Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 18 September 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Parental Influence on Child Social and Emotional Functioning View all 17 articles

Parenting style and children emotion management skills among Chinese children aged 3–6: the chain mediation effect of self-control and peer interactions

  • 1School of Education, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China
  • 2College of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China

Drawing on ecosystem theory, which is based on the interaction of family environment, individual characteristics, and social adaptation, this study aimed to examine the effects of parenting style on emotion management skills and the mediating roles of self-control and peer interactions among Chinese children aged 3–6 years. Some studies have investigated the relationship between parenting style and emotion management skills. However, research on the underlying mechanisms is still deficient. A sample of 2,303 Chinese children completed the PSDQ-Short Version, the Self-Control Teacher Rating Questionnaire, the Peer Interaction Skills Scale, and the Emotion Management Skills Questionnaire. The results show that: (1) Authoritarian parenting style negatively predicted children’s emotion management skills, self-control, and peer interactions; (2) Authoritative parenting style positively predicted children’s emotion management skills, self-control, and peer interactions; (3) Structural equation models indicated that self-control and peer interactions partially mediated the effects of authoritarian and authoritative parenting styles. The parenting style of Chinese children aged 3–6 years is related to emotion management skills, and self-control and peer interactions have chain mediating effects between parenting style and children’s emotion management skills. These results provide further guidance for the prevention and intervention of emotional and mental health problems in children.

Introduction

Behavioral and emotional difficulties commonly occur during childhood, often without recognition or awareness, yet they have a profound impact on mental well-being and behavioral patterns. Early and middle childhood encompass a broad spectrum of behavioral issues in children, including defiance, tantrums, aggression, and destructive tendencies (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In the late 20th century, concerns arose regarding the rising prevalence of emotional and behavioral problems among children and adolescents, leading to an interest in addressing and modifying these secular trends (Rutter and Smith, 1995). Recent nationally representative data from the NSCH reveals the widespread occurrence of depression, anxiety, and conduct problems among children and adolescents in the United States (Ghandour et al., 2019). Based on a report released by the National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities from 2005 to 2011, it is approximated that around 13–20% of children in the United States face a mental, emotional, or behavioral disorder each year (Avenevoli et al., 2013). Increased levels of aggression have been associated with emotional instability (Carlo et al., 2011; Mestre et al., 2012), as well as addictive behaviors, conduct issues, and borderline personality (Mehroof and Griffiths, 2010). However, a preschooler’s ability to conform to societal behavioral standards is influenced by both emotional comprehension and regulation (Di Maggio et al., 2017). Generally, possessing strong emotional management skills indicates adaptability and good health, while inadequate management can contribute to individual social maladjustment and behavioral problems. Neglecting to address these issues leads to considerable societal expenses and places a substantial burden on individuals and communities (Scott et al., 2001; Romeo et al., 2006), underscoring the importance of timely intervention.

Individual variations in emotional tendencies may serve as an implicit underlying factor influencing the diverse externalizing behaviors observed in children. However, the factors that shape children’s ability to regulate their emotions are intricate and multifaceted. Parenting styles within the familial context exert a substantial direct or indirect impact on children’s emotional and mental well-being, receiving considerable attention in the field of child growth and development both domestically and internationally (Ong et al., 2018; Feng et al., 2021; Rongeven et al., 2022). Numerous studies have explored the association between childhood anxiety disorders (Sahithya and Raman, 2021), emotional well-being (Le and Impett, 2019), oppositional defiant disorder (Fooladvand et al., 2021), and parenting styles. Additionally, research has investigated the efficacy of the Emotional Development (ED) module of Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) in treating early childhood depression (Luby et al., 2020). However, few studies have delved into the intricate mechanisms through which various parenting styles influence children’s emotion regulation skills, exploring the complex and intriguing network of relationships involved. Spanish researchers approached psychological research on children aged 4–6 years from the sociocultural perspective of psychological development theory, proposing the Morris triple model of early childhood adjustment (Badenes et al., 2000), which serves as the theoretical foundation for this study. The model comprises three components: (a) family factors influencing individual emotions, (b) the impact of young children’s social adjustment on individual emotions to varying degrees, and (c) the influence of family factors on young children’s adjustment. In this study, parenting style represents the family aspect, children’s emotion regulation skills represent the individual emotion aspect, and factors influencing young children’s social adjustment encompass their self-control abilities (Wang et al., 2019) and peer interactions (Badenes et al., 2000; Domberg et al., 2018). Consequently, parenting styles not only directly affect children’s emotional well-being but also exhibit lasting effects on their psychological characteristics and the development of peer relationships. The development of self-control and peer interactions has been shown to be significantly influenced by family factors, particularly parenting styles and behaviors (Russell et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2019; Ahmetoglu et al., 2022). Therefore, investigating the mechanisms through which parenting style influences children’s emotion regulation skills is essential. The objective of this study was to examine the impact of parenting style on children’s emotion regulation skills and the mediating roles played by self-control and peer interactions.

