Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 March 2023
Sec. Psychopathology

Introduction of the generic sense of ability to adapt scale and validation in a sample of outpatient adults with mental health problems

  • 1Department of Psychology, Health and Technology, Faculty of Behavioural, Management and Social Sciences, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
  • 2GGNet Centre of Mental Health, Apeldoorn, Netherlands
  • 3Mindfit, Deventer, Netherlands

Introduction: The ability to adapt is a core aspect of daily human life. Recent models and theories emphasize its essential role for health and well-being. It concerns the perceived ability to readjust and actively deal with the psychosocial consequences of challenging events. While many questionnaires measure competences related to adaptability to specific conditions, a scale that measures a generic sense of the ability to adapt is lacking. The aim of the present study is to introduce the Generic Sense of Ability to Adapt Scale (GSAAS) and to examine its psychometric properties.

Methods: The article describes two sub-studies. In the first study the items of the GSAAS were generated and field-tested in a cross-sectional non-clinical sample using item analysis, exploratory factor analysis and Rasch analysis.

Results: This resulted in a 10-item questionnaire measuring a single dimension with good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.87). In the second study the 10-item scale was validated using a cross-sectional sample of 496 outpatient adults with mental health problems. Confirmatory factor analysis confirmed the unidimensional structure of the GSAAS and the absence of measurement variance across gender, age and education. Reliability was high (α = 0.89) and moderate to strong correlations between the GSAAS and concurrent validation measures confirmed its convergent validity. Regarding incremental validity, the GSAAS accounted for 7.4% additional explained variance in symptomatic distress above and beyond sense of coherence.

Discussion: In conclusion, the GSAAS appears to be a reliable and valid instrument to assess people’s generic sense of the ability to adapt. It is a practical and quick tool that can be used to measure a vital aspect of health in research and clinical treatment settings.

Introduction

The ability to adapt is a core aspect of daily human life. People need to adapt to both daily hassles and major life-events in private or working conditions including accidents, chronic illnesses, and trauma in order to obtain or retain quality of life (Taylor, 1983; Lu, 1991; Helgeson, 1999; Livneh, 2001; Perrig-Chiello et al., 2015). However, a generic comprehensive measure of perceived ability to adapt is not yet available. To address this, the current study presents the development and psychometric evaluation of the Generic Sense of Ability to Adapt Scale (GSAAS).

Over the years, various theories and models of adaptation have been developed such as Taylor’s cognitive adaptation theory and Roy’s adaptation model. The cognitive adaptation theory defines adaptation as an adjustment process that involves finding ways to give meaning to a threatening event, regaining a sense of mastery, and enhancing and restoring self-esteem (Taylor, 1983). Roy’s adaptation model (Roy, 2009), widely used in nursing practice, considers the individual as a biopsychosocial being that is continuously interacting with and adapting to its environment. The model discerns different adaptive modes that are both physical (physiologic needs) and psychosocial (self-concept, role function, and interdependence) (Nayback, 2009). In order to stay healthy, a person needs to be able to integrate internal stimuli (one’s thoughts and feelings) and external stimuli (the environment) aiming for the ultimate goal of achieving dignity and integrity (Nayback, 2009). Based on the analysis of 89 articles, Londono and McMillan (2015) described psychosocial adaptation as a continuous process driven by interactions with one’s self (e.g., adopting spiritual beliefs) and one’s environment (e.g., social support).

Adaptation theories have in common that adaptation serves the aim to maintain health and well-being as a dynamic equilibrium (Headey and Wearing, 1991; Antonovsky, 1996; Suh et al., 1996; Cummins, 2010). Dodge et al. (2012) define well-being as the balance point between the challenges that a person is facing and its resources. In order to maintain an acceptable level of well-being, individuals should regulate their emotions and modify their actions and beliefs (Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010; Audulv et al., 2016). Kloep et al. (2009, p. 337) describe the adaptation process as follows: “Each time an individual meets a challenge, the system of challenges and resources comes into a state of imbalance, as the individual is forced to adapt his or her resources to meet this particular challenge.”

Based on these theories of adaptation, well-being, and dynamic equilibrium, Bohlmeijer and Westerhof (2020, 2021) developed a model of sustainable mental health, in which the ability to adapt has a central place. The model asserts that a person will continuously experience minor or major life-events challenging one’s ability to adapt. Successful adaptation will result in the absence of severe distress and the presence of mental well-being (Gross and John, 2003; Aldao et al., 2010). The ability to adapt is positively influenced by personal and contextual resources such as optimism (Schiavon, et al.,2017) and high-quality relationships (Amati et al., 2018; Van Loan and Garwood, 2020) and negatively influenced by personal and contextual barriers such as rumination (Willis and Burnett, 2016), dysfunctional emotion-regulation (Rice and Hoffman, 2014; Perry et al., 2020), and non-supportive contexts (Holeva et al., 2001).

In a similar way, Huber et al. (2011) proposed the ability to adapt as a core aspect of health. They argued that the current definition of the World Health Organization (WHO) of health as “a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” underestimates the ability of individuals with chronic illness or psychopathology to adapt to the challenges of life while maintaining life satisfaction. Rather (chronic) illness, psychopathology, and setbacks in life can be considered as challenges that disturb one’s equilibrium and require adaptation. Huber et al. (2016) developed the concept of positive health and defined it as “the ability to adapt and self-manage in the face of social, physical, and emotional challenges.” Recently, a Dutch Delphi study resulted in consensus on a similar definition of mental health as “the way you relate to yourself (personal component) and to others (social component) and how you deal with the challenges in everyday life (functional component) taking into account how you and others in society experience this (perceptive component)” (Van Bon-Martens et al., 2022).

Given its vital role in recent models of (mental) health and well-being, developing a reliable and valid measure of the ability to adapt is an important aim (Jambroes et al., 2016). Prinsen and Terwee (2019) recently tried to develop an outcome instrument to measure the domains and aspects of Huber’s concept of positive health. However, a content validity study to evaluate a preliminary 46-item questionnaire showed major concerns about its relevance, comprehensiveness, and comprehensibility. Further refinement of the conceptualization of positive health and the ability to adapt is thus warranted. In the past, various reliable and valid measures have been developed to measure specific skills and competences that promote the ability to adapt such as psychological flexibility (Brown and Ryan, 2003; McCracken and Yang, 2006; Vowles et al., 2008; Wicksell et al., 2010; McCracken et al., 2015; Yu et al., 2016, 2017; Terhorst et al., 2020), adaptive coping (Chesney et al., 2006; Greenaway et al., 2015; González-López et al., 2022), and optimism and cognitive styles (Conversano et al., 2010; Schiavon et al., 2017). However, a reliable and valid generic comprehensive measure of perceived ability to adapt more focused on the process than on skills, is still lacking. Therefore, we aimed to develop an instrument that captures a person’s trust to successfully cope with the psychosocial consequences of smaller or larger challenges in life.

