Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 05 April 2024
Sec. Gender, Sex and Sexualities

“What do they mean?” a systematic review on the interpretation, usage and acceptability of “they”

  • School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, United Kingdom

The rise of feminist and LGBTQIA+ movements paved the way for many equality reforms. These include language reforms, which facilitate inclusion of multiple groups in society. For example, the shift from the generic “he” to “he or she” and “they” allows for the inclusion of women, transgender, and non-binary individuals in many narratives. For this reason, many institutions worldwide encourage neutral language. It remains unclear how individuals interpret neutral language. One case of neutral language is the pronoun “they,” which has been assigned multiple definitions from the 1970s to 2022. We examine how the pronoun “they” has been interpreted, used, and accepted over time. We discuss trends in the findings and make suggestions for future research directions, including the need for better methods to investigate pronouns and clarification on what the focus of neutral language should be. This timely commentary has implications for action on equality, diversity, and inclusion.

Introduction

Language aids individuals in conceptualizing the world around them. Indeed, research demonstrates the impact of language on cognition and culture (for a review see Boroditsky, 2019). Given the close relationship between culture and language, Sniezek and Jazwinski (1986) suggest that the views of an androcentric society are demonstrated linguistically. Bodine (1975) presents this eloquently: “dictated by an androcentric world-view; linguistically, human beings were to be considered male unless proven otherwise” (p. 133), a statement that remains prevalent today (see Bailey et al., 2019). One way that language influences androcentrism is via personal pronouns, which are terms used to identify an individual. The current systematic review identifies how one solution to androcentrism in language, the pronoun “they,” has been interpreted historically, through a review of literature from as early as 1970 up until 2022. The review aims to inform subsequent research directions and future interventions to support equality, diversity, and inclusion.

Throughout history, there has been a shift in how pronouns, and in particular pronouns used generically, are understood. For example, the pronoun “he” has historically been used as a generic referent, as well as a masculine referent. The pronoun “they” has been interpreted in different ways throughout history. Changes in how pronouns are defined are related to legal and societal changes. For example, the rise of the LGBTQIA+ movement in the 2000s brought legal changes that allowed individuals to identify with their chosen gender. In Sweden, the pronoun “hen” was adopted as an entirely neutral pronoun (Gustafsson Sendén et al., 2015). In English speaking countries, a similar attempt was made with neopronouns (“ze” and “hir”) but has not progressed beyond the LGBTQIA+ communities (Crawford and Fox, 2007). A common alternative is the pronoun “they,” which is defined as a plural referent, a generic referent, a singular indefinite pronoun antecedent, and a referent for a single person who identifies as non-binary (Merriam-Webster, 2022). However, given how many definitions it holds, it is not clear how “they” is understood and interpreted today.

The generic “he”

The concept of androcentrism was first coined by Gilman (1911), who wrote that society rests on the assumption that men are the ideal human type, whereas women are an accompaniment. In an androcentric society, men are seen as “the measure of all things” (Bailey et al., 2019). These beliefs manifest in language.

Others have argued against the androcentric worldview’s impact on language, stating that intention is enough to guarantee both clarity and equity (see Martyna, 1980). Martyna questioned whether “he” could be intentionally utilized in a purely generic form. Moreover, what is the guarantee that the user of “he” means it generically without explicitly stating so? Intent, however, in the usage of “he” is not the only problem. The recipient’s understanding of “he” is key too – utilizing “he” as a generic pronoun does not mean the recipient will understand it in the intended sense and may instead interpret “he” as denoting a man (Sniezek and Jazwinski, 1986). Many (e.g., Kidd, 1971; Soto, 1976; Moulton et al., 1978; MacKay and Fulkerson, 1979; MacKay, 1980; Switzer, 1990; Miller and James, 2009; Khan and Daneman, 2011) have shown that “he” evokes masculine imagery. What this implies is that “he” seems to function as a male-specific pronoun. Therefore, it is not that “he” cannot function generically but that “he” cannot function in a purely generic form.

Movements such as feminism, LGBTQIA+, and transgender communities have argued that a generic form needs to be introduced for equal language. This argument is often misunderstood as a radical language change that would eradicate specific and generic forms of “he,” which feminist linguists noted back in the late 1900s (Martyna, 1980). However, the movements do not aim to exclude men from the narrative. The opposition to the generic “he” is based on a desire for both equal rights and equal language – in other words, to create social equality that would allow for fair communication about genders (Martyna, 1980) and individuals who do not identify with a gender (Wayne, 2005).

Is “he or she” the solution?

Because the pronoun “he” is not appropriate to be a generic referent, researchers have tended to utilize “he or she” as one of the pronoun options. The latter has been referred to as a feminization strategy that aims to produce more gender-neutral language. Research has suggested that “he or she” produces a degree of gender neutrality. For example, Hyde (1984) reported that “he or she” significantly produced the highest percentage of feminine stories (42%) compared to other pronouns (“he” and “they”). For “he or she” there was an almost equal balance of stories with masculine and feminine characters (58% of the characters in the stories for “he or she” were men). Gastil (1990) and Switzer (1990) showed similar findings, reporting that “he or she” produced a balance of feminine and masculine images. Lindqvist et al. (2019) supports such findings. In their study “he or she” did not evoke a masculine-stereotyped bias in a recruitment advert. Overall, what these findings suggest is that the combination of the pronouns “he or she” appear to function more generically than the pronoun “he.”

At the same time, the order of “he or she” is unbalanced in that “he” precedes “she.” Sexist prescriptions have existed in English to order men before women in binomial phrases to mark differences in people’s worth (Bodine, 1975). This concept dates back centuries as early philosophers claimed that linguistically, gender ordering should be according to the worthier of the pair being set first (Wilson, 1553, as cited in Bodine, 1975). Perhaps part of the reason “he or she” cannot be considered entirely gender-neutral may result from a masculine bias in research. Hegarty et al. (2016) note that research that focuses on male-first binomials identify these as gender-neutral or gender-fair language despite an influence of male dominance on order preferences. In sum, it seems that using “he or she” does not lead to inclusivity in language.

