Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 21 December 2023
Sec. Psychopathology

Agency in schizophrenia and autism: a systematic review

  • 1Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 2Monash Centre for Consciousness and Contemplative Studies, School of Philosophical, Historical and International Studies, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 3Centre for Women’s and Children’s Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry, School of Clinical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

Introduction: Previous research suggests that altered experiences of agency are an underlying vulnerability in both schizophrenia and autism. Here, we explore agency as a potential transdiagnostic factor by conducting a systematic review of existing literature investigating agency in autism and schizophrenia individually and together.

Methods: Following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, we conducted three systematic searches on PsycINFO, Embase, Medline, PubMed and Web of Science to identify studies that investigated (1) agency in schizophrenia, (2) agency in autism, and (3) agency in both schizophrenia and autism.

Results: A total of 31 articles met eligibility criteria for inclusion and data extraction, with 24 measuring agency in schizophrenia, 7 investigating agency in autism, and no articles comparing the two. Results show that, compared to control populations, agency is significantly different in every identified schizophrenia study and generally not significantly different in autism.

Discussion: Importantly, we identified a lack of studies using common tasks and a disproportionate number of studies investigating different dimensions of agency across the two conditions, resulting in limited grounds for valid comparison.

Systematic review registration: Prospero, CRD42021273373.

1 Introduction

Experiences of agency include concurrent feelings of control over one’s actions and their sensory consequences (sense of agency) and a retrospective report of “I did that” (judgment of agency) (Synofzik et al., 2008; Moore, 2016). Altered experiences of agency are associated with psychiatric conditions and often result in significant functional impairments and distress (Balconi, 2010; Hur et al., 2014). Agency has been identified as a key transdiagnostic construct under the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC).

Schizophrenia and autism are two (of several) psychological conditions that are thought to be associated with altered experiences of agency (Hur et al., 2014; Sperduti et al., 2014; Zalla and Sperduti, 2015). However, it is unclear to what extent these alterations represent changes to common processes or distinct differences in each condition. Some recent studies have explicitly claimed that these two conditions are diametrically opposed in sense of self including embodiment and agency (Crespi and Dinsdale, 2019; Benítez-Burraco et al., 2021). Further, autism and schizophrenia have a shared history, with autism first being defined as a part of schizophrenia, as the symptom of being withdrawn in the DSM-I and DSM-II, and only becoming its own condition in the DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1952, 1968, 1981). As such, while agency is proposed as a transdiagnostic factor which may be used to distinguish a range of psychiatric conditions, comparing the evidence for agency differences across these two conditions is an important step in developing a broader multidimensional transdiagnostic approach.

If autism and schizophrenia represent polar ends of an agency spectrum, this may serve as a diagnostic boundary and inform treatment decisions in clinical practice. Should the agency alterations be similar instead, altered agency may be a factor shared between schizophrenia and autism. This may inform future research into the underlying mechanisms of these conditions. However, the strength of the evidence supporting these alternatives is yet to be assessed.

Furthermore, it is unknown whether both literatures have addressed the same experiences of agency. Research investigating sense of agency tends to employ implicit measures while research investigating judgment of agency tend to rely on explicit measures (Moore, 2016). For example, the intentional binding paradigm (Haggard et al., 2003) is an implicit measure that uses the shortening of the perceived time interval between a self-generated action and its consequence to indirectly estimate one’s sense of agency. A recent transdiagnostic review of intentional binding showed differences in intentional binding in schizophrenia and its spectrum, but inconsistent findings in autism (Moccia et al., 2023). Meanwhile, explicit measures of agency can be used in conjunction, requiring the participant to directly self-report their agency in words, or using a Likert scale (Moore, 2016). Siebertz and Jansen (2022) found that two distinct intentional binding paradigms generated divergent outcomes in terms of sense of agency. This highlights a critical need to not only distinguish which agentic experience is being investigated, but also the type of measures used in each body of research.

To date, no studies have systematically reviewed the empirical literature across experimental paradigms for evidence regarding different factors that contribute to experiences of agency within these conditions or transdiagnostically for any set of conditions. Notably, a preliminary screen suggests a comparatively low number of autism studies, as such, a scoping review is warranted to provide an assessment of the current state of the literature to inform further research. Hence, this study aims to determine whether individuals with schizophrenia and autism demonstrate altered agency, examine the availability of evidence comparing agency in autism and schizophrenia and whether the independent literatures provide comparable evidence to inform transdiagnostic goals.

