Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 November 2023
Sec. Health Psychology

Thinking twice: examining gender differences in repetitive negative thinking across the adult lifespan

Kieren J. Lilly
&#x;Kieren J. Lilly1*Chloe Howard&#x;Chloe Howard1Elena Zubielevitch&#x;Elena Zubielevitch2Chris G. Sibley&#x;Chris G. Sibley1
  • 1School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • 2Business School, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia

Background: A wealth of literature shows that women report greater levels of repetitive negative thinking, particularly rumination, than men in adolescence and adulthood. However, little research has examined how these gender differences develop or change across the entire adult lifespan.

Methods: The present study addresses these oversights using a nationwide longitudinal probability sample of adults over 12 annual assessment points (N = 64,901; Mage = 42.50, range 18–81; 62.9% women) and a single-item measure of global repetitive negative thinking. Critically, we use multigroup cohort-sequential latent growth modeling to determine whether changes in this construct over time are due to (a) normative aging, (b) generational differences associated with the historical period one was born and raised in, or (c) a combination of these processes.

Results: Our results reveal that rumination peaks in young adulthood for both women and men but declines steadily thereafter, reaching its lowest levels at the end of the adult lifespan. That said, some gender and cohort differences emerged, with young women—particularly young cohorts—reporting higher levels of rumination than their male counterparts and older birth cohorts.

Discussion: Our study suggests that gender differences in rumination may be most prevalent among young birth cohorts, though future research is needed to elucidate the mechanisms underlying these processes.

1. Introduction

Rumination refers to repetitive, persistent, and passive thinking about the symptoms, circumstances, causes, and consequences of depressed mood or distress (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991; Watkins and Roberts, 2020). Rumination has been implicated in the onset of several mental health conditions, including depression (see Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow, 1993; Lyubomirsky et al., 2015), anxiety (Olatunji et al., 2013; Parthi and Kaur, 2022), and eating disorders (for a recent meta-analytic review, see Rickerby et al., 2022). Critically, a robust body of literature posits rumination as a key factor contributing to the known gender differences in these disorders (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994; Hyde et al., 2008; Johnson and Whisman, 2013; Espinosa et al., 2022). For example, women consistently report higher rates of depression than men (World Health Organisation, 2017), and research suggests that this pattern may occur because of emerging gender differences in rumination during early adolescence (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994; Jose and Brown, 2008).

Yet, although gender differences in rumination are firmly established, most research focuses on adolescent and young adult samples (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994; Hampel and Petermann, 2005; Espinosa et al., 2022). Thus, it remains unknown whether the gender disparity in rumination continues into later adulthood. In the current study, we remedy this gap by examining the development of rumination across the adult lifespan using 12 annual waves of a nationwide random panel sample. To do so, we model responses to a single-item measure developed to assess general levels of negative repetitive thinking, in line with the conceptual definition of rumination developed by Nolen-Hoeksema et al. (1993). It is important to note there are different forms of repetitive negative thinking that, while similar in terms of process and function, differ in temporal orientation (i.e., repetitive negative thinking about the past versus the future, see Ehring and Watkins, 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). The present study, however, assesses the global repetitive negative thinking that underlies rumination as directly as possible without specifying the temporal orientation. While we acknowledge the complexities of different forms of repetitive negative thinking, the present study refers to rumination as the focal framework for understanding gender differences in repetitive negative thinking across the lifespan.

Critically, we utilize cohort-sequential latent growth curve modeling (Prinzie and Onghena, 2005) to investigate how rumination develops and changes across adulthood and compare whether this trajectory differs between women and men. Moreover, we also examine the possibility that gender differences in rumination are affected by historical processes (i.e., generational or cohort differences). By examining these processes, we can elucidate the gender differences (or lack thereof) in general levels of rumination across adulthood, as well as the age groups and cohorts most affected by these differences. We begin by reviewing prior research examining gender, age, and cohort differences in response styles, particularly in rumination, before outlining the current study and our hypotheses.

Nolen-Hoeksema (1994) provides a model for understanding gender differences in depression whereby one’s response style—either passive or active—affects their depressed mood (see also Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). A passive style of responding to depression is characterized by rumination, which likely worsens or maintains depressed mood by hindering one’s ability to generate solutions to interpersonal problems (e.g., Lyubomirsky and Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995) and increasing the salience of negative emotions and thoughts (e.g., Teasdale, 1983). In contrast, an active response style typically involves efforts to problem-solve and change or remove the stressor (Tamres et al., 2002), which may reduce levels of depression and anxiety following stressful events (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Importantly, women are more likely than men to engage in ruminative responses (Johnson and Whisman, 2013) which, in turn, may partially explain gender differences in depression (Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011).

Gender differences in rumination likely emerge in pre-adolescence but become more pronounced due to the challenges and stressors of adolescence (see also Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994). Cross-sectional research tentatively corroborates this hypothesis, showing that gender differences in rumination appear around age 12 (Hampel and Petermann, 2005; Jose and Brown, 2008) and remain into adulthood (see Johnson and Whisman, 2013). Greater exposure to uncontrollable, gendered life events may explain why women and young girls are more at risk of rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1999; Michl et al., 2013). For example, adolescent girls report more interpersonal stressors than adolescent boys (e.g., Hankin et al., 2007) and heightened reactivity to these stressors (e.g., Shih et al., 2006). In adulthood, women also report higher rates of life events than men (Kessler and McLeod, 1984; Norris, 1992; Kendler et al., 2001; Turner and Avison, 2003; Howard et al., 2022), which is likely to be a risk factor for greater rumination.

That said, research has primarily focused on establishing when gender differences emerge (see Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994; Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus, 1994; Hyde et al., 2008; Jose and Brown, 2008), meaning that little is known about whether these differences continue over the lifespan. Nevertheless, some cross-sectional research suggests that women report higher levels of rumination and negative self-focus across early, mid, and later adult age groups (e.g., Leach et al., 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011; Meléndez et al., 2012; Zimmermann and Iwanski, 2014; Chen et al., 2020). Providing indirect evidence, studies also show that women consistently report higher rates of depression, anxiety, and other conditions associated with ruminative strategies (e.g., Mirowsky and Schieman, 2008; McLean and Anderson, 2009; Salk et al., 2017; Twenge et al., 2019). Although these gender differences are typically smaller than in adolescence and early adulthood (Johnson and Whisman, 2013; Olatunji et al., 2013), these studies suggest that gender differences in rumination may be relatively persistent across the lifespan.

