2020). Can addiction counselors be trained to deliver mothering from the inside out, a mentalization-based parenting therapy, with fidelity? Results from a community-based randomized efficacy trial. Attach Hum. Dev. 22, 332–351. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1559210

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N. E., DeCoste, C., Leigh, D., and Borelli, J. (2010). Reflective functioning in mothers with drug use disorders: implications for dyadic interactions with infants and toddlers. Attach Hum. Dev. 12, 567–585. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2010.501988

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchman, N., Pajulo, M., DeCoste, C., and Mayes, L. (2006). Parenting interventions for drug-dependent mothers and their young children: the case for an attachment-based approach. Fam. Relat. 55, 211–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2006.00371.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Szilagyi, M. A., Rosen, D. S., Rubin, D., and Zlotnik, S. (2015). Policy statement: health care issues for children and adolescents in foster care and kinship care. Pediatrics 136:e1142, –e1166. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2656

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Target, M., Oandasan, K., and Ensink, K. (2001). Child reflective functioning scale scoring manual for application to child attachment interview. London: University College.

Google Scholar

Taubner, S., Hörz, S., Fischer-Kern, M., Doering, S., Buchheim, A., and Zimmermann, J. (2013). Internal structure of the reflective functioning scale. Psychol. Assess. 25, 127–135. doi: 10.1037/a0029138

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van de Meer, M., Jansen, D., and Anthonio, G. (2019). Barriers by professionals in primary care in discussing children of substance abusing parents. Eur. J. Pub. Health 29:663. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckz186.671

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Velleman, R., Templeton, L., Reuber, D., Klein, M., and Moesgen, D. (2008). Domestic abuse experienced by young people living in families with alcohol problems: results from a cross-european study. Child Abuse Rev. 17, 387–409. doi: 10.1002/car.1047

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, M. B., Dowd, K. L., Jones Harden, B., Landsverk, J., and Testa, M. (Eds.) (2010). Child welfare and child well-being: new perspectives from the national survey of child and adolescent well-being. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Wiedow, A., Ruths, S., Moesgen, D., Pflug, E., Klein, M., and Thomasius, R. (2011). Projekt Trampolin – ein modulares Präventionskonzept für Kinder aus suchtbelasteten Familien: Ergebnisse einer multizentrischen Evaluationsstudie. Suchttherapie 12:S35_3. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1284628

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarnoz, S. (1993). Análisis de la estructura factorial del test Pata Negra. Anal. Psicol. 9, 177–186.

Google Scholar

Zunich, M. (1966). Child behavior and parental attitudes. J. Psychol. 62, 41–46. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1966.10543023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mentalization-based treatment, reflective functioning, foster care, substance use disorder, parenting

Citation: Springer N and Lueger-Schuster B (2024) Navigating foster care: how parental drug use and caregiver attitudes shape children’s mentalization processes—an exploratory longitudinal follow-up study: study protocol. Front. Psychol. 15:1295809. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1295809

Received: 17 September 2023; Accepted: 28 May 2024;
Published: 13 June 2024.

Edited by:

Stefano Eleuteri, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sigita Lesinskiene, Vilnius University, Lithuania
Xin Xie, East Tennessee State University, United States

Copyright © 2024 Springer and Lueger-Schuster. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nadja Springer, nadja.springer@univie.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.