2 Methods

This scoping review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (PRISMA-P Group et al., 2015). The methods were documented in a protocol that was registered and published on the PROSPERO database (CRD42021273373). No informed consent was required.

The searches were conducted across the electronic databases PsycINFO, Embase, Medline, PubMed and Web of Science, and included only studies written in English in peer-reviewed journals. The first database search was conducted on 26 July 2021, and was repeated on 8 May 2023, indicating three new articles for review. Three separate searches were conducted to identify studies that investigated (1) agency in schizophrenia, (2) agency in autism, and (3) agency in both autism and schizophrenia (see Supplementary material for search terms and screening criteria). In brief, to be included, studies needed to include human participants in a diagnosed clinical group and include a neurotypical comparison group in a case-controlled design, outcomes were required to assess the participant’s own judgment/sense/feeling of agency behaviorally (implicitly or explicitly but not solely using biological proxies). See Supplementary material for search strategy details and complete eligibility and selection criteria. The screening process, comprising stages (1) pilot (2) title and abstract screening, and (3) full-text screening, for eligibility was performed independently by DT and DM using Covidence, with conflicts resolved by KP. The resultant papers yielded from screening were then appraised (DT and DM) for the internal and external validity of outcomes using a quality appraisal checklist (The National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence, 2012). See Figures 1, 2 for the PRISMA flow diagrams.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram representing the selection of studies included in review of agency in schizophrenia, autism and both populations (as at 26 July 2021).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PRISMA flow diagram summarizing update of studies included in review of agency in schizophrenia, autism and both populations between 2021 and 8 May 2023.

3 Results

A total of 28 of 1,239 retrieved articles met eligibility criteria for inclusion and extraction of data at the initial search (July 2021), and 3 were added when the search was updated in May of 2023, for a total of 31 eligible articles. Of these articles, 24 investigated agency in schizophrenia, 7 investigated agency in autism, and no articles directly compared the two. A wide range of tasks were employed to measure agency with little overlap in types of measures used across conditions (Figure 3). The most common task used in schizophrenia was the intentional binding task or similar implicit variants of temporal estimation tasks (implicit) and in autism it was the squares task (explicit). The intentional binding task was the sole common task across groups. It should be noted that while classically the rubber hand illusion measures sense of ownership, which is distinct from sense of agency, the rubber hand illusion studies identified were included as part of this study because they included an explicit or implicit measure of agency alongside the classic ownership paradigm. See complete data extracted from reviewed articles in Table 1 for the Schizophrenia search and Table 2 for the Autism search.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. This figure provides an overview of the results indicating a disproportionately greater number of agency studies in schizophrenia than autism, with no studies investigating agency in both conditions. The results are further categorized into type of task, and whether they were implicit or explicit measures. The figure also reveals results of individual studies investigating whether agency was increased, decreased, different, or not different when comparing clinical and control groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of findings from primary outcome measures and participant characteristics relevant to agency in Schizophrenia.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of findings from primary outcome measures and participant characteristics relevant to agency in Autism.

Overall, none of the studies comparing schizophrenia and controls found statistically indistinguishable performance on any agency task, with the most common results (10 of 24) indicating increased agency in participants with schizophrenia (i.e., over-attributed to themselves) relative to controls. A closer inspection suggested that such over-attribution may be associated with a subset of positive symptoms where participants with schizophrenia overestimate the causal reach of their actions in the external world (Voss et al., 2010; Maeda et al., 2012, 2013; Hur et al., 2014; Werner et al., 2014; cf. Foerster et al., 2021). Negative symptom presentations (Maeda et al., 2013) and passivity symptoms (i.e., delusions of alien control) were generally associated with reduced attribution of agency, as seen in 6 out of 24 articles (Graham-Schmidt et al., 2014, 2016, 2018). Additionally, the rubber hand illusion and related delayed feedback paradigms consistently show reduced agency in schizophrenic groups compared to controls (Graham-Schmidt et al., 2014, 2016, 2018; Foerster et al., 2021; Krugwasser et al., 2022; cf. Rossetti et al., 2020).

In studies investigating sense of agency in diagnosed autistic individuals, two of the seven studies showed altered agency (Zalla et al., 2015), with one of these specifically finding reduced agency in autism (Sperduti et al., 2014). The five remaining studies suggest no difference in agency between autistic and control participants.

No study directly compared schizophrenia and autism on measures of agency.