Based on the described theoretical frameworks, it is relevant to arrive at a definition of the sense of ability to adapt, which will be the starting point for the development of a generic instrument. The focus on self-management as a skill to deal with chronic diseases is still too limited. The promotion of health can be seen as a continuous process of adaptation to (negative and positive) complexities in life, both in the environment and in the human being himself. In conclusion, we defined a generic sense of the ability to adapt as the perceived ability to readjust and actively deal with the psychosocial consequences of challenging events.

The importance of the ability to adapt for health has been the subject of research for some time. Antonovsky (1979) introduced the sense of coherence (SOC) as a potential protective factor in dealing with stress and staying healthy. SOC is a relatively stable general orientation to life and includes the ability to assess stressful life events as understandable, meaningful, and manageable (Antonovsky, 1993). An existing instrument that most closely matches the concept of the sense of the ability to adapt is the SOC-13 (Antonovsky, 1987). The external validity of the SOC with various indicators of health has been extensively studied (Morita et al., 2014; Länsimies et al., 2017). However, the items of the SOC-13 emphasize powerlessness and environmental influence. Additionally, Eriksson and colleagues indicate that focusing on health and on people’s resources is more than the measurement of the sense of coherence (Lindström and Eriksson, 2010; Eriksson and Mittelmark, 2017). With the development of the current new instrument, the emphasis on decisiveness and the process of dealing with circumstances is pursued.

The aim of the present study is twofold and elaborated in two sub-studies. The first objective, accommodated in study 1, is to introduce a new reliable and valid instrument measuring people’s generic sense of ability to adapt (GSAAS). In this first study, the items of the GSAAS were generated and field-tested in a non-clinical sample, striving to develop an instrument in which items deal with one’s trust in successful coping with (psychosocial consequences of) challenges and in shaping life, and not so much with the resources or outcome of adaptation. We expected the generic instrument to be unidimensional in nature.

The second objective, accommodated in study 2, is to validate the GSAAS in a sample of outpatient adults with mental health problems. In this second study, further validation research was carried out with the GSAAS, including confirming its factor structure, measurement invariance, reliability, and convergent and incremental validity. Besides confirming its unidimensional structure, the potential role of major characteristics of respondents as age, sex, and education was explored with data-driven multiple group measurement invariance testing. Regarding construct validity, we expect a strong association with emotional and psychological well-being. Psychological well-being is the ability to direct one’s life a meaningful way (Ryff, 1989). As this is dependent on the ability to cope with minor and major life challenges, a strong association between the two constructs is expected. In a similar vein, the absence of a sense of ability to adapt will most likely lead to an increase of feelings of powerlessness and meaninglessness and an increase of worrying. Therefore, we expect strong associations between the GSAAS and indicators of depression (Cuijpers et al., 2021) and anxiety (Roos et al., 2021) as well. Based on the earlier discussed similarities between the GSAAS and SOC-13, we also expect strong associations between these two measures. On substantive grounds, it was expected that an instrument that emphasizes the process, decisiveness, and the way of dealing with positive and negative circumstances would add unique additional variance in symptomatic distress above and beyond the sense of coherence.

1. Study 1: Introduction of the generic sense of ability to adapt scale

1.1. Materials and methods

1.1.1. Phase 1: Content validity: Item generation and pre-testing

For the initial item pool generation of the intended GSAAS, we used a theoretical approach based on the aforementioned theories and models of adaptation. Study of literature about adaptation skills yielded sources for developing an item pool to ensure good content validity (Vaillant, 2000; Fredrickson, 2001; Bjorklund, 2015). Potential items for the GSAAS were produced in an iterative process by two of the authors (EB and PtK) and four psychology students. The aim was to develop an instrument measuring generic adaptation skills applicable to both the general population and to somatic and psychiatric patient groups in line with the new model of sustainable mental health, which integrates positive psychology into clinical models of mental health and endorses the importance of adaptation processes in regulating one’s well-being (Bohlmeijer and Westerhof, 2020, 2021). For this purpose, as mentioned before, we defined a generic sense of the ability to adapt as the perceived ability to readjust and actively deal with the psychosocial consequences of challenging events.

Taking this aim into account and striving for clearly formulated questions, several rounds of review took place. All 23 potential items were positively formulated and a 5-point Likert-type rating scale (not at all, hardly ever, seldom, mostly, always) was used because of its accuracy and reliability and to generate sufficient response variance (Joshi et al., 2015). To ensure the usability, readability, and understandability, the draft questions were cognitively pre-tested among five participants who were asked to verbalize their thoughts while answering the items. Think-aloud methods are a robust and flexible technique and are a proven way to optimize item-sets (Cotton and Gresty, 2006; Schulte-Mecklenbeck et al., 2011). Based on the think-aloud pre-tests, four items were removed due to insufficient clarity for the participants.

1.1.2. Phase 2: Field-testing: Factor structure and item reduction

1.1.2.1. Participants

After the pre-testing, the 19 remaining items were field-tested for item quality evaluation and item reduction purposes in an online survey study among a convenience sample of 250 participants, consisting mostly of students, aged 18–75 years (M = 24.7, SD = 10.7). Most respondents were female, highly educated, living with friends, parents, or a partner and one-fifth had a paid job. Two-third were Dutch and one third had the German nationality (see Table 1). All participants provided complete and valid responses.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Major characteristics of respondents.

1.1.2.2. Procedure

The survey study was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Twente (reference number: 15092). Data collection was performed in the first quarter of 2015 by a group of second-year psychology students. Participants were recruited on a voluntary basis through Sona (the university’s system for obtaining mandatory research credits for first-and second-year psychology and communication students) and the students’ personal networks. Data were collected in the Dutch language.

1.1.2.3. Data analysis

Item analysis and selection followed an iterative process. First, items were screened for skewness and removed when skewness exceeded the range of ±1. Next, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was performed with the remaining items using geomin (oblique) rotation in Mplus 7.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998). Given the ordinal nature of the items, robust maximum likelihood estimation was used. The number of factors underlying the items was determined using both the screen test (Cattell, 1966) and parallel analysis (Horn, 1965) with 50 random data sets. After this EFA, a Rasch analysis was done using the partial credit model in Winsteps 3.65 (Linacre, 1991) to further examine the unidimensionality of the items and their fit to the Rasch model (Bond et al., 2007). Items with an infit and outfit means square statistic between 0.7 and 1.3 were considered to show sufficient fit (Wright, 1994). A second Rasch analysis was performed using only those items demonstrating adequate fit. Finally, based on the content of remaining items, a last selection was made. A new Rasch analysis was done and a final set of items was adopted, which was further administered and examined in study 2.

At different stages, internal consistency of the item set (Cronbach’s alpha) was computed by using the IBM Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 27. 95% confidence intervals (Cis) for Cronbach’s alpha estimates were calculated by bootstrapping 1,000 times. Guidelines by Kline (2000) were applied: values of Cronbach’s alpha below 0.70 were interpreted as low and unacceptable, an alpha between 0.70 and 0.79 fair, an alpha between 0.80 and 0.89 good and an alpha above 0.90 excellent.