Yet, some research suggests that gender differences in rumination may reduce in magnitude rather than persist as people grow older. Indeed, some studies have found smaller or negligible gender differences in depressive disorders among older adults (e.g., Bebbington et al., 2003; Pachana et al., 2012). Thus, rumination may follow a similar trajectory, whereby gender differences become less marked across the adult lifespan. However, no research to our knowledge has explicitly examined how rumination changes and develops across the lifespan. Such longitudinal investigations are needed to determine whether women consistently report higher levels of rumination than men or whether these differences are localized at particular life stages. In doing so, we can determine the specific gender and age groups high in rumination and better prioritize resources to minimize the mental health consequences of rumination.

Although the present study primarily examines lifespan trajectories of gender differences in rumination, it is difficult to disentangle lifespan explanations from the effects of sociohistorical change (i.e., effects due to shifts in culture over time and social differences between generations). After all, the interactions between biological, cognitive, and affective vulnerabilities and adverse life events produce gender differences in mental health (see Hyde et al., 2008; Leach et al., 2008; Hamilton et al., 2015). As such, one’s social and interpersonal context is integral to understanding differences in rumination, and different generations may demonstrate (a) larger (or smaller) gender differences in rumination and (b) distinct trajectories of rumination over time. Accordingly, longitudinal modeling of people across different generations is needed to identify and disentangle cohort differences (or lack thereof) from normative age-related changes in rumination over time.

As mentioned, adolescence is a critical period in the lifespan for rumination due to the uniquely associated physiological changes, life circumstances, and stressors (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994). Yet, emerging and young adults also experience a myriad of unique stressors that may also be relevant to understanding rumination in early adulthood (Schulenberg et al., 2004). Emerging adults (age 18–25; see Arnett, 2000; Arnett, 2007) experience a period of profound change characterized by a transition to greater independence and exploration. Likewise, young adults enter adulthood expecting to complete several personal goals, such as completing education or entering a long-term relationship. Regarding age-specific events, young adults report more stressors than older adults, particularly in relation to education, interpersonal relationships and sexuality, and traumatic events (e.g., assault and suicide; see Norris, 1992; Hatch and Dohrenwend, 2007; Howard et al., 2022).

Conversely, research suggests that older adults (a) endorse more positive forms of emotional regulation and (b) have a more comprehensive range of emotional regulation strategies than young adults (e.g., Blanchard-Fields et al., 1997, 2007; Charles, 2010). For example, older adults experience unique stressors compared to young adults that often center around health, bereavement, and family (Murrel et al., 1984) and, as they age, become more practiced at coping with these stressors (Blanchard-Fields et al., 1997, 2007). In contrast, young adults report greater levels of rumination than older adults (Thomsen et al., 2005; Sütterlin et al., 2012; Zimmermann and Iwanski, 2014), and incidences of mood disorders peak in early adulthood and decline thereafter (e.g., Twenge et al., 2019). Thus, in the present study, we should see a normative trajectory of rumination across the lifespan whereby the highest levels of rumination occur in emerging-to-young adulthood and subsequently decline throughout middle-to-late adulthood.

While rumination should generally follow a normative developmental process, we do expect some cohort-based differences over time. Indeed, historical events and societal changes unfolding during one’s formative years are incredibly important for forming generation-specific attitudes and cognitions (Campbell et al., 2015). For example, children growing up in the 1940s post-World War II period experienced a fundamentally different sociopolitical culture than children in the 1990s. In particular, attitudes toward mental health have changed markedly (e.g., Mojtabai, 2007; Angermeyer et al., 2017), with shifts toward anti-stigma and ‘awareness’ campaigns and media coverage (e.g., Rhydderch et al., 2016). Accordingly, research suggests that young birth cohorts endorse more progressive attitudes toward mental health than older cohorts (e.g., Pescosolido et al., 2021) and greater support for treatment and services (e.g., Pescosolido et al., 2010).

Moreover, older adults were socialized in a time when passive responding styles (i.e., rumination) were discouraged, whereas young generations were encouraged and socialized to recognize their feelings and self-explore (Twenge and Campbell, 2009; Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011). This suggests that younger generations may be more inclined to report negative thoughts, feelings, and behaviors than their predecessors. Thus, while young adults may report higher levels of rumination (relative to older adults) due to a normative developmental difference, the youngest adult cohorts may report the highest levels of rumination due to the shifting attitudes toward mental health and, thus, shifting expectations for young adults. Given the assertion that young women, on average, report higher levels of rumination than men (see Johnson and Whisman, 2013) and experience more gender-specific life events (e.g., Norris, 1992; Turner and Avison, 2003), we expect young cohorts of women, in particular, to report high levels of rumination.

While research has established consistent gender differences in rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987; Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus, 1994; Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011), little is known of how these differences persist across the lifespan. Moreover, while research suggests younger (relative to older) adults report greater levels of rumination, research has yet to examine the potential developmental and generational differences in rumination over time. Given the known associations rumination has with myriad mental health conditions (e.g., Olatunji et al., 2013; Rickerby et al., 2022), the potential aging and cohort trajectories of rumination across time warrant exploration.

The current study examines the development of rumination across the adult lifespan (from age 18 to 81) and whether this trajectory differs among women and men using 12 annual waves of a large, nationwide random probability sample of adults. Specifically, we use a single-item measure of rumination designed to assess general levels of negative repetitive thinking across the lifespan. While there are several forms of repetitive negative thinking (see Ehring and Watkins, 2008), including different forms of rumination (e.g., brooding, reflective, intrusive, and deliberate rumination; see García et al., 2017) across different contexts (e.g., interpersonal or body image domains; Mezulis et al., 2002), our measure mirrors the conceptual definition developed by Nolen-Hoeksema et al. (1993). By using this measure, we aim to identify general ruminative styles of responding in the general population, rather than domain-specific rumination.

Critically, we use cohort-sequential latent growth curve modeling, which allows us to assess age versus cohort-related change in rumination over time (for recent examples, see Milfont et al., 2021; Zubielevitch et al., 2023). More specifically, we estimate three separate cohort-sequential latent growth models that reflect three possible processes underlying the development of rumination across the lifespan: (a) an aging model that assumes common developmental trends across the lifespan, (b) a period model that assumes common trends across birth cohorts due to shared societal change, and (c) a cohort model that assumes differences between birth cohorts due to the unique contexts associated with their formative years. While cohort-sequential models by no means entirely distinguish aging and cohort effects, our ability to model multiple birth cohorts over 12 years is a close approximation. Perhaps more importantly, we estimate models for both women and men, which allows us to inspect gender differences in these trends across the lifespan.