Research on the gendered binomial order demonstrates the ubiquity of a masculine bias. Kesebir (2017) explored the permanence of the “male-first pattern” in a variety of words across a range of English corpora, including “he or she.” Their results show that in texts published in the 20th Century, “he or she” is more likely to start with “he” instead of “she” (97.5%). The lack of variability (i.e., often described as “frozen order” by linguists) of “he or she” poses a problem because linguists have identified word order as relevant to social categories. For example, the more powerful element of a binomial phrase is more likely to be mentioned first (Benor and Levy, 2006). Thus, the “male-first pattern” may reflect a society where “men are the measure of all things” (Bailey et al., 2019). Nevertheless, preferences for male-first forms of several gendered binomials have weakened since the 1970s (Mollin, 2013), which coincides with the increasing support for gender equality and gender-fair language in many English-speaking countries (Hegarty et al., 2016). Nonetheless, the use of male-first linguistic forms serve to reinforce gender-stereotypes in a recurrent pattern. For example, Kesebir (2017) demonstrated that linguistic choice in gendered conjoined phrases such as “mother and father” and “businessman and businesswoman” matter because individuals assign more relevancy to the first mention rather than the second mention in a conjoined phrase. Thus, language such as “he or she” helps maintain androcentric worldviews, and as a result, maintains the oppressive social order that marginalizes non-men.

There is an additional concern with the generic “he” and “he or she”: neither “he” nor “he or she” can be genderless. With the growth of the transgender community, the gender binary of “male and female” is becoming unrepresentative because many individuals do not identify with their birth gender. Whilst much research has found that “he or she” has the potential to evoke gender-neutral imagery (e.g., Hyde, 1984; Gastil, 1990; Lindqvist et al., 2019), that imagery seems exclusive to women and men and not the genders who do not fit in the binary. In fact, in their investigation of which pronouns native English speakers use to refer to a genderless pronoun, LaScotte (2016) found that participants did not always envision someone outside of the gender binary when they used “he or she.” As a result, “he or she” reinforces the gender binary (Saguy and Williams, 2022), which researchers have suggested should be replaced with a multiple-category gender/sex system, of which the categories are not mutually exclusive (Hyde et al., 2019). To solve this problem, in terms of gender-fair language, we need to determine which alternative pronoun possesses the ability to evoke imagery beyond the gender binary.

A genderless and gender-neutral pronoun

To identify an effective alternative to the problems mentioned in this review (essentially, the generic “he” and “he or she” as masculine-biased pronouns and thus, unrepresentative of women and those who identify beyond the gender binary), we need to establish an alternative that represents everyone. The representative would need to include individuals who identify with a gender and individuals who do not identify with any gender. Therefore, the pronoun is expected to be gender-neutral and genderless simultaneously.

In 2012, Sweden introduced a third person pronoun that is exclusively genderless/gender-neutral: “hen.” “Hen” is meant to be used generically when the gender is unknown or irrelevant, or to identify a transgender or genderless individual (Gustafsson Sendén et al., 2015). Sendén et al. explored the attitudes to the introduction of “hen” from 2012 to 2015, as well as the use of the Swedish third-person pronoun. Attitudes to “hen” have changed over time, beginning with negative attitudes that became more positive as of 2015. There was a concomitant change in behavior. In 2013 and 2014, participants reported to rarely using “hen.” By 2015 the use of “hen” increased by 25%. Left-wing orientation and low sexism predicted individuals’ use of “hen” (19% of participants). Conversely, higher levels of right-wing orientation and sexism was associated with lower use of “hen” (15% of participants). Attitudes to “hen” changed faster than behavior. Making grammatical changes to a language is a difficult process (Paterson, 2014). However, “hen” demonstrates that language can change. In fact, Gustafsson Sendén et al. (2021) reported that, from 2015 to 2018, individuals have continued to perceive “hen” more positively. The introduction of “hen” and the changes in attitudes with regards to the gender-neutral/genderless Swedish pronoun give way for other languages to introduce their own gender-neutral/genderless pronoun. A similar attempt has been made in English-speaking countries with less success. Indeed, transgender communities have introduced “ze” and “hir” as genderless pronouns. However, these pronouns have not been very widespread outside LGBTQIA+ and transgender communities (Crawford and Fox, 2007).

The grammatical structure in the English language as it exists facilitates the use of a gender-neutral/genderless pronoun because it already contains a pronoun that has been interpreted as gender-neutral by researchers and even linguists: “they” (Quirk et al., 1972, 1985; Wales, 1996). As a result, gender-fair language has been encouraged by a variety of institutions. For example, the American Psychological Association (2020) recommended that authors “do not use the generic ‘he’ or ‘he or she’ to refer to a generic person; instead, authors must rewrite the sentence or use the singular ‘they’. When writing about a known individual, use that person’s identified pronouns” (paragraph 29). Additionally, in 2008, the European Parliament released guidelines that encourage the use of gender-neutral language and advise members to avoid the use of the generic “he” and “man.” The United Nations in 2018 also released guidelines on gender-fair language, advising users to only make gender visible when it is relevant for communication as well as providing inclusive examples of language, e.g., “humankind.” In countries like Australia, for example, use of non-sexist language has been encouraged for several decades (e.g., the new national anthem was changed to become more inclusive, “Australia sons” became “Australians all”; Strahan, 2008). Similarly, the Canadian’s Department of Justice encourages the use of gender-neutral language (Department of Justice Canada, 2023). In non-English speaking countries, gender-fair language has been encouraged as well, e.g., the Netherlands and Germany (Weida, 2022). Furthermore, there are many countries whose language is already genderless, such as Armenian, Persian and Swahili. Many academic institutions worldwide encourage gender-fair language. The National Centre for State Courts has released guidance on how courts can encourage gender-inclusive language (Wirkus and Zarnow, 2023). In 2022, the Supreme Court of the Philippines approved guidelines on the use of gender-fair language in the Judiciary and Gender-Fair Courtroom Etiquette, and the Courts and Tribunals Judiciary in the United Kingdom released guidelines on gender equality in 2013, encouraging users to use gender-neutral language. It is worth noting that there are countries that oppose the inclusion of gender-fair language. For example, Argentina implemented a policy in June 2022 that forbade public educational institutions from using gender-neutral language on the basis of grammatical sanctity (Lankes, 2022). In France, the French Senate passed a bill banning inclusive language (Bollinger, 2023), claiming that their masculine form acts as a neutral form. Nevertheless, these examples suggest that a wide variety of institutions are becoming increasingly aware of the importance of fair and inclusive language and are encouraging their members to follow.