1.2. Results

Three of the 19 items demonstrated skewness values greater than 1. EFA with the 16 remaining items resulted in three factors with eigenvalues >1. However, the scree plot indicated a clear single-factor solution (see Figure 1). The eigenvalue of the first factor was 6.819 whereas it was 1.303 and 1.168 for the second and third factor, respectively. The parallel test confirmed this single-factor solution by showing that only the first factor in the real data had an eigenvalue higher than its counterpart average eigenvalue from the random data (average random eigenvalue = 1.461). All items loaded strongly on this first factor, with factor loadings ranging from 0.465 to 0.711. Internal consistency of the 16 items was excellent with Cronbach’s α = 0.907 (95% CI: 0.890–0.929).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scree plot of observed eigenvalues and average eigenvalues from parallel analysis (50 random datasets).

To further examine the fit of the items to a unidimensional model, a Rasch analysis followed the EFA. Two items showed a poor fit to the Rasch model, with, respectively, outfit mean square statistic of 1.37 and 1.48, and being removed. A 14-item scale remained that showed no remaining misfitting items in subsequent Rasch analysis (infit mean square statistics: M = 1.00, range 0.82–1.28; outfit mean square statistics: M = 1.00, range 0.82–1.22).

Based on the content of these 14 items, a final selection was made on substantive considerations. To guarantee optimal applicability, also by people in non-clinical populations, we decided to remove the four items focusing on the skill of self-management. These four items specifically addressed dealing with diseases, whereas the other items are more focused on the process of dealing with all kinds of complexities in life. Given that psychosocial adaptation was defined as a process by which a person interacts with the psychosocial consequences of smaller or larger challenges in life (e.g., daily hassles, chronic and/or severe physical diseases, life-events such as divorce, accidents, and job-losses) and goes beyond coping with illness, we decided to not include them in the final instrument.

A new Rasch analysis was done with the 10 remaining items, which showed no substantial consequence of the removal of these items for the measurement range and precision of the scale. There was only marginal misfit for one item (infit mean square statistic = 1.31), which also had a lower factor loading (0.487) in the initial factor analysis. A final Rasch analysis showed that removing or retaining this item would not have consequences for either measurement range or measurement precision (see Table 2). Based on its content relevance (“I have influence over my personal circumstances”), this item was retained in the scale. The final 10 items (see Table 3) demonstrated a reasonable spread of item difficulty levels across the scale. Internal consistency of these 10 items was good: Cronbach’s α = 0.872 (95% CI: 0.847–0.897).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Rasch analysis results of the final 10 items of the GSAAS.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Completely standardized factor loadings for the 10 item one-factor solution of the GSAAS.

The final set of 10 items was further investigated in study 2 to test the reliability and validity of the GSAAS in a clinical population.

2. Study 2: The validation of the GSAAS in a clinical population

2.1. Materials and methods

2.1.1. Participants

The psychometric validation involved 496 Dutch adults (62.1% females) with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) classification (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), aged 17–75 (M = 36.7; SD = 14.37), and with educational levels ranging between low (12.3%), intermediate (50%), and high (37.7%) (see Table 1). All participants completed questionnaires (see 2.1.3.) prior to their intake and were referred by their general practitioner, based on its estimated mild-to-moderate DSM-5 classification, to receive outpatient treatment in primary mental health care at MindFit Mental Health Centre. In comparison with normative OQ-45 scores from outpatients from three Dutch public mental health care organizations (De Jong et al., 2007), current Symptomatic Distress scale scores (M = 46.7; SD = 20) and Social Role scale scores (M = 12.5; SD = 5) were similar. The total Psychopathology score was considerably lower in the current population (M = 50.5; SD = 20), primarily due to a lower average Interpersonal Relations scale score (M = 10.5; SD = 5). Each registered patient at MindFit during July 2018 and March 2019 was asked to participate on a voluntary basis in this second study and no incentives were given.

2.1.2. Procedure

Study 2 was evaluated by The Medical Ethics Review Committee (METC) from Twente, which declared that the current study was not subject to ethical review under the Law on Research Involving Human Subjects (file number: K18-19). MindFit’s Routine Outcome Monitoring (ROM) system was used to digitally collect data. As part of a larger cross-sectional study on sustainable mental health, participants filled out seven different self-report measures before their intake interview of which we used four in the present study. Participants provided their informed consent for the use of data for scientific purposes. Participants were included from 18 years, on the terms that they had a valid email-address, access to internet and a proficiency in Dutch.

2.1.3. Measures

2.1.3.1. Ability To adapt

The Generic Sense of Ability to Adapt Scale (GSAAS) is the instrument under evaluation in this study. It is a 10-item scale measuring the extent to which an individual feels to be able to readjust and actively deal with the psychosocial consequences of personally challenging events (see Table 3). All items were scored on a 5-point Likert scale, which ranged from not at all (0) to always (4). Included items are for example “I see plenty of interesting challenges” and “If something unexpected happens, I can easily adapt.” A higher total average score reflects a higher generic sense of ability to adapt and to adjust to daily life challenges and adverse life-events, whereas low scores suggest low perceived ability to adapt.

To measure convergent and incremental validity, the scales mentioned below were used.

2.1.3.2. Sense of coherence

Sense Of Coherence (SOC) was measured using Antonovsky’s SOC-13, also known as the Orientation to Life Questionnaire. The SOC-13 comprises 13 items with three subscales (Antonovsky, 1987). The first subscale is comprehensibility, measuring the extent to which stimuli are considered predictable, structured, explicable, and understandable. The second subscale is meaningfulness, which measures the extent to which someone experiences adversities as challenges with emotional meaning and valuable to adapt to. The third subscale is manageability, measuring the availability of resources to deal with circumstances and demands (Mittelmark et al., 2016). All items were scored on a 7-point semantic differential scale with two opposite response options (e.g., never to very often) tailored to each question. Five of the items were reversed (1, 2, 3, 7, 10) so a higher mean reflected a higher level of the SOC. Although the scale consists of three subscales, Antonovsky (1987) intended to measure SOC as a general factor. Various studies evaluating the SOC scale support this idea of SOC as an overarching factor with three dimensions (Klepp et al., 2007; Drageset and Haugan, 2016; Mittelmark et al., 2016). In the present study the scale proved to have a good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.871, 95% CI: 0.835–0.896).

