Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 05 December 2023
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Analysing Writing Processes of People with Language, Mental, Cognitive or Physical Disorders View all 8 articles

Bridging the writing gap in studying language related disorders: the process and the product

  • 1Department of Swedish, Multilingualism, Language Technology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
  • 2Department of Health and Rehabilitation, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
  • 3Faculty of Business Economics, Department of Management, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium

Proficiency in writing is essential for effective functioning in today's digitalized world. As pointed out by, for instance, Brandt (2014) and Rønneberg (2018), the world is full of text, and as society becomes increasingly digitalized the demand for written communication grows substantially. In the Western world, we find ourselves writing and possibly debating students' writing skills more frequently than ever before. Writing not only constitutes a vital competency across various academic subjects but also serves as a pivotal tool in professional settings and as a prerequisite for active engagement within democratic societies. These evolving dynamics pose formidable challenges for individuals grappling with language-related difficulties, ranging from developmental language disorders and dyslexia to aphasia and dementia. In fact, many individuals contending with language difficulties express writing as their most significant area of struggle (Connelly et al., 2006). This also extends to individuals with other disorders that might impede the acquisition of written language, such as hearing impairments (Breland et al., 2022).

For those with developmental challenges, like dyslexia or developmental language disorder (DLD), it is common for writing difficulties to persist even after overcoming reading obstacles (Berninger and Amtmann, 2003). Likewise, individuals recovering from post-stroke aphasia often find that writing difficulties linger long after regaining other capabilities and completing rehabilitation. The loss of writing proficiency is deeply felt, greatly impacting the quality of life and sense of independence for affected individuals (Parr, 2007; Knollman-Porter et al., 2015; Kjellén et al., 2017; Thiel and Conroy, 2022).

Despite successful research in identifying potential causes and solutions for reading challenges, writing difficulties surprisingly remain largely un(der)explored (Connelly et al., 2006). Back in 2008, Berninger et al., for instance, published a paper titled “Writing problems in developmental dyslexia: under-recognized and under-treated,” shedding light on the often disregarded and inadequately addressed nature of writing issues in those with developmental dyslexia. They emphasized not only the under-recognition of writing difficulties associated with dyslexia but also the necessity for comprehensive assessments and targeted interventions. They argued that heightened awareness among educators, parents, and the broader community regarding the impact of writing challenges was crucial for effectively addressing writing impediments in individuals with dyslexia and for developing efficient support and intervention strategies. Nevertheless, subsequent to the publication by Berninger et al. (2008), the need for deeper understanding has intensified, rather than diminished. Although the escalating demand for written communication skills has fostered a greater awareness of the substantial hindrance posed by writing difficulties in various contexts, our understanding of these challenges has not advanced at a proportional pace.

Previous research on writing difficulties

Research into writing difficulties experienced by individuals with various language or hearing disorders, such as dyslexia, DLD, aphasia, and congenital hearing impairment, sheds light on the intricate relationship between linguistic and cognitive challenges and the written mode of communication.

Investigations into writing challenges faced by individuals with dyslexia have primarily centered around deficient spelling skills, often arising from struggles with phoneme-to-grapheme correspondences. For a review, see Sumner et al. (2014). These struggles impede the accurate translation of auditory word aspects into written form. Alongside spelling difficulties, issues related to grammar, punctuation, organizational structure, and overall composition quality have been frequently documented—even after texts have been corrected for spelling and capitalization (Tops et al., 2013). These challenges are believed to stem from cognitive bottlenecks during the transcription phase of writing (McCutchen, 1996; Berninger et al., 2002). However, the conclusive evidence supporting this notion remains elusive. While some studies, such as Sterling et al. (1998) and Wengelin (2007), revealed associations between spelling-related dysfluencies and lexical attributes in texts composed by individuals with dyslexia, other studies, including those by Connelly and colleagues, did not identify any discernible variance in lexical diversity (Connelly et al., 2006) or in the frequency of spelling revisions (Sumner and Connelly, 2020) between dyslexic and non-dyslexic individuals. However, Sumner and Connelly did indeed note that spelling revisions constituted a higher proportion of the total number of revisions for the dyslexic writers. These inconsistencies between studies may partly result from variations in demographics, input modalities, tasks, and languages across studies. Another possible explanation for the relatively impaired structure and content observed in texts by many writers with dyslexia is, as suggested by Torrance et al. (2016), that their reading deficit, along with their limited reading experience, has influenced their prior learning.