When individuals use “they,” it can have positive effects. “They” produces gender-neutral/inclusive imagery in comparison to “he” and, to some extent, “he or she.” For example, Switzer (1990) presented children with a brief scenario (in which the pronoun differed for each participant) before asking multiple questions about the character. “They” aided children in producing inclusive imagery. Additionally, Conkright et al. (2000) explored the effects of pronoun type on children’s recall and interpretation of stories. In their findings, the authors showed that the pronoun “they” was evenly interpreted as either generic or as a specific gender. LaScotte (2016) investigated which pronouns native English speakers used when referring to a genderless person (i.e., “the ideal student). Most participants (79%) used a gender-inclusive approach to describe the target (i.e., using “he or she” or singular “they”), with 68% of the participants using singular “they.” What this may mean is that “they” as a generic pronoun may be useful to help individuals express less gendered ideals and help them create a more inclusive worldwide view. Indeed, utilizing the pronoun “they” as gender-neutral/genderless may be an alternative to the generic “he” and “he or she.”

Nevertheless, even if “they” appears to be the alternate pronoun to encourage genderless imagery and ideals, research has found mixed results with regard to the pronoun “they.” For example, some report that “they” produces a mix of gender-neutral imagery (Switzer, 1990; Conkright et al., 2000), whereas others report that “they” also works to produce gender specific imagery (Lindqvist et al., 2019). Martyna (1980) argued that although users intend to use the pronoun “he” generically, its use is not restricted to these uses. Similar issues arise with “they.” Bodine (1975) explained that grammarians identified the pronoun “they” as plural in the 19th Century, which means that users may use “they” as a plural referent, as well as a specific-gender singular referent, and not just a purely genderless pronoun.

Lindqvist et al. (2019) investigated the effect of three language strategies on the reduction of the masculine bias through a recruitment candidate advert. The language utilized in the advert varied according to the language strategy. In the paired strategy, the advert used “he or she.” In the traditional neutral strategy, the advert either used a gender-neutral noun (“the applicant”) or the singular gender-neutral “they.” Finally, in the new gender-neutral strategy, the non-gendered pronoun “ze” was used. Lindqvist et al. found that the wording used in the description of the candidate significantly impacted the participants’ gender associations. The gender-neutral noun “the applicant” and the singular gender-neutral pronoun “they” possessed a masculine bias. In other words, participants were more likely to choose a man for the candidate. When “he or she” or “ze” was utilized, there was no masculine bias. With regards to “ze,” familiarity with the pronoun did not significantly impact participants’ gender association of the candidate. Most participants were familiar with the singular “they” but were less familiar with the other pronouns, such as “ze.” Lindqvist et al. suggest that the singular “they” is not in the same category as the newly created gender-neutral alternatives. Instead, “they” is a familiar pronoun with a different use (i.e., singular rather than the conventional plural).

Combined, the evidence suggests two main conclusions. First, “he” does not function generically, and it is not a gender-neutral nor a genderless pronoun. Secondly, binomials (“he or she”) are a better alternative to the generic “he” as a gender-neutral pronoun but not interpreted as a genderless pronoun. As a result, this systematic review will not investigate either of these topics. We focus on understanding how people use, accept, and interpret the pronoun “they” over recent history. Thus, this review examines what “they” means, how individuals use the pronoun “they,” and the acceptance of the pronoun “they” over time. This review will examine literature from the 1970s to 2022. Based on Gustafsson Sendén et al.’s (2015) findings, which demonstrate a change over time regarding the Swedish pronoun “hen,” we hypothesize that “they” will become more gender-neutral, increase in use, and rise in acceptability over time.

Method

Search strategy

We searched two digital databases, PsychINFO and Web of Science, for empirical research that investigated the interpretation of the pronoun “they.” We chose PsychINFO because it contains information related to psychology, mental health, and the behavioral and social sciences. Web of Science allows the search to be conducted across a wide range of science, social science, and humanities databases simultaneously. In Supplementary Table S1 we show the initial search terms, their combinations, and the results from the searches.

After the initial search, we re-evaluated the terms and searched synonyms. Supplementary Table S2 displays these terms, their combinations, and the results.

Because the two main searches did not yield many results, we conducted a third search with terms taken from Ansara and Hegarty (2013). We display the results from the third search in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Final search terms, combinations, and results.

An additional digital database, Google Scholar, was used in the search for empirical research after the initial searches did not yield many results. However, this search did not result in new papers. We conducted manual searches from the reference lists of the included articles, as well as Google Scholar citation searches, which resulted in 28 papers. Studies were selected based on being published between 1970 and 2022, written in English, and investigating the pronoun “they” in the English language. A Zotero digital library was used to safekeep the studies selected. Studies were included if they included empirical research investigating participants’ interpretation of the pronoun “they” – that is, whether the pronoun “they” refers to individuals of the gender binary, beyond the gender binary or a combination. Studies that investigated other pronouns, including “they,” were included in the review. Figure 1 includes a PRISMA flow diagram of the process by which the papers were identified and then eliminated.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram for search results (Page et al., 2021).

Study sample

Table 2 displays a summary of the characteristics (e.g., the year of publication, the samples, the methodology of the empirical studies) in the review. Table 3 shows the relevant outcomes.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. A summary of the research articles’ samples and methods.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. A summary of the research articles’ relevant outcomes and ratings for the quality assessment of each research article.

Information extraction

Information such as year of publication, study samples, and methodology were extracted if included in the research paper. We extracted and then thematically analyzed findings relating to the pronoun “they.” Three categories emerged: meaning of “they,” use of “they,” and acceptance of “they.”

Search results

In total, the database search revealed 4,014 publications, of which 23 were initially selected. However, of these 23 papers, 19 were excluded. The exclusion criterion for the search was papers that were not investigating the pronoun “they” and its interpretation. Duplicate papers were merged in the original database search and not retrieved for consideration. Overall, 4,010 publications were removed entirely. Further manual searches were conducted from the reference lists of the included articles, as well as Google Scholar citation searches. Twenty-eight papers emerged from these searches, resulting in 32 papers in total reviewed by two independent reviewers (see Figure 1 for PRISMA Flow Chart).

Quality check

The 32 articles identified in the search results were assessed using the Quantitative Assessment Tool by Kmet et al. (2004). The tool’s criteria contains 14 items (see Table 4), which are scored on the basis of the extent to which the criteria was met: “Yes,” “Partial,” “No” and “Not Applicable.” For each research paper, a summary score was calculated using the assessment tool guidelines (see Table 5). Two reviewers independently performed quality assessments, and disagreements were solved by consensus. A score of 75% or over indicated strong quality, a score between 55 and 75% indicated moderate quality and a score of 55% or less indicated weak quality (from Van Cutsem et al., 2017).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Criteria for assessing the quality of quantitative studies and overall results (from Kmet et al., 2004).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Quality assessment of research articles included in Systematic Review (from Kmet et al., 2004).