2.1.3.3. Well-being

Well-being was measured using the Mental Health Continuum-Short Form (MHC-SF). The MHC-SF is designed to measure positive mental health and consists of 14 items divided into three subscales (Keyes et al., 2008). The first subscale is Emotional Well-being (EW), which measures positive and negative affect and life-satisfaction. The second subscale is Social Well-being (SW), measuring someone’s functioning in society. The third subscale is Psychological Well-being (PW), reflecting positive functioning in the individual’s life. On all items, participants were asked to rate how frequently they have experienced a given feeling during the past month. Items were answered on a 6-point Likert scale which ranged from never (0) to every day (5). Studies evaluating psychometric properties of the Dutch MHC-SF show its reliability and validity (Lamers et al., 2011; De Vos et al., 2017; Franken et al., 2018; De Beurs et al., 2022). Within the sample of the current study, the subscales and total scale proved to have fair to excellent internal consistency in the present study (EW Cronbach’s α = 0.886, 95% CI: 0.864–0.903; SW α = 0.799, 95% CI: 0.764–0.826; PW α = 0.855, 95% CI: 0.833–0.873; total scale α = 0.920, 95% CI: 0.908–0.930).

2.1.3.4. Psychopathology

Psychopathology was measured using the Outcome Questionnaire 45 (OQ-45) (De Jong et al., 2007). The OQ-45 consists of 45 items comprising three subscales. The first subscale is Symptomatic Distress (OQ-SD), consisting of items with the most common symptoms in psychopathology (e.g., anxiety and depression). The second subscale is Interpersonal Relations (OQ-IR), measuring the functioning of patients in relationships with family and friends. The third subscale is Social Role (OQ-SR) that assesses a patient’s functioning in work and leisure. Items were answered on a 5-point Likert scale which ranged from never (0) to almost always (4). Nine items were reversed so a higher mean reflects a higher level of psychological complaints. In the present study, both the OQ-SD scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.950, 95% CI: 0.937–0.959) and the total scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.925, 95% CI: 0.863–0.952) showed excellent internal consistency. The internal consistency of the OQ-IR scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.733, 95% CI: 0.656–0.785) and the OQ-SR scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.723, 95% CI: 0.662–0.770) was acceptable.

2.1.4. Data analysis

SPSS version 27 was used for determining the descriptive and correlational statistics and for calculating the internal consistency of the scale (Cronbach’s alpha). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed using Mplus 7.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998).

2.1.4.1. Factorial validity

Confirmatory factor analysis using robust maximum likelihood estimation was applied to evaluate whether the single-factor structure of the GSAAS, found in the non-clinical sample (study 1), could be confirmed in the clinical population. The data were tested against a strict 10-item model in which all items loaded on a single latent factor. The model’s fit was determined using the Satorra-Bentler (SB) scaled Chi-square statistic (χ2) in which a small non-significant value is indicative for good fit. Because the χ2 is overly sensitive to misfit, a ratio of <5 between χ2 and the degrees of freedom was considered indicative of an acceptable fit (Watkins, 1989). In addition, the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) were used to test the model’s fit (Hu and Bentler, 1998). An acceptable fit is achieved when TLI ≥ 0.90, CFI ≥ 0.90, SRMR ≤0.10, and RMSEA ≤0.08, while a good model fit is established when TLI ≥ 0.95, CFI ≥ 0.95, SRMR ≤0.08, and RMSEA ≤0.06 (Browne and Cudeck, 1992; Hu and Bentler, 1999). Standardized factor loadings were considered sufficient and meaningful if they exceeded 0.40 (Floyd and Widaman, 1995; Hair et al., 2006).

2.1.4.2. Measurement invariance

To explore whether meaningful comparisons across different groups can be made using the GSAAS, multiple-group confirmatory factor analyses were executed to establish measurement invariance. People were grouped based on the demographics gender (male vs. female), age (<35 vs. >35 years) and educational level (low/moderate vs. high). The data were analyzed on three increasingly restrictive levels of invariance: configural invariance (same factor structure), metric invariance (same factor structure and equal factor loadings), and scalar invariance (same factor structure and equal factor loadings and intercepts). The χ2 difference test (Δχ2) was only used for descriptive purposes because of its sensitivity to sample size (Chen, 2007). Instead, Chen (2007) suggests to use the absolute changes in CFI and RMSEA indices to determine the evidence for sufficient measurement invariance because they appear to be equally sensitive to the three types of invariance tests. Based on the current sample size (N > 300), a change of ≥ −0.010 in ΔCFI, supplemented by a change of ≥0.015 in ΔRMSEA or ≥0.030 in ΔSRMR, would indicate non-invariance (i.e., a different meaning of the construct to different groups) on the metric invariance level. For scalar testing a change of ≥ −0.010 in ΔCFI, supplemented by a change of Δ ≥ 0.015 in RMSEA or a change of ≥0.010 in ΔSRMR indicates non-invariance.

2.1.4.3. Convergent validity

Convergent validity was explored by calculating Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the scores on the GSAAS and scores on the convergent constructs: SOC-13 (sense of coherence), MHC-SF (well-being) and OQ-45 (psychopathology). In addition, the correlation of the GSAAS and the subscales of the OQ-45 and the MHC-SF were assessed. Values between 0.50 and 1.00 were considered strong, between 0.30 and 0.50 moderate, between 0.10 and 0.30 small, and values <0.10 weak (Cohen, 1988).

The perceived ability to adapt was expected to show strong positive relations to both the sense of coherence measure and the three core components of well-being (Keyes, 1998, 2002; Keyes et al., 2008). It was expected that the presence of ability to adapt will result in higher levels of positive affect and life-satisfaction (EW) and will be related to indicators of better positive functioning such as experiencing autonomy, mastery and meaning (PW), and social embedding (SW). Londono and McMillan (2015) and van Leeuwen et al. (2012) suggest that aspects of emotional well-being are outcomes of successful adaptation. Aspects of psychological well-being, for example finding meaning, environmental mastery, and psychological functioning, are also attributed to adaptation (Taylor, 1983; Stanton et al., 2007; Helgeson et al., 2014). Moreover, social well-being can promote successful adaptation (Stanton et al., 2007; Brüssow, 2013; Londono and McMillan, 2015; Audulv et al., 2016). Consequently, a strong positive relation between the GSAAS and general well-being was expected. Additionally, in the current sample with mild-to-moderate psychopathology, the perceived ability to adapt was expected to show a strong negative relation with symptomatic distress and moderate negative relations with functioning in relations with friends or family and in work or leisure. These expectations are in line with the model of Sustainable Mental Health, stating that well-being is seen as an outcome of successful adaptation and psychopathological symptoms are negatively related to adaptation (Bohlmeijer and Westerhof, 2020, 2021).

2.1.4.4. Incremental validity

Finally, using a hierarchical linear regression analysis, incremental validity of the GSAAS was examined by determining the degree of variability in symptomatic distress (measured by OQ-SD) explained by the GSAAS beyond a sense of coherence (SOC-13). A significant increase (p < 0.05) in explained variance was considered indicative for incremental validity of the GSAAS. In the present study, we expected that the GSAAS, measuring the generic sense of ability to adapt, can significantly explain symptomatic distress beyond the sense of coherence.