The effect of parenting style on emotion management skills

The family plays a pivotal role in a child’s growth, yet other influential environments, including schools (Chao, 2001; Veiga et al., 2021) and peer groups (Maccoby and Martin, 1983; Martinez-Escudero et al., 2023), also contribute to their development. Both parental involvement theory and bioecological theory indicate that parenting style significantly influences the healthy development of children’s emotion management skills (Morris et al., 2007; Luo et al., 2019). Parenting styles encompass a range of parental attitudes and behaviors toward raising children (Darling and Steinberg, 1993), reflecting the quality of children’s interactions with family members and their emotional well-being. Parenting approaches can be characterized by integrating two core dimensions: responsiveness and demandingness (Lamborn et al., 1991; Villarejo et al., 2023). These dimensions, namely responsiveness and demandingness, are typically seen as independent of each other (Darling and Steinberg, 1993; Fuentes et al., 2022). The term “responsiveness” captures a parent’s warmth, active involvement, and ability to nurture a child’s unique personality (Baumrind, 1991; Climent-Galarza et al., 2022). On the other hand, “demandingness” describes the level of strictness and expectations parents set for their children in aligning with societal or familial norms (Steinberg et al., 1994; Palacios et al., 2022). Within this dual-dimensional framework, several scholars have outlined specific parenting categories. These include authoritative (high in both responsiveness and demandingness), authoritarian (high demandingness, low responsiveness), neglectful (low in both dimensions), and indulgent (high responsiveness, low demandingness) as common classifications in familial research (Lamborn et al., 1991; Steinberg et al., 1994; Villarejo et al., 2023). Baumrind’s work (Baumrind, 1968; Baumrind, 1971) has distinguished between permissive-indulgent and permissive-neglectful parents, noting both a variance in levels of parental responsiveness and resultant child outcomes (Lamborn et al., 1991; Martinez-Escudero et al., 2020). Interestingly, children from families emphasizing responsiveness but lacking in demandingness often exhibit better adaptability and skills compared to their counterparts from neglectful households. Building on Baumrind’s foundational theoretical approach, Maccoby and Martin (1983) advanced the classification of parenting styles into four distinct categories: authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful. Interestingly, parenting styles and their impacts vary across cultures (Pinquart and Kauser, 2018; Martinez-Escudero et al., 2023). The cultural backdrop can significantly influence the dynamics between parenting methods and child adjustment (Pinquart and Kauser, 2018; Garcia et al., 2019). Considering China’s rich cultural tapestry deeply rooted in Confucian principles, parenting often mirrors its hierarchies and moral teachings. Such teachings promote understanding and respecting familial hierarchies and social statuses. Chinese societal norms value a child’s proper upbringing, which emphasizes elements of guidance and discipline. Therefore, parents may employ stern disciplinary measures if children fall short of set expectations, viewing this strictness as their responsibility (Chao, 1994). This rigorous parenting instills in the young an understanding of their roles within family and society, facilitating their smooth societal integration (Pan and Shang, 2023). In studies of European-American families, an authoritative approach frequently emerges as optimal for child development (Maccoby and Martin, 1983; Lamborn et al., 1991; Steinberg et al., 1994). However, China’s competitive social fabric has intensified parental involvement in children’s upbringing. There’s an evident trend of parents being increasingly directive yet responsive to their children’s needs, with neglectful parenting being a rarity (Henry et al., 2020; Liu F. et al., 2020; Liu J. et al., 2020; Vogel et al., 2021). Contemporary young Chinese parents are veering toward promoting children’s independence, challenging the traditional norm of grandparents’ indulgence. This change has led to a decline in overly permissive parenting practices (Scharff, 2022; Lin et al., 2023; Guo J. et al., 2023). Research by Wu (1996) suggests that Chinese parents frequently resort to reprimands to guide their children’s behavior, particularly in academic realms, reflecting an authoritarian approach (Kriger and Kroes, 1972; Lin and Fu, 1990). Interestingly, recent studies from European and South American contexts highlight the potential advantages of high responsiveness combined with low demandingness (Gimenez-Serrano et al., 2022; Martinez-Escudero et al., 2023; Villarejo et al., 2023). Historically, China has emphasized social harmony and order. Consequently, in Chinese familial contexts, most parents not only respond to their children’s expressions and actions but also set expectations for their emotional and behavioral conduct. An authoritative approach resonates with many Chinese parents, aligning with the cultural ethos of balance. In general, several studies focusing on Chinese-American families have identified the benefits of the authoritative style (Chao, 1994, 2001). Therefore, it can be argued that the two primary parenting styles, authoritative (characterized by both responsiveness and demandingness) and authoritarian (characterized by responsiveness without demandingness), are most prevalent in Chinese families. This research aimed to determine which of these two styles is optimal.

Researchers have long studied the mechanisms through which parenting styles influence children’s emotional health (Li J. B. et al., 2019; Li Y. et al., 2019). This research complements and builds upon Gottman’s classification of parenting styles related to emotion (Gottman et al., 1996). However, studies have typically reported differential manifestations of children’s emotional dysregulation (Camisasca et al., 2022), emotional monitoring and behavioral problems (Haslam et al., 2020) under different parenting styles, or the profound impact of children’s perceived parenting style on their mental health (Feng et al., 2021). Nevertheless, little in-depth research has been conducted on the complete system of children’s emotion management skills. According to family systems theory (Bowen, 1993), the degree of individual socialization determines how individuals experience emotional states, and at the same time, the degree of individual emotionalization is closely related to personal, family, and cultural contexts (Lewis, 1995). Thus, the family has been described as the primary environment for children’s emotional establishment and social adjustment patterns. Research has shown that authoritarian parenting styles (e.g., discipline, control, say no) negatively affect children’s emotional and psychological stability (Kawabata et al., 2011). Additionally, in Helping Families Change, Satir et al. (1994) suggest the importance of family communication theory, which states that 93% of emotional states in parent–child conversations are perceived by each other, and that an emotionally unstable or impulsive child who elicits a hostile parental response may make the child’s response more aggressive than the child’s response prior to the parent’s response (Wahl and Metzner, 2012). In contrast, authoritative parents employ positive emotional communication, characterized by active listening, understanding, and positive recognition, along with consistent rules for their children, which positively influences their children’s emotional management and psychological adjustment (Khaleque, 2013). Family systems theory highlights the critical impact of overall family system stability, harmony, and health on children’s emotional and psychological development (Lindahl et al., 2012). While numerous theoretical and empirical studies have demonstrated the influence of parenting styles on children’s emotion management skills, the role of protective factors has been relatively understudied. Firstly, research on self-control in school-aged children suggests that parent-grandparent co-parenting can predict children’s self-control through maternal authoritative parenting, and authoritative parenting itself is associated with children’s self-control (Yang et al., 2023). Pratt and Cullen (2000) meta-analysis have unequivocally shown that low self-control is significantly and positively correlated with involvement in crime and deviant behaviors. Additionally, Gibbs et al. (1998) found that low self-control mediated a substantial portion of the effect of parenting on college deviance, encompassing behaviors such as alcohol use, class-cutting, academic dishonesty, and school suspension or expulsion. Secondly, punishment and harsh parenting styles can have adverse effects on children, which can be magnified within peer interactions, leading to disharmony in social relationships (Shen et al., 2015). The effects of these variables may manifest not only in the short term but also during long-term developmental processes. In fact, detrimental effects on peer relationships, such as becoming targets of exclusion and bullying, can arise from characteristics of the parent–child relationship, including abusive, neglectful, or maladaptive parenting (Reijntjes et al., 2010, 2011; Lereya et al., 2013). Both children’s self-control and peer interactions exert powerful influences on their emotion management skills. Studies have indicated that individuals with higher levels of peer interactions and self-control tend to hold more positive beliefs about managing negative emotions and expressing positive emotions (Chow et al., 2015; Meng et al., 2020). Consequently, we hypothesize that both self-control and peer interactions mediate the relationship between parenting style and children’s emotion management skills. We proposed that parenting style is related to children’s emotion management skills (hypothesis one). We expect that authoritative parenting will have a positive correlation with children’s emotion management skills, while authoritarian parenting will have a negative correlation.