Results and discussion

Overview

This study examined how the pronoun “they” is interpreted. We hypothesized that there would be a change of interpretation, use, and acceptability of “they” over time in which “they” would become more inclusive over the years. Through the examination of papers and analysis, we examined the meaning of “they,” the use of “they,” and the acceptance of “they.”

Figure 1 illustrates how we selected the studies. Thirty-two studies were included after full-text review: 21 on the meaning of “they,” nine on the use of “they” and six on the acceptance of “they.” Year of publication ranged from 1978 to 2022, with two studies conducted in the 1970s, three in the 1980s, five in the 1990s, three in the 2000s, six in the 2010s, and 13 studies in the 2020s. Out of the 32 studies, five had a child sample (3 to 12 years of age) and the remaining 27 were conducted with an adult sample. Twenty-one of the studies took place in the United States, whilst five were set in the United Kingdom, two in Australia, Finland, and Sweden, and one each in China, Hong Kong, and Canada. Two studies were corpus based, one of which extracted papers from multiple countries worldwide (see Table 4). One of the studies did not report which country they took place in. We present a summary of the studies’ year of publications, samples, method, and relevant outcomes in Tables 2, 3.

The meaning of “they”

Because this review investigates the interpretation of the pronoun “they” across time, the results are presented in chronological order to present a timeline of how the pronoun “they” has changed over the years. Two early studies (Moulton et al., 1978; Hyde, 1984) report the pronoun “they” to be more masculine-biased, with fewer than 50% of participants referring to women in their story-creation tasks. Thus, early work suggests that “they” was masculine biased initially. This masculine bias seems to change in the mid-1980s. In 1985, the pronoun “they” was reported to function in a non-masculine biased manner, with 68% of choices in the character identification task being feminine (Fisk, 1985). This finding continues through 1989 to 1997, in which studies report that the pronoun “they” generates more inclusive imagery (Khosroshahi, 1989; Kroepelin, 1989; Gastil, 1990; Switzer, 1990; Prentice, 1994; Bennett-Kastor, 1996; Prögler-Rössler, 1997).

From the year 2000 onwards, we see a variety of results. For example, girls appear to use the pronoun “they” more generically than boys do (Conkright et al., 2000), which may be an effect of a continued prevalent androcentric worldview (see Bailey et al., 2019) where a masculine bias is still observed in language. Thus, girls use the pronoun “they” more generically than boys because it represents them too. The pronoun “they” also has a plural meaning as documented by Lee (2007). “They” also seems to have a “gender not relevant” meaning (Strahan, 2008, p. 28). This means that if the gender of the referent is not relevant or necessary to the conversation, the pronoun “they” was used. The pronoun “they” is a more inclusive pronoun than the pronoun “he” because the findings show more feminine connotations than “he” (Noll et al., 2018), and even acts as a gender-neutral pronoun (LaScotte, 2016), which is seen in 2019 as well (Bradley et al., 2019). However, in 2019, the pronoun “they” continues to possess a masculine bias (Lindqvist et al., 2019). In a study in 2020, “they” has a feminine association (von der Malsburg et al., 2020). In fact, data demonstrates that individuals use a range of pronouns (i.e., “he,” “she,” “they,” or “he or she”) to refer to a singular, non-gender antecedent (LaScotte, 2021).

In the 2020s, “they” is also used to refer to individuals who are non-binary who personally use the pronouns “they/them” (Arnold et al., 2021). Thus, in the 2020s, there are different interpretations of the singular pronoun “they.” Indeed, “they” is used as a non-binary personal pronoun, as a universal gender-neutral pronoun, and as an indefinite pronoun (Saguy and Williams, 2022).

Overall, there appears to be a linear progression in interpretation. In sum, the pronoun “they” was interpreted with a masculine bias in the 1980s and this linguistically reflected the views of an androcentric society (Sniezek and Jazwinski, 1986), as “human beings were to be considered male unless proven otherwise” (Bodine, 1975, p. 133). However, the second wave of feminism between the 1960s and 1970s (Pauwels, 2003) drew attention to gender biases in language as women felt excluded because of specific linguistic choices such as the use of the pronoun “he” as a generic pronoun (i.e., the pronoun “he” could refer to men and women). This shift in Western society may explain the move from the pronoun “they” possessing a masculine bias in the late 1970s and early 1980s to becoming more inclusive in the 1990s. However, this definition of inclusivity is restricted to the gender binary. It seems that in some of the aforementioned studies (e.g., Moulton et al., 1978; Hyde, 1984; Fisk, 1985; Khosroshahi, 1989; Kroepelin, 1989; Gastil, 1990; Switzer, 1990; Prentice, 1994), “they” possessing feminine connotations suggested it was an inclusive pronoun, but this does not represent genders other than women and men.

With the rise of the LGBTQIA+ movement in the 2000s, we witness the pronoun “they” shifting into a more neutral and genderless territory. The movement has impacted many legislative efforts to recognize gender identity all over the world. For example, the introduction of the 2004 Gender Recognition Act in the United Kingdom, which allows transgender people to identify with their chosen gender fully and legally and acquire a new birth certificate. In Canada, there was an adjustment of the Canadian Human Rights Act and the Criminal Code in 2017. Australia has passed multiple legislations since 2014 to recognize transgender rights, whilst Sweden has had legislations in place since 1972 and continues to re-evaluate their laws up until the current day. China and Hong Kong, on the other hand, have laws in place that recognize gender identity, however, only after post-sexual reassignment surgery. Similarly, in Finland, a legal gender reassignment on official documents is only recognized if the person is sterilized or for other reasons infertile. In the United States, as of February 2021, the Equality Act, which prohibits discrimination on the basis of sex, sexual orientation, and gender identity, was passed in the House of Representatives, and is awaiting consideration in the Senate. Laws regarding transgender rights differ from state to state, with many states in 2022 passing anti-LGBTQIA+ bills (Lavietes and Ramos, 2022). Nevertheless, many institutions such as the European Parliament (2008), the United Nations (2018), and the American Psychological Association (2020) encourage non-sexist and non-gendered language in their rules in recognition of fluid gender identity. Simultaneous with increased legal protection, a shift in interpretation is observed up until recent years, where the data reflects more fluid interpretations of the pronoun “they.” For example, “they” has masculine and feminine associations, as well as gender-neutral and non-binary interpretations. Although transgender rights differ worldwide, changes have occurred; research suggests that these changes are reflected in the pronoun “they” (Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Timeline to show the meaning of the pronoun “they” through the years.