2.2. Results

2.2.1. Factor structure, measurement invariance, and internal consistency

The strict unidimensional factor model showed an acceptable fit based on the CFI (0.91) and the SMRS (0.05). In addition, all items (N = 10) showed strong factor loadings (0.544–0.762; see Table 3). However, the χ2/df ratio (5.36), TLI (0.88) and the RMSEA (0.10, 90% CI: 0.08–0.11) indicated a poor fit to the data. Because of an unacceptable fit of these indices, it was tested whether allowance of an error correlation between items with substantive overlap would result in a better fit. After investigating the modification indices, we allowed for an error correlation between item 2 (“I feel energetic”) and 3 (“I see plenty of interesting challenges”), which improved the fit indices (χ2/df = 4.37, TLI = 0.91, CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.08, SRMR = 0.05). The model, in which the error correlation between item 2 and 3 (r = 0.34) was allowed, showed a significantly better fit based on a Satorra-Bentler scaled χ2 difference test (Δχ2 (1) = 31.21, p < 0.001) and adequate to good fit on all indices.

Results of the measurement invariance tests supported measurement invariance on both the metric and the scalar level for all tested demographics (see Table 4). Although χ2 difference tests were mostly significant (as expected), none of the used models showed relevant changes on the CFI and RMSEA or SRMR indices. This indicates that scores on the GSAAS are equivalent between these subgroups (gender, age, and educational level). The scale showed good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s α value of 0.892 (95% CI: 0.875–0.907).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Measurement invariance between gender, age and educational level on the GSAAS.

2.2.2. Convergent and incremental validity

Figure 2 shows the distribution of the mean scores of the GSAAS. With a skewness of 0.338 and a kurtosis of −0.148, the distribution of scores was approximately normal. The mean total score on the GSAAS was 1.61 (SD = 0.76) with a range from 0 to 4. Most scores were centered just below the midpoint of the scale. The descriptive statistics of the GSAAS, the SOC-13, the OQ-45, and the MHC-SF can be found in Table 5. This table also presents the correlations between the GSAAS and its validation measures.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Frequency distribution of the mean scores of the GSAAS (N = 496).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Means, standard deviations and correlations for outcome variables with the GSAAS.

In line with our expectations, there were strong positive correlations between the GSAAS and the SOC-13 (r = 0.657) and the GSAAS and the MHC-SF (r = 0.662) and its subscales PW (r = 0.665) and EW (r = 0.568) and SW (r = 0.537). As expected, a strong negative correlation was found between the GSAAS and the OQ-45 (r = −0.687) and its subscale OQ-SD (r = −0.698). Moderate correlations were found for the OQ-45 subscales OQ-IR (r = −0.403) and OQ-SR (r = −0.386).

Incremental validity was established using a hierarchical regression analysis (see Table 6). The GSAAS accounted for 7.4% additional variance in symptomatic distress (OQ-SD) beyond a Sense Of Coherence (SOC-13). This increase in explained variance was significant (p < 0.001).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Hierarchical regression analysis: the GSAAS as predictor of SD (OQ-45) beyond SOC (SOC-13).

3. Discussion

The current study aimed to evaluate the GSAAS, a new measure to assess people’s generic sense of the ability to adapt, and to evaluate its psychometric properties in a Dutch sample of outpatient adults with mental health problems. The ability to adapt can be considered a vital aspect of health (Jambroes et al., 2016) and a process indicator for treatment (Bohlmeijer and Westerhof, 2021).

Overall, the results of our study suggest that the GSAAS factor structure was best represented by a single-factor model in both samples, with all items tapping into a single underlying construct. The GSAAS showed good internal consistency and convergent validity by demonstrating the expected relationships with theoretically related constructs, such as sense of coherence (Mittelmark et al., 2016), well-being (Taylor, 1983; Diener, 1984; Ryff, 1989; Stanton et al., 2007; van Leeuwen et al., 2012; Brüssow, 2013; Helgeson et al., 2014; Londono and McMillan, 2015; Audulv et al., 2016), and psychopathology (Eriksson and Lindström, 2007; Keyes et al., 2008; Grevenstein et al., 2016).

We found a strong relationship between the generic sense of ability to adapt (GSAAS) and a sense of coherence (SOC-13). Both instruments measure to what extent people might use their resources to overcome resistance and feel that stressors in their environment are manageable. Previous studies already proved the conceptually related construct of sense of coherence to be a strong predictor of psychopathology (Eriksson and Lindström, 2007; Grevenstein et al., 2016). The moderate to strong negative correlations between the GSAAS and symptomatic distress also confirm the convergent validity of the scale. In previous research into concepts strongly related to the ability to adapt, it has been conceptualized that lower experienced levels of ability to adapt will be associated with higher levels of anxiety (Roos et al., 2021) and depression (Cuijpers et al., 2021). Apart from the aforementioned theory about the sense of coherence, this also corresponds with theories about similar constructs such as self-management (Barlow et al., 2002; Lorig and Holman, 2003), psychological flexibility (Hayes, 2004; Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010), sense of mastery (Bohlmeijer et al., 2003; Pinquart and Forstmeier, 2012), and identifying, describing, integrating, and regulating emotions (Bohlmeijer et al., 2003; Dunn, 2007; Pinquart and Forstmeier, 2012; Kramer, 2014). This underlines the importance further developing the more widely concept of the ability to adapt, and thus the application of a generic questionnaire as the GSAAS. Compared with the relations of the ability to adapt with psychopathology and symptomatic distress, the observed relations with family and friends and observed functioning in work and leisure were moderate and relatively low. One explanation for this lower correlation could be that relations and roles can be seen as sources of adaptation, where psychopathology and symptomatic distress are the outcome. This also fits into the model of sustainable mental health (Bohlmeijer and Westerhof, 2020, 2021).

An important finding of the current study is that the GSAAS accounted for 7.4% additional unique variance in symptomatic distress in comparison to the SOC-13 confirming its incremental validity. This finding shows the added value of the GSAAS. Where the SOC-13 focuses on powerlessness and environmental influence, the GSAAS focuses on decisiveness and the process of dealing with circumstances. In conclusion, with the GSAAS we now have an instrument with good psychometric properties to measure a generic sense of the ability to adapt. This may have several important implications. The scale could be used in clinical studies to assess the impact of psychological treatments on adaptation. The recently introduced theoretical model of sustainable mental health proposes the ability to adapt as a core indicator of recovery and resilience (Bohlmeijer and Westerhof, 2020, 2021). While distress and mental well-being may fluctuate across time depending on current life-events, a sense of the ability to adapt is an indicator whether a person is able to successfully cope with difficult life-events and restore good mental health. The availability of the GSAAS allows more empirical studies to validate the assumptions of the model of sustainable mental health. For example, it can be studied whether a sense of the ability to adapt indeed mediates the impact of dysfunctional and functional cognitions, emotions, and behavior on psychopathology and well-being.

