Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 08 April 2024
Sec. Performance Science
This article is part of the Research Topic International Symposium on Performance Science 2023 View all 15 articles

Focus of attention in musical learning and music performance: a systematic review and discussion of focus instructions and outcome measures

  • Freiburg Institute for Musicians’ Medicine, University of Music Freiburg, Medical Center of Albert-Ludwigs-University Freiburg, Freiburg Center for Music Research and Teaching, Freiburg, Germany

The topic of attentional focus (focus of attention, FOA) in musical learning and performance has recently received increasing interest, as the growing number of empirical studies inspired by the established FOA paradigm in sports by Wulf and colleagues in 1998. The current systematical review aims at collecting, abstracting, and categorizing relevant data to show which kinds of FOA instructions were applied in experimental designs and what kinds of dependent variables were used to measure the effects of FOA instruction on musical performance. The three main inclusion criteria in the selection process were experimental design, detailed descriptions of FOA instructions, and outcome measures (OMs). A systematic search was conducted with a complex search term in four scientific databases in March 2023. For presenting and synthesizing results, we used data collection and an inductive-deductive data categorization. Fifteen studies with a total sample size of 401 participants were included out of 387 records initially identified. We collected 53 different FOA instruction citations from the 15 studies and classified them into 9 FOA subcategories, of which the most applied were bodily focus (21%), sound focus (15%), and visual focus (14%). Selected studies used 63 OMs that were abstracted to 10 different OM categories with expert ratings (27%) and acoustical analysis (22%) as the most applied dependent variables. Data categorization and abstraction of additional study information show multiple combinations of FOA instructions, OMs, participants’ instruments and expertise, and musical tasks. Finally, studies show no consistent results of superiority of either external or internal or otherwise different FOA considering positive effects on musical performance. Limitations of the review lie in the small study sample, possible criticism of applied eligibility criteria, and subjectivity of data categorization. We propose a research agenda with a more exploratory approach that comprehensively and qualitatively examines the dimensions of musical goals to create a database that could provide a foundation for developing a music-specific FOA model.

1 Introduction

The general questions of how we locate our attention while we perform and why we do so play a crucial role in physiological and psychological processes in performances of various everyday life areas as well as in many professional, high-performance domains (e.g., sports, music, dance, etc.). Considering these performance areas in which motor control and motor learning are highly important, we can add many questions that are of special interest to certain research fields, performers, trainers, and educators. Assuming there is a performance effect based on attentional mechanisms, questions arise as to how these processes manifest in different learning or performing situations. What degree of influence does the type of movement, movement task, expertise, pressure, anxiety, state of consciousness, and, finally, instructions have on performance quality and how can we measure it in a domain-specific way? The current systematic review aims to shed light upon some open challenges of focus of attention (FOA) research in general in the music domain by systematically collecting and abstracting data on the two much-discussed aspects of instruction and outcome measures (OMs).

The idea that attentional processes could affect motor performances is not new, nor is it the scientific discourse on it that began at the end of the 19th century and continues today. James (1890, p. 520) describes in chapter XXVI of his book The Principles about the production of movement “[…] that we fail of accuracy and certainty in our attainment of the end whenever we are preoccupied with much ideal consciousness of the means.” Moreover, Bliss (1895, p. 55) said in the last sentence of his report, Investigations in reaction-time and attention, that it is “[…] a well-known fact that we can perform numerous actions much better when only half attending to them.” From the 1960s, movement and sports research shaped the debate about positive or negative attentional effects on performance from the motor learning perspective. Many of the well-established motor learning theories and concepts refer to development from conscious and highly controlled movements or actions at the beginning of the acquisition of motor skill to a more unconscious and highly free, automatic performance of movements at an expert stage. Meinel (1960) defined this stage as Variable Verfügbarkeit (variable availability), in which a performer can detach from movement execution and focus on movement expression (for the role of variability in this regard, see also Bernstein, 1967). Other phase concepts described this as the autonomous phase (Fitts and Posner, 1967) that contains automatic mechanisms and the ruggedness of movement execution against external resistances.

1.1 Attention under pressure: explicit monitoring and distraction theories

Following this tradition, experimental psychologists of the 1970s and 1980s started to ask and examine whether self-awareness, self-consciousness, and certain attentional processes could aid or detract from performance success (Martens and Landers, 1972; Langer and Imber, 1979), especially in contexts, in which performers are under pressure (Baumeister, 1984; see also Masters, 1992). Later, Beilock and Carr (2001) subsume the explanations for this phenomenon as explicit monitoring theory (EMT). The terms execution focus theory (see also Beilock and Carr, 2001) and conscious processing hypothesis (CPH; Mullen and Hardy, 2000; Wilson et al., 2007) are in line with EMT and highlight that a step-by-step focus on execution degrades performance and this control or self-control disrupts fluency as well as automaticity of movement on the expert level. Although Baumeister (1984) also emphasized the role of anxiety in his concept, other theories put the fear of failing in situations under pressure at the center of their arguments (Wine, 1971; Eysenck, 1979). This distraction theory describes processes of involuntary shifts of attention to task-irrelevant information. Another attempt by Eysenck and Calvo (1992) claims that anxiety does not directly impair performance effectiveness but negatively impacts the efficiency of the on-task effort—and further leads to a reduction of processing capacity that results in performance degrading (processing efficiency theory, see also Smith et al., 2001; Murray and Janelle, 2003). The first comparative experimental studies from the music and sports domains show that EMT or CPH seems to be a more useful explanation theory for choking under pressure processes than the distraction or processing efficiency attempts (Wan and Huon, 2005; Wilson et al., 2007). However, more recent results tended to contradict those insights (Lee and Grafton, 2015; Buchanan et al., 2018; Furuya et al., 2021), whereas other studies seemed to confirm it (Reeves et al., 2007; Gray et al., 2013; Carson and Collins, 2016)—the discussion and research on this topic is ongoing (see also Saikley and Haroush, 2021).

1.2 Attentional focus in motor learning and motor performance

In 1998, Gabriele Wulf and colleagues published the results of an experimental study that has received much attention in the last two decades up to the current discourse. Referring to the abovementioned concepts of Baumeister (1984) and Masters (1992), explaining performance degrading under pressure, Wulf et al. (1998) developed an experimental paradigm to examine the effects of different attentional focus instructions on motor learning, independent from the existence of high-pressure situations or anxiety processes. In the first experiment, participants should perform a repetitive skiing-like movement task multiple times with a ski-simulator. The instruction was either “[…] to try to exert force on the outer foot (e. g., the right foot) as long as the platform moved in the respective direction (e. g., to the right side)” (internal-focus group) or “to try to exert force on the outer wheels as long as the platform moved in the respective direction” (external-focus group; Wulf et al., 1998, p. 172). The results show significantly higher mean amplitude (derived from the platform position data) of movements of the external-focus group compared to the internal-focus group (and a control group that got no specific instruction) in both practice trials and retention tests. A second experiment containing a balance task somehow confirmed these results by showing fewer balance errors in the external-focus group than in the internal-focus group during a retention test after 2 days of practice, in which no further instructions were given. Although a concrete theoretical foundation of that outcome could not be found by the authors at that time, the common-coding theory (Prinz, 1997)—describing a common representation of perception and action in the brain—serves as a theoretical framework due to its link to distal events in the form of perception–action coupling mechanisms (an external FOA is a “distal event”). Wulf herself later interpreted that link as insufficient: “Yet, because the theory is rather abstract, it does not specifically predict the differential learning effects of external versus internal attentional foci. It also does not explain any underlying mechanisms of this effect” (Wulf, 2013, p. 91).

Due to the promising results, and despite missing adequate theoretical constructs, Wulf and other researchers applied the paradigm to other movement tasks and could confirm the claimed benefit of an external FOA for motor learning (e.g., Shea and Wulf, 1999; McNevin et al., 2000; Wulf et al., 2000) before formulating the constrained action hypothesis (CAH; Wulf et al., 2001a,b). The CAH describes a negative effect chain from (1) focusing consciously on body movement execution or trying to control it, (2) interfering with automatic control motor processes, to (3) a performance degrading or action constraining effect. In addition, or as an expansion, a less beneficial focus on the self through an over-evaluation of one’s own actions could enhance the interference effect (self-invoking trigger hypothesis, Wulf and Lewthwaite, 2010; McKay et al., 2015). Turning the CAH—which focuses on disadvantageous processes—into an assumption, what kind of FOA could be beneficial for supporting the motor system to be automatic and self-organized, Wulf and colleagues formulate the general instruction advice to focus “[…] on the intended movement effect or task goal” (Wulf and Lewthwaite, 2016, p. 1402). Another hypothesis describes the importance of distance in this regard. The further away a goal appears that refers to external FOA instructions, the greater the effect on motor learning and performance (distal foci effect hypothesis; Bell and Hardy, 2009; Duke et al., 2011; McKay and Wulf, 2012; Stambaugh, 2017). Current results from a meta-analysis confirmed the hypothesis that distal-external foci instructions have a more positive impact on motor learning than proximal-external foci instructions (Chua et al., 2021).

An impressive number of experimental studies demonstrate the superiority of the effects of external FOA instructions on motor learning and motor performance. However, there are critical discussions on some aspects of the big picture, e.g., regarding various theoretical issues (Ehrlenspiel and Maurer, 2007; Oudejans et al., 2007; Poolton et al., 2007; Raab, 2007; Peh et al., 2011), methodical questions (Mullen, 2007), and the missing of a theoretical construct that could explain learning benefits of an external FOA (Maurer and Zentgraf, 2007; for an overview, collection of critical commentaries, and responses by Wulf, see the special issue of Bewegung und Training [Movement and Training], 2007).