The use of “they” in spoken and written English

If the pronoun “they” is interpreted as more generic through time, then it is expected to become more common in spoken and written English language. The studies examined in this systematic review investigated whether the pronoun “they” was used in speech or written language, how commonly it was used in comparison to other pronouns, and how the pronoun “they” can be used (as gender-neutral, masculine, feminine or non-binary). Martyna (1978) was the first to report findings on the use of the pronoun “they.” The pronoun “they” was more commonly used in spoken responses than in written responses possibly because “they” is easier to say than “he or she” and individuals are more used to “they” in colloquial language. In the late 1990s, Bennett-Kastor (1996) reported children frequently chose the pronoun “they” (13.1%) more when writing a story than the pronouns “he” (11.9%) and “she” (5.6%). In addition, Prögler-Rössler (1997) recorded a written usage of 59% of the pronoun “they” by their student population compared to “s/he” (7%), “she” (9%), “he” (12%) and other (13%). More recently, there have been more reports on the usage of the pronoun “they.” For example, in written responses, the pronoun “they” was used 20.6% of the time (Ahokas, 2020) and in speech, the pronoun “they” was the most common choice. Hekanaho (2020) found singular “they” was the most commonly used (80%) pronoun in written responses. Stormbom (2020) conducted a search of gendered language in open research articles and reported that singular “they” was the most commonly used pronoun (46.8%). Similarly, Yakut et al. (2021) found that the usage of the pronoun “they” as a non-binary singular pronoun considerably increased from the year 2010 to the year 2019 in Social Science academic journals. In Chinese University students’ English writing, Zhang and Yang (2021) found that singular “they” accounted for 15% of the total pronoun usage in comparison to gendered pronouns.

Overall, with time, the pronoun “they” became a common choice of pronoun, even in countries where the participants’ first language was not English (e.g., Ahokas, 2020; Hekanaho, 2020; Zhang and Yang, 2021). This increase in usage fits with historical context. With the rise of the LGBTQIA+ movement, and in particular, individuals in the movement who do not identify with their assigned sex or any specific gender (i.e., non-binary individuals, transgender individuals, and others), linguistic choices that refer to identity are changing. For example, non-binary people may adopt the pronoun “they” as their referent of choice. The history of the interpretation of the pronoun “they” suggests it has become more fluid and its meaning open to interpretation. For example, early research (Quirk et al., 1972, 1985; Wales, 1996) reported that the pronoun “they” was already interpreted and used as gender-neutral by researchers and even linguists. Moreover, more institutions are encouraging the use of non-sexist language. For example, the change in the lyrics of the Australian National anthem previously outlined, demonstrates a move away from masculine oriented language (“Australia sons”) to more gender neutral and inclusive language (“Australians all”; Strahan, 2008) (Figure 3).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Timeline to show the use of the pronoun “they” through the years.

The acceptance of “they”

Given the increased familiarity of the pronoun “they” over time, we next turn to an examination of the acceptance of the pronoun “they.” More specifically, research has examined whether this pronoun has been accepted as a generic pronoun or a plural pronoun, and whether language structure impacts the rates of acceptance. Garnham et al. (2012) discovered that the pronoun “they” received positive judgment (i.e., participants approved of the sentence construction) when constructed with a plural word (“singers”) and binary genders (“men” and “women”). Doherty and Conklin (2017) reported that their participants rated the pronoun “they” as less natural than pronouns that matched the gender of the referent in the task but more natural than pronouns that did not match the gender of the referent shown. Konnelly and Cowper (2020) found participants objected to the use of singular “they” for social reasons – in particular, to defend the sanctity of grammar. Similarly, Bradley (2020) found that prescriptivist attitudes contributed to the rejection of certain uses of singular “they.” Moulton et al. (2020) reported that their participants thought sentences with “they” were just as acceptable as sentences with singular gendered pronouns (i.e., participants rated sentences for naturalness on a seven-point scale), despite whether the gender of the referent was known or unknown. Hekanaho (2020) reported that singular “they” was the most commonly accepted generic pronoun (94%), as well as the most accepted non-binary pronoun (67%). When looking at gender differences, 97% of transgender participants accepted non-binary pronouns. Meanwhile, 61% of cisgender male participants accepted “they” as a non-binary pronoun and 73% of cisgender female participants accepted “they” as a non-binary pronoun.

The results reflect a shift in acceptance, although it appears that there is still a debate of prescriptivist bias in terms of acceptance. Bodine (1975) explained that from the 19th century onwards, prescriptive grammarians identified the pronoun “they” as plural only, and it appears this interpretation is still rooted in the English language today. However, the data suggests that the pronoun “they” is still accepted as a generic and as a non-binary pronoun, although its meaning and interpretation has varied in recent years. It may be that new alternative pronouns (neopronouns), such as “ey,” “em,” “xe,” and “ze,” which have no historical attachments to them, are easier to perceive and be accepted as non-binary and neutral. Lindqvist et al. (2019), who reported that the pronoun “they” possessed a masculine bias, also examined the neopronoun “ze,” and concluded that this pronoun could eliminate a masculine bias. In contrast, the pronoun “they” is not in the same category as neopronouns because of its familiar nature. When “they” is compared to gendered pronouns such as “he or she” it may appear to be more inclusive, but when compared to neopronouns, it possesses a masculine bias, whereas the neopronouns are perceived as neutral (Figure 4).

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Timeline to show the acceptance of the pronoun “they” through the years.

Overall, the findings support all three hypotheses: that “they” would become more gender-neutral in meaning, as well as increase in use and in acceptability over time. The hypothesized change is observed as the pronoun “they” is shown to be fluid and flexible, as well as a common choice of pronoun. The pronoun “they” may be gendered, neutral and genderless. Despite the prescriptivist bias debate, the pronoun “they” is still declared singular and plural. In essence, the pronoun “they” has become inclusive for all members of society and adaptable as needed.

At the same time, “they” might continue to evolve and follow the trajectory of “you,” a generic for second person singular and plural. Speakers often use additional morphology, such as “y’all” to differentiate when “you” refers to more than one individual as opposed to an individual. Over time, we may begin to hear phrases such as “they all” to differentiate the third person singular and plural. Future research will be able to detail this trajectory.