The mediating effect of self-control between parenting style and emotion management skills

The ability of children to consciously regulate their behavior in order to meet their specific needs contributes to enhanced focus, improved social adjustment, and effective coping with negative emotional experiences such as pain and frustration (Chen et al., 2012). Self-control refers to an individual’s capacity to regulate behavior, emotions, and various other responses in a timely manner to accomplish specific goals and serves as a significant indicator of early socialization (Moffitt et al., 2011). It encompasses attentional aspects, such as the ability to shift or sustain attention, as well as behavioral aspects, such as inhibiting impulses or undesirable behaviors (Duckworth and Kern, 2011). The initiation hypothesis of the psychological mechanisms of self-control (Mischel and Ayduk, 2002) suggests that children’s attention, cognition, and emotions mature as they develop self-control, with higher levels of self-control reducing an individual’s arousal in emotionally charged situations and regulating emotional expression and impulsive behaviors. Prior studies have established a significant correlation between self-control and prosocial behavior among children and adolescents, indicating that individuals with higher levels of self-control are more likely to exhibit prosocial acts (Carlo et al., 2012). Children experiencing challenges in self-control are susceptible to emotional and behavioral issues (Chui and Chan, 2015). Inadequate regulation of negative emotions can contribute to chronic emotional distress, leading to elevated levels of anxiety and depression (Nigra et al., 2015), ultimately manifesting as irritable moods and pathological behaviors commonly associated with autism. Therefore, adequate self-control facilitates individuals in planning their actions, managing their emotions, conserving ego-depleting resources, and effectively anticipating conflict precursors, making it easier to maintain emotional stability in the face of arbitrary outcomes (Hofmann et al., 2009).

Furthermore, the development of self-control is significantly influenced by family dynamics, particularly parenting styles (Russell et al., 2013). Authoritarian parenting styles, characterized by frequent negative emotions and caregiver disengagement, can hinder children’s adaptive efforts to regulate their behavior and impede the cultivation of self-control (Harrist and Waugh, 2002; Li et al., 2015). Parental rejection and excessive intervention have been found to predict children’s low self-control, as well as heightened levels of anger, anxiety, and aggression (Wang et al., 2015). The imposition of high levels of parental control and intervention may suppress children’s intrinsic motivation, subsequently limiting their exploration and adaptation abilities (Guan et al., 2018). Additionally, research suggests that individuals with higher levels of self-control demonstrate better regulation of mood and emotions, whereas poor self-control is associated with anxiety and depression (Özdemir et al., 2014; Yen et al., 2014; Pan et al., 2021). Given that poor self-control serves as a risk factor for mood disorders, it may contribute to comorbidity between anxiety-depression, substance use, and Internet addiction during later childhood (Na et al., 2017). Some empirical studies have shown that authoritative parenting facilitates children’s perceptions of behavioral autonomy, so that children believe they can make and stick to their own decisions (Yu et al., 2013), have a better sense of control and accomplishment over themselves, and reinforce their positive behaviors. Children with high levels of proprioceptive self-control have fewer externalizing problems and less dissociative distress, and are more socially competent with skills to control negative emotions, get along with others, and follow social rules (Spinrad et al., 2007; Sun et al., 2022). Calm thinking and stable emotion management allow them to focus their energy and time on solving practical problems, prefer a positive view of things, and have an optimistic personality (Baesu, 2019; Li J. B. et al., 2019; Li Y. et al., 2019). The presence of positive interactions, coupled with authoritative parenting, is linked to enhanced self-control outcomes in children throughout their developmental stages. For instance, children who engage in more mutually positive and prosocial interactions with their caregivers exhibit improved behavioral and emotional regulation (Kim and Kochanska, 2012; Davis et al., 2017). Therefore, based on the aforementioned research, we hypothesized that self-control could mediate the effects of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills (hypothesis two).

The mediating effect of peer interactions between parenting style and emotion management skills

Group dynamics theory suggests that as children grow older, the proportion of peer group participation in children’s lives will increase substantially, and that interactions in different social situations will satisfy children’s intrinsic values (Lewin, 1948; Burnes and Bargal, 2017). Peer acceptance is closely related to emotional perception and positive personality (Beazidou and Botsoglou, 2016). Peer interactions is the comprehensive ability of young children to perceive, adapt, coordinate, and manage peer relationships during interactions with peers of the same or similar age (Jiang et al., 2015). Children’s peer interactions play an important role in children’s emotion management skills, the better children’s peer interactions are, the more they promote children’s ability to identify their own and others’ emotions, manage their bad emotions, and use positive emotions to make better decisions (Mayer et al., 2004). Children who are unpopular, rejected, and ostracized by their peers often develop inappropriate emotional expressions and behavioral conflicts, but inappropriate emotions and behaviors can be observed, learned, imitated, and corrected through peer interactions to reshape emotional perceptions and have stable emotion management. When children’s ability to recognize expressions and select emotion perspectives is better developed, they are more likely to be accepted by their peers (Denham and Couchoud, 1990). Therefore, children’s peer interactions are one of the necessary conditions to determine their level of emotion management skills.

The tendency to exhibit behavioral deficits or excessive behavior increases with poorer peer interactions, and the lack of social skills leading to peer alienation predisposes to the development of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), which interferes with normal interpersonal interactions in adulthood (Corbett et al., 2014). Social learning theory states that children tend to develop their own patterns of interpersonal interactions by observing their parents’ everyday interactive behaviors (Bandura, 1976; Ladd and Kochenderfer-Ladd, 2019). Safdar and Zahrah (2016) found that children raised by authoritative parents were more dominant in peer interactions, more likely to integrate into groups and be accepted by peers, and had a significant positive correlation with peer cooperation behaviors (Lau and Power, 2020). Children with authoritarian parenting, on the other hand, are overly dependent on parents, have less self-control, are more likely to exhibit disobedient behavior, have poorer peer interactions, and often suffer from peer rejection (Lee and Wong, 2009). This situation can increase children’s pride or low self-esteem, and even lead to school bullying, social withdrawal, and self-blocking behaviors, resulting in negative emotional experiences and negative social relationship evaluations (Zheng et al., 2019).