Given the mixed consensus on the persistence of gender differences in rumination across the lifespan, we make no specific predictions for the magnitude of differences between women and men across adulthood. We do, however, expect differences to be most pronounced between young women and men, given the emergence of differences in rumination in adolescence (Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus, 1994; Jose and Brown, 2008) and continuing differences in young adulthood (Thomsen et al., 2005; Leach et al., 2008). Moreover, the extant literature suggests normative age-based differences in rumination and regulation strategies for both women and men (e.g., Blanchard-Fields et al., 2007; Sütterlin et al., 2012; Twenge et al., 2019). We thus expect rumination to generally follow a normative aging process—specifically, the highest levels of rumination should occur in emerging-to-young adulthood and subsequently decline throughout middle-to-late adulthood. That said, given the shifting attitudes toward mental health among young cohorts (Mojtabai, 2007; Angermeyer et al., 2017), we also expect young adult cohorts—particularly young cohorts of women—to report the highest levels of rumination across time. Overall, we aim to elucidate when and how rumination differs across the lifespan, as well as the age groups and cohorts most susceptible to ruminative thinking.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and sampling procedure

We use data from the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study (NZAVS)—an ongoing, nationwide longitudinal panel study that began in 2009. Although the NZAVS began in 2009, we first assessed rumination at Time 2 (2010). As such, we focus on participants who responded to one or more assessment occasions from Time 2 (2010) to Time 13 (2021)—the most recently completed wave of the NZAVS (Ntotal = 64,901). Notably, the NZAVS sample has relatively low rates of attrition, with 33.52% of participants at Time 13 (2021) retained from Time 1 (2009) and good wave-to-wave retention (67.9–85.6%; see Sibley, 2023). Most of our participants were women (62.9%), and the average age of participants at Time 2 was 42.50 years (SD = 13.16; rangetotal: 18–81 years). Concerning ethnicity, most of the sample were New Zealand European (77.8%) or Māori (12.7%), with the remaining participants identifying as Asian (5.1%) and Pasifika (2.7%). Our sample also represents the New Zealand population in socioeconomic status as measured using the New Zealand Deprivation Index (M = 4.89, SD = 2.79, range: 1–10), a decile-based measure assessing neighborhood deprivation across the country from 1 (most affluent) to 10 (most deprived; see Atkinson et al., 2019). Sibley (2023) provides full details of the sampling procedure, demographic information, methodology, and ethics approvals for the NZAVS (see also the OSF page: https://osf.io/75snb/).

We grouped participants into 5-year cohorts for our analyses based on their birth year and gender. Table 1 displays each birth cohort’s sample size and age by gender across the 12 assessment occasions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Age and sample size for birth cohorts by gender.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Rumination

Rumination was measured at each wave using a single item: “During the last 30 days, how often did…you have negative thoughts that repeated over and over?” (Matika et al., 2017). This item was developed for the NZAVS based on the conceptual definition of rumination described by Nolen-Hoeksema et al. (1993). The item was measured on a scale from 0 (None of the time) to 4 (All of the time), with higher scores indicating higher levels of rumination. Table 2 displays the bivariate correlations between our measures of rumination at each time point.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between our focal variables.

2.2.2. Gender

Gender was measured differently over time following a move to a more inclusive measure. In Times 2–5, gender was measured by asking participants a forced binary choice question (“Are you male or female?). From Time 6 onwards, gender was measured using an open-ended question, “What is your gender?” (Fraser et al., 2020). For our analyses, we dummy-coded participants’ gender (0 = woman, 1 = man) and excluded gender-diverse participants due to the small sample size (0.5% of the total NZAVS sample).

2.3. Analytic approach

To examine the trajectory of rumination over time—and whether this trajectory differs between women and men—we estimated multigroup cohort-sequential latent growth models based on 5-year birth cohorts using Mplus version 8.8 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998-2023). While traditional latent growth models permit us to examine mean growth trajectories for a particular cohort, cohort-sequential designs estimate mean growth trajectories for different cohorts simultaneously, allowing us to identify (a) common developmental trends across adulthood (i.e., overlapping estimates in different birth cohorts), and (b) any potential differences between birth cohorts over time (see Prinzie and Onghena, 2005).

While this approach has been well-documented elsewhere (for a recent example, see Zubielevitch et al., 2023), we provide a brief overview here. First, we sorted our sample into 12 sequential birth cohorts based on birth year, spanning 1995 to 1936 (ages 18 to 81). We used the youngest age within each birth cohort to indicate participants’ age at Time 2 (2010); for example, the 1990–1986 cohort reflected change from ages 19 to 31. There is one exception to this—the 1995–1991 birth cohort spanned a shorter time period (from age 18 to 26) as the youngest theoretical age (15) is beyond the observed range of ages in the NZAVS, which only surveyed those over the age of 18. Additionally, we separated each birth cohort by gender to model change for both women and men. To handle missing data, we conducted all analyses using full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML; see Enders and Bandalos, 2001).

We first estimated an aging model that examined trajectories of rumination across the lifespan as a function of normative developmental change. That is, the model assumes that different birth cohorts will have comparable initial levels of rumination and rates of change across time. Specifically, the model constrains the intercepts and slopes to equality across all 12 birth cohorts. To account for possible curvilinear rates of change over time, we estimated linear and quadratic slopes in our analyses. The model was then age-centered at 45 years and conditioned by age so that we could plot the point estimates across ages 18 to 81 and identify any age-related trends in rumination across the adult lifespan for both women and men.

We then estimated a period model—an intermediate model that allows birth cohorts to differ in their initial levels of rumination (i.e., the intercepts) but constrains the rate of change to equality across birth cohorts. Accordingly, we constrained the slopes to equality but freely estimated intercepts for each birth cohort. Finally, we estimated a cohort model that examined the possibility that generational differences uniquely affect rumination across the lifespan. That is, initial levels of rumination and rates of change over time differ across birth cohorts due to the contextual factors associated with the period in which they were born. As such, we freed the intercepts and the slopes for each birth cohort. As with our aging model, we conditioned the point estimates by age to plot the trends for each birth cohort across the12 annual assessments.

To assess model fit and determine whether changes in rumination across the adulthood lifespan reflect aging or cohort effects, we inspected widely used model fit indices for each model. However, determining aging versus cohort processes requires more nuance than provided by global fit indices (see Steiger, 2007). Given this, we use global fit indices and our plots of the aging and cohort estimates to holistically inspect whether birth cohort estimates broadly follow an aging trend or whether there are unique trajectories across birth cohorts over time.