Limitations

There are limitations in the current review that should be noted. Our review is based on articles that we were able to access. Although we contacted 10 researchers for unpublished research, we did not receive replies. In addition, our systematic review is confined to the articles we found in the databases we searched.

Future directions

There are other future directions that should be addressed based on gaps in the research base. For example, research that required participants to describe a character often asked the binary (i.e., woman or man) gender of the character (Hyde, 1984; Fisk, 1985; Gastil, 1990; Bennett-Kastor, 1996) or for participants to name the character (Moulton et al., 1978; Khosroshahi, 1989; Switzer, 1990), which were then labeled per the binary gender. This method is restrictive because it does not allow for a gender outside of the binary to be considered. This limitation may have resulted because of the year the research articles were published in. In addition, it may have occurred because the focus of some of the previous studies was to understand whether the pronoun “he” was generic or possessed a masculine bias. Nevertheless, when assessing whether pronouns elicit gendered meanings, all categories of gender need to be considered, and the choices given to the participants must not be restricted to the binary. Two exceptions are Bradley et al. (2019) and Lindqvist et al. (2019), which included photographs of individuals who were non-binary (judged by reliability rating) and in the gender binary.

The population of each study must be considered as well, and future studies must consider recruiting varied samples. Hyde (1984), Fisk (1985), Switzer (1990), Bennett-Kastor (1996), and Conkright et al. (2000) were the only researchers to explore pronouns with a child sample (3–12 years-old). This omission suggests children are underrepresented in the topic of pronouns and how these are interpreted, and it cannot be established how children understand the pronoun “they.” The available literature suggests that “they” possesses masculine connotations (Hyde, 1984; Switzer, 1990) and at the same time, functions in a non-masculine biased way (Fisk, 1985), such as holding a gender-neutral association (Bennett-Kastor, 1996), when compared to “he or she” alternatives (Switzer, 1990); girls perceive the pronoun “they” as more generic than boys (Conkright et al., 2000). There is not sufficient research on the pronoun “they” with children to suggest what their interpretation of the pronoun “they” is and how children obtained this understanding. Of the remaining studies that conducted empirical research, their samples were all adults. Thus, the conclusions that emerged from the review can be applied to adults. However, the majority of adult samples were university students, and whilst an accessible sample, it may be considered a biased sample due to demand characteristics or their educational level. Future research should expand on the kind of adult sample recruited so that samples vary in age, levels of education, ethnicity, sexuality, and gender identity to obtain a representative idea of how multiple adult samples interpret the pronoun “they.”

Conclusion

Overall, what the data suggests is that there has been a shift in terms of interpretation, usage, and acceptance of the pronoun “they.” However, given the gaps in the literature (e.g., age of sample), further research is necessary. First, future research needs to investigate neopronouns and how to integrate neopronouns in mainstream society, including their interpretation, acceptance, usage, etc. As mentioned, Sweden introduced a third person pronoun, exclusively genderless/generic: “hen.” There was an increase in positive attitudes between 2012 to 2018 (Gustafsson Sendén et al., 2015, 2021). Thus, whilst language reforms are difficult to achieve (Paterson, 2014), it is not impossible, and further research is required into neopronouns to assess whether a similar reform can be achieved in the English language.

Second, research should continue to investigate the pronoun “they” with multiple population types (i.e., children and adults) to decipher how it is interpreted (i.e., a masculine biased pronoun, a feminine biased pronoun, a generic pronoun, a non-binary pronoun, a plural pronoun). There are many benefits that come with understanding how the pronoun “they” is interpreted, and in turn, how the pronoun can be integrated into multiple aspects of society. For example, research needs to investigate if “they” can be integrated into education and weaken related gender-stereotypes. Future research could focus on the impact the pronoun “they” may have on gender-stereotypes as well as the impact the pronoun “they” has on prejudice. For example, integrating the use of “they” in everyday language may reduce levels of sexism or racism. Language is a powerful tool, and as the linguistic relativity hypothesis suggests, the languages we speak influence the way we think about the world (Whorf, 1956). This influence of language on thought has been demonstrated in various domains, including grammatical gender (e.g., Bassetti, 2007; Sato and Athanasopoulos, 2018). Thus, further investigation of the pronoun “they” is crucial. Whilst a language reform may not solve these societal impacts of bias, it may contribute to creating greater inclusivity and equality for all.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

MBDC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization. HT: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing, Visualization. AG: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing, Visualization.

Funding

The University of Surrey’s Doctoral College for awarding a PhD bursary to MBDC to enable this research, and the University of Surrey’s Open Research Team for funding publication.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Peter Hegarty for his contribution to the initial design and concept of the research project. Thanks must also go to Margarida Pedrosa and Emily Clark for their help in the quality check of the articles. We would also like to acknowledge the University of Surrey’s Doctoral College for awarding a PhD bursary to MBDC to enable this research, and the University of Surrey’s Open Research Team for funding publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1253356/full#supplementary-material

References

Ahokas, O. (2020). He or she or they: generic pronoun use among Finnish and Swedish L2 speakers of English. Bachelor’s thesis, Stockholm University. Diva Portal. Available at: https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/record.jsf?pid=diva2%3A1388806&dswid=5369

Google Scholar

American Psychological Association (2020) Gender. Available at: https://apastyle.apa.org/style-grammar-guidelines/bias-free-language/gender

Google Scholar

Ansara, Y. G., and Hegarty, P. (2013). Misgendering in English language contexts: applying non-cisgenderist methods to feminist research. Int. J. Multiple Res. Approaches 7, 160–177. doi: 10.5172/mra.2013.7.2.160

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, J. E., Mayo, H. C., and Dong, L. (2021). My pronouns are they/them: talking about pronouns changes how pronouns are understood. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 28, 1688–1697. doi: 10.3758/s13423-021-01905-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, A. H., LaFrance, M., and Dovidio, J. F. (2019). Is man the measure of all things? A social cognitive account of Androcentrism. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 23, 307–331. doi: 10.1177/1088868318782848

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassetti, B. (2007). Bilingualism and thought: grammatical gender and concepts of objects in Italian–German bilingual children. Int. J. Biling. 11, 251–273. doi: 10.1177/13670069070110030101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennett-Kastor, T. L. (1996). Anaphora, nonanaphora, and the generic use of pronouns by children. Am. Speech 71, 285–301. doi: 10.2307/455551

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benor, S., and Levy, R. (2006). The chicken or the egg? A probabilistic analysis of English binomials. Language 82, 233–278. doi: 10.1353/lan.2006.0077