In a study conducted in Spain, the examination of parenting style and peer attachment as predictors of emotional instability in late childhood revealed that both parenting style and peer group played equally significant roles in predicting emotional instability among children (Llorca-Mestre et al., 2017). The findings align with the principles of ecological theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979), which emphasize the continuous interaction between individual characteristics and their context. According to this theory, individual characteristics evolve and develop over time through ongoing interactions with their environment (Bronfenbrenner and Ceci, 1994). Therefore, when children’s emotional needs are not met by their parents, contextual interactions among peers play a compensatory role in children’s emotional management. Children may be able to learn from their peers how to control their emotions, suppress unreasonable desires, coordinate with others, and rationally handle peer conflicts (Wang et al., 2019). When children can correctly understand their peers’ basic mental states, such as thoughts, intentions, and emotions, and can predict and interpret their peers’ behavior through emotional transference, they will have normal, friendly interactions with their peers and perform better on tasks (Astington and Barriault, 2001). Children who are unpopular, rejected, and ostracized by their peers are more likely to experience bullying (Zhu et al., 2020) and may become more sensitive to frustration, which can further exacerbate levels of problem behavior and contribute to impaired interactions and poor social adjustment in adolescence (Buhs and Ladd, 2001). These negative childhood experiences can further increase a child’s withdrawal behaviors and rejection of peer interactions, resulting in a withdrawn personality disorder (Guina, 2016; Bang et al., 2018). There is a strong correlation between children’s emotion management and well-being and parenting styles and children’s peer interactions (Malonda et al., 2019; Zhang and Deng, 2022). Therefore, we hypothesized that parenting styles may be predictors of peer interactions, such that authoritarian parenting styles negatively predict peer interactions, whereas authoritative parenting styles positively predict peer interactions. Based on the aforementioned research, we hypothesized that peer interactions would mediate the effects of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills (hypothesis three).

The mediating effect of self-control and peer interactions between parenting style and emotion management skills

Existing research has demonstrated the protective influence of both self-control and peer interactions on the development of children’s emotion management skills (Li et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2021). Moreover, parenting style indirectly affects children’s emotion management skills through the mediating factors of self-control and peer interactions, forming a chain mediating process within the relationship (Erkan and Sop, 2018; Farzadi et al., 2021). Consequently, there is a crucial need for comprehensive investigations into the mechanisms through which parenting styles impact children’s emotion management skills. However, limited studies have directly explored the interplay between self-control, peer interactions, and their mediating effects on the association between parenting styles and children’s emotion management skills. Therefore, this study aims to examine the relationship among these four variables by drawing upon relevant theoretical frameworks and empirical evidence.

Multiple research studies have consistently demonstrated that children’s ability to exercise self-control contributes positively to their social adjustment and interactions with peers. Drawing from a situational-developmental theory perspective, children’s self-control plays a significant role in shaping their preferences for peers, thereby influencing the development of emotional stability in later stages (Llorca-Mestre et al., 2017). Children with high levels of self-control exhibit better regulation of their behavior and emotions, leading to more appropriate and positive interactions with peers. Consequently, favorable group preferences contribute to higher levels of peer status and act as a protective factor against internalization problems in children (Heinrich and Gullone, 2006). In contrast, children with low self-control tend to interpret social cues in group settings as threatening or hostile toward themselves, which can trigger intense emotional reactions and a hostile disposition, making them more prone to engaging in bullying behaviors (Plexousakis et al., 2019; Camodeca and Nava, 2022). It is evident that high levels of self-control are associated with increased opportunities for social interactions, which, in turn, impact children’s ability to engage with peers, form social relationships, enhance their sense of belonging, and foster positive emotional experiences during peer interactions. Additionally, Chinese parents place a strong emphasis on fostering their children’s socialization, including the control of negative emotions and the suppression of desires to prioritize group interests and attain a favorable position within peer relationships (Liu et al., 2017). Given the link between children’s self-control and their peer interactions—both of which jointly and adaptively moderate and manage their emotions and behaviors—this study proposes a third pathway. We speculate that parenting styles impact children’s emotion regulation skills through a chain mediation process that involves both self-control and peer interactions (hypothesis four).

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 2,397 participants were recruited from 16 kindergartens (both public and private) in 10 provinces in seven geographic regions (northern, eastern, southern, central, northwestern, southwestern, and northeastern) of China using stratified cluster sampling. Data collection took place at the start of the spring school year in 2023. Prior permission was obtained from the schools, and written informed consent was obtained from both parents and teachers of the participants. The informed consent forms ensured anonymity and confidentiality and did not require signatures or include subject names. Parents and teachers were clearly informed that participation in the study was voluntary, and they had the right to withdraw at any time. Those who agreed to participate independently completed the paper-and-pencil questionnaire during the parent-teacher conference, with the guidance of a trained research assistant. After completion, the questionnaires provided by the children’s parents and teachers were sealed in envelopes. A small token of appreciation, such as a flower or snack, was given to participants as a gesture of gratitude. Upon screening, 94 questionnaires were identified as invalid due to significant missing information. Thus, a total of 2,303 questionnaires were included in the final analysis, resulting in an overall response rate of 96.08%. Participants were aged 3–6 years (M = 4.00, SD = 0.499), 1,180 (51.2%) were boys and 1,123 (48.8%) were girls, 1,082 (34.0%) were in junior class, 782 (34.0%) in middle class, 737 (32.0%) in senior class. The two questionnaires were completed by 476 fathers and 1,827 mothers. These participants were categorized into three age groups (M = 31.00, SD = 0.532): 229 were under 30 years of age, 1,702 were between 30 and 40 years of age, and 372 were over 40 years of age.

Before the survey was started, the local education department and the survey schools were consulted to obtain their approval to conduct the study. During the survey, we asked the principal and teachers about the basic conditions of the campus, and the researcher informed the parents and teachers about the purpose and procedures of the study and administered the questionnaires. The questionnaires were administered in the classroom, where teachers conducted parent-teacher conferences with parents. Participants provided written consent before completing the study questionnaire and were informed of the purpose, confidentiality, and anonymity of this study.

All study materials in this study were reviewed and approved by the research ethics committee of the corresponding author’s university.

Instruments

Parenting styles and dimensions questionnaire-short version

The PSDQ-Short Version (Robinson et al., 2001) with 27 items is a modified version of the original 62-item PSDQ (Robinson et al., 1996), developed to measure parent’s self-own behavior and report their behavior with children. The shortened version of the PSDQ consists of two dimensions: authoritarian parenting style and authoritative parenting style, and was completed by the parents. An example item is “I punish my children by excluding them, without explanation”. Each item based on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always). The scale has been shown to have good reliability and validity. Also, it has shown excellent psychometric properties in the Chinese subject population (Robinson et al., 2001; Li et al., 2012). In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the subscales of authoritarian parenting style and authoritative parenting style were 0.95 and 0.92, respectively.

