Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 04 December 2023
Sec. Neuropsychology

An exploratory study on counterfactual thinking in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis

\r\nBarbara Poletti,
&#x;&#x;Barbara Poletti1,2*†‡Edoardo Nicol Aiello&#x;Edoardo Nicolò Aiello1Sofia TaginiSofia Tagini3Federica SolcaFederica Solca1Silvia TorreSilvia Torre1Eleonora ColomboEleonora Colombo1Alessio MaranzanoAlessio Maranzano1Ruggero BonettiRuggero Bonetti4Francesco SchevegherFrancesco Schevegher4Claudia MorelliClaudia Morelli1Alberto DorettiAlberto Doretti1Federico Verde,Federico Verde1,5Sergio BarbieriSergio Barbieri6Francesca MameliFrancesca Mameli6Alberto Priori,Alberto Priori7,8Roberta Ferrucci,Roberta Ferrucci2,6Vincenzo Silani,Vincenzo Silani1,5Paolo Cherubini,Paolo Cherubini9,10Gabriella Pravettoni,&#x;Gabriella Pravettoni2,11Nicola Ticozzi,&#x;Nicola Ticozzi1,5
  • 1Department of Neurology and Laboratory of Neuroscience, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milano, Italy
  • 2Department of Oncology and Hemato-Oncology, University of Milan, Milano, Italy
  • 3“Rita Levi Montalcini” Department of Neurosciences, University of Turin, Torino, Italy
  • 4Neurology Residency Program, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milano, Italy
  • 5Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, “Dino Ferrari” Center, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milano, Italy
  • 6Fondazione IRCCS Ca' Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milano, Italy
  • 7Aldo Ravelli Center for Neurotechnology and Experimental Brain Therapeutics, Department of Health Sciences, International Medical School, University of Milan, Milano, Italy
  • 8ASST Santi Paolo e Carlo, San Paolo University Hospital, Milano, Italy
  • 9Department of Neural and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • 10Milan Center for Neuroscience, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
  • 11Applied Research Division for Cognitive and Psychological Science, IEO, European Institute of Oncology, IRCCS, Milano, Italy

Objectives: This study aimed at exploring (1) the motor and non-motor correlates of counterfactual thinking (CFT) abilities in non-demented amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) patients and (2) the ability of CFT measures to discriminate these patients from healthy controls (HCs) and patients with and without cognitive impairment.

Methods: N = 110 ALS patients and N = 51 HCs were administered two CFT tasks, whose sum, resulting in a CFT Index (CFTI), was addressed as the outcome. Patients further underwent an in-depth cognitive, behavioral, and motor-functional evaluation. Correlational analyses were run to explore the correlates of the CFTI in patients. Logistic regressions were performed to test whether the CFTI could discriminate patients from HCs.

Results: The CFTI was selectively associated (p ≤ 0.005) with fluency and memory subscales of the Edinburgh Cognitive and Behavioral ALS Screen (ECAS), but not with other variables. CFTI scores discriminated patients from HCs (p < 0.001) with high accuracy (82%), but not patients with a normal vs. defective performance on the ECAS-Total.

Conclusion: CFT measures in non-demented ALS patients were associated with verbal fluency and memory functions, and they were also able to discriminate them from HCs.

1 Introduction

Counterfactual thinking (CFT) is defined as one's own ability to cognitively simulate alternatives to events that have occurred in order to regulate complex behaviors at both individual and social levels (Roese, 1997; Epstude and Roese, 2008). Counterfactual thoughts are believed to be “physiologically” triggered (“activation” stage of CFT) by affective states and/or situational occurrences that lead individuals to think (“content” stage of CFT) about how the outcomes of a given course of events could have been better—i.e., “upward” CFT or worse and “downward” CFT—had they acted differently (Roese, 1997; Epstude and Roese, 2008). In these terms, CFT processes are theorized to functionally allow humans to regulate future behaviors based on past events (Roese, 1997; Epstude and Roese, 2008).

As far as neural substrates are concerned, CFT abilities are believed to be subserved by widespread cortical-subcortical frontotemporal networks involved in both executive and episodic memory functions (De Brigard et al., 2013, 2015; Van Hoeck et al., 2013; De Brigard and Parikh, 2019).