4 Discussion

This scoping review revealed that no study has directly compared agency between schizophrenia and autism. The types of measures used in schizophrenia are heterogenous, using a broad range of implicit and explicit measures of both sense and judgment of agency, while the autism literature is predominated by explicit measures of judgment.

The results indicate that agency is altered in schizophrenia, with more studies finding reports of both increased and reduced agency relative to control participants. This apparent contradiction may reflect difference in symptomatology (positive vs. negative symptoms) or stage of illness progression. Evidence suggests that people exhibiting increased agency are more likely to experience positive symptoms like delusions of reference, paranoia/persecution, and control over external objects, where they overestimate the causality of their actions in the external world (Haggard et al., 2003; Schimansky et al., 2010; Garbarini et al., 2016, cf. Foerster et al., 2021). In support of this explanation, this review identified four studies that focused on patients with predominantly paranoid-type delusions and consistently reported excessive agency (Voss et al., 2010; Maeda et al., 2012, 2013; Werner et al., 2014).

Papers that reported reduced agency in schizophrenia suggested two candidate explanations for this pattern of results. On one hand, people exhibiting reduced agency may be more likely to experience a different subset of positive symptoms than those showing increased agency including delusions of being controlled and thought insertion, underestimating the causality of their actions to themselves rather than external stimuli (Fourneret et al., 2001). This was observed in three papers reporting reduced agency that investigated patients experiencing predominantly passivity symptoms, referring to delusions of alien control/ being controlled by others (Graham-Schmidt et al., 2014, 2016, 2018). On the other hand, our synthesis also showed that patients with predominantly negative symptoms generally have reduced agency. Notably, Maeda et al. (2013) was the first and only study to date that investigated agency in schizophrenia comparing patients with predominantly negative symptoms and paranoid-type symptoms.

A possible explanation for this mixed/inconsistent association between symptomatology and agentic experience is the relationship between stage of illness progression and severity of altered agency. Although the number of studies investigating this relationship are limited, three studies have suggested a unified story that agency alterations increase across stage of disorder from prodromal ultra-high risk (where negative symptoms and attenuated positive symptoms predominate the clinical presentation) to the acute phase of first-episode psychosis (where previously attenuated symptoms have crystallized into full-blown positive and negative psychotic symptoms) (Wilquin and Delevoye-Turrell, 2012; Kozáková et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2020). The severity of altered agency appears related to the severity of positive symptoms reflected by stage of illness. A 1-year follow study by Kozáková et al. (2020) further suggests that the severity of agency alterations is state-dependent and can attenuate as patients enters the remissive phase of the illness, which is often characterized by reduced psychotic symptoms and improved functioning. As such, it appears that severity of altered agency is more related to the severity of positive symptoms. However, given the paucity of studies examining this relationship, it will be important for future research to consider the stage of illness, comparisons of symptomatology across stages and longitudinal follow-ups. Together with the above findings, the overall finding is that agency is generally over-attributed in schizophrenia and is generalized to patients with predominantly positive symptoms that may reflect stage of illness.

Only a small number of studies have examined agency in autism. Within these, 5 studies suggested that agency is not different, and only 2 suggested that agency is altered, with 1 indicating reduced agency in autism compared to neurotypical participants (Sperduti et al., 2014). This pattern of results stands in contrast to assumptions that agency is altered in autism (Sperduti et al., 2014; van Laarhoven et al., 2019), possibly because the full range of agentive processes have not been studied (Zalla and Sperduti, 2015; Perrykkad and Hohwy, 2020). Of note, Finnemann et al. (2021) focused on agency in autism, but also found higher schizotypal traits in an autistic group compared to neurotypical controls. This reinforces a further need to consider diagnostic and trait-based overlaps when using dimensional approaches to distinguish autism and schizophrenia.

Overall, these findings suggest that agency in autism and schizophrenia are neither diametrically opposed nor decidedly similar in alteration, in contrast to such claims in the literature (Crespi and Dinsdale, 2019; Benítez-Burraco et al., 2021). Differences in self-processing in schizophrenia but not in autism is consistent with recent findings showing a significantly stronger association between self-concept clarity and schizotypy than with autism traits (Perrykkad and Hohwy, 2022). Further, there is little ground for comparison in the existing literature as not only have no studies compared agency in autism and schizophrenia directly, but there is also little overlap in the measures used in the two conditions.