Self-control teacher rating questionnaire

The 22-item Self-Control Teacher Rating Questionnaire (SCTRQ) (Dong, 2005) is a modified version of the original 33-item Self-Control Rating Scale (SCRS-C) by Kendall and Wilcox (Kendall and Wilcox, 1979). The SCRS was developed to assess children’s levels of self-control and is evaluated by their teachers. An example item is “The child can stop what he or she is doing to listen when the teacher starts talking.” Items in the questionnaire are scored on a five-point scale. There are questions with reverse scoring (questions 17 and 22), and the questionnaire encompasses a total of four dimensions. Evidence has been provided in studies of the effects of parenting practices and self-control on problem behavior in left-behind and non-left-behind children in rural China (Zhang et al., 2023). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the scale in this study was 0.94.

Peer interaction skills scale

The Peer Interaction Skills Scale (PISS) (Zhang, 2002; Qin, 2022; Qin and Caizhen, 2022) was designed to assess children’s peer interaction skills. It encompasses four dimensions: social initiative, verbal and non-verbal interactions, social barriers, and pro-social behavior. Vandell (2000) noted that teachers can influence children’s peer interaction skills through their own perceptions of the classroom. Therefore, the questionnaire was completed by the teachers based on the children’s daily learning and living performance. A sample item reads, “The child can proactively introduce himself/herself to new peers.” The PISS contains four dimensions measured on a four-point scale. The higher the total score, the higher the level of peer interaction skills. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.84.

Emotion management skills questionnaire

The 30-item Emotion Management Skills Questionnaire (EMSQ) (Wu, 2013; Qiu et al., 2021) comprises three dimensions and is completed by teachers, who assess children’s daily emotional expressions and reactions. The questionnaire contains 11 items related to emotion perception skills, 13 items focused on emotion regulation skills, and 6 items assessing emotion utilization skills. Emotion perception refers to the child’s ability to accurately identify both their own and others’ emotional states, as well as to perform preliminary analyses to express emotions appropriately (e.g., “The child notices when the moods of friends around them change”). Emotion regulation pertains to the child’s ability to manage their emotions in accordance with organizational norms specific to a given situation (e.g., “The child takes the initiative to stop a fight between peers”). Emotion utilization skills involve the child’s capability to convey emotions appropriately through facial expressions and behavior to foster positive interpersonal relationships (e.g., “When they are happy or sad, they do not display it overtly and tend to appear emotionally reserved”). Each item is measured using a 5-point Likert scale to evaluate the child’s ability to manage their emotions effectively. The higher the score of children’s emotional management skills scale,the better the children’s ability to maintain their own emotional stability. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the subscale in the present study was 0.94.

Data analysis

Statistical analysis in this study was conducted using SPSS 26.0 and Mplus 8.3. First, we used Harman’s one-way method to test for the presence of common method bias (Zhou and Long, 2004). Second, we completed descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation analysis on all study variables by running SPSS 26.0. Considering that gender, age, and grade level may be additional influencing factors for children aged 3–6 years, we decided to control for these variables in the analysis. Third, structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to examine mediating effects. To explore the mediating effects of children’s self-control and their peer interactions in the pathway of parenting styles on children’s emotion management skills, we conducted variable-by-variable analyses of authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles to assess a diverse set of hypotheses. Finally, the bootstrap method (Tibshirani and Efron, 1993) was used to repeat the sample 5,000 times to test the mediating effect of children’s self-control and peer interactions in the pathway of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills.

Results

Common method bias test

Harman’s one-way method was used to test for the existence of common method bias (Zhou and Long, 2004). Exploratory factor analysis resulted in 12 factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. The first factor accounted for 36.90% of the total variance, which is less than 40% of the critical standard, indicating that common method bias is not apparent. Therefore, data analysis can proceed.

Descriptive statistics and significance test

Bivariate correlations between the core variables were performed using Pearson correlation analysis. The results are presented in Table 1, including the means, SDs, and Pearson correlation coefficients of the core variables. The results indicated that all the core variables were significantly related. Emotion management skills were negatively associated with authoritarian parenting style (r = −0.640, p < 0.001), but positively associated with authoritative parenting style (r = 0.733, p < 0.001), self-control (r = 0.723, p < 0.001), and peer interactions (r = 0.700, p < 0.001). Authoritarian parenting style was negatively associated with self-control (r = −0.656, p < 0.001) and peer interactions (r = −0.570, p < 0.001). Whereas authoritative parenting style was positively associated with self-control (r = 0.586, p < 0.001) and peer interactions (r = 0.602, p < 0.001). In addition, self-control was positively associated with peer interactions (r = 0.663, p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis of all the variable.

Total effect, direct effect, and indirect effect of the chain mediating effect

A chain mediation model was tested, which consisted of three indirect effects as follows: (1) self-control played a mediating role in the relationship between parenting styles and children’s emotion management skills; (2) peer interactions mediated the relationship between parenting styles and children’s emotion management skills; and (3) parenting styles may indirectly affect children’s emotion management skills through the chain mediation of self-control and peer interactions (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The mediating roles of self-control and peer interactions between authoritarian parenting style and emotion management ability. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

The SEM was used to examine the mediating effects of self- control and peer interactions between parenting styles and emotion management skills. The results showed that authoritarian parenting style and emotion management skills established significant and negative relationships (β = −0.640, t = −52.028, p < 0.001). After accounting for the influences of control variables, the mediation model showed that authoritarian parenting style had a significant negative direct effect on both self-control (β = −0.659, t = −49.244, p < 0.001) and emotion management skills (β = −0.100, t = −5.080, p < 0.001). Meanwhile, self-control had a significant positive direct effect on both the peer interactions (β = 0.725, t = 40.725, p < 0.001) and emotion management skills (β = 0.329, t = 12.201, p < 0.001). The peer interactions had a significant positive direct effect on emotion management skills (β = 0.532, t = 122.453, p < 0.001; see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Research hypothesis model.