CFT deficits have been documented in a variety of brain disorders of focal, developmental, degenerative, and demyelinating etiologies, as well as in psychiatric conditions (Tagini et al., 2021). With specific regard to neurodegenerative diseases, CFT deficits have been previously described in Parkinson's (McNamara et al., 2003) and Huntington's disease (Solca et al., 2015) patients only. Therein, CFT measures were found to be associated with tests tapping on both executive and social-cognitive functions.

However, no study to date has explored CFT abilities in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), up to 50% of whom present with cognitive deficits within the spectrum of frontotemporal degeneration (Strong et al., 2017). Nevertheless, given the likely link between CFT and both executive and social-cognitive functions (McNamara et al., 2003; Solca et al., 2015), which are acknowledged to negatively affect prognosis in this population by undermining decision-making and adherence within care settings (Huynh et al., 2020), the assessment of CFT abilities in ALS patients might convey relevant clinical entailments.

Given the above premises, the present study aimed at exploratively assessing (1) the motor and non-motor correlates of CFT abilities in a cohort of non-demented ALS patients, (2) the capability of CFT measures to discriminate patients from healthy controls (HCs), and (3) patients with vs. without cognitive impairment.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

N = 110 consecutive, El Escorial-diagnosed ALS patients referred to IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milano, Italy, between 2021 and 2022 were consecutively recruited, along with N = 51 healthy controls (HCs) identified via the authors' acquaintances. No patient had a comorbid diagnosis of either behavioral variant-frontotemporal dementia or progressive non-fluent aphasia/semantic dementia. Both patients and HCs were free of (1) other neurological/psychiatric conditions, (2) severe/unstable general medical diseases, and (3) uncorrected vision/hearing deficits. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano (I.D.: 2021_03_23_03); participants provided written informed consent.

2.2 Materials

Both patients and HCs were administered two CFT tasks, i.e., the Counterfactual Inference Test (CIT) (Hooker et al., 2000) and the Spontaneous Counterfactual Generation Test (SCGT) (Roese and Hur, 1997). The CIT requires the examinee to determine who, among two characters having experienced similar, but not identical, circumstances, is more likely to generate “if only”-like thoughts. The CIT is a 4-item, forced-choice task ranging from 0 to 4. By contrast, the SCGT first requires the examinee to recall a past life event and analyze it in detail for 3 min; he/she is then asked to propose alternative conclusions to such an occurrence. The outcome of the SCGT is represented by the number of alternative conclusions provided by the examinee; it is thus an open-range task. Both the CIT and the SCGT have been previously adapted for administration to Italian examinees (Zago et al., 2014; Solca et al., 2015).

Additionally, patients were assessed for global cognition [Edinburgh Cognitive and Behavioral ALS Screen, ECAS (Poletti et al., 2016)], executive [Frontal Assessment Battery, FAB (Appollonio et al., 2005; Aiello et al., 2023a)], social-cognitive functions [Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test, RMET (Aiello et al., 2022; Maddaluno et al., 2022); Emotion Attribution subtest of the Story-Based Empathy Task, SET-EA (Dodich et al., 2015; Aiello et al., 2023b)], behavior [State- and Trait-Anxiety Inventory-Form Y, STAI-Y1/-Y2 (Spielberger et al., 1971); Beck Depression Inventory, BDI (Beck et al., 1961); Beaumont Behavioral Inventory, BBI (Iazzolino et al., 2022); Dimensional Apathy Scale, DAS (Santangelo et al., 2017)], and motor-functional status [ALS Functional Rating Scale-Revised, ALSFRS-R (Cedarbaum et al., 1999)]. Progression rate (ΔFS) was computed as (48-ALSFRS-R score)/disease duration in months (Kimura et al., 2006), and disease staging was retrieved via King's (Roche et al., 2012) and Milano-Torino (MiToS) (Chiò et al., 2015) systems.

2.3 Statistics

Due to the limited ranges of both the CIT and the SCGT, a CFT index (CFTI) was computed as the sum of the two tasks and addressed as the outcome for the analyses, as the two tasks proved to be unrelated both in patients [r(110) = 0.15; p = 0.122] and in HCs [r(51) = −0.05; p = 0.709].

In patients, the association between age/education and sex and CFTI scores was assessed via Pearson's correlations and an independent-sample t-test, respectively, as indexed by both skewness and kurtosis values < |1| and |3|, respectively, as well as by the absence of visual abnormalities within their histogram and Q-Q plot.