The GSAAS can be implemented in clinical practice to validate the new sustainable mental health model to contribute to the recently initiated transition of focus from disease to health and thus stimulate a more balanced treatment in (mental) health care (Bohlmeijer and Westerhof, 2021). Treatments may focus on promoting adaptive regulations of difficult emotions or replacing or defusing from unrealistic and unhelpful cognitions. When these interventions are successful, a strengthening of the sense of the ability to adapt should be observed. In addition, interventions promoting personal resources should also enhance a sense of the ability to adapt. The GSAAS could thus be a practical, easy, and quick tool measuring a vital process of change in treatment.

A few limitations of the studies should be considered. First, in study 1, participants were mainly students with a high level of education, who possibly interpreted and evaluated their generic sense of ability to adapt in a specific way. Future research should study generalizability of the validity in other age- and education groups. However, in study 2 where the age range was wider and the level of education was mainly moderate, the GSAAS also demonstrated good psychometric properties. Also, across gender, age, and education, the factor structure showed sufficient measurement invariance. Second, any temporal directional nature of the relationship between the ability to adapt and psychopathology cannot be deduced from the results of the current cross-sectional study. Future longitudinal studies should also reveal if the GSAAS is sensitive to detect changes over time and can thus be used accurately as an evaluation tool in treatment settings. Likewise, in a follow-up study of psychometric qualities, adding a retest reliability study could substantiate the reliability of the GSAAS even more strongly. Furthermore, we suggest measurement invariance analyses to be applied in several psychiatric and somatic clinical and also non-clinical studies to investigate whether GSAAS scores can be meaningfully compared across clinical populations. Finally, in the present study, we only investigated incremental validity over sense of coherence. This was done because the sense of coherence is theoretically strongly related to adaptation and both can be considered as determinants or mediators of health. We found a strong association between well-being and the ability to adapt, as has been found earlier with the SOC (Getnet and Alem, 2019). In the future, it might be interesting to investigate the association between the quality of life and the ability to adapt, as the association with the SOC is also strong (Eriksson and Lindström, 2007).

4. Conclusion

All in all, the current study introduced the GSAAS as a valid and reliable tool, measuring the perceived ability to adapt. The unidimensional scale appears to be promising in assessing people’s generic sense of the ability to adapt and can be used in both (non-)clinical settings and research settings. The GSAAS shows its potential to contribute to the general understanding of mental health, where the concept of well-being did this before by leaving the one-sided psychopathology-oriented approach (Franken et al., 2018). Further research is recommended to establish stronger evidence for the psychometric properties of the scale in different target groups and its contribution to the evaluation and support of the model of Sustainable Mental Health in particular and the positive psychology framework in general (Bohlmeijer and Westerhof, 2020, 2021).

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Medical Ethics Review Committee (METC) from Twente University, under the Law on Research Involving Human Subjects (File numbers: 15092 and K18-19). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KF: first authorship, responsible for data processing and analysis, and display in manuscript. PS and KD: support and collect the data. PK: senior authorship and supporting data analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amati, V., Meggiolaro, S., Rivellini, G., and Zaccarin, S. (2018). Social relations and life satisfaction: the role of friends. Genus 74, 1–18. doi: 10.1186/s41118-018-0032-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association . (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (Vol. 5). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC

Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress, and coping: New perspectives on mental and physical well-being. San Francisco, CA: Wiley & Sons 12–37.

Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay well. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass

Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of the sense of coherence scale. Soc. Sci. Med. 36, 725–733. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(93)90033-Z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1996). The salutogenic model as a theory to guide health promotion. Health Promot. Int. 11, 11–18. doi: 10.1093/heapro/11.1.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Audulv, Å., Packer, T., Hutchinson, S., Roger, K. S., and Kephart, G. (2016). Coping, adapting or self-managing–what is the difference? A concept review based on the neurological literature. J. Adv. Nurs. 72, 2629–2643. doi: 10.1111/jan.13037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, J., Wright, C., Sheasby, J., Turner, A., and Hainsworth, J. (2002). Self-management approaches for people with chronic conditions: a review. Patient Educ. Couns. 48, 177–187. doi: 10.1016/S0738-3991(02)00032-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F. (2015). Developing adaptations. Dev. Rev. 38, 13–35. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohlmeijer, E., Smit, F., and Cuijpers, P. (2003). Effects of reminiscence and life review on late-life depression: a meta-analysis. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 1088–1094. doi: 10.1002/gps.1018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohlmeijer, E. T., and Westerhof, G. J. (2020). A new model for sustainable mental health: Integrating well-being into psychological treatment, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Bohlmeijer, E. T., and Westerhof, G. J. (2021). The model for sustainable mental health: future directions for integrating positive psychology into mental health care. Front. Psychol. 12, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.747999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bond, T. G., Fox, C. M., and Lacey, H. (2007). “Applying the Rasch model” in Fundamental measurement in the human sciences. 2nd Edn. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Psychology Press).

Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol. Methods Res. 21, 230–258. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brüssow, H. (2013). What is health? Microb. Biotechnol. 6, 341–348. doi: 10.1111/1751-7915.12063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1, 245–276. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr0102_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chesney, M. A., Neilands, T. B., Chambers, D. B., Taylor, J. M., and Folkman, S. (2006). A validity and reliability study of the coping self-efficacy scale. Br. J. Health Psychol. 11, 421–437. doi: 10.1348/135910705X53155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 84, 19–74.

Google Scholar

Conversano, C., Rotondo, A., Lensi, E., Della Vista, O., Arpone, F., and Reda, M. A. (2010). Optimism and its impact on mental and physical well-being. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Mental Health 6, 25–29. doi: 10.2174/1745017901006010025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotton, D., and Gresty, K. (2006). Reflecting on the think-aloud method for evaluating e-learning. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 37, 45–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8535.2005.00521.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuijpers, P., Karyotaki, E., Ciharova, M., Miguel, C., Noma, H., and Furukawa, T. A. (2021). The effects of psychotherapies for depression on response, remission, reliable change, and deterioration: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 144, 288–299. doi: 10.1111/acps.13335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, R. A. (2010). Subjective wellbeing, Homeostatically protected mood and depression: a synthesis. J. Happiness Stud. 11, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s10902-009-9167-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Beurs, E., Kosterman, S., Anten, S., Bohlmeijer, E., and Westerhof, G. J. (2022). Psychometrische evaluatie van de Mental Health Continuum–Short Form (MHC-SF). Constructvaliditeit, responsiviteit voor verandering, normen en T-scores. Gedragstherapie 55, 1–32.