As shown in Figure 3, authoritative parenting style had a significant positive direct effect on self-control (β = 0.591, t = 39.157, p < 0.001), peer interactions (β = 0.304, t = 15.867, p < 0.001), and emotion management skills (β = 0.315, t = 19.139, p < 0.001). Self-control had a significant positive direct effect on both peer interactions (β = 0.633, t = 35.437, p < 0.001) and emotion management skills (β = 0.338, t = 14.527, p < 0.001). Peer interactions had a significant positive direct effect on emotion management skills (β = 0.377, t = 14.988, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The mediating roles of self-control and peer interactions between authoritative parenting style and emotion management ability. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Furthermore, as shown in Table 2, the total effect of authoritarian parenting style on emotion management skills was −0.641 (SE = 0.018, 95% CI [−0.674, −0.604], p < 0.001) and the direct effect was −0.100 (SE = 0.020, 95% CI [−0.139, −0.062], p < 0.001), indicating that both the total effect and the direct effects were statistically significant. The indirect effect was −0.217 (SE = 0.018, 95% CI [−0.254, −0.183], p < 0.001) in the pathway of authoritarian parenting style → self-control → emotion management skills, and the mediation effect accounted for 33.801% of the total effect (−0.642). And the indirect effect was −0.071 (SE = 0.010, 95% CI [−0.092, −0.051], p < 0.001) in the pathway of authoritarian parenting style → peer interactions → emotion management skills, and the mediation effect accounted for 11.059% of the total effect. Finally, the indirect effect was −0.254(SE = 0.013, 95% CI [−0.280, −0.230], p < 0.001) in the pathway of authoritarian parenting style → self-control → peer interactions → emotion management skills, and the mediation effect accounted for 39.564% of the total effect. Then, as shown in Table 3, the total effect of authoritative parenting style on emotion management skills was 0.770 (SE = 0.012, 95% CI [0.747, 0.793], p < 0.001), and the direct effect was 0.315 (SE = 0.016, 95% CI [0.282, 0.346], p < 0.001), indicating that both the total effect and the direct effect were statistically significant. The indirect effect was 0.200 (SE = 0.014, 95% CI [0.173, 0.230], p < 0.001) in the pathway of authoritative parenting style → self-control → emotion management skills, and the mediation effect accounted for 25.940% of the total effect (0.771). And the indirect effect was 0.115 (SE = 0.010, 95% CI [0.095, 0.136], p < 0.001) in the pathway of authoritative parenting style → peer interactions → emotion management skills, and the mediation effect accounted for 14.916% of the total effect. Finally, the indirect effect was 0.141 (SE = 0.011, 95% CI [0.121, 0. 163], p < 0.001) in the pathway of authoritative parenting style → self-control → peer interactions → emotion management skills, and the mediation effect accounted for 18.288% of the total effect. Given that the bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals do not include zero, the statistical significance of these three indirect effects is established. The data analysis revealed that the indirect effect of parenting styles (authoritarian parenting style and authoritative parenting style) on emotion management skills was contingent upon self-control and peer interactions, which served as significant and positive partial mediators in the association between parenting styles (authoritarian parenting style and authoritative parenting style) and emotion management skills.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Direct, indirect, and total effects of the hypothesized model.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Direct, indirect, and total effects of the hypothesized model.

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the impact of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills, considering the factors of self-control and peer interactions. Our findings validate parental involvement theory (McCurdy and Daro, 2001), bioecological theory (Tudge et al., 2022), and Morris’ triple model of early childhood adjustment (Morris et al., 2017), providing support for the notion that self-control mediates the relationship between parenting style and emotion management skills. Furthermore, peer interactions mediate the relationship between negative parenting style and emotion management skills. Additionally, both self-control and peer interactions serve as chain mediators in the association between parenting style and emotion management skills. Finally, the results of this study support Gottman’s emotion-based approach to parenting and lay the groundwork for determining whether children express constructive or destructive affective tendencies (Jespersen et al., 2021).

First, this study found a significant relationship between parenting style and children’s emotion management skills between the ages of 3–6 (hypothesis one). The evidence confirms that parenting style is the primary factor influencing differences in children’s emotion management skills, and that the more authoritative parenting attitudes and behaviors are, the stronger children’s emotion management skills have, with a positive association. Whereas authoritarian parenting styles negatively affect children’s emotion management skills. The results of this pathway are consistent with previous studies (Tao et al., 2010; Acar et al., 2019; Cantekin and Akduman, 2020; Kashyap et al., 2020). The direct link between parenting style and emotion management skills provides theoretical support for parental involvement theory (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2005), which states that parents who take an active role tend to listen patiently to their children rather than reprimanding and punishing them, and are more likely to participate in children’s activities and guide children’s emotional perception, expression, and control either directly or subliminally. In such a family environment, children gain a sense of security, increase their self-confidence and independence, and further improve their emotion management skills balanced by their self-control (Guo Y. et al., 2023). High-quality parent–child interactions result in children feeling supported by their families, accumulating positive energy, being more advantaged in complex social situations, having better self-control, and having positive and optimistic stability (Dinkha et al., 2023; Li et al., 2023).

A longitudinal study of parental behaviors related to children in the United States highlighted warmth and emotionally democratic parenting as determinants of good social and emotional development in children (Garthe et al., 2015). However, according to social learning theory, the more often parents adopt authoritarian parenting attitudes that are less supportive and more intrusive, the more difficult it is for children to regulate their emotions during parent–child interaction tasks (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2004). Children tend to hide their emotions or have variable emotions, intimacy is relatively difficult to establish, and the parent–child bonding is in an unhealthy state of development (Hollenstein and Lewis, 2006; Duncan et al., 2009; Tao et al., 2020). The “helicopter parenting” of a collectivist society produces “good children who know what they are doing” and “subordinates who do not think for themselves but follow orders (Ho et al., 2022; Hwang et al., 2023). “Helicopter parents” interfere and manipulate their children’s thoughts and behavior always, and help them decide everything. In this kind of authoritarian family in China, children are only responsible for studying, working and living according to the path planned by their parents or other adults, so there are a large number of “giant babies” with high scores and low abilities who can only study but not actually live (Zhang et al., 2022). Their emotional changes are completely dependent on their parents’ approval, and their emotional stability is brought about by getting used to being planned every step of the way. When they lose the guidance of their parents or teachers, they experience anxiety symptoms of restlessness, worry, confusion, and are in emotional dysregulation and self-doubt (Cui et al., 2019), which seriously affect their physical and mental health (Gao et al., 2023). Based on the above discussion, firstly, in the process of parenting, parents should not only play the role of modeling emotions to improve the quality of parent–child bonding interaction, but also convey positive emotions and emotion management strategies. Secondly, parents should provide guidance and effective communication about children’s hidden and variable emotions, and focus on their inadequate emotion management or behavioral withdrawal, as both may be the cause and effect of children’s behavioral problems.