By contrast, since the vast majority of remaining measures did not distribute normally according to the abovementioned criteria, non-parametric techniques were adopted to test associations of interest.

More specifically, three different sets of Bonferroni-corrected Spearman's correlations were run to test the association between the CFTI and (1) cognitive (i.e., ECAS-Language/-Fluency/-Executive/-Memory/-Visuospatial scores as well as FAB, RMET, and SET scores), (2) behavioral (i.e., STAI-Y1/-Y2, BDI, BBI, and DAS scores), and (3) motor-functional outcomes (i.e., disease duration in months, ALSFRS-R scores, and ΔFS).

Finally, two logistic regressions were performed in order to test the capability of the CFTI to (1) discriminate patients from HCs and, among the patient cohort, (2) discriminate cognitively impaired patients from cognitively unimpaired ones [i.e., those with a below- vs. above-cutoff ECAS-Total according to current, age- and education-stratified Italian norms (Poletti et al., 2016)]. Since patients and HCs were matched for education [t(159) = 1; p = 0.321] but not for age [t(159) = −6.01; p < 0.001] and sex [χ(1)2 = 10.02; p = 0.002], the last two variables were covaried when running the first model. At variance, patients with a normal (N = 81) vs. defective performance (N = 29) on the ECAS-Total were matched for age [t(108) = 0.28; p = 0.778], education [t(40.31) = −1.42; p = 0.163], and sex [χ(1)2 = 0.24; p = 0.621].

Analyses were run via jamovi 2.3 (https://www.jamovi.org/). Missing data were excluded pairwise.

3 Results

Table 1 summarizes the participants' background and clinical and neuropsychological measures. In patients, CFTI scores were unrelated to age [r(110) = 0.08; p = 0.401], education [r(110) = 0.10; p = 0.302], and sex [t(108) = −0.84; p = 0.402].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants' background and neuropsychological measures.

At αadjusted = 0.017, no associations were detected between CFTI scores and motor-functional outcomes (ps ≥ 0.104). Similarly, at αadjusted = 0.01, the CFTI was unrelated to STAI-Y1, STAI-Y2, BDI, BBI, and DAS scores (ps ≥ 0.160). By contrast, at αadjusted = 0.006, CFTI scores were associated with both ECAS-Fluency [rs(110) = 0.28; p = 0.003] and ECAS-Memory scores [rs(110) = 0.27; p = 0.005], while not with ECAS-Language/-Executive/-Visuospatial, RMET, SET-EA, and FAB scores (ps ≥ 0.074).

The CFTI was able to discriminate patients from HCs [b = −0.50, z = −3.80, p < 0.001; OR = 0.60, CI 95% (0.47, 0.78)] with high accuracy (82%) (Figure 1), but was unable to discriminate patients with vs. without a defective ECAS-Total score [b = 0.19, z = −1.43, p = 0.153; OR = 0.60, CI 95% (0.47, 0.78)].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Box-plot showing the comparisons of the CFTI between ALS patients and HCs. ALS, amyotrophic laterals sclerosis; CFTI, counterfactual thinking index; HCs, healthy controls.

4 Discussion

The present study, for the first time, explored the status of CFT abilities in non-demented ALS patients. CFT measures were herewith found to be selectively associated with verbal fluency and memory functions, while being independent of demographic, motor-functional, and behavioral features.

Such findings are, in the first place, likely to be accounted for by the nature of the tasks herewith adopted for measuring such a construct. Indeed, both the CIT and the SCGT require, albeit to different extents, to recall and generate verbal items—similarly to verbal fluency and verbal memory tasks included within the ECAS-Fluency and the ECAS-Memory, respectively. At the same time, they appear to align with the notion of executive and memory functions being related to CFT abilities (De Brigard and Parikh, 2019). However, in this respect, the non-negligible, inherent overlap between the current CFT measures and the tasks enclosed within the ECAS-Fluency/-Memory—both requiring the elicitation of reconstructive-recalling processes and relying on verbal responses—suggests that the associations herewith detected might be of a spurious nature. Hence, further investigations are needed in order to elucidate the cognitive underpinnings of CFT abilities in ALS.