Google Scholar

De Jong, K., Nugter, M. A., Polak, M. G., Wagenborg, J. E. A., Spinhoven, P., and Heiser, W. J. (2007). The outcome questionnaire (OQ-45) in a Dutch population: a cross-cultural validation. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. Int. J. Theory Pract. 14, 288–301. doi: 10.1002/cpp.529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vos, J. A., LaMarre, A., Radstaak, M., Bijkerk, C. A., Bohlmeijer, E. T., and Westerhof, G. J. (2017). Identifying fundamental criteria for eating disorder recovery: a systematic review and qualitative meta-analysis. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s40337-017-0164-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 95, 542–575. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, R., Daly, A. P., Huyton, J. L., and Sanders, L. (2012). The challenge of defining wellbeing. Int. J. Wellbeing 2, 222–235. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v2i3.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drageset, J., and Haugan, G. (2016). Psychometric properties of the orientation to life questionnaire in nursing home residents. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 30, 623–630. doi: 10.1111/scs.12271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, W. (2007). Supporting children to participate successfully in everyday life by using sensory processing knowledge. Infants Young Child. 20, 84–101. doi: 10.1097/01.IYC.0000264477.05076.5d

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, M., and Lindström, B. (2007). Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale and its relation with quality of life: a systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 61, 938–944. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.056028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, M., and Mittelmark, M. B. (2017). “The sense of coherence and its measurement” in The handbook of salutogenesis (Cham: Springer), 97–106.

Google Scholar

Floyd, F. J., and Widaman, K. F. (1995). Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psychol. Assess. 7, 286–299. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franken, K., Lamers, S. M. A., Ten Klooster, P. M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., and Westerhof, G. J. (2018). Validation of the mental health continuum-short form and the dual continua model of well-being and psychopathology in an adult mental health setting. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 2187–2202. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getnet, B., and Alem, A. (2019). Construct validity and factor structure of sense of coherence (SoC-13) scale as a measure of resilience in Eritrean refugees living in Ethiopia. Confl. Heal. 13, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s13031-019-0185-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-López, J. R., Serrano-Gómez, D., Velasco-González, V., Alconero-Camarero, A. R., Cuesta-Lozano, D., García-García, E., et al. (2022). Design and validation of a questionnaire on risk perception, coping behaviors and preventive knowledge against COVID-19 among nursing students. J. Pers. Med. 12:515. doi: 10.3390/jpm12040515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenaway, K. H., Louis, W. R., Parker, S. L., Kalokerinos, E. K., Smith, J. R., and Terry, D. J. (2015). “Measures of coping for psychological well-being” in Measures of personality and social psychological constructs. eds. G. J. Boyle, D. H. Saklofske, and G. Matthews (Elsevier), 322–351.

Google Scholar

Grevenstein, D., Bluemke, M., and Kroeninger-Jungaberle, H. (2016). Incremental validity of sense of coherence, neuroticism, extraversion, and general self-efficacy: longitudinal prediction of substance use frequency and mental health. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 14, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s12955-016-0412-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., and Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis, Vol. 6.

Google Scholar

Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behav. Ther. 35, 639–665. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(04)80013-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Headey, B., and Wearing, A. (1991). “Subjective well-being: a stocks and flows framework” in Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective. eds. F. Strack, M. Argyle, and N. Schwarz, Vol. 21 (Oxford: Pergamon Press), 49–73.

Google Scholar

Helgeson, V. S. (1999). Applicability of cognitive adaptation theory to predicting adjustment to heart disease after coronary angioplasty. Health Psychol. 18, 561–569. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.18.6.561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helgeson, V. S., Reynolds, K. A., Siminerio, L. M., Becker, D. J., and Escobar, O. (2014). Cognitive adaptation theory as a predictor of adjustment to emerging adulthood for youth with and without type 1 diabetes. J. Psychosom. Res. 77, 484–491. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holeva, V., Tarrier, N., and Wells, A. (2001). Prevalence and predictors of acute stress disorder and PTSD following road traffic accidents: thought control strategies and social support. Behav. Ther. 32, 65–83. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80044-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, J. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of components from the matrix of partial correlations. Psychometrika 30, 179–185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L.-T., and Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychol. Methods 3, 424–453. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, M., Knottnerus, J., Green, L., van der Horst, H., Jadad, A. R., Kromhout, D., et al. (2011). How should we define health. BMJ 2011:343 doi: 10.1136/bmj.d4163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, M., van Vliet, M., Giezenberg, M., Winkens, B., Heerkens, Y., Dagnelie, P., et al. (2016). Towards a ‘patient-centred’ operationalisation of the new dynamic concept of health: a mixed methods study. BMJ Open 6:e010091. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jambroes, M., Nederland, T., Kaljouw, M., Van Vliet, K., Essink-Bot, M.-L., and Ruwaard, D. (2016). Implications of health as ‘the ability to adapt and self-manage’ for public health policy: a qualitative study. Eur. J. Public Health 26, 412–416. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckv206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, A., Kale, S., Chandel, S., and Pal, D. K. (2015). Likert scale: explored and explained. Br. J. Appl. Sci. Technol 7, 396–403. doi: 10.9734/BJAST/2015/14975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., and Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. L. (1998). Social well-being. Soc. Psychol. Q. 61, 121–140. doi: 10.2307/2787065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. L. (2002). The mental health continuum: from languishing to flourishing in life. J. Health Soc. Behav. 43, 207–222. doi: 10.2307/3090197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. L., Wissing, M., Potgieter, J. P., Temane, M., Kruger, A., and Van Rooy, S. (2008). Evaluation of the mental health continuum–short form (MHC–SF) in setswana-speaking south Africans. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 15, 181–192. doi: 10.1002/cpp.572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klepp, O. M., Mastekaasa, A., Sørensen, T., Sandanger, I., and Kleiner, R. (2007). Structure analysis of Antonovsky's sense of coherence from an epidemiological mental health survey with a brief nine-item sense of coherence scale. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 16, 11–22. doi: 10.1002/mpr.197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, P. (2000). Handbook of psychological testing. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Kloep, M., Hendry, L., and Saunders, D. (2009). A new perspective on human development. Int. J. Arts Sci. 1, 332–343.

Google Scholar

Kramer, L. (2014). Learning emotional understanding and emotion regulation through sibling interaction. Early Educ. Dev. 25, 160–184. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2014.838824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamers, S. M., Westerhof, G. J., Bohlmeijer, E. T., ten Klooster, P. M., and Keyes, C. L. (2011). Evaluating the psychometric properties of the mental health continuum-short form (MHC-SF). J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 99–110. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Länsimies, H., Pietilä, A. M., Hietasola-Husu, S., and Kangasniemi, M. (2017). A systematic review of adolescents’ sense of coherence and health. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 31, 651–661. doi: 10.1111/scs.12402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linacre, J. M. (1991). A user’s guide to Winsteps Ministeps Rasch-model computer programs. Chicago, IL: John M. Linacre.

Google Scholar

Lindström, B., and Eriksson, M. (2010). The hitchhiker’s guide to salutogenesis. Salutogenic pathways to health promotion. Helsinki: Folkhälsan Research Centre.