1.3 Focus of attention in music

Madsen and Geringer (1990), Geringer and Madsen (1996), Madsen (1997) started a series of studies investigating the focus of attention on different musical elements of musicians and non-musicians while listening to music. Whereas this research is not in line with the attentional effects of focus instructions on motor learning, the results show how different the attentional focus on diverse musical parameters can be in relation to musical expertise or musical stimuli. Wulf and Mornell (2008, see also Mornell, 2007) were the first to investigate the transfer and adaptation of FOA findings from sports to music and dance (see also Guss-West and Wulf, 2016; Mornell and Wulf, 2019). This was followed by an experimental study by Duke et al. (2011) on the effects of attentional instructions on various aspects of solving a short piano task. In their study, participants focused on the fingers, the keys, the hammers, and the sound of the music while playing as part of a repeated measures design. The authors investigated the effect of different FOAs on the evenness of playing movements and showed that non-experts play significantly more consistently in the transfer test when focusing on the hammers and the sound of the music.

Following these results, the FOA paradigm of Wulf et al. (1998), and different explanatory hypotheses (e.g., CAH), some music-related experimental and exploratory studies have been conducted, e.g., in singing (Atkins and Duke, 2013; Atkins, 2017, 2018; Treinkman, 2022a), on the effect in piano playing (Cheng et al., 2011; Lipke-Perry et al., 2022; Jentzsch and Braun, 2023), violin playing (Allingham et al., 2021; Allingham and Wöllner, 2022), wind instrument playing (Stambaugh, 2017, 2019; Williams et al., 2023), or in music education settings (Silvey and Montemayor, 2014; Montemayor et al., 2016; Parsons and Simmons, 2021). The results of these studies vary widely, with some evidence of a positive effect of an external FOA on certain aspects of musical learning and musical performances and some results showing no significant differences between different FOA instructions.

Most of the studies investigating FOA in music predominantly used motor learning and performance under pressure models as theoretical underpinnings and transferred those to create an experimental paradigm with a musical task (e.g., Duke et al., 2011; Atkins and Duke, 2013; Atkins, 2017; Stambaugh, 2017; Mornell and Wulf, 2019). Allingham et al. (2021) used an additional music-specific theoretical framework by Jensenius (2007) that presents an action-sound chain describing the process from neurological activity in the brain at the start to the production of sound at the end. In more detail, he outlined a paired connection mechanism between the involved performance part and its location area (Brain–Neurological, Muscle–Physiological, Limb–Physical, Instrument–Mechanical, Sound–Acoustical; see Jensenius, 2007, p. 24). In all these parts of the process, a multimodal feedback-loop takes place. This model could be used as an explanation for sound as an external FOA because, at least in the dimension of time, it is the furthest point. Williams et al. (2023, p. 3) provided an attentional focus continuum model for musicians, classifying focus instructions into four main categories from proximal to distal, namely internal focus, external focus, distal external focus, and very distal external focus. It is a movement-oriented approach that subsumes insights from the motor learning and music research field but lacks precise sources and theoretical underpinnings regarding what dimension the continuum is grounded on—it could be time, room, and mental capacity. There are only very few approaches to investigating the attention processes of musicians in an explorative and qualitative manner to discover which music-specific aspects condition the direction of an attentional focus. Buma et al. (2015) used a collection of statements from experienced professional musicians to examine different thoughts before and during a performance situation under pressure. The various statements were categorized using cluster analysis and inductively assigned to six focus categories. The individual statements were then evaluated by musicians for their importance and frequency of occurrence during stage performances. The results show that a musical focus is mentioned most frequently, but the relevance and application of a musical focus are not considered as important as that of a focus on physical aspects. A study by Oudejans et al. (2017), which builds on this, deals with the focus shortly before and after moments of choking under pressure and assigns an important significance to a focus on musical aspects here. Another qualitative approach was made by Treinkman (2022a), who examines attentional focus processes in singing by asking more than 200 singers about their foci while practicing and performing. The deductive assignment of the singer’s open statements, where they direct their attention to Wulf’s paradigm of internal vs. external FOA, showed no convincing results regarding which kind of foci were preferred, used, or useful in singing. A qualitative thematic analysis of open-format questionnaires with string players conducted by Lubert et al. (2023) shows four main themes of reported attentional foci during music performance under pressure, that is, navigation of music-related aspects, physical and emotional performance experience, critical thoughts and attempts and control, and quality and dynamic of focus. These explorative studies and inductively performed qualitative analyses are very important for the field due to the differentiated perspective on attentional foci in various musical situations.

1.4 Challenges of attentional focus instruction and outcome measures in music

A critical aspect in assessing the effect of attentional instructions in music is the heterogeneity in terms of participants (amateur and professional musicians), instruments (vocals, strings, winds, etc.), and musical tasks or tests (high internal validity and low ecological validity or very application-oriented tasks or music practice interventions). However, the variety of reported verbal attentional instructions and their mainly deductive classification to the internal vs. external FOA categorization by Wulf et al. (1998) lead to difficulties in the interpretation of effects as well as study results. There are some limitations of a dichotomous assignment of instructions for executing a musical task into categories of internal and external FOA (or complementarily possibly far-external and proximal-external FOA). Instructions used in the experiments do not exclusively refer to one movement execution (internal FOA) or one (near or far) movement target (external FOA) as Wulf and colleagues’ paradigm purports (Wulf et al., 1998; see also Wulf, 2013; Wulf and Lewthwaite, 2016). They refer to many different aspects that play a role in making music—namely, body movements, breathing, sound, visual orientation, consistency, communication, visual and auditory imagination, metaphors, musical instruments, physical resistance, creativity, expressivity, musical articulation, etc.—and a music-specific theoretical FOA model that could explain, connect, or differentiate these aspects from another is missing. Recently, Herrebrøden (2023) argued in his critical review that the superiority of external FOA instructions in motor learning experiments in the sports and music domains could alternatively be explained with the direction of instructions on task-relevant information—whereas internal foci instruction often refers to task-irrelevant information.

Finally, the measurement methods used in the experiments are as heterogenic as the various aspects mentioned before. Measuring musical performance is a fundamental problem that plays a major role in a transfer or commonality of sports and musical performance models (“First, we have the problem of measurement,” Schmidt and Lee, 2012, p. 17). In sports and movement research, there is no discussion of the outcomes of gross movement tasks or specific types of sports scoring systems. We can easily measure how high we jump, and we can count baskets, holes, bull’s eyes, or detect errors while trying to reach a task goal (for an overview of outcome measures in the FOA motor learning field, see Chua et al., 2021, p. 622, footnote 2 and Appendix). One of the few exceptions in the FOA research field is expert ratings in gymnastics (Lawrence et al., 2011). In music performance research, the discussion of how to assess musical performances validly and reliably has a long tradition (Saunders, 1993; McPherson and Thompson, 1998; Thompson and Williamon, 2003) and is still up to date (Wesolowski and Wind, 2019a,b; for an overview of different perspectives on the issue from education and research see Brophy, 2019).

1.5 Review aims and research questions

Although there was no empirical research on the effects of FOA in music at the time of Wulf and Mornell’s (2008) contribution, the authors formulated implications for music education based on the findings from the field of motor learning: “Teachers will ideally look for verbal instructions that direct attention away from small muscle movements or body, so that automatic motor programs are not disrupted by cognitive interference” (p. 14). Similar deductions are also made based on other results, although the study situation and less evidence do not (yet) provide clear pedagogical or didactical implications for musicians and singers while they practice or perform on stage.

Thus, this review first aims to contribute to a broader discourse in the FOA field by systematically displaying the genesis and actual research situation, mainly in the sports and music domains. Second, we intend to highlight theoretical and methodical challenges and examine to what extent a movement-based model can be transferred to the specifics of musical skill acquisition and music performance. Two of the main questions in this context serve as a framework for the current review and future directions of examining FOA effects in music: What should we focus on and how can we measure it? In more detail and in the context of the present review, we have the following three research questions:

1. How many experimental studies investigate the effects of different attentional focus instructions on learning and performance in the music domain?

2. What kind of FOA instructions, outcome measures, and classifications do they use?

3. Which concrete aspects of FOA research in music should be discussed in the field in the future, and what directions of an application-oriented agenda could there be in music performance research?

2 Methods

The present systematical analysis and its methods are strongly oriented to the PRISMA statement (Moher et al., 2009) and the updated guidelines for reporting a systematic review (Page et al., 2021). The abstract was written in line with the PRISMA 2020 for Abstracts checklist (Page et al., 2021, p. 185). The application of the PRISMA guidelines in this systematic review lies in both the methodological process and the structure of illustration by continuously following the PRISMA 2020 item checklist (see Page et al., 2021, pp. 183–184). Due to the research topic and main research aims of reviewing FOA instructions and outcome measures, and not effects, the current study did not consider items from the checklist related to meta-analysis recommendations (11. Study risk of biases; 12. Effect measures; 13. Synthesis methods; 14. Reporting risk of bias; 15. Certainty assessment; 18. Risk of bias in studies; 20. Results of syntheses; 21. Reporting biases; 22. Certainty of evidence, see Page et al., 2021).

A detailed and comprehensive review protocol, which can be found in the Supplementary material (Review protocol), contains different tables of datasets to understand the review process better. However, important outcomes referring to the research questions of the current study and additional findings are implemented in the text.

2.1 General eligibility criteria

Regarding the whole study selection process recommended by Moher et al. (2009), the eligibility check of reports contains three main levels, i.e., identification of records, screening of records, and a final, full-text eligibility check that includes data collection as well as data abstraction. Beyond singular methodical steps, we defined eight eligibility criteria, which have been reviewed throughout different stages of the selection process. Included studies should meet the following criteria:

a. Be published in the English language.

b. Be published between February 1998 and March 2023 (due to the first publication of Wulf et al. (1998) presenting the FOA paradigm in movement science).

c. Be published in a peer-reviewed journal.

d. Refer to the research topic focus of attention on music in a broad sense.

e. Apply an experimental paradigm referring to Wulf et al. (1998).

f. Address the processes of learning or performing a musical skill.

g. Contain a precise description of FOA instructions.

h. Contain a precise description of outcome measures.