Notably, the present results suggest that CFT abilities in this population are not accounted for by executive-attentive processes per se—this is in contrast with previous evidence reported in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease patients (McNamara et al., 2003; Solca et al., 2015). Such a discrepancy might be accounted for by the fact that Parkinson's and Huntington's disease are by default characterized by a primary neurodegeneration of networks subserving cognitive functions; by contrast, cognitive involvement in ALS is not ubiquitous since it appears only when extra-motor regions are involved.

As to the lack of associations between CFT measures and both motor-functional and behavioral features, it might be hypothesized that such findings are due to the fact that the patients herewith enrolled were in the early stages of the disease.

Interestingly, CFT measures were shown to be able to accurately discriminate non-demented ALS patients from HCs, suggesting that CFT abilities might be, at least to some extent, defective in this population. Nevertheless, the unavailability of Italian normative data for the CIT and the SCGT prevented estimating the actual prevalence of CFT deficits within the present cohort.

At the same time, the CFTI herewith proved to be unable to differentiate cognitively impaired from unimpaired patients. Hence, it might be postulated that the decrease in CFT abilities in this population is independent of the presence of overall cognitive impairment. In this respect, at least two putative explanations can be brought up. The first relates to the fact that the proportion of cognitive impairment within the present cohort was lower when compared to those commonly accepted within the relevant literature (i.e., up to 50%) (Murphy et al., 2016; Strong et al., 2017). This might have reduced the statistical power of the concerning comparison, thus confounding a potentially significant difference. As to the second, a measurement-related issue might be taken into account: indeed, cognition has been herewith assessed via a first-level test (i.e., the ECAS). It cannot be ruled out that a significant difference in CFT abilities might have been detected if function-/domain-specific tests had been employed for classifying patients' cognitive status. Thus, further investigations are needed that address a larger and more deeply phenotyped sample of patients in order to determine whether CFT measures could discriminate between ALS patients with and without cognitive dysfunctions. Relatedly, future studies should also include ALS patients with comorbid frontotemporal dementia.

Finally, a number of limitations of this study need to be highlighted. First, it is worth noting that the current investigation mostly embraced the “positive” polarity of CFT—i.e., its functional effects in terms of behavior regulation. However, it has been postulated that the excessive generation of counterfactuals might be linked to ruminative thoughts, which in turn might lead to anxiety and depressive symptoms (Epstude and Roese, 2008). Hence, while no associations were herewith detected between the CFTI and BDI/STAI-Y scores, it is advisable that future studies also focus on exploring whether excessive CFT might negatively contribute to ALS patients' psychological wellbeing. Second, it has to be borne in mind that the correlations herewith run for testing the association between CFT measures and cognitive scores have not been covaried for potentially relevant confounders, such as behavioral, psychopathological, and motor-functional features. At variance, the association between such confounders and the CFTI has been separately tested within different correlational sets. Although this was the aim of this explorative investigation, future studies are advisable that focus on exploring the determinants of CFT abilities in these patients by employing multivariate models. A third issue then lies in the fact that the patient cohort was much larger when compared to the HP group; this could have led, at least to some extent, to biases in the results of between-group comparisons, thus calling for future investigations to be adequately balanced in such terms.

Overall, while the present study is intended to be exploratory, it prompts further research aimed to determine whether CFT measures convey clinical entailments in non-demented ALS patients. Future studies are indeed advised to focus on assessing whether CFT abilities in this population are linked to decision-making and adherence within care settings, especially within the context of advanced care planning.

In conclusion, CFT abilities in non-demented ALS patients are associated with verbal fluency and memory functions; moreover, CFT measures are able to discriminate such patients from HCs.

Data availability statement

Raw data have been stored on an online repository (https://zenodo.org/records/10159681) and can be made available upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano (I.D.: 2021_03_23_03). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

BP: Conceptualization, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EA: Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. STa: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. FSo: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. STo: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. EC: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. AM: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. RB: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. FSc: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. CM: Data curation, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. AD: Data curation, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. FV: Data curation, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SB: Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. FM: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. AP: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. RF: Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. VS: Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. PC: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. GP: Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. NT: Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by Italian Ministry of Health - Ricerca Corrente.

Acknowledgments

The authors are thankful to patients and their caregivers.