3. Results

3.1. Aging model

Table 3 shows the aging model fits these data well [χ2(2150) = 9930.67, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.07]. The parameter estimates that best fit all birth cohorts are displayed in Table 4. For women, Table 4 reveals a significant curvilinear change in rumination over time (s = −0.14, SE = 0.00, p < 0.001; q = 0.02, SE = 0.00, p < 0.001). Rumination among men displayed a similar aging trend (s = −0.13, SE = 0.00, p < 0.001; q = 0.01, SE = 0.00, p < 0.001). Indeed, an inspection of the aging lines in Figures 1, 2 (i.e., the black lines) reveals a decline in rumination from age 18 onwards but that this rate of change slowed over time for both women and men. However, a follow-up Wald test did reveal a small but significant difference between the trajectories of women and men across time [Wald(3) = 9.73, p = 0.021]. These subtle gender differences are displayed in Figure 3 and reveal greater curvilinear changes in rumination among women than men.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Model fit for aging, period, and cohort models.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Parameter coefficients for the aging models for rumination by gender.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Change trajectories and comparisons of aging and cohort models for rumination among women. The aging trajectory for rumination among women is shown by the black line from ages 18 to 81. The gray lines within each 5-year birth cohort panel demonstrate longitudinal change in rumination over 12 years for each birth cohort, estimating intercepts (i), linear slopes (s), and quadratic slopes (q). Error bars reflect 95% confidence intervals for each point estimate. Due to graphical constraints, we plot the estimates for the 1995–1991 birth cohort (ages 18–26, n = 2,615) but could not display them: i = 3.27*, s = 1.89*, q = 0.46*. *p < 0.05.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Change trajectories and comparisons of aging and cohort models for rumination among men. The aging trajectory for rumination among men is shown by the black line from ages 18 to 81. The gray lines within each 5-year birth cohort panel demonstrate longitudinal change in rumination over 12 years for each birth cohort, estimating intercepts (i), linear slopes (s), and quadratic slopes (q). Error bars reflect 95% confidence intervals for each point estimate. Due to graphical constraints, we plot the estimates for the 1995–1991 birth cohort (ages 18–26, n = 1,223) but could not display them: i = 0.51, s = −0.48, q = −0.06. *p < 0.05.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparisons of the aging trajectories for rumination among women (N = 40,793) and men (N = 24.108). The black line depicts the aging trajectory for women, while the gray line depicts the aging trajectory for men. The graph depicts a 0–2 scale to facilitate interpretation of gender differences, but rumination was measured on a 0–4 scale. Error bars reflect 95% confidence intervals for each point estimate.

3.2. Period model

The period model also fits these data well [χ2(2128) = 9616.47, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.07; Table 3]. Table 5 displays the parameter estimates for this model and reveals that the values for the freely estimated intercepts were generally lower for each successive birth cohort for both women and men. That is, young birth cohorts had higher mean levels of rumination than older birth cohorts, irrespective of gender. Interestingly, only women in the two youngest birth cohorts (1995–1986) had higher initial mean levels of rumination than men in the corresponding birth cohorts (i.e., the confidence intervals of the estimates did not overlap). Similar to the aging model, the period model suggests a linear decline in rumination among women over time (s = −0.03, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001) but that this rate of change slowed over time (q = 0.01, SE = 0.00, p < 0.001). A similar trend emerged for men over time, albeit with greater linear declines in rumination across the lifespan (s = −0.08, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001; q = 0.01, SE = 0.00, p = 0.015).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Parameter estimates for the period model for rumination by gender.

3.3. Cohort model

Finally, the cohort model also fits these data well [χ2(2084) = 9930.67, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.07; Table 3]. In terms of women, Table 6 reveals that 8 out of 12 birth cohorts showed significant changes in rumination over 12 years (ps < 0.05). The gray lines in Figure 1 display the trends for each birth cohort and reveal that the 1995–1991 (s = 1.89, SE = 0.51, p < 0.001; q = 0.46, SE = 0.11, p < 0.001), 1990–1986 (s = 1.12, SE = 0.31, p < 0.001; q = 0.32, SE = 0.08, p < 0.001), 1985–1981 (s = 0.58, SE = 0.22, p = 0.008; q = 0.22, SE = 0.08, p = 0.006), 1980–1976 (s = 0.30, SE = 0.12, p = 0.016; q = 0.18, SE = 0.07, p = 0.012), and 1975–1971 (s = 0.13, SE = 0.05, p = 0.010; q = 0.17, SE = 0.06, p = 0.009) birth cohorts experienced curvilinear increases in rumination over time. The 1970–1966 birth cohort showed positive curvilinear change (q = 0.17, SE = 0.06, p = 0.003). Finally, the 1955–1951 (s = −0.45, SE = 0.20, p = 0.001; q = 0.20, SE = 0.06, p = 0.044) and 1950–1946 (s = −1.56, SE = 0.35, p < 0.001; q = 0.37, SE = 0.08, p < 0.001) birth cohorts showed linear decreases in rumination over time but that this rate of change slowed over time. The remaining cohorts showed no significant change in rumination over the 12 assessment occasions (ps ≥ 0.289).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Parameter estimates for the cohort model for rumination by gender.

Regarding men, only 4 of the 12 birth cohorts showed significant changes in rumination over time (see Table 6). As shown in Figure 2, these cohorts mirrored the findings for women in that the 1990–1986 cohort displayed curvilinear increases in rumination over time (s = 1.24, SE = 0.47, p = 0.008; q = 0.35, SE = 0.12, p = 0.005). The 1970–1966 cohort showed linear decreases that slowed over time (s = −0.12, SE = 0.04, p = 0.001; q = 0.22, SE = 0.08, p = 0.005), whereas the 1960–1956 cohort showed linear decreases (s = −0.39, SE = 0.16, p = 0.012) but no significant curvilinear change (q = 0.12, SE = 0.07, p = 0.060). Finally, the 1955–1951 birth cohort showed linear decreases in rumination, but this rate of change slowed over time (s = −0.08, SE = 0.011, p < 0.001; q = 0.01, SE = 0.004, p = 0.015).

To formally examine differences between women and men in the cohort model, we constrained the cohort estimates for women and men to equality within each birth cohort. Constraining all 11 birth cohorts to equality revealed that (some of) the birth cohorts differed in their trajectories over time by gender [Wald(36) = 71.13, p < 0.001]. Subsequent comparisons of each birth cohort revealed differences between women and men among the 1995–1991 [Wald(3) = 15.50, p = 0.001] and 1960–1956 [Wald(3) = 9.58, p = 0.023] birth cohorts. The remaining birth cohorts did not demonstrate significant gender differences over time [Walds(3) = 0.87–6.72, ps ≥ 0.081], suggesting only small cohort-specific gender differences. Specifically, women in the 1995–1991 were generally higher in rumination (relative to men) and increased over time, while men in this cohort experienced (nonsignificant) declines in rumination. Similarly, in the 1960–1956 cohort, men reported significant declines in rumination, while women in this cohort remained relatively stable in rumination across time.