Possible limitations of the current review include missing any articles that did not appear in the search results using the selected terms at the time of the searches, but otherwise appeared to fit the inclusion and exclusion criteria, including Franck et al. (2005). The findings of this study, showing increased temporal binding for some conditions (though notably not the agentive condition) for a diagnosed schizophrenia group compared to control participants are consistent with findings of the studies included in this review (Franck et al., 2005). Another limitation is the focus on only behavioral results and the exclusion of findings based on neural data. While neural data is indeed important, especially for mechanism discovery, psychological disorders remain defined clinically by behavioral symptomatology and clinician-patient interactions, so we focused here on behavioral and self-report findings. In the future, when inevitably more research has been done to fill some of the gaps identified by this review, researchers should consider repeating a review like this for a wider range of agency-related disorders, such as functional movement disorders, and a broader range of levels of analysis (such as those described by the RDoC matrix including for instance neurotransmitters, systems neuroscience, behavior, and self-report).

Taken together, the review highlights an urgent and clear need to develop harmonized ways of assessing agency. Future research needs to either develop a gold-standard comprehensive agency task, or to use a battery of comparable tasks when exploring agency within and across conditions. Agreed-upon measure(s) of agency will undeniably provide a stronger foundation for identifying similarities and differences across disorders. In our view, an ideal task (or battery of tasks) would involve: (i) a cognitive task contrasting an active condition with a passive condition to facilitate quantification of over- and under-attribution of agency, (ii) in predictable and uncertain conditions to compare baseline best-case-scenario performance and simulate the complexities of everyday inferences, (iii) incorporating agency an implicit measure (the best likely candidate being a temporal perception measure, though alternatives exist, e.g., neural measures), (iv) as well as explicit questions about the attribution of agency to task actions and events. This should be supported by generalised questions about agentive experience in daily life, such as the Sense of Agency Scale questionnaire (Tapal et al., 2017). Together, a measure with these features would create a rich picture to uncover the differences in agency processes and experience between groups. Further, future research investigating different dimensions of agency, across clinical stages, and using longitudinal data will clarify if altered agency is a useful transdiagnostic dimension that reflects of illness progression, and may contribute to identifying a set of informative psychiatric traits in the healthy spectrum.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

DT: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – original draft. OC: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. D-RM: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. KP: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1280622/full#supplementary-material

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1952). DSM-I diagnostic and statistical manual: Mental disorders (1 ed.). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (1968). DSM-II diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (1981). DSM-III: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington: APA.

Google Scholar

Balconi, M. (2010). “Neuropsychology of the sense of agency: from consciousness to action” in Neuropsychology of the sense of agency: From consciousness to action (Springer-Verlag Publishing).

Google Scholar

Benítez-Burraco, A., Adornetti, I., Ferretti, F., and Progovac, L. (2021). An evolutionary account of impairment of self in cognitive disorders. Cogn. Process. 24, 107–127. doi: 10.1007/s10339-022-01110-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulot, V., Thomas, P., and Delevoye-Turrell, Y. (2007). A pre-reflective indicator of an impaired sense of agency in patients with schizophrenia. Exp. Brain Res. 183, 115–126. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1027-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespi, B., and Dinsdale, N. (2019). Autism and psychosis as diametrical disorders of embodiment. Evol. Med. Public Health 2019, 121–138. doi: 10.1093/emph/eoz021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

David, N., Gawronski, A., Santos, N. S., Huff, W., Lehnhardt, F. G., Newen, A., et al. (2008). Dissociation between key processes of social cognition in autism: impaired mentalizing but intact sense of agency. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 593–605. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0425-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finnemann, J. J. S., Plaisted-Grant, K., Moore, J., Teufel, C., and Fletcher, P. C. (2021). Low-level, prediction-based sensory and motor processes are unimpaired in autism. Neuropsychologia 156:107835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foerster, F. R., Weibel, S., Poncelet, P., Dufour, A., Delevoye-Turrell, Y. N., Capobianco, A., et al. (2021). Volatility of subliminal haptic feedback alters the feeling of control in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 130, 775–784. doi: 10.1037/abn0000703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fourneret, P., Franck, N., Slachevsky, A., and Jeannerod, M. (2001). Self-monitoring in schizophrenia revisited. Available at: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=sso&db=edsbl&AN=RN094352617&site=eds-live&scope=site&custid=s2775460

Google Scholar

Franck, N., Posada, A., Pichon, S., and Haggard, P. (2005). Altered subjective time of events in schizophrenia. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 193, 350–353. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000161699.76032.09