Conflict of interest

VS received compensation for consulting services and/or speaking activities from AveXis, Cytokinetics, Italfarmaco, Liquidweb S.r.l., Novartis Pharma AG and Zambon, receives or has received research supports from the Italian Ministry of Health, AriSLA, and E-Rare Joint Transnational Call. He is in the Editorial Board of Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis and Frontotemporal Degeneration, European Neurology, American Journal of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Frontiers in Neurology, and Exploration of Neuroprotective Therapy. BP received compensation for consulting services and/or speaking activities from Liquidweb S.r.l. BP is Associate Editor for Frontiers in Neuroscience. NT received compensation for consulting services from Amylyx Pharmaceuticals and Zambon Biotech SA. He is Associate Editor for Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aiello, E. N., Carelli, L., Solca, F., Torre, S., Ferrucci, R., Priori, A., et al. (2022). Validity and diagnostics of the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) in non-demented amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) patients. Front. Psychol. 13, 1031841. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1031841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aiello, E. N., Solca, F., Torre, S., Carelli, L., Ferrucci, R., Priori, A., et al. (2023a). Feasibility and diagnostics of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurol. Sci. 44, 587–592. doi: 10.1007/s10072-022-06438-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aiello, E. N., Solca, F., Torre, S., Colombo, E., Maranzano, A., Olivero, M., et al. (2023b). Clinical usability of the Story-Based Empathy Task (SET) in non-demented ALS patients. Neurol. Sci. 44, 3181–3187. doi: 10.1007/s10072-023-06791-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appollonio, I., Leone, M., Isella, V., Piamarta, F., Consoli, T., Villa, M. L., et al. (2005). The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB): normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol. Sci. 26, 108–116. doi: 10.1007/s10072-005-0443-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cedarbaum, J. M., Stambler, N., Malta, E., Fuller, C., Hilt, D., Thurmond, B., et al. (1999). The ALSFRS-R: a revised ALS functional rating scale that incorporates assessments of respiratory function. J. Neurol. Sci. 169, 13–21. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(99)00210-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiò, A., Hammond, E. R., Mora, G., Bonito, V., and Filippini, G. (2015). Development and evaluation of a clinical staging system for amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 86, 38–44. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-306589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Brigard, F., Addis, D. R., Ford, J. H., Schacter, D. L., and Giovanello, K. S. (2013). Remembering what could have happened: neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking. Neuropsychologia 51, 2401–2414. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Brigard, F., and Parikh, N. (2019). Episodic counterfactual thinking. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 28, 59–66. doi: 10.1177/0963721418806512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Brigard, F., Spreng, R. N., Mitchell, J. P., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Neural activity associated with self, other, and object-based counterfactual thinking. Neuroimage 109, 12–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodich, A., Cerami, C., Canessa, N., Crespi, C., Iannaccone, S., Marcone, A., et al. (2015). A novel task assessing intention and emotion attribution: Italian standardization and normative data of the Story-based Empathy Task. Neurol. Sci. 36, 1907–1912. doi: 10.1007/s10072-015-2281-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstude, K., and Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 168–192. doi: 10.1177/1088868308316091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hooker, C., Roese, N. J., and Park, S. (2000). Impoverished counterfactual thinking is associated with schizophrenia. Psychiatry 63, 326–335. doi: 10.1080/00332747.2000.11024925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huynh, W., Ahmed, R., Mahoney, C. J., Nguyen, C., Tu, S., Caga, J., et al. (2020). The impact of cognitive and behavioral impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Expert Rev. Neurotherap. 20, 281–293. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2020.1727740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iazzolino, B., Pain, D., Laura, P., Aiello, E. N., Gallucci, M., Radici, A., et al. (2022). Italian adaptation of the Beaumont Behavioral Inventory (BBI): psychometric properties and clinical usability. Amyotr. Lateral Scler. Frontotemp. Degener. 23, 81–86. doi: 10.1080/21678421.2021.1946085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimura, F., Fujimura, C., Ishida, S., Nakajima, H., Furutama, D., Uehara, H., et al. (2006). Progression rate of ALSFRS-R at time of diagnosis predicts survival time in ALS. Neurology 66, 265–267 doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000194316.91908.8a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maddaluno, O., Aiello, E. N., Roncoroni, C., Prunas, A., and Bolognini, N. (2022). The Reading the mind in the eyes test, Iowa gambling task and interpersonal reactivity index: normative data in an Italian population sample. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 37, 929–938. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acab100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNamara, P., Durso, R., Brown, A., and Lynch, A. (2003). Counterfactual cognitive deficit in persons with Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74, 1065–1070. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.8.1065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J., Factor-Litvak, P., Goetz, R., Lomen-Hoerth, C., Nagy, P. L., Hupf, J., et al. (2016). Cognitive-behavioral screening reveals prevalent impairment in a large multicenter ALS cohort. Neurology 86, 813–820. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poletti, B., Solca, F., Carelli, L., Madotto, F., Lafronza, A., Faini, A., et al. (2016). The validation of the Italian Edinburgh cognitive and behavioural ALS screen (ECAS). Amyotr. Lateral Scler. Frontotemp. Degener. 17, 489–498. doi: 10.1080/21678421.2016.1183679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roche, J. C., Rojas-Garcia, R., Scott, K. M., Scotton, W., Ellis, C. E., Burman, R., et al. (2012). A proposed staging system for amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Brain 135, 847–852. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychol. Bull. 121, 133–148. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roese, N. J., and Hur, T. (1997). Affective determinants of counterfactual thinking. Soc. Cogn. 15, 274–290. doi: 10.1521/soco.1997.15.4.274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santangelo, G., Raimo, S., Siciliano, M., D'Iorio, A., Piscopo, F., Cuoco, S., et al. (2017). Assessment of apathy independent of physical disability: validation of the Dimensional Apathy Scale in Italian healthy sample. Neurol. Sci. 38, 303–309. doi: 10.1007/s10072-016-2766-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solca, F., Poletti, B., Zago, S., Crespi, C., Sassone, F., Lafronza, A., et al. (2015). Counterfactual thinking deficit in Huntington's disease. PLoS ONE 10, e0126773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gonzalez-Reigosa, F., Martinez-Urrutia, A., Natalicio, L. F., and Natalicio, D. S. (1971). The state-trait anxiety inventory. Inter Am. J. Psychol. 5, 145–158.