3.4. Model comparisons

While the aging, period, and cohort models all fit these data comparably (see Table 3), a visual inspection of the aging and cohort estimates in Figures 1, 2 reveals apparent aging effects for rumination among women and men, albeit with some very minor cohort differences. Namely, while the cohort estimates for women largely overlapped between birth cohorts (i.e., the 95% error bars for each point estimate largely overlapped; see Figure 1), there was slightly less overlap among the young birth cohorts. Moreover, the youngest cohort (1995–1991) demonstrated higher levels of rumination than estimated in the aging line and slightly less overlap with its adjacent birth cohort. Thus, these findings suggest a primarily normative developmental trajectory of rumination across the lifespan for women, with some limited evidence of cohort-based differences among young birth cohorts.

Concerning men, Figure 2 reveals a significant overlap between birth cohorts and between the broader cohort and aging models. That is, the cohort estimates largely follow the aging trajectory. As such, while there may be a distinct trajectory for the youngest birth cohort among women, these findings suggest that the course of rumination across the adult lifespan primarily reflects an aging process among men, irrespective of one’s birth cohort.

4. Discussion

The present study examined how rumination develops and changes across the adult lifespan using a large longitudinal nationwide random probability sample of adults over 12 years. Specifically, we utilized cohort-sequential latent growth modeling, which allowed us to inspect whether rumination follows a normative aging process across the lifespan or whether the development of rumination across time differed between cohorts. Critically, we examined trajectories of rumination over time among women and men separately, given that the extant literature suggests gender differences in rumination across different life stages (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994; Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011; Johnson and Whisman, 2013). In examining these processes, we aimed to elucidate gender differences in rumination (or lack thereof) across the lifespan, extending prior research that has predominantly focused on understanding the emergence rather than the continuity of gender differences in rumination.

Overall, our findings revealed that mean levels of rumination generally decreased from age 18 to 81, albeit at a rate that slowed over time. This trajectory appeared to reflect a normative aging process, corroborating previous research suggesting ruminative strategies are highest in young adulthood and decrease as one ages and gains life experience over time (e.g., Carstensen, 1987; Charles and Carstensen, 2007; Sütterlin et al., 2012; Zimmermann and Iwanski, 2014). Our findings, however, advance this understanding by demonstrating this normative trajectory in a large, nationwide sample of adults over 12 years. Indeed, previous research has predominantly focused on identifying the age groups most at “risk” for rumination rather than how rumination develops and changes across the lifespan. Our data provide a unique opportunity to explore this by directly comparing aging and cohort trajectories of rumination across time.

Interestingly, trajectories of rumination across the lifespan were relatively consistent across women and men, suggesting a common developmental trend irrespective of gender. That said, small gender differences did emerge in our aging model, and our period model revealed higher initial mean levels of rumination in the two youngest cohorts among women (relative to their male counterparts). Moreover, formal tests of gender differences in our cohort model revealed significant differences between women and men in the 1995–1991 birth cohort (and, to some extent, the 1960–1956 cohort). Thus, our findings align with previous research suggesting that young women face unique stressors that may predispose vulnerability to ruminative responding styles (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1999; Michl et al., 2013), and align with wider societal shifts in attitudes toward mental health over time (e.g., Pescosolido et al., 2021).

These findings suggest that gender differences in rumination may be better understood as age- and cohort-specific rather than as a consistent difference across the lifespan. While young female cohorts reporting the highest levels of rumination is perhaps unsurprising, these findings alleviate concerns that women are higher in rumination across the entire lifespan and that these patterns of responding may, in turn, put women at increased risk for depression and other mental health disorders e.g., (see Nolen-Hoeksema and Harrell, 2002; Johnson and Whisman, 2013; Rickerby et al., 2022). Although the differences between women and men in depressive disorders is well-established, these findings suggest that rumination may not play a significant role in this gender difference at later lifestages. Instead, the focus should be toward educating young women on the adverse consequences of rumination, and the benefits of active problem-solving strategies, to lessen the prevalence of adverse mental health in these groups.

Additionally, while both women and men experienced normative changes in rumination across the lifespan, more cohort differences emerged among women, suggesting that women’s reported rumination may be more sensitive to societal shifts than men’s and further supporting the assertion that women experience gender-specific events that impact their mental health (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1999). Given the generational shifts in how masculinity is defined and expressed, it is somewhat surprising, however, that young male cohorts were not considerably higher in rumination than their older counterparts (Anderson, 2018). Indeed, conformity to traditional masculine norms is associated with lower reporting of emotional distress (Ridge et al., 2011) and less willingness for men to seek mental health support (e.g., Pederson and Vogel, 2007; Lorber and Garcia, 2010; Johnson et al., 2012). However, changing priorities toward emotional intelligence and “soft skills” in contemporary society means that how men “perform” masculinity is changing (Gough, 2018). As such, one might expect young male cohorts to be more likely to disclose their emotional responses to stressors than their older counterparts. While our cohort model did not reveal any substantial cohort-based differences among men, these generational differences may only become apparent as young generations age and enter older life stages. Future research should consider this possibility and continue to examine how new generations of men navigate their emotions and mental health.

4.1. Limitations and future research directions

The present study contributes rare insights into the development of rumination across the lifespan. There are, however, limitations worthy of consideration. Namely, space constraints associated with a longitudinal survey restricted our assessment of rumination to a single item. Our measure of rumination was designed to assess general negative repetitive thoughts as directly as possible (see Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1993), allowing us to examine general levels of rumination in the population. While we could not assess particular forms of rumination, our analyses provide insight into the development of general ruminative thinking across the lifespan. Nonetheless, it is possible that gender differences only emerge consistently for particular forms of rumination (e.g., brooding, reflecting, deliberate, and intrusive rumination) across the lifespan (see García et al., 2017) or in response to specific life domains (e.g., body image or interpersonal domains; see Mezulis et al., 2002). Future research should examine these possibilities using indicators of rumination with multiple items across different domains.

Although a strength of our study is the use of a large, nationwide random sample, our participants generally scored low on rumination across time which may contribute to the lack of gender differences. Mean rumination scores were well below the scale’s mid-point across the lifespan—as expected of a nonclinical population—with only 5.6–7.8% of the sample scoring above the mid-point at each wave. Thus, the relatively low levels of rumination in the sample may occlude gender differences, or these differences may only be apparent in clinical samples. Future research should consider this possibility when assessing the development and change in rumination across time, particularly when assessing gender or age differences.