Second, this study revealed that self-control mediates the relationship between parenting style and children’s emotion management skills, i.e., authoritarian parenting mitigates the negative impact on children’s ability to manage their emotions through greater self-control (hypothesis two). This is similar to findings from previous studies (Özdemir et al., 2013; Briki, 2020; Pan et al., 2021). Overall, the internal mechanisms by which parenting style affect children’s emotion management skills are consistent. As an extension of previous research, the present study shows that parenting style influence children’s emotion management skills through their self-control. This suggests that self-control may be an important factor in protecting against emotional disorders.

During early childhood, the development of self-control is influenced by a range of environmental stressors and supports, shaping its trajectory (Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University, 2011). Individual variations in child self-control have been found to predict outcomes in adulthood, including physical and mental health, criminal behavior, and socioeconomic status (Gagne, 2017). Parenting style plays a significant role in shaping individual differences in self-control among children and adolescents (Gibson et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2016; Li J. B. et al., 2019; Li Y. et al., 2019). On the one hand, with a more stable parent–child relationship, children have a greater sense of integration and trust in the family environment, better control over themselves, and high self-control to better regulate their attentional and behavioral problems so as to display appropriate emotional states (Schmeichel and Baumeister, 2010). Establishing good intimacy between children and parents also promotes mutual emotional contagion and emotional management skills. Therefore, a positive, enthusiastic, and responsive authoritative parenting style can support and promote children’s ability to self-regulate, whereas a negative, harsh, and insensitive authoritarian parenting style appears to have detrimental effects (Gajos and Beaver, 2016). On the other hand, when children’s emotion management skills are not available from the foundational source of parenting style, the ontological factor of self-control comes into play. Research has shown that people with higher levels of self-management tend to have rational self-perceptions and positive self-initiatives (Vohs et al., 2014). Emotional stability and switching between different emotions require the ego to have a more sensitive sense of perception and a stronger level of control. As a result, children’s mastery of self-control will build up their strong willpower from their inner being, promote the development of their good personalities and the successive formation of comprehensive abilities. External emotional management and behavioral dominance will increase in response to more positive evaluations from others, allowing children to integrate more quickly and comfortably into their social or natural environments. At the same time, children’s emotional responses and behaviors become more ethical and promote the development of socialization.

Third, the present study also found a mediating effect of peer interactions between parenting style and emotion management skills, where authoritative parenting style could improve children’s emotion management skills by influencing their peer interactions. This finding is consistent with national and international research that higher levels of peer interactions are associated with authoritative parenting (Bornstein and Bornstein, 2007; Morris et al., 2021; Lanjekar et al., 2022), suggesting that peer interactions play an important mediating role in the way parenting styles influence children’s emotion management skills. Authoritative parenting styles contribute positively to children’s peer interactions in a study of the relationship between effortful control and character anger in Chinese children, and that higher peer interactions enhances children’s good social adjustment function and cultivates individual pro-sociality and a sense of collective responsibility (Zhou et al., 2004). Well-functioning, well-adjusted families have the capacity to successfully mitigate a wide range of developmental threats to children, while at the same time reducing the likelihood of maladjustment in at-risk groups of children (Azimi et al., 2012).

The most significant difference between peer relationships and parent–child and teacher-student relationships is the “equality” of peer groups, which makes it clear that peer interactions are egalitarian and can be constructed by choice, which helps children express their emotions, transfer information, and learn from each other (Reitz et al., 2014; Leung et al., 2021). In this model, children are more active and willing to accept each other’s expressions of opinions and emotions, further developing children’s interpersonal skills. Children’s current and future cognitive development and social adjustment will be negatively affected if they have problems interacting with their peers (Bosacki, 2015). The present study found that authoritative parenting styles positively predicted children’s peer interactions, consistent with previous research (Hong et al., 2021; Marcone et al., 2021). The higher the ability to interact with peers, the better their ability to manage their emotions, which is consistent with Eisenberg’s findings (Eisenberg et al., 2001a). Summary of the above, peer interactions play a partial mediating role in the relationship between parenting style and emotion management skills (hypothesis three). First, as children are exposed to a complex and diverse environment, along with the demands of physiological development, they gradually pay attention to external perceptions and imitate the behaviors and emotional expressions of members of that environment, both consciously and unconsciously. Children with authoritative parents are more emotionally stable, have better peer interactions, have fewer externalizing and implicit problem behaviors, and are more socially adjusted, resulting in higher academic achievement (Kim et al., 2018; Steele and McKinney, 2019). Children who form secure attachments with positive parenting styles are more likely to develop responsive, harmonious peer interactions in kindergarten and elementary school, and these children will have more peer support (Coleman, 2003). Second, children’s peer interactions play a mitigating or ameliorating role in the process of the negative effects of authoritarian parenting styles on children’s emotion management skills, consistent with ecosystem theory (Bronfenbrenner and Morris, 2007). Children in authoritarian parenting styles tend to have social impairments, more academic problems, exhibit higher levels of negative emotions, and experience both explicit and implicit problem behaviors (Ng-Knight et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2018). They are often unable to develop a healthy emotional management system that is conducive to the development of psychological distress in children. When children face unpleasant situations and obstacles in such negative parenting, peers can re-engage their own emotional perceptions through communication or distraction, and their ability to manage emotions will provide time to consider emotional control, enabling children to choose the final emotion presented and promoting the continued occurrence of positive emotions. Such children who are able to use a constructive approach to managing negative emotions also achieve higher social efficacy, reducing their chances of rejection and exclusion by peers (Eisenberg et al., 2001b) and re-establishing better emotion management skills in peer interactions. Therefore, peer interactions are a highly contextualized imitation and learning process in which external and internal emotions are more likely to be displayed. Children discover themselves and others as emotional individuals, develop unconscious emotional imitation and conscious emotional awareness (Izard et al., 2008), become more aware of their emotional problems, and continue to acquire skills for managing their emotions.