Considering the study selection flow, the first four criteria (a–d) were reviewed in the screening phase, whereas the latter four (e–h) were examined during the full-text eligibility check.

2.2 Information sources and search strategy

To find appropriate reports referring to FOA in music as much as possible and guarantee a high degree of transparency, we defined a search term suitable for various scientific search engines and databases. It contains keywords, Boolean operators, truncations, quotation marks, and parentheses. The search with the term (“focus of attention” OR “attentional focus” OR “external focus” OR “internal focus”) AND (music* OR music OR singer OR singing OR voice) was performed using a title/abstract filter in PubMed, SAGE journals, Taylor & Francis Online, and Web of Science. In addition, we conducted an open search with the term “focus of attention” music in Google Scholar and examined the first 300 records (as recommended by Haddaway et al., 2015, who analyzed the procedure, usefulness, and weaknesses of Google Scholar for systematic scientific literature searches in detail). Finally, we scanned the reference lists of representative articles in the field. All actions regarding the systematic search of records were performed by the first author (JH). The search was conducted on 30 March 2023.

2.3 Study selection process

After the identification of records and exclusion of duplicates, two reviewers (JH and AI) independently scanned the publishing date, publication type, journal name, record title, and abstract under consideration of the first four eligibility criteria (a–d). If the record did not meet one of the four criteria, it was rated with [EC] for exclusion; otherwise, we assigned the code [TM] for transmission to the next level. In the case of a mismatch rating (a record was rated with [TM] by the first reviewer, but the second reviewer assigned [EC] or vice versa), the records in question were looked at together again and discussed before a decision was made (see the Review protocol for review methods, code explanation, and contents of the screening categories, Supplementary material).

Records that met the criteria were transmitted to the final eligibility check, performed by two reviewers (JH and AI) together. At the beginning of this stage, we collected additional basic information about the reports (e.g., Authors, APA citation, and DOI). One report could contain two or more studies (experiments); in such cases, both studies were reviewed. There were no studies published twice, so we did not exclude double-published contributions. Subsequently, we performed a first collection of relevant data in line with the research aim of this review to check the studies under consideration of the latter four eligibility criteria (e–h) and finally, to decide on inclusion or exclusion. Therefore, we created six main data abstraction variables, which are important for the eligibility check, i.e., type of report, research approach, research design (each of these was filled with data by JH and AI, who followed a variable-specific categorization system; see Supplementary material), FOA instruction description, musical task description, outcome measure description (each of these was filled by JH and AI with either [YS] for reported or [NO] for not reported). In the next step, we checked the four criteria (e–h) for eligibility and finally decided on the inclusion [IC] or exclusion [EC] of the study. In two cases, the decision for selection was made after intensive discussion. Furthermore, the decision about the criteria being fulfilled or missing was distinct.

2.4 Data collection, data abstraction, data categorization, and frequency analysis

The included studies were reviewed in more detail with the help of a complex dataset that was divided into five major sections, namely design, participants, focus of attention, outcome measures, and results. In each of the sections, we created different variables and deductively developed various information categories and an associated code system. The dataset contains four types of variables, namely dichotomous variables (with the codes [YS] for reported or [NO] for not reported), categorization variables (either with a category system created by us or by the authors of the reviewed studies), and citation variables (with relevant original content from the reviewed studies) or quantitative variables (e.g., sample size). First, this structure provides the basis for an overview of the objective, methods, and outcomes, and second, it lets us focus on the important data to answer the research questions, i.e., (1) the precise wording of FOA instructions and (2) the type of outcome measures trying to assess the effect of those instructions. We either collected FOA instruction classifications (as assigned by the authors of the reviewed studies) or categorized instructions inductive-deductively to give an overview of the field in this regard. Furthermore, we conducted an inductive-deductive categorization of outcome measures, aiming to overview which dependent variables were used in the studies. To display the actual research situation as comprehensively as possible, we finally ran frequency analyses of the most relevant variables in relation to the research questions of this review and displayed percentage distributions accordingly.

3 Results

The systematic search identified 387 records, of which 163 were duplicates and thus sorted out. The publishing date, title of the journal, record title, and abstract of the remaining 224 records were screened by the two reviewers independently. The decision for exclusion [EC] or transmission [TM] was made with a total agreement rate of 91% (see review protocol, Tab. III, Supplementary material). After discussing critical records and a final agreement about the decision, 185 records (79%) were not transmitted to the final eligibility check. Most records (75%, n = 139) were excluded since the contribution did not refer to the research topic FOA in music at all (criteria d), and 4% (n = 7) were sorted out because the report was not published in a peer-reviewed journal (c). Five records (3%) were not published between February 1998 and March 2023 (b). The remaining 18% of records (n = 34) were not transmitted due to a failure of more than one eligibility criteria (see Figure 1 and Tab. III, Review protocol, Supplementary material). Finally, 39 records passed the screening criteria and were reviewed in detail in the next step.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study selection process.

Within the final full-text eligibility check, we reviewed 39 reports on 41 studies by proofing relevant data in the manuscripts, and we discussed important criteria to decide on study inclusion or exclusion. Finally, 14 reports (36%) with 15 studies were included in the review. One report (Mornell and Wulf, 2019) contains two experiments that should be interpreted as two studies following the Glossary of terms of the PRISMA 2020 statement (see Page et al., 2021, p. 181). Almost all reports (n = 25, 96%) were excluded due to the failing of more than one of the four eligibility criteria (e–h). For an overview of the study selection flow, see Figure 1.

After collecting and abstracting relevant data from the studies for the eligibility check, we did another data collection, abstraction, and categorization step for the 15 studies included in the review (see Review protocol, Tab. V, Supplementary material). Many variables containing citations of aims and hypotheses, participant information (e.g., sample size, participants’ instrument, and participants’ expertise), descriptions of musical tasks and material, and detailed information about the results (e.g., post-hoc results) were added to the protocol. Finally, we collected and categorized the important data to answer the review research questions, that is, the correct citations of FOA instructions within the 15 studies and all outcome measures used to measure the effects of experimentally manipulated attentional focus instructions in different experiments. Furthermore, we collected the classifications of FOA instructions by the authors of the original studies and inductive-deductively developed a code system that helps to categorize all focus instructions and assign these categories to the two main FOA classifications by Wulf et al. (1998), that is, internal and external. The coding and categorization process of dependent variables was conducted with an inductive-deductive attempt to cluster outcome measures into 10 categories (e.g., expert rating, acoustical analysis, and self-evaluation rating).

3.1 Attentional focus instruction in music

In total, we collected 53 different experimental FOA instructions. On the first abstraction level, we assigned these instructions to 9 different FOA subcategories inductive-deductively, i.e., either the category is strongly oriented to the original instruction citation or it was assigned by the authors of this review by abstracting on a broader aspect of the FOA field. Fourteen out of 15 included studies designed an experimental paradigm that contains—albeit in different ways—a comparison of different movement (or playing or teaching) instructions made by the experimenter, which refer at least to one external and one internal aspect of the required task. This fact can be seen as a minimal match across all studies. The study by Williams et al. (2023) serves as an exception due to comparing a practice program that contains external foci with a control group without any FOA instruction. However, the collection, abstraction, and categorization of instruction data show a wide variety of attentional focus instructions with reference to many different goals (see Tables 1, 2).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant information and attentional focus (FOA) instructions of included studies.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Frequencies of FOA instruction categories, experimental groups, or conditions used in included studies.

The most frequently used instructions refer to an attentional focus on the body (n = 12, 21%), a FOA on sound (n = 9, 16%), and a control condition or control group, in which generally no specific focus instructions were given (n = 9, 16%). Instructions referring to a visual focus were solely used in studies with singers and investigating FOA instructions on different singing tasks (Atkins and Duke, 2013; Atkins, 2017, 2018). The reason for this specificity lies in the behavior that singers do not have naturally to visually focus their instruments, as they are hidden inside the body, so they can adopt an attentional focus while visualizing different focal points in their environment. An instrumental focus was instructed in 4 of the 15 studies reviewed. Furthermore, there are a few experimental FOA instructions that we could not assign reasonably to one of the other 9 subcategories because they were part of a whole practice (Williams et al., 2023) or education program (Silvey and Montemayor, 2014; Montemayor et al., 2016) and their content between and within the program was very different. That is why they were subsumed in the category Other focus (see Table 2).

Those 10 subcategories were assigned to three main categories of a category system that is oriented on the original experimental paradigm of Wulf et al. (1998); see also (Wulf, 2013) consisting of FOA instructions either as internal, external, neutral, or with no specific focus. In the current review, we abstracted each of the FOA instructions used and 10 subcategories to one of these three main categories based on either a note for a link in the original manuscript of the study or due to an interpretation of the original group/condition classification in terms of Wulf’s definitions of an external focus (to focus “[…] on the intended movement effect or task goal,” Wulf and Lewthwaite, 2016, p. 1,402) and an internal focus (“[…] concentration on body movements,” Chua et al., 2021, p. 619). Within the 15 studies reviewed, 27 (51%) external foci have been used, 32% of all FOA instructions refer to internal attentional processes (n = 17), and 9 instructions had no special focus and served as control or baseline conditions (17%, see Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Frequencies of FOA instruction categories referring to the paradigm by Wulf (2013).