Similar gaps in research exist for DLD, a condition characterized by significant oral language production, and in severe cases, comprehension difficulties. Tucci and Choi (2023) conducted a comprehensive review of DLD's effects on writing skills, revealing challenges encompassing handwriting, spelling, organization, cohesion, planning, and narrative quality. Weaker grammatical abilities and the production of writing samples comparable in length to peers with typical language development were also noted. However, the impacts of DLD on both higher-level processes such as planning, revision, organization, and writing speed, and word-level processes, such as morphology, and spelling remain underexplored, and the results are therefore once again inconclusive. For example, while Shen and Troia (2018) found that, before receiving a writing strategy intervention, their participants with DLD did not plan or revise their writing at all, Koutsoftas (2016) found no differences between children with and without DLD.

Studies on writing by individuals with acquired writing difficulties, arising from brain damage caused by stroke, trauma, or neurodegenerative diseases like dementia are limited, and have like DLD primarily been investigated through the lens of spoken language when it comes to studying language production. While recent research on writing by individuals with aphasia have offered some new and cautionsly promising insights into the possibilities presented by intervention and compensatory tools (Behrns et al., 2009; Johansson-Malmeling et al., 2020), dementia research has primarily centered on differentiating between speech and writing in relation to aging. Its main focus has been on disease identification and progression rather than intervention. Notably, for Alzheimer's disease, there remains a lack of consensus regarding whether writing deficits manifest in the early stages of the disease or whether writing abilities are affected only in the later stages of the disease. In this context, investigations into the writing process can serve as an inconspicuous means for early detection of atypical aging and offer deeper insights into the linguistic regression associated with it (Van Waes et al., 2017; Afonso et al., 2019).

Also in the case of congenitally deaf or hard of hearing (DHH) individuals, research has predominantly emphasized reading and spoken skills. Their disorder is not intrinsically linguistic in its nature, but they often struggle with literacy, and writing may present unique challenges. DHH individuals often struggle to transfer morphosyntactic structures from speech to writing (Arfé, 2015; Mayer et al., 2016; Breland et al., 2022) both due to the high cognitive cost of perceiving and translating auditory sounds to writing, and for some, due to the written language being a second language with different structures than their first, signed, language (Wolbers et al., 2014). As DHH children increasingly participate in mainstream classrooms, there is a growing demand for greater understanding and awareness of their unique writing challenges.

In summary, research into writing difficulties across various language disorders highlights the multifaceted nature of linguistic and cognitive challenges when transitioning to the written mode. While spelling and transcription struggles are evident among individuals with dyslexia, DLD, and acquired language disorders, congenital hearing impairment also pose distinct obstacles to successful writing. A deeper understanding of these challenges can guide effective interventions and support individuals in overcoming barriers to written communication.

Discussion

As shown in previous sections, a significant void persists in the domain of language disorders concerning the intricacies of writing difficulties and the underlying processes. While extensive research has shed light on reading and spoken language impairments, investigations into writing remain notably limited, hindering our ability to recognize, comprehend, and address writing-related challenges across various disorders. It is imperative that we address this gap through comprehensive studies that delve, not only into the final products of writing—the texts—, but into the writing process itself, as this knowledge is pivotal for both recognizing difficulties and devising effective remediation strategies.

For example, the findings from research on individuals with dyslexia underline the importance of unraveling the complexities of writing, particularly the connection between transcription skills, spelling, and overall composition quality. Such insights can provide a solid foundation for the identification of key intervention points to alleviate writing challenges. Similarly, developmental language disorder (DLD) research has revealed distinct hurdles in writing, from morphosyntactic transfers to organization and planning. This field remains underexplored in terms of understanding the precise processes that contribute to these difficulties and developing tailored interventions.