Relatedly, although our study aimed to be representative of the New Zealand population, there were some discrepancies compared to the general population. Namely, our sample overrepresented women and New Zealand Europeans (see Sibley, 2023, for full information about the NZAVS sample). Concerning gender, our sample size for male participants is sufficient to detect change in rumination over time, and differences in sample sizes for women and men should not significantly affect our results. That said, the sample sizes for older male cohorts are small relative to other groups, which may occlude potential gender differences between women and men at older ages. Relatedly, recent research among transgender and gender-diverse individuals suggests a nuanced relationship between rumination and minority stress (e.g., Sarno et al., 2020) that we could not capture in our analyses. Future research should thus consider how rumination may develop and change differently across different identities and contexts, with particular attention to male and gender-diverse cohorts.

Additionally, our sample consisted of predominantly New Zealand Europeans, which may bias our findings in favor of Western WEIRD populations. Indeed, research suggests that the prevalence and consequences of rumination differ across cultures (e.g., Bonanno et al., 2005; Chang et al., 2010). These differences are primarily attributed to differences in maladaptive versus adaptive rumination (Grossmann and Kross, 2010), perceptions of change (Choi and Miyamoto, 2023), and holistic versus analytic thinking (De Vaus et al., 2018). Notably, research examining rumination among Māori (i.e., New Zealand’s Indigenous people) reveals that Māori with a greater sense of cultural efficacy report lower levels of rumination (Matika et al., 2017). Thus, culture and ethnicity are important factors that shape rumination. While examining cultural and ethnic-based differences in rumination is beyond the scope of the present study, we encourage future research to consider how cultural differences may impact developmental and cohort-based changes in rumination across the lifespan.

It is also important to note that we cannot fully distinguish between aging and cohort effects, as doing so would require extensive data across the lifespan within individuals. For example, cohort differences may be more (or, counterintuitively, less) pronounced if each cohort was followed across their entire lifespan. Likewise, although we provide some theoretical explanations for our findings, our approach does not elucidate why rumination declines as people age, nor can it explain the differences between birth cohorts. For example, rumination may decrease across the lifespan (or differ between birth cohorts) because of differences in life experiences and stressors (Kessler and McLeod, 1984; Kendler et al., 2001; Howard et al., 2022) or shifting attitudes toward mental health (e.g., Mojtabai, 2007; Rhydderch et al., 2016; Angermeyer et al., 2017). That said, such data is beyond the scope of this particular study. Nonetheless, our use of 12 annual assessments and a broad range of birth cohorts provides tentative evidence of the normative trajectory of rumination across the adult lifespan. These findings lay the critical foundations for future studies examining the mechanisms underlying the development and change of rumination as people age.

4.2. Conclusion

While the extant literature suggests gender differences in rumination persist across the lifespan, no research to date has directly examined this hypothesis. Likewise, research has yet to elucidate whether changes in rumination over time reflect a normative aging process or birth cohort differences. As such, the present study utilized multigroup cohort-sequential latent growth models to directly compare aging, period, and cohort effects in rumination across the adult lifespan (ages 18–81) and whether these trajectories differed between women and men. Our findings suggest a normative aging process whereby rumination is highest in young adulthood and declines across the adult lifespan, albeit at a rate that slows over time. This trajectory was largely similar among women and men. However, key cohort differences emerged among young women that suggest some context- and age-specific gender differences in rumination over time. Taken together, our results provide critical insights into the development of rumination in adulthood and demonstrate the importance of disentangling aging and cohort effects across the lifespan.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: The dataset presented in this article is not readily available because ethical restrictions and the need to protect the confidentiality of study participants prevent public deposition of raw data. The data described in the paper are part of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study (NZAVS). Full copies of the NZAVS data files are held by all members of the NZAVS management team and advisory board. A de-identified dataset containing the variables analyzed in this manuscript is available upon request from the corresponding author or any member of the NZAVS advisory board for the purposes of replication or checking of any published study using NZAVS data. The Mplus syntax used to test all models reported in this manuscript are available on the NZAVS OSF page: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/75SNB. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to CS, c.sibley@auckland.ac.nz.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the University of Auckland Human Participants Ethics Committee (Reference Number: 22576). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KL wrote the original manuscript and performed the statistical analysis. CS organized the database and funding for the study. CH, EZ, and CS provided extensive feedback on the manuscript. All authors approved the manuscript in its final form.

Funding

This manuscript was supported by a grant from the Templeton Religion Trust (TRT-2021-10418) awarded to the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Anderson, E. (2018). Generational masculinities. J. Gend. Stud. 27, 243–247. doi: 10.1080/09589236.2017.1406088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angermeyer, M. C., Van Der Auwera, S., Carta, M. G., and Schomerus, G. (2017). Public attitudes towards psychiatry and psychiatric treatment at the beginning of the 21st century: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. World Psychiatry 16, 50–61. doi: 10.1002/wps.20383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: a theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. Am. Psychol. 55, 469–480. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.55.5.469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnett, J. J. (2007). Emerging adulthood: what is it, and what is it good for? Child Dev. Perspect. 1, 68–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00016.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J., Salmond, C., and Crampton, P. (2019). NZDep2018 index of deprivation, interim research report, December 2019. Wellington: University of Otago.

Google Scholar

Bebbington, P., Dunn, G., Jenkins, R., Lewis, G., Brugha, T., Farrell, M., et al. (2003). The influence of age and sex on the prevalence of depressive conditions: report from the National Survey of psychiatric morbidity. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 15, 74–83. doi: 10.1080/0954026021000045976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard-Fields, F., Chen, Y., and Norris, L. (1997). Everyday problem solving across the adult life span: influence of domain specificity and cognitive appraisal. Psychol. Aging 12, 684–693. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard-Fields, F., Mienaltowski, A., and Seay, R. B. (2007). Age differences in everyday problem-solving effectiveness: older adults select more effective strategies for interpersonal problems. J Gerontol B 62, P61–P64. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.1.P61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Zhang, N., and Noll, J. G. (2005). Grief processing and deliberate grief avoidance: a prospective comparison of bereaved spouses and parents in the United States and the People's Republic of China. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 86–98. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.1.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, W. K., Campbell, S. M., Siedor, L. E., and Twenge, J. M. (2015). Generational differences are real and useful. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 8, 324–331. doi: 10.1017/iop.2015.43

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L. (1987). “Age-related changes in social activity” in Handbook of clinical gerontology (Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press), 222–237.

Google Scholar

Chang, E. C., Tsai, W., and Sanna, L. J. (2010). Examining the relations between rumination and adjustment: do ethnic differences exist between Asian and European Americans? Asian Am. J. Psychol. 1, 46–56. doi: 10.1037/a0018821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integration: a model of emotional well-being across adulthood. Psychol. Bull. 136, 1068–1091. doi: 10.1037/a0021232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, S. T., and Carstensen, L. L. (2007). “Emotion regulation and aging” in Handbook of emotion regulation (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 307–327.