For internal focus instructions used by experimenters and authors of the studies, the wording appears often sharp, precise, and goal-oriented with a clear link to body parts or body motions, e.g., “focus on your fingers” (Duke et al., 2011), “focus on your right arm” (Allingham et al., 2021), “focus on the precision of their finger movements” (Mornell and Wulf, 2019), or “focus on your soft-palate” (Atkins, 2017). In the included studies, 21% of all instructions refer to the body (n = 12, see Table 2). On the other hand, there are internal FOA instructions relating to different dimensions, such as a type of auditory imagery (“[…] and development of an aural image of the music,” Silvey and Montemayor, 2014) or reference to notes or the score (“[…] and correct notes,” Mornell and Wulf, 2019). Some authors classify a focus on technical performance aspects as an internal FOA, e.g., “[…] keeping their vibrato steady and consistent” (Atkins, 2017) or “[…] and creating staccato articulation” (Lipke-Perry et al., 2022), even if the words refer to outcomes that could be interpreted by definition (see Wulf, 2013) as movement goals – that is, in reference to the abovementioned instructions, move in a certain manner to sing consistently or to create staccato.

Within the package of instruction wordings of the 15 included studies referring to external attentional foci in music (n = 27), the interpretation, abstraction, or classification is challenging, at least due to the variety of musical tasks, materials, participants’ experiences, and participants’ instruments. However, one crucial aspect is the interpretation of sound as the central goal of musical movements or musical tasks in equivalence or as a modification of the definition of external FOA in motor learning, focusing on the movement effect (see Wulf, 2013). This is supported by the percentage of instructions categorized as sound focus (n = 9, 33% of all external FOA instructions). A further subcategory of special relevance for research in singing is classified as visual focus (n = 8, 30% of external FOA instructions). An important difference compared to sound as a movement effect is the action dimension of visualizing a certain point (either near or far away) in the room (see Atkins and Duke, 2013; Atkins, 2017, 2018). Adopting a visual focus while making music is not a focus on a movement effect or movement goal; it can be seen as a supporting moderator between movement execution and sound to optimize sound. Obviously, concentrating on a visual task during a music performance is easier when musicians are not physiologically and perceptually tied because hand–eye coordination is not essential, as for singers.

Another important type of external FOA can be described as an instrumental focus in general (n = 5, 19% of external FOA instructions). Looking back at the original experimental design and experimentally manipulated instruction in the ski-simulator study by Wulf et al. (1998), an attentional instruction focusing on the musical instrument (or maybe certain aspects of voice as the pendant for singers) has the biggest theoretical overlap to the original external instructions in motor learning, focusing on not the feet but the wheels of the ski-simulator platform. Allingham et al. (2021); see also (Allingham and Wöllner, 2022) used the term somatic focus to depict the importance of physical resistance and the tactile sensory feedback while focusing on the instrument. Duke et al. (2011) and Stambaugh (2017) also used the instrumental focus on keys to find differences between concentrating on essential parts of the piano or the wind instrument and concentrating on the sounds that arise through actuating these essential parts.

3.2 Outcomes measures in FOA studies in the music domain

Across all included studies, we collected 63 descriptions of dependent variables to measure the outcomes and, thus, the effects of experimentally manipulated attentional focus instructions on the performance of a musical task. Thereby, the distribution of used outcome measures per study is very heterogeneous and reaches from 1 (Stambaugh, 2019; Temporal evenness) to 14 (Allingham et al., 2021; 3 different OM categories). A detailed collection of OM and other relevant information can be overviewed in Table 4. An inductive-deductive data categorization of all OM descriptions of the original manuscripts resulted in 9 different OM categories (see Table 5), of which expert ratings (EXR) were used most to measure FOA effects (n = 17; 27%). In total, 7 out of 15 studies used EXR. Furthermore, another 4 studies used different kinds of 14 acoustical analyses of recorded performances as dependent measures (ACU; 22%), e.g., roughness, spectral centroid, formant frequencies, or harmonic-to-noise ratio (see Table 5). Electromyography analysis (EMG) for measuring the effects of FOA on muscle activity or muscle energy—and therefore on movement efficiency—was solely conducted by two studies of the same research group (Allingham et al., 2021; Allingham and Wöllner, 2022). However, six different EMG measures were applied (10%), the same amount as for self-evaluation ratings (SER; 10%) and error detection (ERD; 10%). One study used another’s evaluation ratings (OER; 3%; Silvey and Montemayor, 2014), that is, an evaluation rating of performance conducted neither by an expert nor by oneself. Furthermore, one study measured FOA effects with the help of movement analysis (MVA) and used five different movement parameters (8% of all OM).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Musical tasks, outcome measures (OMs), and results of included studies.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Frequencies of outcome measures (OMs) categories used in included studies.

In general, the total frequency of different OM used in FOA studies in the music domain is very high considering the small number of experimental studies in the field (on average, 4.2 OM per study, sd = 3.2). One explanation of this result could lie in the explorative character of the included studies, although all studies applied an experimental design with a relatively fixed paradigm. Due to the tenuous research situation in this regard and the lack of clear results yet, a one-sided focus on one OM may not be adequate for the explorative aim of most of the research groups.

3.3 Further outcomes

In addition to the main research aim, to depict the actual research work in terms of FOA instructions and OM, a few other important outcomes emerged based on the full-text analysis and the data abstraction process. In line with the argument of the explorative character of the studies, frequency analysis showed that just 4 studies out of 15, to the best of our knowledge, formulated a clearly directed hypothesis (Mornell and Wulf, 2019, Exp1&2; Allingham et al., 2021; Allingham and Wöllner, 2022), in which the type of FOA was assumed to be superior regarding the music performance or music learning effect.

In addition to the illustration of FOA instructions, FOA sub, and main categories, Table 1 depicts the sample size, participants’ expertise (experts, novices, and both), and participants’ instrument category. Across all included studies, the sample size ranges from 7 to 52, with a total sample size of 401 and an average of 27.7 participants per study (sd = 13.8). In terms of participants’ expertise, the frequency distribution is homogenous, with six studies investigating experts, five studies that defined either two groups of experts and novices or no distinction at all (both), and four studies solely with participants and authors defined as novices or amateurs. As expertise plays a big role in the discussion of FOA effects in sports or motor learning (see Introduction and Singh and Wulf, 2020), this differentiation was discussed in many of the included studies (e.g., Stambaugh, 2019; Allingham and Wöllner, 2022; Jentzsch and Braun, 2023). Regarding the participants’ instruments category, which is strongly correlated with the type of musical task within the experimental design (see Table 4), all classical instrumental groups (wind, string, piano, and voice) are present in the included studies except for percussion instruments. However, there are two additional studies that examined music educational skills in ensemble teaching (Silvey and Montemayor, 2014; Montemayor et al., 2016) and two studies that apply the same task to various instrumental groups (Mornell and Wulf, 2019, Exp1 & 2).

Table 4 shows the research design (within-subject, between-subject, and mixed), musical tasks, the task paradigm (performance or learning paradigm) of each study, and a summarized display of post-hoc results for each OM. There are two included studies investigating FOA effects without a within-subject factor and, hence, a between-subject design containing one internal FOA group and one external FOA group (Silvey and Montemayor, 2014; Montemayor et al., 2016). In addition to that, we found three studies with a mixed design, all of them with the experimental and control conditions as within-subject factor and expertise as between-subject factor (Allingham et al., 2021; Allingham and Wöllner, 2022; Jentzsch and Braun, 2023). Musical tasks are very particular, each of them specially adjusted for the participants’ instrument, research design, and performance paradigm. The two studies that applied a traditional learning paradigm (Duke et al., 2011; Stambaugh, 2017)—i.e., acquisition/training block, retention test, and transfer test—used more controlled and internally valid experimental tasks. On the other hand, investigations of more authentic music performances designed more externally valid musical tasks (for an overview, see Table 4). Finally, a summary of post-hoc results, as described by the authors of the original manuscripts of the studies, is depicted in Table 4, if available. This style of illustration is oriented to Wulf’s (2013) review of FOA effects on motor learning, although the current review does not explicitly focus on FOA effects for the aforementioned reasons. Nevertheless, the results of the small number of included studies convey an impression of how heterogeneous the interplay of participant information, musical tasks, FOA instructions, and OM is (see Tables 1, 4). This fact confirmed the complexity and difficulty of outcome interpretation.

4 Discussion

This review aims to overview the current research situation of investigating attentional focus mechanisms in the music performance field by systematically collecting, abstracting, and categorizing relevant data referring to FOA instructions and outcome measures used in studies with experimental paradigms, in accordance with Wulf et al. (1998). In this context, we specifically asked (1) what is the current state of research, (2) what type of FOA instructions and dependent variables are used, and (3) to what extent can a future research agenda be derived from the findings. Out of 387 records initially identified, 15 studies could be included in a more in-depth investigation through several selection steps oriented on the PRISMA statement (Moher et al., 2009) and the PRISMA 2020 item checklist (Page et al., 2021). Thereby, different types of interesting additional information were collected, that is, aspects of research design, participant information, experimental tasks, and finally, attentional focus effects. Although that data collection was not the main goal of the review, it provides useful information as a basis for the following discussion and supports taking a comprehensive view of the research field and the actual discourse.