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodine, A. (1975). Androcentrism in prescriptive grammar: singular ‘they’, sex-indefinite ‘he’, and ‘he or she’. Lang. Soc. 4, 129–146. doi: 10.1017/S0047404500004607

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bollinger, A. (2023), French president Emmanuel Macron supports ban on gender-inclusive language. Available at: https://www.lgbtqnation.com/2023/11/french-president-emmanuel-macron-supports-ban-on-gender-inclusive-language/

Google Scholar

Boroditsky, L. (2019). Language and the brain. Science 366:13. doi: 10.1126/science.aaz6490

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, E. D. (2020). The influence of linguistic and social attitudes on grammaticality judgments of singular ‘they’. Lang. Sci. 78:101272. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2020.101272

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, E. D., Salkind, J., Moore, A., and Teitsort, S. (2019). Singular 'they' and novel pronouns: gender-neutral, nonbinary, or both? Proceed. Linguist. Soc. Am. 36, 1–7. doi: 10.3765/plsa.v4i1.4542

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conkright, L., Flannagan, D., and Dykes, J. (2000). Effects of pronoun type and gender role consistency on children’s recall and interpretation of stories. Sex Roles J. Res. 43, 481–497. doi: 10.1023/A:1007167432657

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, M., and Fox, A. (2007). IX. From sex to gender and back again: co-optation of a feminist language reform. Fem. Psychol. 17, 481–486. doi: 10.1177/0959353507084333

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Department of Justice Canada. (2023) Gender-neutral Language. Government of Canada. Available at: https://www.justice.gc.ca/eng/rp-pr/csj-sjc/legis-redact/legistics/p1p15.html

Google Scholar

Doherty, A., and Conklin, K. (2017). How gender-expectancy affects the processing of “them”. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 718–735. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.11545

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

European Parliament (2008). Gender-neutral language in the European parliament. Available at: https://www.europarl.europa.eu/cmsdata/151780/GNL_Guidelines_EN.pdf

Google Scholar

Fisk, W. R. (1985). Responses to "neutral" pronoun presentations and the development of sex-biased responding. Dev. Psychol. 21, 481–485. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.3.481

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnham, A., Gabriel, U., Sarrasin, O., Gygax, P., and Oakhill, J. (2012). Gender representation in different languages and grammatical marking on pronouns: when beauticians, musicians, and mechanics remain men. Discourse Process. 49, 481–500. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2012.688184

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gastil, J. (1990). Generic pronouns and sexist language: the oxymoronic character of masculine generics. Sex Roles J. Res. 23, 629–643. doi: 10.1007/BF00289252

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilman, P. C. (1911). The Man-Made World or Our Androcentric Culture. Johnson.

Google Scholar

Gustafsson Sendén, M., Bäck, E. A., and Lindqvist, A. (2015). Introducing a gender-neutral pronoun in a natural gender language: the influence of time on attitudes and behavior. Front. Psychol. 6:893. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00893

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafsson Sendén, M., Renström, E., and Lindqvist, A. (2021). Pronouns beyond the binary: the change of attitudes and use over time. Gend. Soc. 35, 588–615. doi: 10.1177/08912432211029226

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gygax, P., Gabriel, U., Sarrasin, O., Oakhill, J., and Garnham, A. (2008). Generically intended, but specifically interpreted: When beauticians, musicians and mechanics are all men. Language and Cognitive Processes. 23, 464–485.

Google Scholar

Hegarty, P., Mollin, S., and Foels, R. (2016). “Binomial word order and social status” in Advances in intergroup communication. eds. H. Giles and A. Maass (New York, NY: Peter Lang Publishing), 119–135.

Google Scholar

Hekanaho, L. (2020). Generic and nonbinary pronouns: usage, acceptability and attitudes. Doctoral thesis, University of Helsinki

Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S. (1984). Children’s understanding of sexist language. Dev. Psychol. 20, 697–706. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.20.4.697

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., and van Anders, S. M. (2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: five challenges to the gender binary. Am. Psychol. 74, 171–193. doi: 10.1037/amp0000307

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesebir, S. (2017). Word order denotes relevance differences: the case of conjoined phrases with lexical gender. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113, 262–279. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000094

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, M., and Daneman, M. (2011). How readers spontaneously interpret man-suffix words: evidence from eye movements. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 40, 351–366. doi: 10.1007/s10936-011-9173-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khosroshahi, F. (1989). Penguins don't care, but women do: a social identity analysis of a Whorfian problem. Lang. Soc. 18, 505–525. doi: 10.1017/S0047404500013889

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidd, V. (1971). A study of the images produced through the use of male pronouns as the generic. Moments Contemp. Rhetoric Commun. 1, 25–30.

Google Scholar

Kmet, L. M., Cook, L. S., and Lee, R. C. (2004). Standard quality assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields. Heritage Found. Med. Res. 6, 1–20. doi: 10.7939/R37M04F16

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Konnelly, L., and Cowper, E. (2020). Gender diversity and morphosyntax: an account of singular they. J. Gen. Linguist. 5:40. doi: 10.5334/gjgl.1000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroepelin, J. M. (1989). Is" he" considered a gender neutral pronoun? Proceed. Deseret Lang. Linguist. Soc. Symposium 15, 86–103.

Google Scholar

Lankes, A. (2022) One of the World’s First Bans on Gender-Neutral Language. The New York Times. Available at: https://www.nytimes.com/2022/07/20/world/americas/argentina-gender-neutral-spanish.html

Google Scholar

LaScotte, D. K. (2016). Singular they: an empirical study of generic pronoun use. Am. Speech 91, 62–80. doi: 10.1215/00031283-3509469

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

LaScotte, D. K. (2021). Nonnative English learners’ use and understanding of singular they. Lang. Aware. 30, 84–94. doi: 10.1080/09658416.2020.1869753

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavietes, M., and Ramos, E. (2022), Nearly 240 anti-LGBTQ bills filed in 2022 so far, most of them targeting trans people. NBC News. Available at: https://www.nbcnews.com/nbc-out/out-politics-and-policy/nearly-240-anti-lgbtq-bills-filed-2022-far-targeting-trans-people-rcna20418