Google Scholar

Chen, X., Pu, J., Shi, W., and Zhou, Y. (2020). The impact of neuroticism on symptoms of anxiety and depression in elderly adults: the mediating role of rumination. Curr. Psychol. 39, 42–50. doi: 10.1007/s12144-017-9740-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. H., and Miyamoto, Y. (2023). Cultural differences in rumination and psychological correlates: the role of attribution. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 49, 1213–1230. doi: 10.1177/01461672221089061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vaus, J., Hornsey, M. J., Kuppens, P., and Bastian, B. (2018). Exploring the east-west divide in prevalence of affective disorder: a case for cultural differences in coping with negative emotion. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 22, 285–304. doi: 10.1177/1088868317736222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehring, T., and Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic process. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 1, 192–205. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2008.1.3.192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enders, C. K., and Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 8, 430–457. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espinosa, F., Martin-Romero, N., and Sanchez-Lopez, A. (2022). Repetitive negative thinking processes account for gender differences in depression and anxiety during adolescence. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 15, 115–133. doi: 10.1007/s41811-022-00133-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraser, G., Bulbulia, J., Greaves, L. M., Wilson, M. S., and Sibley, C. G. (2020). Coding responses to an open-ended gender measure in a new Zealand National Sample. J. Sex Res. 57, 979–986. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2019.1687640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, F. E., Duque, A., and Cova, F. (2017). The four faces of rumination to stressful events: a psychometric analysis. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 9, 758–765. doi: 10.1037/tra0000289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gough, B. (2018). “Affective masculinities: emotions and mental health” in Contemporary masculinities: embodiment, emotion and wellbeing (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 39–55.

Google Scholar

Grossmann, I., and Kross, E. (2010). The impact of culture on adaptive versus maladaptive self-reflection. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1150–1157. doi: 10.1177/0956797610376655

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, J. L., Stange, J. P., Abramson, L. Y., and Alloy, L. B. (2015). Stress and the development of cognitive vulnerabilities to depression explain sex differences in depressive symptoms during adolescence. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 702–714. doi: 10.1177/2167702614545479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampel, P., and Petermann, F. (2005). Age and gender effects on coping in children and adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 34, 73–83. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-3207-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hankin, B. L., Mermelstein, R., and Roesch, L. (2007). Sex differences in adolescent depression: stress exposure and reactivity models. Child Dev. 78, 279–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatch, S. L., and Dohrenwend, B. P. (2007). Distribution of traumatic and other stressful life events by race/ethnicity, gender, SES and age: a review of the research. Am. J. Community Psychol. 40, 313–332. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9134-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, C., Zubielevitch, E., Overall, N. C., and Sibley, C. G. (2022). The broad inventory of specific life events (BISLE): development, validation, and population prevalence. N. Z. J. Psychol. 51, 59–74.

Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S., Mezulis, A. H., and Abramson, L. Y. (2008). The ABCs of depression: integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression. Psychol. Rev. 115, 291–313. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. L., Oliffe, J. L., Kelly, M. T., Galdas, P., and Ogrodniczuk, J. S. (2012). Men’s discourses of help-seeking in the context of depression. Sociol. Health Illn. 34, 345–361. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2011.01372.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, D. P., and Whisman, M. A. (2013). Gender differences in rumination: a meta-analysis. Personal. Individ. Differ. 55, 367–374. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jose, P. E., and Brown, I. (2008). When does the gender difference in rumination begin? Gender and age differences in the use of rumination by adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 37, 180–192. doi: 10.1007/s10964-006-9166-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., and Prescott, C. A. (2001). Gender differences in the rates of exposure to stressful life events and sensitivity to their depressogenic effects. Am. J. Psychiatr. 158, 587–593. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.4.587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., and McLeod, J. D. (1984). Sex differences in vulnerability to undesirable life events. Am. Sociol. Rev. 49, 620–631. doi: 10.2307/2095420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leach, L. S., Christensen, H., Mackinnon, A. J., Windsor, T. D., and Butterworth, P. (2008). Gender differences in depression and anxiety across the adult lifespan: the role of psychosocial mediators. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 43, 983–998. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0388-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorber, W., and Garcia, H. A. (2010). Not supposed to feel this: traditional masculinity in psychotherapy with male veterans returning from Afghanistan and Iraq. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. Train. 47, 296–305. doi: 10.1037/a0021161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., Layous, K., Chancellor, J., and Nelson, S. K. (2015). Thinking about rumination: the scholarly contributions and intellectual legacy of Susan Nolen-Hoeksema. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 11, 1–22. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 176–190. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matika, C. M., Manuela, S., Muriwai, E., Houkamau, C. A., and Sibley, C. G. (2017). Cultural efficacy predicts increased self esteem for Māori: the mediating effect of rumination. N. Z. J. Psychol. 46, 176–185.

Google Scholar

McLean, C. P., and Anderson, E. R. (2009). Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 496–505. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meléndez, J. C., Mayordomo, T., Sancho, P., and Tomás, J. M. (2012). Coping strategies: gender differences and development throughout life span. Span. J. Psychol. 15, 1089–1098. doi: 10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n3.39399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., and Hyde, J. S. (2002). Domain specificity of gender differences in rumination. J. Cogn. Psychother. 16, 421–434. doi: 10.1891/jcop.16.4.421.52524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michl, L. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Shepherd, K., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 339–352. doi: 10.1037/a0031994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milfont, T. L., Zubielevitch, E., Milojev, P., and Sibley, C. G. (2021). Ten-year panel data confirm generation gap but climate beliefs increase at similar rates across ages. Nat. Commun. 12:4038. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-24245-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirowsky, J., and Schieman, S. (2008). Gender, age, and the trajectories and trends of anxiety and anger. Adv. Life Course Res. 13, 45–73. doi: 10.1016/S1040-2608(08)00003-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mojtabai, R. (2007). Americans' attitudes toward mental health treatment seeking: 1990–2003. Psychiatr. Serv. 58, 642–651. doi: 10.1176/ps.2007.58.5.642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murrel, S. A., Norris, F. H., and Hutchins, G. L. (1984). Distribution and desirability of life events in older adults: population and policy implications. J. Community Psychol. 12, 301–311. doi: 10.1002/1520-6629(198410)12:4<301::AID-JCOP2290120403>3.0.CO;2-I