4.1 What is the goal of movement in a musical performance?

The original FOA paradigm of comparing internal and external foci using their effects on motor learning and performance is well-established in the sports domain and movement science research. Significant results of a superior external FOA can be found in studies examining various types of sports and different kinds of gross motor skills (for reviews and meta-analysis, see Wulf, 2007a; Wulf, 2013; Wulf and Lewthwaite, 2016; Chua et al., 2021). Explanations of these effects, even the specification of distinguishing between proximal and distal external foci, are mainly based on the theory that a shift of attention onto the movement effect or a task goal prevents the constrained action effect and, thus, leads to improved motor performance. With small exceptions, included studies of the current review refer to its research questions, aims (and hypotheses), research designs, experimental paradigms, as well as outcome interpretations to Wulf’s motor theory, and music-specific theoretical constructs, play a marginal role, even during outcome discussions. Overviewing the variety of external FOA instructions in the reviewed studies, the question arises, how a movement goal or movement effect is manifested in music. Many of the authors decided to operationalize the external FOA instructions with wording that targets the sound (see Table 1). The challenge with this implementation in the music domain lies in its self-evidence because no musician aims to execute musical movements or sound-producing gestures (Dahl et al., 2010; Jensenius et al., 2010) without aiming to produce sound. Transferring that problem back to the sports domain, we could compare this operationalization with the external FOA instruction to focus on playing basketball or darts (not to shoot baskets, e.g., see Zachry et al., 2005, p. 306; or not to throw bull’s eyes, e.g., see Lohse et al., 2014, p. 124). A few of the included studies from the music domain therefore added some instructions referring to sound quality (“thinking about filling the room with their sound,” Atkins, 2017, p. 425; “focus on playing for the audience and the expressive sound of the music,” Mornell and Wulf, 2019, p. 379; “focusing on creating the style of the dance,” Lipke-Perry et al., 2022, p. 425)—as it is a quality to shoot baskets in basketball.

4.2 Dimensions of musical goals and musical technique

However, these focus-instruction extensions in external FOA music instructions refer to a variety of musical performance dimensions, namely, music communication aspects (“filling the room” or “playing for the audience”), musical expression (“expressive sound”), or musical auditory/visual imagery or musical metaphors (“style of a dance”). These elements of musical performances and musical learning processes could be seen as musical goals, as it is not the sound production itself that lies in the musician’s focus of attention, but how the sound is produced, whom the sound is addressed to, and what it should express. Another methodical issue appears when combining different FOA directions in one instruction. As Wulf herself stated, “[…], we have always attempted to make external and internal focus instructions so similar that they differed in only one or two words to avoid confounds with other variables” (Wulf, 2013, p. 92). Subsequently, she mentioned that “contradictory results” (p. 92)—that is, study outcomes that showed no difference between internal and external FOA or a superiority of internal FOA—could be explained by this aspect. This suggestion of a highly controlled experimental manipulation of FOA instructions was made to optimally trace back FOA effects on motor learning effects to the fact that one word makes the difference. Across all included FOA studies of this systematic review, only two studies are close to transposing this advice (Duke et al., 2011; Stambaugh, 2019). The others more or less failed to control FOA instruction in this regard (see Table 1), at least when paying attention to the number of differently used words.

In addition, there is a big difference between changing words in FOA instructions or consciously referring the instruction to a different musical performance dimension, which took place as well while operationalizing internal FOA instructions. A few of the collected internal FOA instructions relate to technical aspects of musical learning or music performance processes (“focus on the precision of their finger movements (or lip movements for singers) and correct notes,” Mornell and Wulf, 2019, p. 379; “focusing on the fingertips and creating staccato articulation,” Lipke-Perry et al., 2022, p. 4; “focusing their attention on keeping their vibrato steady and consistent,” Atkins, 2017, p. 425). Two points should be discussed concerning aspects of musical technique. On the one hand, the first listed FOA instruction citation refers to the precision of playing (see Mornell and Wulf, 2019). When looking at the control/baseline condition of Stambaugh (2019), she used the wording to play “[…] as accurately as possible” (p. 239), which refers to the same kind of technical dimension. This wording is also used as the basic instruction for all experimental conditions in another study (see Stambaugh, 2017, p. 48). To summarize, we have the same reference of instruction used in three different studies with different functions within the experimental FOA paradigm, i.e., as a control condition, internal FOA, and general instruction underlying all conditions. Reflecting the methodical advice by Wulf (2013) in this context, contradictory results may not be surprising. On the other hand, a technical FOA instruction (“staccato articulation” or “consistent and steady vibrato”) conceptually lacks a precise assignment to one of the two traditional FOAs due to the point that it could be referred to a style of playing or a specific type of sound-production as defined as an external FOA in many studies of this review. Still, all included studies classified technical foci as internal FOA.

4.3 FOA in music and multimodal action–perception coupling processes

From a sensory perception perspective, performing music and learning to perform music are processes with many related action–perception coupling mechanisms (Jensenius, 2007; Leman and Naveda, 2010), which are linked with sensory feedback processes as auditory (e.g., Bangert and Altenmüller, 2003; Pfordresher and Chow, 2019), visual (e.g., Wöllner and Williamon, 2007; Bishop and Goebl, 2015), somatosensory (tactile or kinesthetic; e.g., Goebl and Palmer, 2008; Kuchenbuch et al., 2014), or multisensory feedback (for a recent review see Nunes-Silva et al., 2021). In general, musicians spend their whole lives practicing, concentrating consciously or unconsciously on body movements and sound, as the two are inextricably linked. Therefore, it is difficult to argue that a specific focus on one of the two aspects can succeed while not focusing on the other. Furthermore, when it comes to perception, multimodal perception processes, mainly audiovisual integration, are present in making music (see, e.g., Schutz and Lipscomb, 2007). Wulf (2007b) answered a proposal of Hegele and Erlacher (2007) to consider the perceptual dimension of FOA in motor learning, or more concretely, dimensions of the movement effect, with the following statement: The “[…] suggestion that ‘temporal’ and ‘perceptional’ dimensions of movement effects should be considered, […], is interesting. Examining those factors independently would appear to be challenging, however” (Wulf, 2007b, p. 62). She added later: “Nevertheless, examining different dimensions of movement effects would seem like a worthwhile endeavor, as it may provide more insight into the effects of attentional focus on motor control” (p. 62). In the visual perception domain, a few studies (Moore et al., 2012; Klostermann et al., 2014; Rienhoff et al., 2015) investigated the relationship between FOA effects and effects of a visual fixation duration, such as the quiet eye phenomenon (for an overview, see Vickers, 2007; Lebeau et al., 2016). Neugebauer et al. (2020) recently found a significant effect of perceptional-directed attentional foci by implementing a 2 × 2 experimental design using a dart-throwing task (visual–internal vs. visual–external vs. kinesthetic–internal vs. kinesthetic–external), that is, quiet-eye duration was increased in the visual instruction groups. On the other hand, kinesthetic instruction leads to a decrease in visual fixation duration, indicating that the perceptual dimension is highly relevant within FOA research. This outcome is also worthy of discussion because Duke et al. (2011) mentioned this topic from a methodical perspective: “It is important to note that the term focus in this research does not refer to visual focus but to focus of attention (i.e., what one is thinking about). In fact, in much of the laboratory research in this domain, participants look at a visual fixation point throughout all the experimental conditions” (p. 46). To the best of our knowledge, there is just one FOA research attempt to study the auditory perceptual dimension of movement goals in music by Cheng et al. (2011). The group experimentally manipulated the auditory feedback (normal, mute, and delayed)—that was seen by the authors as the external FOA—of a piano task performed by professional pianists. Interestingly, although Cheng et al. (2011) did not use the FOA paradigm by Wulf et al. (1998), they had no verbal FOA instructions. Instead, they apparently pre-supposed that the sound of the piano is the movement goal of the pianists, and the absence or manipulation of fingering while playing was associated with the presence or the absence of an internal focus. Nevertheless, the results show that even when the fingering was manipulated on instruction, the most important performance factor was auditory feedback. Based on this outcome, it is worthwhile to examine the relationships between auditory or audiovisual sensory feedback and FOA effects in music.

4.4 Musical performance outcomes: measuring more or less?

One of the core aims of the current review was to shed light on the type of application of outcome measures possible within FOA research in the music performance domain. We, therefore, collected and categorized all data of dependent variables used to measure FOA effects on the accomplishment of the different musical tasks. As already mentioned, the number of different kinds of outcome measures was surprisingly high in relation to the small number of studies that met the eligibility criteria of inclusion and compared to the amount of applied OM in many motor learning studies from the sports domain (see Wulf, 2013; Chua et al., 2021). It almost seems as if many studies reviewed in this article used several dependent measures to find the effects of experimentally manipulated FOA instructions. One of the reasons for that high amount of OM could lie in the dimension of the movement goal in music, the sound. Identifying sound as the external FOA means finding a solution to measure the performance effect, and this challenge was solved partially by conducting an acoustical analysis (Atkins and Duke, 2013; Atkins, 2018; Allingham et al., 2021; Allingham and Wöllner, 2022; see Table 4). Considering the amount of existing information of (post-hoc) results within the original manuscripts (see Table 4) of the studies, the question arises, how exploratory in its origin research designs of FOA studies in music are. Directed hypotheses are rare and, if available, a theoretically and/or empirically derived answer to why the experimental manipulation should affect this certain outcome measure, if at all, is slightly posed. Vice versa, the methodical sharpness of the FOA paradigm in its original implementation in motor learning research—and the pervasive number of significant results supporting the superiority of adopting an external FOA—provides a solid base for a precisely formulated, directed hypothesis. We argue that one of the key aspects of this methodical issue or challenge of analysis selection is a missing theoretical underpinning of FOA effects in music in general. If we do not know how it works, we know even less how to measure it. Another issue lies in the general challenge of measuring musical performance (see Schmidt and Lee, 2012). Expert ratings have been established through their extensive use within different music educational settings, grading in the music study context, performance evaluations in music competitions, and judgments with scores for various musical performance characteristics in auditions for positions as professional musicians. However, the high amount of different OMs used in the growing FOA music field somehow prevents comparable results. Thus, the interpretation of outcomes is challenging. Moreover, this problem is grounded on the inconsistency of applied musical tasks and the musical instruments used in the studies (see Table 4). Singers, wind instrument players, and pianists have different motor skills, different sensory awareness, gestural flexibility, and finally, a different perceptual-directed focus of attention during musical performance. There is a lack of replication studies and a lack of studies with different subjective performance ratings, such as ratings of competitors or the audience, which play a big part in the discourse of musical goals.