Google Scholar

Lee, J. F. (2007). Acceptability of sexist language among young people in Hong Kong. Sex Roles 56, 285–295. doi: 10.1007/s11199-006-9170-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindqvist, A., Renström, E. A., and Gustafsson Sendén, M. (2019). Reducing a male bias in language? Establishing the efficiency of three different gender-fair language strategies. Sex Roles J. Res. 81, 109–117. doi: 10.1007/s11199-018-0974-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay, D. (1980). On the goals, principles, and procedures for prescriptive grammar. Lang. Soc. 9, 349–367. doi: 10.1017/S0047404500008253

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay, D., and Fulkerson, D. (1979). On the comprehension and production of pronouns. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 18, 661–673. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90369-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martyna, W. (1978). What does ‘he’ mean?: use of the generic masculine. J. Commun. 28, 131–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01576.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martyna, W. (1980). Beyond the "he/man" approach: the case for nonsexist language. Signs J. Women Cult. Soc. 5, 482–493. doi: 10.1086/493733

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merriam-Webster (2022). They. In Merriam-Webster.com dictionary. Available at: https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/they (Accessed August 9, 2022)

Google Scholar

Miller, M. M., and James, L. E. (2009). Is the generic pronoun he still comprehended as excluding women? Am. J. Psychol. 122, 483–496. doi: 10.2307/27784423

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mollin, M. (2013). Radical pacifism in modern America: Egalitarianism and protest. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press

Google Scholar

Moulton, K., Han, C. H., Block, T., Gendron, H., and Nederveen, S. (2020). Singular they in context. J. Gen. Linguist. 5:122. doi: 10.5334/gjgl.1012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moulton, J., Robinson, G. M., and Elias, C. (1978). Psychology in action: sex Bias in language use, "neutral" pronouns that Aren't. Am. Psychol. 33, 1032–1036. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.33.11.1032

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noll, J., Lowry, M., and Bryant, J. (2018). Changes over time in the comprehension of he and they as epicene pronouns. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 47, 1057–1068. doi: 10.1007/s10936-018-9577-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, L. (2014). British pronoun use, prescription, and processing. Palgrave Macmillan: London

Google Scholar

Pauwels, A. (2003). “Linguistic sexism and feminist linguistic activism” in The handbook of language and gender. eds. J. Holmes and M. Meverhoff (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing)

Google Scholar

Prentice, D. A. (1994). Do language reforms change our way of thinking? J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 13, 3–19. doi: 10.1177/0261927X941310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prögler-Rössler, K. (1997). Sexism in language–a study on pronoun usage in generic contexts. Vienna English Working Papers 6, 34–48.

Google Scholar

Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., and Svartvik, J. (1972). A grammar of contemporary English. Essex, England: Longman.

Google Scholar

Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., and Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. Essex, England: Longman.

Google Scholar

Saguy, A. C., and Williams, J. A. (2022). A little word that means a lot: a reassessment of singular they in a new era of gender politics. Gend. Soc. 36, 5–31. doi: 10.1177/0891243221105792

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, S., and Athanasopoulos, P. (2018). Grammatical gender affects gender perception: evidence for the structural-feedback hypothesis. Cognition 176, 220–231. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sniezek, J. A., and Jazwinski, C. H. (1986). Gender Bias in English: in search of fair language. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 16, 642–662. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1986.tb01165.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soto, D. (1976). Alternatives to using masculine pronouns when referring to the species. In Paper presentation Western speech communication association 46th annual meeting, San Francisco, United States.

Google Scholar

Stormbom, C. (2020). Gendering in open access research articles: the role of epicene pronouns. Engl. Specif. Purp. 60, 193–204. doi: 10.1016/j.esp.2020.08.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strahan, T. E. (2008). ‘They’ in Australian English: non-gender-specific or specifically non-gendered? Austr. J. Linguist. 28, 17–29. doi: 10.1080/07268600701877473

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Switzer, J. Y. (1990). The impact of generic word choices: an empirical investigation of age- and sex-related differences. Sex Roles 22, 69–82. doi: 10.1007/BF00288155

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations (2018). United Nations Gender-Inclusive Language. Available at: https://www.un.org/en/gender-inclusive-language/guidelines.shtml

Google Scholar

Van Cutsem, J., Marcora, S., De Pauw, K., Bailey, S., Meeusen, R., and Roelands, B. (2017). The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. 47, 1569–1588. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

von der Malsburg, T., Poppels, T., and Levy, R. P. (2020). Implicit gender bias in linguistic descriptions for expected events: the cases of the 2016 United States and 2017 United Kingdom elections. Psychol. Sci. 31, 115–128. doi: 10.1177/09567976198906

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wales, K. (1996). Personal pronouns in present-day English. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Wayne, L. D. (2005). Neutral pronouns: a modest proposal whose time has come. Can. Woman Stud. 24, 85–91.

Google Scholar

Weida, K. (2022), Your guide to inclusive language around the world. Medium. Available at: https://medium.com/@kazweida/your-guide-to-inclusive-language-around-the-world-22dc1b2cb221

Google Scholar

Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality: Selected writing of Benjamín Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Wilson, T. (1553). Arte of rhetorique. Gainsville: Scholars Facsimiles and Reprints, 1962.

Google Scholar

Wirkus, A., and Zarnow, Z. (2023). Gender inclusivity in the courts: How to treat everyone with fairness, dignity, and impartiality. Williamsburg, Virginia: National Center for State Courts.

Google Scholar

Yakut, I., Genç, B., and Bada, E. (2021). Epicene pronoun usage in the social sciences: the case of research articles. J. Engl. Acad. Purp. 52:101005. doi: 10.1016/j.jeap.2021.101005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., and Yang, H. (2021). Gender voices in Chinese university students’ English writing: a corpus study. Linguist. Educ. 64:100935. doi: 10.1016/j.linged.2021.10093

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gender, language, pronouns, gender-neutral language, systematic review

Citation: Batista da Costa M, Tenenbaum HR and Grandison A (2024) “What do they mean?” a systematic review on the interpretation, usage and acceptability of “they”. Front. Psychol. 15:1253356. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1253356

Received: 06 July 2023; Accepted: 16 February 2024;
Published: 05 April 2024.

Edited by:

Anelise Gregis Estivalet, Stanford University, United States

Reviewed by:

Syahdan, Lancang Kuning University, Indonesia
Gregory Robinson, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, United States
Karolina Hansen, University of Warsaw, Poland

Copyright © 2024 Batista da Costa, Tenenbaum and Grandison. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mafalda Batista da Costa, mbdc1@leicester.ac.uk

Present address: Mafalda Batista da Costa, The University of Leicester, Leicester, United Kingdom

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.