Google Scholar

Strong, M. J., Abrahams, S., Goldstein, L. H., Woolley, S., Mclaughlin, P., Snowden, J., et al. (2017). Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis-frontotemporal spectrum disorder (ALS-FTSD): Revised diagnostic criteria. Amyotr. Lateral Scler. Frontotemp. Degener. 18, 153–174. doi: 10.1080/21678421.2016.1267768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tagini, S., Solca, F., Torre, S., Brugnera, A., Ciammola, A., Mazzocco, K., et al. (2021). Counterfactual thinking in psychiatric and neurological diseases: a scoping review. PLoS ONE 16, e0246388. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0246388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Hoeck, N., Ma, N., Ampe, L., Baetens, K., Vandekerckhove, M., and Van Overwalle, F. (2013). Counterfactual thinking: an fMRI study on changing the past for a better future. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 556–564. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zago, S., Delli Ponti, A., Mastroianni, S., Solca, F., Tomasini, E., Poletti, B., et al. (2014). Counterfactual thinking in Tourette's syndrome: a study using three measures. Behav. Neurol. 2014, 256089. doi: 10.1155/2014/256089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: counterfactual thinking, cognition, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, frontotemporal degeneration, neuropsychology, dementia

Citation: Poletti B, Aiello EN, Tagini S, Solca F, Torre S, Colombo E, Maranzano A, Bonetti R, Schevegher F, Morelli C, Doretti A, Verde F, Barbieri S, Mameli F, Priori A, Ferrucci R, Silani V, Cherubini P, Pravettoni G and Ticozzi N (2023) An exploratory study on counterfactual thinking in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Front. Psychol. 14:1281976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1281976

Received: 23 August 2023; Accepted: 31 October 2023;
Published: 04 December 2023.

Edited by:

Sara Palermo, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Fausto Viader, Université de Caen Normandie, France
Graziella Orrù, University of Pisa, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Poletti, Aiello, Tagini, Solca, Torre, Colombo, Maranzano, Bonetti, Schevegher, Morelli, Doretti, Verde, Barbieri, Mameli, Priori, Ferrucci, Silani, Cherubini, Pravettoni and Ticozzi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Barbara Poletti, b.poletti@auxologico.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work

ORCID: Barbara Poletti orcid.org/0000-0003-4398-2051

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.