4.5 Limitations of the review study

The limitations of the review study are mainly due to the small number of experimental studies. In addition, the inconsistent application of analysis between those studies leads to problems in classifying the results and reported effects. Challenges to the interpretation of study results from the FOA in the music field impede formulations of credible insights. Furthermore, it is not possible to estimate the effects of internal, external, or otherwise different attentional focus instructions on musical learning, music performance, or motor learning in music based on this investigation. Another limitation lies in various steps of the PRISMA guidelines to conduct and display systematic reviews, which are in their implementation—even if the claim is to be as transparent and objective as possible—inherently still subjective. We tried to strictly follow the checklist and explain why we deviated from it in some places. Moreover, data abstraction, data categorization, and the design of the categorization systems in terms of FOA instructions and outcome measures are explorative in nature. However, the specific goal of this review lies in focusing on the methodical aspects in detail and not evaluating the results and effects of the reviewed studies. Transparent elucidations of conducting this inductive-deductive style of analysis were given so they can be discussed or criticized in the field and may serve as an impulse for further investigations in this regard. Finally, the definition of eligibility criteria in systematic reviews is often challenging as it determines the study selection process. Formulations of the eight criteria are somehow worthy of discussion, e.g., the requirement that studies must apply an experimental FOA paradigm in reference to the Wulf et al. (1998) attempt (criteria e) and contain a precise description of FOA instruction (criteria g). There are a few theoretical contributions overviewing the situation of FOA research in singing (e.g., Helding, 2015, 2016; Brand, 2021; Treinkman, 2021, 2022b), but they failed these criteria. Other studies used qualitative methods to provide important ideas on studying FOA in music (e.g., Buma et al., 2015; Guss-West and Wulf, 2016; Oudejans et al., 2017; Parsons and Simmons, 2021; Treinkman, 2022a; Lubert et al., 2023); however, they were not included due to missing criteria (e) and (g). The study by Cheng et al. (2011); published in conference proceedings and a few unpublished dissertations (e.g., Atkins, 2013; Mentzel, 2016; Williams, 2019; Allingham, 2022) approaching aspects of FOA in music were excluded—besides other reasons—because they failed criteria (c), i.e., reports were not published in a peer-reviewed journal or we could not finally be sure of it. With the exception of the study by Mentzel (2016), the mentioned unpublished dissertations described the same studies that were included in the review. Our decision to apply somehow strict criteria in this regard is grounded on the aim of deepening face up to two of the relevant aspects of the discussion, namely FOA instructions and outcome measures.

4.6 Implications and future studies

Almost every discussion or conclusion section of the study reports included in this review contains ideas of music pedagogical implications from their study results. In other cases, the authors have concrete suggestions of didactical implications for practicing music in general or, in more detail, for singing and playing an instrument. Considering the inconclusive results and effects of FOA instructions on music performance, we would not make concrete music pedagogical suggestions for using shifts of FOA while practicing music at this point. One of the main reasons for that is the absence of FOA studies in music with a learning paradigm in an application-oriented educational setting. Concerning on-stage musical performances in performance under pressure contexts, the research situation manifests as tighter due to a long tradition of multidisciplinary investigations in relation to anxiety and stress in high-pressure performing settings from psychology, sports, and music, e.g., the growing field of music performance anxiety research (for current reviews, see Fernholz et al., 2019; Osbourne and Kirsner, 2022; Kenny, 2023). However, studies showed that dealing with music-specific high-pressure challenges is highly individual and diverse, and depends on multiple factors (see, e.g., Buma et al., 2015; Oudejans et al., 2017). When dealing with the wide range of practice routines at different practice stages (see Antonini Philippe et al., 2020) and the very different pedagogical approaches of instrumental and vocal teachers, it is very difficult to assess the impact of conscious changes in attentional focus on musical learning. This is also supported by anecdotic evidence from discussions with many music students in different lectures, where practice and teaching routines, the practice stage, and the musical literature play a major role.

In relation to the last-mentioned issue and as a preliminary conclusion, future studies of FOA effects in the music domain should first go a step back. For the construction of a music-specific theoretical model, it is necessary to take the musical goals of musicians seriously. Undoubtedly, basic principles of motor learning and motor control have an important influence on how successful musicians learn and perform, but when supposing an integral understanding of musical goals, mental and physiological health and wellbeing should be noted as well. We would argue that it is necessary to conduct big qualitative and quantitative questionnaire surveys to collect as much as possible of existing FOA routines and pedagogical instructions in various music-relevant learning and performance situations of musicians with different goals. Subsequently, it could be interesting to do some in-depth investigations of singular practicing and on-stage performances, e.g., by applying video-stimulated-recall settings (see, e.g., Després, 2022), in which the relation between conscious or involuntary shifts of attentional foci, musical material, and sound characteristics could be analyzed solidly. Maybe a strict inductive strategy of data analysis will generate clusters, categories, or structures that somehow confirm the reasonableness of a transfer of the original binary FOA paradigm by Wulf et al. (1998) from sports to music—maybe not, however, because dimensions of musical performances could be more complex.

5 Conclusion

To conclude the current review, we start with a reference to the three main research questions. Out of 387 records, a small number, 15 studies, were included in the study, which, by definition of certain eligibility criteria, applied a FOA instruction paradigm in an experimental design. FOA instructions could be abstracted into 10 different subcategories, e.g., sound focus, visual focus, bodily focus, or instrumental focus, which again could be assigned to the two classifications of internal and external FOAs. We classified 63 outcome measures into 9 outcome measure categories, e.g., acoustical analysis and expert rating. Future scientific discourse in the field should focus on exploring musical goals as one of the critical aspects when comparing FOA effects in sports with those that could be expected in music. We could show that the current research situation is lacking in various points that must be considered before generalizing insights and offering music pedagogical implications. Finally, it seems to be promising to pursue a new application-oriented attempt at exploratory research on attentional focus routines and FOA shifts of musicians in different practice stages to get a solid database of music-specific attentional foci.

Data availability statement

The datasets for this study can be found in the Supplementary material (see Review protocol).

Author contributions

JH: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AI: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Charlotte Eckler for proofreading the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1290596/full#supplementary-material

References

Allingham, E. (2022). Preparing to perform: Focus of attention and slow practice in the preparation for instrumental music performance. Unpublished Dissertation. Hamburg, Germany: University of Hamburg.

Google Scholar

*Allingham, E., Burger, B., and Wöllner, C. (2021). Motor performance in violin bowing: effects of attentional focus on acoustical, physiological and physical parameters of a sound-producing action. J. New Music Res., 50, 428–446, doi: 10.1080/09298215.2021.1978506

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Allingham, E., and Wöllner, C. (2022). Effects of attentional focus on motor performance and physiology in a slow-motion violin Bow-control task: evidence for the constrained action hypothesis in bowed string technique. J. Res. Music. Educ., 70, 168–189, doi: 10.1177/00224294211034735

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonini Philippe, R., Kosirnik, C., Vuichoud, N., Clark, T., Williamon, A., and McPherson, G. E. (2020). Conservatory musicians’ temporal organization and self-regulation processes in preparing for a music exam. Front. Psychol. 11:89. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkins, R. L. (2013). Focus of attention affects singer’s tone production. Unpublished Dissertation. Austin, USA: University of Texas.

Google Scholar

*Atkins, R. L., and Duke, R. A. (2013). Changes in tone production as a function of focus of attention in untrained singers. Int. J. Choral Sing., 4, 28–36.

Google Scholar

*Atkins, R. L. (2017). Effects of focus of attention on tone production in trained singers. J. Res. Music. Educ., 64, 421–434, doi: 10.1177/0022429416673842

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Atkins, R. L. (2018). Focus of attention in singing: expert listeners’ descriptions of change in trained singers’ tone quality. Int. J. Choral Sing., 6, 3–24.

Google Scholar

Bangert, M., and Altenmüller, E. O. (2003). Mapping perception to action in piano practice: A longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neurosci. 4:26. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-4-26

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 610–620. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.610

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beilock, S. L., and Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 701–725. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.701

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, J. J., and Hardy, J. (2009). Effects of attentional focus on skilled performance in golf. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 21, 163–177. doi: 10.1080/10413200902795323

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergee, M. J. (1992). A scale assessing music student teachers’ rehearsal effectiveness. J. Res. Music. Educ. 40, 5–13. doi: 10.2307/3345770

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon.

Google Scholar

Bishop, L., and Goebl, W. (2015). When they listen and when they watch: pianists’ use of nonverbal audio and visual cues during duet performance. Music. Sci. 19, 84–110. doi: 10.1177/1029864915570355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bliss, C. B. (1895). Investigations in reaction time and attention. Stud. Yale Psychol. Lab. 1, 1–55,

Google Scholar

Brand, S. (2021). Attentional focus effects and singing: enhancing vocal performance through body movements and gestures as external foci of attention. Int. J. Arts Educ. 16, 1–12. doi: 10.18848/2326-9944/CGP/v16i02/1-12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brophy, T. S. (Ed.) (2019). The Oxford handbook of assessment policy and practice in music education. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Buchanan, J. J., Park, I., Chen, J., Mehta, R. K., McCulloch, A., Rhee, J., et al. (2018). Expert monitoring and verbal feedback as sources of performance pressure. Acta Psychol. 186, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.04.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buma, L. A., Bakker, F. C., and Oudejans, R. R. D. (2015). Exploring the thoughts and focus of attention of elite musicians under pressure. Psychol. Music 43, 459–472. doi: 10.1177/0305735613517285

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, H. C., and Collins, D. (2016). The fourth dimension: A motoric perspective on the anxiety–performance relationship. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 9, 1–21. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2015.1072231

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, F. P-H ., Heiß, P., Großbach, M., and Altenmüller, E. (2011). Attentional foci in piano performance. Proceedings of International Symposium on Performance Science (ISPS), Toronto, Canada, 1–6.