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garbarini, F., Mastropasqua, A., Sigaudo, M., Rabuffetti, M., Piedimonte, A., Pia, L., et al. (2016). Abnormal sense of agency in patients with schizophrenia: evidence from bimanual coupling paradigm. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:43. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham-Schmidt, K. T., Martin-Iverson, M. T., Holmes, N. P., Jablensky, A., and Waters, F. (2014). Deficits in agency in schizophrenia, and additional deficits in body image, body schema, and internal timing, in passivity symptoms. Front. Psych. 5:126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham-Schmidt, K. T., Martin-Iverson, M. T., Holmes, N. P., and Waters, F. A. (2016). When one's sense of agency goes wrong: absent modulation of time perception by voluntary actions and reduction of perceived length of intervals in passivity symptoms in schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. Int. J. 45, 9–23. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham-Schmidt, K. T., Martin-Iverson, M. T., and Waters, F. A. (2018). Self- and other-agency in people with passivity (first rank) symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 192, 75–81. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, C., Williams, D. M., and Lind, S. E. (2014). Online action monitoring and memory for self-performed actions in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 1193–1206. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1987-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggard, P., Martin, F., Taylor-Clarke, M., Jeannerod, M., and Franck, N. (2003). Awareness of action in schizophrenia. NeuroReport: for rapid communication of. Neurosci. Res. 14, 1081–1085. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200305230-00035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauser, M., Knoblich, G., Repp, B. H., Lautenschlager, M., Gallinat, J., Heinz, A., et al. (2011). Altered sense of agency in schizophrenia and the putative psychotic prodrome. Psychiatry Res. 186, 170–176. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hur, J. W., Kwon, J. S., Lee, T. Y., and Park, S. (2014). The crisis of minimal self-awareness in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr. Res. 152, 58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koreki, A., Maeda, T., Fukushima, H., Umeda, S., Takahata, K., Okimura, T., et al. (2015). Behavioral evidence of delayed prediction signals during agency attribution in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 230, 78–83. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozáková, E., Bakštein, E., Havlíček, O., Bečev, O., Knytl, P., Zaytseva, Y., et al. (2020). Disrupted sense of agency as a state marker of first-episode schizophrenia: a large-scale follow-up study. Front. Psych. 11:570570. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.570570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krugwasser, A. R., Stern, Y., Faivre, N., Harel, E. V., and Salomon, R. (2022). Impaired sense of agency and associated confidence in psychosis. Schizophrenia 8:32. doi: 10.1038/s41537-022-00212-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeda, T., Kato, M., Muramatsu, T., Iwashita, S., Mimura, M., and Kashima, H. (2012). Aberrant sense of agency in patients with schizophrenia: forward and backward over-attribution of temporal causality during intentional action. Psychiatry Res. 198, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeda, T., Takahata, K., Muramatsu, T., Okimura, T., Koreki, A., Iwashita, S., et al. (2013). Reduced sense of agency in chronic schizophrenia with predominant negative symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 209, 386–392. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metcalfe, J., Van Snellenberg, J. X., DeRosse, P., Balsam, P., and Malhotra, A. K. (2012). Judgements of agency in schizophrenia: an impairment in autonoetic metacognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 1391–1400. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moccia, L., di Luzio, M., Conte, E., Modica, M., Ambrosecchia, M., Ardizzi, M., et al. (2023). Sense of agency and its disturbances: a systematic review targeting the intentional binding effect in neuropsychiatric disorders. Psych. Clin. Neurosci. doi: 10.1111/pcn.13601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. (2016). What is the sense of agency and why does it matter? Front. Psychol. 7:272. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, B., Lavoie, S., Gaweda, L., Li, E., Sass, L., Koren, D., et al. (2020). The neurophenomenology of early psychosis: an integrative empirical study. Conscious. Cogn. Int. J. 77:102845. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrykkad, K., and Hohwy, J. (2020). Modelling me, modelling you: the autistic self. Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 7, 1–31. doi: 10.1007/s40489-019-00173-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrykkad, K., and Hohwy, J. (2022). How selves differ within and across cognitive domains: self-prioritisation, self-concept, and psychiatric traits. BMC Psychol. 10:165. doi: 10.1186/s40359-022-00870-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