Considering persons with acquired disorders, such as aphasia and dementia, who in most cases have previously been skilled writers, it is somewhat surprising that the landscape of writing challenges has been left largely uncharted for so long. An awakening interest in understanding the production processes through methods like keystroke logging, exemplifies the need for a deeper exploration of writing difficulties in these contexts. Interestingly, dementia research stands out in that the focus has predominantly been on identifying disease-related linguistic changes rather than designing interventions. This disparity highlights the large range of opportunities offered by studies of the writing processes, for our understanding of the various aspects of language difficulties.

Furthermore, as mentioned above, the congenitally DHH population faces unique obstacles when it comes to writing. While reading and spoken skills have garnered more attention, research suggests that morphosyntactic transfers from speech to writing present significant challenges. This underscores the importance of understanding the cognitive demands of transcription for this group and underscores the significance of addressing writing-related difficulties in diverse populations.

In conclusion, the scarcity of research on writing and writing processes in both developmental and acquired language disorders is a critical issue. Enhanced studies focusing on these areas are pivotal for recognizing, understanding, and addressing writing challenges effectively. A balanced approach that mirrors the attention given to reading and spoken language can uncover the intricacies of writing impairments, thus paving the way for targeted interventions that empower individuals to overcome barriers in written communication, while also revealing some of the nuanced mechanisms underlying unaffected written expression. The six articles in this Frontiers Research Topic offer a significant contribution to the intricate realm of writing difficulties across various disorders. Through meticulous investigation, employing methodologies such as keystroke logging and handwriting capturing, they dissect the mechanics and cognitive aspects of writing as well as methods for exploring them. As our understanding of the writing process thus deepens, we will be able to provide more comprehensive support to those navigating the complex landscape of language disorders.

Author contributions

ÅW: Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. IH: Writing—review & editing. LV: Writing—review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Riksbanken's jubileumsfond (SAB20-0018) funded the ÅW's writing of this paper.

Acknowledgments

ÅW acknowledges the financial support from Riksbanken's jubileumsfond (SAB20-0018), which funded the writing of this paper.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Afonso, O., Álvarez, C. J., Martínez, C., and Cuetos, F. (2019). Writing difficulties in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Read. Writ. 32, 217–233. doi: 10.1007/s11145-017-9813-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arfé, B. (2015). Oral and written discourse skills in deaf and hard of hearing children: the role of reading and verbal working memory. Top. Lang. Disord. 35, 180–197. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrns, I., Hartelius, L., and Wengelin, Å. (2009). Aphasia and computerised writing aid supported treatment. Aphasiology 23, 1276–1294. doi: 10.1080/02687030802436892

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Graham, S., and Richards, T. (2002). Writing and Reading. J. Learn. Disabil. 35, 39–56. doi: 10.1177/002221940203500104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., and Amtmann, D. (2003). “Preventing written expression disabilities through early and continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: research into practice,” in Handbook of Learning Disabilities, eds. H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, and S. Graham. New York City: The Guilford Press, 345–363.

Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., and Raskind, W. (2008). Writing problems in developmental dyslexia: Under-recognized and under-treated. J. Sch. Psychol. 46, 1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandt, D. (2014). The Rise of Writing: Redefining Mass Literacy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Breland, L., Lowenstein, J. H., and Nittrouer, S. (2022). Disparate oral and written language abilities in adolescents with cochlear implants: evidence from narrative samples. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 53, 193–212. doi: 10.1044/2021_LSHSS-21-00062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., and Barnes, J. (2006). Contribution of lower order skills to the written composition of college students with and without dyslexia. Dev. Neuropsychol. 29, 175–196. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansson-Malmeling, C., Wengelin, Å., and Henriksson, I. (2020). Aphasia and spelling to dictation: analysis of spelling errors and editing. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 56, 145–160. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjellén, E., Laakso, K., and Henriksson, I. (2017). Aphasia and literacy—the insider's perspective. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 52, 573–584. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knollman-Porter, K., Wallace, S. E., Hux, K., Brown, J., and Long, C. (2015). Reading experiences and use of supports by people with chronic aphasia. Aphasiology 29, 1448–1472. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2015.1041093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutsoftas, A. D. (2016). Writing process products in intermediate-grade children with and without language-based learning disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 59, 1471–1483. doi: 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, C., Watson, L., Archbold, S., Ng, Z. Y., and Mulla, I. (2016). Reading and writing skills of deaf pupils with cochlear implants. Deafness Educ. Int. 18, 71–86. doi: 10.1080/14643154.2016.1155346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: working memory in composition. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 8, 299–325. doi: 10.1007/BF01464076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parr, S. (2007). Living with severe aphasia: tracking social exclusion. Aphasiology 21, 98–123. doi: 10.1080/02687030600798337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rønneberg, V. (2018). My Spelling is Wobbly – Causes and Consequences of Word-Level Disfluencies in Written Composition (Ph.D. Thesis). Bergen: University of Bergen.

Google Scholar

Shen, M., and Troia, G. A. (2018). Teaching children with language-learning disabilities to plan and revise compare–contrast texts. Learn. Disab. Quart. 41, 44–61. doi: 10.1177/0731948717701260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterling, C., Farmer, M., Riddick, B., Morgan, S., and Matthews, C. (1998). Adult dyslexic writing. Dyslexia 4, 1–15.

Google Scholar

Sumner, E., and Connelly, V. (2020). Writing and revision strategies of students with and without dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 53, 189–198. doi: 10.1177/0022219419899090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumner, E., Connelly, V., and Barnett, A. L. (2014). “A review of dyslexia and expressive writing in English,” in Writing Development and Instruction in Children with Hearing, Speech and oral Language Difficulties, eds. B. Arfé, J. Dockrell, and V. Berninger. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 188–200.

Google Scholar

Thiel, L., and Conroy, P. (2022). ‘I think writing is everything': An exploration of the writing experiences of people with aphasia. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 57, 1381–1398. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, W., Callens, C., Van Cauwenberghe, E., et al. (2013). Beyond spelling: the writing skills of students with dyslexia in higher education. Read. Writing 26, 705–720. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9387-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, M., Rønneberg, V., Johansson, C., and Uppstad, P. H. (2016). Adolescent weak decoders writing in a shallow orthography: process and product. Scient. Stud. Read. 20, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2016.1205071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucci, A., and Choi, E. (2023). Developmental language disorder and writing: a scoping review from childhood to adulthood. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 66, 2900–2920. doi: 10.1044/2023_JSLHR-22-00583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Waes, L., Leijten, M., Mariën, P., and Engelborghs, S. (2017). Typing competencies in Alzheimer's disease: an exploration of copy tasks. Comput. Human Behav. 73, 311–319. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.03.050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wengelin, Å. (2007). “The word-level focus in text production by adults with reading and writing difficulties,” in Writing and Cognition: Research and Applications, eds. M. Torrance, L. Van Waes, and D. Galbraith. Oxford: Elsevier, 67–82.

Google Scholar

Wolbers, K. A., Graham, S. C., Dostal, H. M., and Bowers, L. M. (2014). A description of ASL features in writing. Ampersand 1, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.amper.2014.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: writing difficulties, writing processes, language disorder, cognitive disorders, dyslexia, keystroke logging, hearing impairment, handwriting recording

Citation: Wengelin Å, Henriksson I and Van Waes L (2023) Bridging the writing gap in studying language related disorders: the process and the product. Front. Psychol. 14:1292602. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1292602

Received: 11 September 2023; Accepted: 20 November 2023;
Published: 05 December 2023.

Edited by:

Patricia E. Román, Loyola Andalusia University, Spain

Reviewed by:

Francisca Serrano, University of Granada, Spain
Kristina Hansson, Lund University, Sweden

Copyright © 2023 Wengelin, Henriksson and Van Waes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Åsa Wengelin, YXNhLndlbmdlbGluJiN4MDAwNDA7Z3Uuc2U=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.