Google Scholar

Chua, L. K., Jimenez-Diaz, J., Lewthwaite, R., Kim, T., and Wulf, G. (2021). Superiority of external attentional focus for motor performance and learning: systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 147, 618–645. doi: 10.1037/bul0000335

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahl, S., Bevilacqua, F., Bresin, R., Clayton, M., Leante, L., Poggi, I., et al. (2010). “Gestures in performance” in Musical gestures. Sound, movement and meaning. eds. R. I. Godøy and M. Leman (New York: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Després, J. P. (2022). First-person, video-stimulated recall method for studying musical improvisation strategies. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 44, 34–51. doi: 10.1177/1321103X20974803

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Duke, R. A., Cash, C. D., and Allen, S. E. (2011). Focus of attention affects performance of motor skills in music. J. Res. Music. Educ., 59, 44–55, doi: 10.1177/0022429410396093

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrlenspiel, F., and Maurer, H. (2007). Aufmerksamkeitslenkung beim sportmotorischen Lernen: Ein Überblicksartikel zwischen Empirie. Theorie und Perspektiven. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie 14, 114–122. doi: 10.1026/1612-5010.14.3.114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W. (1979). Anxiety, learning, and memory: A reconceptualization. J. Res. Pers. 13, 363–385. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(79)90001-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., and Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cognit. Emot. 6, 409–434. doi: 10.1080/02699939208409696

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernholz, I., Mumm, J., Plag, J., Noeres, K., Rotter, G., Willich, S., et al. (2019). Performance anxiety in professional musicians: A systematic review on prevalence, risk factors and clinical treatment effects. Psychol. Med. 49, 2287–2306. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001910

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitts, P. M., and Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Monterey, CA: Brooks.

Google Scholar

Furuya, S., Ishimaru, R., and Nagata, N. (2021). Factors of choking under pressure in musicians. PLoS One 16:e244082. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244082

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geringer, J. M., and Madsen, C. K. (1996). Focus of attention to elements: listening patterns of musicians and nonmusicians. Bull. Counc. Res. Music. Educ. 127, 80–87,

Google Scholar

Goebl, W., and Palmer, C. (2008). Tactile feedback and timing accuracy in piano performance. Exp. Brain Res. 186, 471–479. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1252-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, R., Allsop, J., and Williams, S. E. (2013). Changes in putting kinematics associated with choking and excelling under pressure. Int. J. Sport. Psychol. 44, 387–407. doi: 10.7352/I]SP2013.44.387

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guss-West, C., and Wulf, G. (2016). Attentional focus in classical ballet. A survey of professional dancers. J. Dance Med. Sci. 20, 23–29. doi: 10.12678/1089-313X.20.1.23

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haddaway, N. R., Collins, M. A., Coughlin, D., and Kirk, S. (2015). The role of Google scholar in evidence reviews and its applicability to grey literature searching. PLoS One 10:e0138237. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138237

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hegele, M., and Erlacher, D. (2007). Focusing along multiple dimensions: spatial, temporal, and modal aspects of distality. E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 21–22,

Google Scholar

Helding, L. (2015). Motor learning and voice training: locus of attention. J. Sing. 72, 87–91,

Google Scholar

Helding, L. (2016). Motor learning and voice training, part II. Locus of attention: internal or external? That is the question. J. Sing. 72, 621–627,

Google Scholar

Herrebrøden, H. (2023). Motor performers need task-relevant information: proposing an alternative mechanism for the attentional focus effect. J. Mot. Behav. 55, 125–134. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2022.2122920

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, vol. 2. New York: Holt.

Google Scholar

Jensenius, A. R. (2007). Action-sound: Developing methods and tools to study music-related body movement. Unpublished dissertation. Norway: University of Oslo.

Google Scholar

Jensenius, R. A., Wanderley, M. M., Godøy, R. I., and Leman, M. (2010). “Musical gestures: concepts and methods in research” in Musical gestures. Sound, movement and meaning. eds. R. I. Godøy and M. Leman (New York: Routledge).

Google Scholar

*Jentzsch, I., and Braun, Y. (2023). Effects of attention focus instructions on amateur piano performance. Psychol. Music, 51, 579–591, doi: 10.1177/03057356221101431

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T. (2023). The Kenny music performance anxiety inventory (K-MPAI): scale construction, cross-cultural validation, theoretical underpinnings, and diagnostic and therapeutic utility. Front. Psychol. 14:1143359. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1143359

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klostermann, A., Kredel, R., and Hossner, E. J. (2014). On the interaction of attentional focus and gaze: the quiet eye inhibits focus-related performance decrements. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 36, 392–400. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2013-0273

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuchenbuch, A., Paraskevopoulos, E., Herholz, S. C., and Pantev, C. (2014). Audio-tactile integration and the influence of musical training. PLoS One 9:e85743. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085743

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, E. J., and Imber, L. G. (1979). When practice makes imperfect: debilitating effects of overlearning. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 2014–2024. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.11.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, G. R., Gottwald, V. M., Hardy, J., and Khan, M. A. (2011). Internal and external focus of attention in a novice form sport. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 82, 431–441. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2011.10599775

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebeau, J. C., Liu, S., Saenz-Moncaleano, C., Sanduvete-Chaves, S., Chacon-Moscoso, S., Becker, B. J., et al. (2016). Quiet eye and performance in sport: a meta-analysis. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 38, 441–457. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0123

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, T. R., and Grafton, S. T. (2015). Out of control: diminished prefrontal activity coincides with impaired motor performance due to choking under pressure. NeuroImage 105, 145–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leman, M., and Naveda, L. (2010). Basic gestures as spatiotemporal reference frames for repetitive dance/music patterns in Samba and Charleston. Music. Percept. 28, 71–91. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.71

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lipke-Perry, T., Levy, M., and Dutto, D. J. (2022). Probing focus of attention: multiple case-study analysis of pianists’ pedaling under different foci conditions in performance of Bartók’s Romanian folk dance Sz. 56, no. 2. Music Sci., 5, 1–14, doi: 10.1177/20592043221123225

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohse, K. R., Sherwood, D. E., and Healy, A. F. (2014). On the advantage of an external focus of attention: A benefit to learning or performance? Hum. Mov. Sci. 33, 120–134. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2013.07.022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubert, V. J., Chełkowska-Zacharewicz, M. E., and Gröpel, P. (2023). “My mind varied its focus quite a bit”: A thematic analysis of the attentional focus of aspiring professional violinists and violists during performance [manuscript submitted for publication]. Department of Psychology, University of Vienna.

Google Scholar

Madsen, C. K. (1997). Emotional response to music. Psychomusicology: A journal of research in music. Cognition 16, 59–67. doi: 10.1037/h0094067

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Madsen, C. K., and Geringer, J. M. (1990). Differential patterns of music listening: focus of attention of musicians versus nonmusicians. Bull. Counc. Res. Music. Educ. 105, 45–47.

Google Scholar

Martens, R., and Landers, D. M. (1972). Evaluation potential as a determinant of coaction effects. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 8, 347–359. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(72)90024-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masters, R. S. W. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: the role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. Br. J. Psychol. 83, 343–358. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02446.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, H., and Zentgraf, K. (2007). On the how and why of the external focus advantage. E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 31–32.

Google Scholar

McKay, B., and Wulf, G. (2012). A distal external focus enhances novice dart throwing performance. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 10, 149–156. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2012.682356

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McKay, B., Wulf, G., Lewthwaite, R., and Nordin, A. (2015). The self: your own worst enemy? A test of the self-invoking trigger hypothesis. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 68, 1910–1919. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.997765

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McNevin, N. H., Wulf, G., and Carlson, C. (2000). Effects of attentional focus, self-control, and dyad training on motor learning: implications for physical rehabilitation. Phys. Ther. 80, 373–385. doi: 10.1093/ptj/80.4.373

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McPherson, G. E., and Thompson, W. F. (1998). Assessing music performance: issues and influences. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 10, 12–24. doi: 10.1177/1321103X9801000102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meinel, K. (1960). Bewegungslehre: Versuch einer Theorie der sportlichen Bewegung unter pädagogischem Aspekt. Berlin: Volk und Wissen.

Google Scholar

Mentzel, M. (2016). The effect of attentional focus on singing voice quality: Towards the interdisciplinary experimental investigation of singing pedagogy. Unpublished Dissertation. College Park: University of Maryland.

Google Scholar

Miles, R. (Ed.) (1997–1998). Teaching music through performance in band (Vols. 1–2). Chicago, IL: GIA.

Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G.and the PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and Meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 151:264. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Montemayor, M., Silvey, B. A., Adams, A. L., and Witt, K. L. (2016). Effects of internal and external focus of attention during novices’ instructional preparation on subsequent rehearsal behaviors. J. Res. Mus. Educ., 63, 455–468. doi: 10.1177/0022429415612201

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, L. J., Vine, S. J., Cooke, A., Ring, C., and Wilson, M. R. (2012). Quiet eye training expedites motor learning and aids performance under heightened anxiety: the roles of response programming and external attention. Psychophysiology 49, 1005–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01379.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mornell, A. (2007). Opening musicians’ ears to attentional focus. E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 35–36.