Google Scholar

Livneh, H. (2001). Psychosocial adaptation to chronic illness and disability: a conceptual framework. Rehab. Couns. Bull. 44, 151–160. doi: 10.1177/003435520104400305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Londono, Y., and McMillan, D. E. (2015). Psychosocial adaptation: an evolutionary concept analysis exploring a common multidisciplinary language. J. Adv. Nurs. 71, 2504–2519. doi: 10.1111/jan.12723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorig, K. R., and Holman, H. R. (2003). Self-management education: history, definition, outcomes, and mechanisms. Ann. Behav. Med. 26, 1–7. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2601_01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, L. (1991). Daily hassles and mental health: a longitudinal study. Br. J. Psychol. 82, 441–447. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1991.tb02411.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCracken, L. M., Chilcot, J., and Norton, S. (2015). Further development in the assessment of psychological flexibility: a shortened committed action questionnaire (CAQ-8). Eur. J. Pain 19, 677–685. doi: 10.1002/ejp.589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCracken, L. M., and Yang, S.-Y. (2006). The role of values in a contextual cognitive-behavioral approach to chronic pain. Pain 123, 137–145. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.02.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mittelmark, M. B., Sagy, S., Eriksson, M., Bauer, G. F., Pelikan, J. M., Lindström, B., and Espnes, G. A. eds (2016). “The sense of coherence and its measurement” in The Handbook of Salutogenesis (Cham: Springer), 1–461.

Google Scholar

Morita, Y., Ohta, M., Inoue, T., Honda, T., Konno, Y., Eguchi, Y., et al. (2014). Sense of coherence is significantly associated with both metabolic syndrome and lifestyle in Japanese computer software office workers. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 27, 967–979. doi: 10.2478/s13382-014-0322-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles. CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Nayback, A. M. (2009). PTSD in the combat veteran: using Roy's adaptation model to examine the combat veteran as a human adaptive system. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 30, 304–310. doi: 10.1080/01612840902754404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrig-Chiello, P., Hutchison, S., and Morselli, D. (2015). Patterns of psychological adaptation to divorce after a long-term marriage. J. Soc. Pers. Relationsh. 32, 386–405. doi: 10.1177/0265407514533769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, J. C., Banon, E., and Bond, M. (2020). Change in defense mechanisms and depression in a pilot study of antidepressive medications plus 20 sessions of psychotherapy for recurrent major depression. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 208, 261–268. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000001112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M., and Forstmeier, S. (2012). Effects of reminiscence interventions on psychosocial outcomes: a meta-analysis. Aging Ment. Health 16, 541–558. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2011.651434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinsen, C. A., and Terwee, C. B. (2019). Measuring positive health: for now, a bridge too far. Public Health 170, 70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2019.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, T. R., and Hoffman, L. (2014). Defense mechanisms and implicit emotion regulation: a comparison of a psychodynamic construct with one from contemporary neuroscience. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 62, 693–708. doi: 10.1177/0003065114546746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roos, A.-L., Goetz, T., Krannich, M., Jarrell, A., Donker, M., and Mainhard, T. (2021). Test anxiety components: an intra-individual approach testing their control antecedents and effects on performance. Anxiety Stress Coping 34, 279–298. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2020.1850700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, C. (2009). The Roy adaptation model. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson Education.

Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiavon, C. C., Marchetti, E., Gurgel, L. G., Busnello, F. M., and Reppold, C. T. (2017). Optimism and hope in chronic disease: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 7:2022. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Kühberger, A., and Johnson, J. G. (2011). A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: A critical review and user’s guide. Oxford: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Stanton, A. L., Revenson, T. A., and Tennen, H. (2007). Health psychology: psychological adjustment to chronic disease. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 565–592. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suh, E., Diener, E., and Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: only recent events matter. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 1091–1102. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.1091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: a theory of cognitive adaptation. Am. Psychol. 38, 1161–1173. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terhorst, Y., Baumeister, H., McCracken, L. M., and Lin, J. (2020). Further development in the assessment of psychological flexibility: validation of the German committed action questionnaire. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 18, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12955-020-01497-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaillant, G. E. (2000). Adaptive mental mechanisms: their role in a positive psychology. Am. Psychol. 55, 89–98. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.89

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Bon-Martens, M., Kleinjan, M., Hipple Walters, B., Shields-Zeeman, L., and Van den Brink, C. (2022). Delphistudie 'Definitie Mentale Gezondheid': Resultaten van een consensusprocedure met verschillende perspectieven. Utrecht: Trimbos-instituut.

Google Scholar

van Leeuwen, C., Post, M. W., Van Der Woude, L. H., De Groot, S., Smit, C., Van Kuppevelt, D., et al. (2012). Changes in life satisfaction in persons with spinal cord injury during and after inpatient rehabilitation: adaptation or measurement bias? Qual. Life Res. 21, 1499–1508. doi: 10.1007/s11136-011-0073-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Loan, C. L., and Garwood, J. D. (2020). Facilitating high-quality relationships for students with emotional and behavioral disorders in crisis. Interv. Sch. Clin. 55, 253–256. doi: 10.1177/1053451219855740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vowles, K. E., McCracken, L. M., McLeod, C., and Eccleston, C. (2008). The chronic pain acceptance questionnaire: confirmatory factor analysis and identification of patient subgroups. Pain 140, 284–291. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, D. (1989). The role of confirmatory factor analysis in cross-cultural research. Int. J. Psychol. 24, 685–701. doi: 10.1080/00207598908246806

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wicksell, R. K., Lekander, M., Sorjonen, K., and Olsson, G. L. (2010). The psychological inflexibility in pain scale (PIPS)–statistical properties and model fit of an instrument to assess change processes in pain related disability. Eur. J. Pain 14, 771.e1–771.e14. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.11.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willis, K. D., and Burnett, H. J. (2016). The power of stress: perceived stress and its relationship with rumination, self-concept clarity, and resilience. N. Am. J. Psychol. 18, 483–498.

Google Scholar

Wright, B. (1994). Reasonable mean-square fit values. Rasch Meas Trans 8:370.

Google Scholar

Yu, L., McCracken, L. M., and Norton, S. (2016). The self experiences questionnaire (SEQ): preliminary analyses for a measure of self in people with chronic pain. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 5, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, L., Norton, S., and McCracken, L. M. (2017). Change in “self-as-context”(“perspective-taking”) occurs in acceptance and commitment therapy for people with chronic pain and is associated with improved functioning. J. Pain 18, 664–672. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: scale development, positive psychology assessment, reliability, validation, well-being, adaptation, generic sense of ability to adapt scale, mental health problems

Citation: Franken K, Schuffelen P, ten Klooster P, van Doesum K, Westerhof G and Bohlmeijer E (2023) Introduction of the generic sense of ability to adapt scale and validation in a sample of outpatient adults with mental health problems. Front. Psychol. 14:985408. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.985408

Received: 12 July 2022; Accepted: 02 March 2023;
Published: 29 March 2023.

Edited by:

Tanja Stamm, Medical University of Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

André Luiz De Carvalho Braule Pinto, Federal University of Minas Gerais, Brazil
Machteld Ouwens, GGz Breburg, Netherlands

Copyright © 2023 Franken, Schuffelen, ten Klooster, van Doesum, Westerhof and Bohlmeijer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katinka Franken, c.p.m.franken@utwente.nl

These authors share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.