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998-2023). Mplus user's guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: evidence and theory. Psychol. Bull. 101, 259–282. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 569–582. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1994). An interactive model for the emergence of gender differences in depression in adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 4, 519–534. doi: 10.1207/s15327795jra0404_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 704–708. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychol. Bull. 115, 424–443. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Harrell, Z. A. (2002). Rumination, depression, and alcohol use: tests of gender differences. J. Cogn. Psychother. 16, 391–403. doi: 10.1891/jcop.16.4.391.52526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., and Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 1061–1072. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognit. Emot. 7, 561–570. doi: 10.1080/02699939308409206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., and Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 102, 20–28. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norris, F. H. (1992). Epidemiology of trauma: frequency and impact of different potentially traumatic events on different demographic groups. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 60, 409–418. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.60.3.409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olatunji, B. O., Naragon-Gainey, K., and Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B. (2013). Specificity of rumination in anxiety and depression: a multimodal meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 20, 225–257. doi: 10.1037/h0101719

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pachana, N. A., McLaughlin, D., Leung, J., Byrne, G., and Dobson, A. (2012). Anxiety and depression in adults in their eighties: do gender differences remain? Int. Psychogeriatr. 24, 145–150. doi: 10.1017/s1041610211001372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parthi, K., and Kaur, S. S. (2022). Rumination, depression, anxiety, stress, physical health and neuroticism in young adults. Indian J. Health Well Being 13, 29–34.

Google Scholar

Pederson, E. L., and Vogel, D. L. (2007). Male gender role conflict and willingness to seek counseling: testing a mediation model on college-aged men. J. Couns. Psychol. 54, 373–384. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.54.4.373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pescosolido, B. A., Halpern-Manners, A., Luo, L., and Perry, B. (2021). Trends in public stigma of mental illness in the US, 1996-2018. JAMA Netw. Open 4:e2140202. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.40202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pescosolido, B. A., Martin, J. K., Long, J. S., Medina, T. R., Phelan, J. C., and Link, B. G. (2010). “A disease like any other”? A decade of change in public reactions to schizophrenia, depression, and alcohol dependence. Am. J. Psychiatr. 167, 1321–1330. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09121743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinzie, P., and Onghena, P. (2005). “Cohort sequential design” in Encyclopedia of statistics in behavioral science. eds. B. S. Everitt and D. C. Howell (Chichester: John Wiley), 319–322.

Google Scholar

Rhydderch, D., Krooupa, A. M., Shefer, G., Goulden, R., Williams, P., Thornicroft, A., et al. (2016). Changes in newspaper coverage of mental illness from 2008 to 2014 in England. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 134, 45–52. doi: 10.1111/acps.12606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rickerby, N., Krug, I., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Forte, E., Davenport, R., Chayadi, E., et al. (2022). Rumination across depression, anxiety, and eating disorders in adults: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. doi: 10.1037/cps0000110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridge, D., Emslie, C., and White, A. (2011). Understanding how men experience, express and cope with mental distress: where next? Sociol. Health Illn. 33, 145–159. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2010.01266.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salk, R. H., Hyde, J. S., and Abramson, L. Y. (2017). Gender differences in depression in representative National Samples: Meta-analyses of diagnoses and symptoms. Psychol. Bull. 143, 783–822. doi: 10.1037/bul0000102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarno, E. L., Newcomb, M. E., and Mustanski, B. (2020). Rumination longitudinally mediates the association of minority stress and depression in sexual and gender minority individuals. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 129, 355–363. doi: 10.1037/abn0000508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulenberg, J. E., Sameroff, A. J., and Cicchetti, D. (2004). The transition to adulthood as a critical juncture in the course of psychopathology and mental health. Dev. Psychopathol. 16. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, J. H., Eberhart, N. K., Hammen, C. L., and Brennan, P. A. (2006). Differential exposure and reactivity to interpersonal stress predict sex differences in adolescent depression. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 35, 103–115. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3501_9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, C. G. (2023). Sampling procedure and sample details for the New Zealand attitudes and values study.

Google Scholar

Steiger, J. H. (2007). Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modeling. Personal. Individ. Differ. 42, 893–898. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sütterlin, S., Paap, M. C. S., Babic, S., Kübler, A., and Vögele, C. (2012). Rumination and age: some things get better. J. Aging Res. 2012, 1–10. doi: 10.1155/2012/267327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamres, L. K., Janicki, D., and Helgeson, V. S. (2002). Sex differences in coping behavior: a meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 2–30. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0601_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: cause, effect, or reciprocal relationship? Adv. Behav. Res. Ther. 5, 3–25. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(83)90013-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomsen, D. K., Mehlsen, M. Y., Viidik, A., Sommerlund, B., and Zachariae, R. (2005). Age and gender differences in negative affect—is there a role for emotion regulation? Personal. Individ. Differ. 38, 1935–1946. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. J., and Avison, W. R. (2003). Status variations in stress exposure: implications for the interpretation of research on race, socioeconomic status, and gender. J. Health Soc. Behav. 44, 488–505. doi: 10.2307/1519795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twenge, J. M., and Campbell, W. K. (2009). The narcissism epidemic: living in the age of entitlement New York: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Twenge, J. M., Cooper, A. B., Joiner, T. E., Duffy, M. E., and Binau, S. G. (2019). Age, period, and cohort trends in mood disorder indicators and suicide-related outcomes in a nationally representative dataset, 2005–2017. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 128, 185–199. doi: 10.1037/abn0000410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, E. R., and Roberts, H. (2020). Reflecting on rumination: consequences, causes, mechanisms and treatment of rumination. Behav. Res. Ther. 127:103573. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2020.103573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organisation (2017). Depression and other common mental disorders: global health estimates. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., and Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 38, 182–194. doi: 10.1177/0165025413515405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zubielevitch, E., Osborne, D., Milojev, P., and Sibley, C. G. (2023). Social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism across the adult lifespan: an examination of aging and cohort effects. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 124, 544–566. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cohort-sequential design, rumination, gender differences, longitudinal analysis, generational differences

Citation: Lilly KJ, Howard C, Zubielevitch E and Sibley CG (2023) Thinking twice: examining gender differences in repetitive negative thinking across the adult lifespan. Front. Psychol. 14:1239112. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1239112

Received: 12 June 2023; Accepted: 18 October 2023;
Published: 08 November 2023.

Edited by:

Diego Gomez-Baya, University of Huelva, Spain

Reviewed by:

Francisco José García-Moro, University of Huelva, Spain
Antonio David Martin Barrado, University of Huelva, Spain
Carolina Inostroza, University of Concepcion, Chile

Copyright © 2023 Lilly, Howard, Zubielevitch and Sibley. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kieren J. Lilly, kieren.lilly@auckland.ac.nz

ORCID: Kieren J. Lilly, https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1826-9254
Chloe Howard, https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9043-9735
Elena Zubielevitch, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5751-3152
Chris G. Sibley, http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4064-8800

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.