Google Scholar

*Mornell, A., and Wulf, G. (2019). Adopting an external focus of attention enhances musical performance. J. Res. Music. Educ., 66, 375–391. doi: 10.1177/0022429418801573

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullen, R. (2007). Attentional focus and motor learning? Answers and open questions. E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 39–41.

Google Scholar

Mullen, R., and Hardy, L. (2000). State anxiety and motor performance: testing the conscious processing hypothesis. J. Sports Sci. 18, 785–799. doi: 10.1080/026404100419847

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, N. P., and Janelle, C. M. (2003). Anxiety and performance: A visual search examination of the processing efficiency theory. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 25, 171–187. doi: 10.1123/jsep.25.2.171

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neugebauer, J., Baker, J., and Schorer, J. (2020). Looking to Learn Better–Training of Perception-Specific Focus of Attention Influences Quiet Eye Duration but Not Throwing Accuracy in Darts. Front. Sports and Active Living 2:79. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2020.00079

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunes-Silva, M., Janzen, T. B., Rodrigues, R. G., and Da Luz, A. R. (2021). Sensory feedback in music performer–instrument interactions. Psychol. Music 49, 1285–1302. doi: 10.1177/0305735620928397

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osbourne, M. S., and Kirsner, J. (2022). “Music performance anxiety” in The Oxford handbook of music performance. ed. G. E. McPherson (New York: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Oudejans, R. R. D., Koedijker, J. M., and Beek, P. J. (2007). An outside view on Wulf’s external focus: three recommendations. E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 41–42.

Google Scholar

Oudejans, R. R. D., Spitse, A., Kralt, E., and Bakker, F. C. (2017). Exploring the thoughts and attentional focus of music students under pressure. Psychol. Music 45, 216–230. doi: 10.1177/0305735616656790

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. A., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 134, 178–189. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, J. E., and Simmons, A. L. (2021). Focus of attention verbalizations in beginning band: a multiple case study. J. Res. Music. Educ. 69, 152–166. doi: 10.1177/0022429420973638

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peh, S. Y.-C., Chow, J. Y., and Davids, K. (2011). Focus of attention and its impact on movement behaviour. J. Sci. Med. Sport 14, 70–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2010.07.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfordresher, P. Q., and Chow, K. (2019). A cost of musical training? Sensorimotor flexibility in musical sequence learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26, 967–973. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1535-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poolton, J. M., Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W., and Van der Kamp, J. (2007). Moving with an external focus: automatic or simply less demanding? E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 43–44.

Google Scholar

Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. Euro. J. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 129–154. doi: 10.1080/713752551

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raab, M. (2007). On the value of the attentional focus concept: elaborate and specify! E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 45–46,

Google Scholar

Reeves, J. L., Tenenbaum, G., and Lidor, R. (2007). Choking in front of the goal: the effects of self-consciousness training. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 5, 240–254. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2007.9671834

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rienhoff, R., Fischer, L., Strauss, B., Baker, J., and Schorer, J. (2015). Focus of attention influences quiet-eye behavior: an exploratory investigation of different skill levels in female basketball players. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 4, 62–74. doi: 10.1037/spy0000031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saikley, A., and Haroush, K. (2021). Toward a neurobiological model of human performance under pressure. PNAS 118:e2113777118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2113777118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, T. (1993). The assessment of music performance: techniques for classroom and rehearsal. MENC Special Research Interest Group in Measurement and Evaluation Newsletter, 15, 7–11.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A., and Lee, T. D. (2012). “Principles of practice for the development of skilled actions: implications for training and instruction in music” in Art in motion II – Motor skills, motivation, and musical practice. ed. A. Mornell (Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Peter Lang).

Google Scholar

Schutz, M., and Lipscomb, S. (2007). Hearing gestures, seeing music: vision influences perceived tone duration. Perception 36, 888–897. doi: 10.1068/p5635

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shea, C. H., and Wulf, G. (1999). Enhancing motor learning through external-focus instructions and feedback. Hum. Mov. Sci. 18, 553–571. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(99)00031-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, H., and Wulf, G. (2020). The distance effect and level of expertise: is the optimal external focus different for low-skilled and high-skilled performers? Hum. Mov. Sci. 73:102663. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2020.102663

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Silvey, B. A., and Montemayor, M. (2014). Effects of internal and external focus of attention on novices’ rehearsal evaluations. J. Res. Music. Educ., 62, 161–174. doi: 10.1177/0022429414530434

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, N. C., Bellamy, M., Collins, D. J., and Newell, D. (2001). A test of processing efficiency theory in a team sport context. J. Sports Sci. 19, 321–332. doi: 10.1080/02640410152006090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Springer, D. G., and Silvey, B. A. (2022). Effects of focus of attention instructions on listeners’ evaluations of solo instrumental performance. Int. J. Music. Educ. 40, 205–216. doi: 10.1177/02557614211033312

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Stambaugh, L. A. (2017). Effects of internal and external focus of attention on woodwind performance. Psychomusicol. Music Mind Brain, 27, 45–53. doi: 10.1037/pmu0000170

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

*Stambaugh, L. A. (2019). Effects of focus of attention on performance by second year band students. J. Res. Music. Educ., 67, 233–246. doi: 10.1177/0022429419835841

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, S., and Williamon, A. (2003). Evaluating evaluation: musical performance assessment as a research tool. Music. Percept. 21, 21–41. doi: 10.1525/mp.2003.21.1.21

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Treinkman, M. (2021). Focus of attention research: A review and update for teachers of singing. J. Sing. 77, 407–418.

Google Scholar

Treinkman, M. (2022a). Focus of attention in voice training. J. Voice 36:733, doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2020.08.035

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Treinkman, M. (2022b). Focus of attention in voice training and performance: applications to the voice studio. J. Sing. 79, 21–28. doi: 10.53830/IVWF2556

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vickers, J. N. (2007). Perception, cognition and decision training: The quiet eye in action. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Wan, C., and Huon, G. F. (2005). Performance degradation under pressure in music: an examination of attentional processes. Psychol. Music 33, 155–172. doi: 10.1177/0305735605050649

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wesolowski, B. C., and Wind, S. A. (2019a). Investigating rater accuracy in the context of secondary-level solo instrumental music performance. Music. Sci. 23, 157–176. doi: 10.1177/1029864917713805

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wesolowski, B. C., and Wind, S. A. (2019b). “Validity, reliability, and fairness in music testing” in The Oxford handbook of assessment policy and practice in music education. ed. T. S. Brophy (New York: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Williams, S. G. (2019). Finding focus: Using external focus of attention for practicing and performing music. Unpublished Dissertation. Netherlands: Leiden University.

Google Scholar

*Williams, S. G., van Ketel, J. E., and Schaefer, R. S. (2023). Practicing musical intention: the effects of external focus of attention on musicians’ skill acquisition. Music Sci., 6, 11512–11514. doi: 10.1177/20592043231151416

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M., Smith, N. C., and Holmes, P. S. (2007). The role of effort in influencing the effect of anxiety on performance: testing the conflicting predictions of processing efficiency theory and the conscious processing hypothesis. Br. J. Psychol. 98, 411–428. doi: 10.1348/000712606X133047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychol. Bull. 76, 92–104. doi: 10.1037/h0031332

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wöllner, C., and Williamon, A. (2007). An exploratory study of the role of performance feedback and musical imagery in piano playing. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 29, 39–54. doi: 10.1177/1321103X07087567

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G. (2007a). Attentional focus and moto learning: A review of 10 years of research. E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 4–14.

Google Scholar

Wulf, G. (2007b). Methods, findings, explanations, and future directions: response to commentaries on attentional focus and motor learning. E-J. Bewegung und Training 1, 57–64.

Google Scholar

Wulf, G. (2013). Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 6, 77–104. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2012.723728

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., Höß, M., and Prinz, W. (1998). Instructions for motor learning: differential effects of internal versus external focus of attention. J. Mot. Behav. 30, 169–179. doi: 10.1080/00222899809601334

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., and Lewthwaite, R. (2010). “Effortless motor learning?: an external focus of attention enhances movement effectiveness and Efficiancy” in Effortless attention. A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action. ed. B. Bruya (Cambridge: MIT Press).

Google Scholar

Wulf, G., and Lewthwaite, R. (2016). Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 1382–1414. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0999-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., McNevin, N. H., Fuchs, T., Ritter, F., and Toole, T. (2000). Attentional focus in complex skill learning. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 71, 229–239. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2000.10608903

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., McNevin, N. H., and Shea, C. H. (2001a). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 54, 1143–1154. doi: 10.1080/713756012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., and Mornell, A. (2008). Insights about practice from the perspective of motor learning: a review. Music Perform. Res. 2, 1–25.

Google Scholar

Wulf, G., Shea, C. H., and Park, J. H. (2001b). Attention and motor learning: preferences for and advantages of an external focus. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 72, 335–344. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2001.10608970

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zachry, T., Wulf, G., Mercer, J., and Bezodis, N. (2005). Increased movement accuracy and reduced EMG activity as the result of adopting an external focus of attention. Brain Res. Bull. 67, 304–309. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

* References of reports included in the review are marked with an asterisk.

Google Scholar

Keywords: attentional focus, motor learning, musical performance, focus instruction, outcome measures, systematic review

Citation: Hohagen J and Immerz A (2024) Focus of attention in musical learning and music performance: a systematic review and discussion of focus instructions and outcome measures. Front. Psychol. 15:1290596. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1290596

Received: 07 September 2023; Accepted: 26 February 2024;
Published: 08 April 2024.

Edited by:

Liliana Silva Araújo, McGill University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Claudia Bullerjahn, Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Germany
Eckart Altenmüller, Hanover University of Music Drama and Media, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Hohagen and Immerz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jesper Hohagen, amVzcGVyLmhvaGFnZW5AdW5pa2xpbmlrLWZyZWlidXJnLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.