PRISMA-P GroupMoher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 4, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyasik, M., Capozzi, F., Sigaudo, M., Cardillo, S., Pia, L., Rocca, P., et al. (2019). I do not know whether you did that: abnormal implicit attribution of social causality in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 210, 291–293. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.12.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renes, R. A., Vermeulen, L., Kahn, R. S., Aarts, H., and van Haren, N. E. (2013). Abnormalities in the establishment of feeling of self-agency in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 143, 50–54. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossetti, I., Repossi, M., Florio, V., Demartini, B., Conca, A., Gambini, O., et al. (2022). Sense of body ownership and body agency in schizophrenia. Sci. Rep. 12:19369. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-23988-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossetti, I., Romano, D., Florio, V., Doria, S., Nistico, V., Conca, A., et al. (2020). Defective embodiment of alien hand uncovers altered sensorimotor integration in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 46, 294–302. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbz050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J., and Hill, E. L. (2001). Action-monitoring and intention reporting in children with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 317–328. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schimansky, J., David, N., Rossler, W., and Haker, H. (2010). Sense of agency and mentalizing: dissociation of subdomains of social cognition in patients with schizophrenia [empirical study; quantitative study]. Psychiatry Res. 178, 39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siebertz, M., and Jansen, P. (2022). Diverging implicit measurement of sense of agency using interval estimation and Libet clock. Conscious. Cogn. 99:103287. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2022.103287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperduti, M., Pieron, M., Leboyer, M., and Zalla, T. (2014). Altered pre-reflective sense of agency in autism spectrum disorders as revealed by reduced intentional binding. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 343–352. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1891-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., and Newen, A. (2008). Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Conscious. Cogn. Int. J. 17, 219–239. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapal, A., Oren, E., Dar, R., and Eitam, B. (2017). The sense of agency scale: a measure of consciously perceived control over One's mind, body, and the immediate environment. Front. Psychol. 8:1552. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. (2012). Appendix G Quality Appraisal Checklist – Quantitative Studies reporting correlations and associations: Methods for the development of Nice Public Health Guidance (third edition). Available at: https://www.nice.org.uk/process/pmg4/chapter/appendix-g-quality-appraisal-checklist-quantitative-studies-reporting-correlations-and (Accessed September 26, 2012).

Google Scholar

Van Haren, N., Van Der Weiden, A., Aarts, H., and Prikken, M. (2019). Sense of ownership and sense of agency in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bull. Conf. 45:S107. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbz022.046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Laarhoven, T., Stekelenburg, J. J., Eussen, M. L. J. M., and Vroomen, J. (2019). Electrophysiological alterations in motor-auditory predictive coding in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. Off. J. Int. Soc. Autism Res. 12, 589–599. doi: 10.1002/aur.2087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M., Moore, J., Hauser, M., Gallinat, J., Heinz, A., and Haggard, P. (2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: a specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain: a. J. Neurol. 133, 3104–3112. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, J. D., Trapp, K., Wustenberg, T., and Voss, M. (2014). Self-attribution bias during continuous action-effect monitoring in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 152, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D., and Happé, F. (2009). Pre-conceptual aspects of self-awareness in autism spectrum disorder: the case of action-monitoring. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 251–259. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0619-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilquin, H., and Delevoye-Turrell, Y. (2012). Motor agency: a new and highly sensitive measure to reveal agency disturbances in early psychosis. PLoS One 7:e30449. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalla, T., Miele, D., Leboyer, M., and Metcalfe, J. (2015). Metacognition of agency and theory of mind in adults with high functioning autism. Conscious. Cogn. 31, 126–138. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalla, T., and Sperduti, M. (2015). The sense of agency in autism spectrum disorders: a dissociation between prospective and retrospective mechanisms? Front. Psychol. 6:1278. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sense of agency, judgment of agency, transdiagnostic, schizophrenia, autism

Citation: Tan DPW, Carter O, Marshall D-R and Perrykkad K (2023) Agency in schizophrenia and autism: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1280622. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1280622

Received: 21 August 2023; Accepted: 04 December 2023;
Published: 21 December 2023.

Edited by:

Massimo Tusconi, University of Cagliari, Italy

Reviewed by:

Philip Gerrans, University of Adelaide, Australia
Marianna Ambrosecchia, University of Parma, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Tan, Carter, Marshall and Perrykkad. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kelsey Perrykkad, a2Vsc2V5LnBlcnJ5a2thZEBtb25hc2guZWR1

ORCID: Denise P. W. Tan, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1823-7597

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.