Forth, this study revealed a chain mediating effect, with self-control and peer interactions playing intermediary roles between parenting style and emotion management skills (hypothesis four). These findings support the idea that children with authoritative parenting styles tend to exhibit higher levels of self-control and engage in more positive peer interactions, thereby facilitating the development of their emotion management skills (Chase-Lansdale et al., 1995; Pan et al., 2021; Papadopoulos, 2021). Conversely, children with authoritarian parenting styles tend to have lower levels of self-control and limited peer interactions, impeding the development of their emotion management skills and leading to emotional dysregulation and behavioral problems. These results are consistent with prior research findings (De Minzi, 2007; Gagnon et al., 2014). According to the situational developmental perspective, the family, community, and culture significantly influence children’s emotional stability, while their attitudes, judgments, emotional perceptions, and behavioral expressions are highly susceptible to external influences from others’ behaviors and group norms (Lamm et al., 2018). Inhibitory control skills are crucial for successful school adjustment, positive peer relationships, and social–emotional functioning (Rueda et al., 2005; Bell and Deater-Deckard, 2007; Crandall et al., 2015). Both inhibitory control and effortful control are important aspects of self-control in children (Scholtes et al., 2021). Given that weaker self-control is associated with lower academic achievement, difficulties in establishing positive peer relationships, and deficits in social–emotional functioning, further research should investigate the ways in which interactive coordination can support healthy developmental outcomes for children (Blair, 2002).

Children who are guided by their parents’ positive emotional concepts and emotional behaviors at an early age will develop better self-control. Simultaneously, children who interact with their peers will develop more positive emotions about themselves and others to create happy atmosphere, and such positive emotions will infect the entire peer group from individual to individual, influencing each other emotion management skills for better or worse. Children with high levels of emotion management have better empathy and are able to put themselves in the shoes of their peers, which not only enhances children’s observational and analytical skills, but also motivates them to share emotional feelings, speculate on emotion-generating motivations, and improve self-social adaptability, consistent with previous research on similar models (Llorca-Mestre et al., 2017). Overall, this finding extends previous research that has examined the complex relationships among parenting style, self-control, peer interactions, and children’s emotion management skills within the framework of Morris’ three-model of early childhood adjustment. Authoritative parenting styles help children develop better self-control and become popular in peer interactions, both of which promote children’s ability to manage their emotions. Conversely, children in authoritarian parenting styles develop poorer levels of self-control and peer interactions that hinder the development of their emotion management skills. This highlights the need to pay more attention to family education and parenting style choices to improve children’s emotional management and mental health, then to shape physically and mentally healthy children through better self-control and peer interactions.

Implications

This study examined the influence of parenting styles on children’s emotion management skills from the perspective of self-control and peer interactions, and provided theoretical and practical guidance on how families and schools can actively and rationally promote children’s emotion management skills. At the same time, it established a reference direction for parents to choose which parenting styles to implement. Our findings showed that self-control plays an important role in the influence of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills. Parents should promote their children’s self-control by using parenting behaviors that are high in support and understanding, low in rejection and over-interference. Our findings also indicated that peer interactions play an important role in the influence of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills and affect their personality development. Therefore, parents should pay attention not only to the establishment of appropriate self-control but also to the development of their children’s peer interactions. Parents need to cultivate their children’s self-control and peer interactions through parenting attitudes that are high in emotional warmth, high in supportive acceptance, low in rejection, and low in excessive control, which, in turn, promote the growth of their children’s emotion management skills. Specifically, parents must first be sensitive to children’s emotional changes and provide positive emotional experiences to satisfy children’s emotional needs and psychological demands. For example, in their relationship with their children, parents are not only listeners but also companions. They need to excel at observing and guiding their children’s emotional responses and fostering their awareness of emotion management. When parents listen attentively and provide warmth, acceptance, and emotional support, children will perceive more positive emotional feedback from their caregivers and demonstrate stronger emotional skills (Feng et al., 2021). Second, children will learn and regulate uncomfortable emotions in peer interactions, redirecting emotional attention, reducing both the intensity and maintenance of negative emotions, and maintaining emotional health without stressing the mind and body (Li J. B. et al., 2019; Li Y. et al., 2019). Parents and peers play a crucial role in children’s emotional growth and development. Thus, emotional harmony within the family and positive peer relationships are essential for healthy physical and mental development in childhood. This is why early childhood educators should emphasize family education and the cultivation of beneficial peer relationships for children. Ultimately, the combined strengths of the family, preschool, and community create opportunities for children to develop healthy emotion management skills by taking full advantage of multiple environmental resources to learn and experience.

Limitations and future work

The results of the current study should be considered in light of its limitations. First, due to the cross-sectional design of the data, we could not infer causal relationships among the variables. For example, it is not clear that children’s lower emotional competence is caused by negative parenting or poor peer interactions, a longitudinal study would be needed to explore these possibilities. Second, we controlled for the age factor of children aged 3–6 years in the study, and all variables were reported using other-rated questionnaires. Although multi-subject reports increase the validity of the data, they lack data on children’s self-perceptions, so future studies suggest that researchers can collect data directly from children’s responses and performance through play-based tests combined with other-rated questionnaires to improve measurement accuracy. Third, this study only explored the mediating mechanism of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills without considering individual differences, and future studies could conduct a more in-depth analysis of the relationship between parenting style and emotion management skills from the perspective of regulation mechanism. Fourth, considering the Chinese cultural context in which parental socialization occurs, we focused on comparing and analyzing the two dominant parenting styles, without considering the permissive style from the three-style model. While we have observed the benefits of the demanding and responsive authoritative parenting styles for children’s development, it might be worthwhile to explore the potential advantages of the authoritarian parenting style for children in future research. Fifth, our subjects were selected from China, and culture is also one of the important contextual factors. For example, as a collectivist country, China will show higher collective will and more harmonious consistency (Marginson and Yang, 2020). Therefore, cultural and geographical differences should be considered in the subjects, and the relationship between the variables discussed in the model may be different. Therefore, a larger sample is needed in future studies to build research models that can be generalized to different countries or regions.

Conclusion

The present research investigated the status of emotion management skills among Chinese children aged 3–6 years, while exploring various preceding factors that exert influence on emotion management and emotional stability. Specifically, this study reveals the differential effects of two different types of parenting styles on children’s emotion management skills. Furthermore, self-control and peer interactions were identified as a potential mediator in the relationship between parenting style and children’s emotion management skills. In addition, this study examined the following mediating pathways: authoritarian parenting style → self-control → peer interactions → emotion management skills and authoritative parenting style → self-control → peer interactions → emotion management skills. As children’s levels of self-control and peer interactions increase, the impact of parenting style on children’s emotion management skills becomes more pronounced. This study contributes both theoretically and practically. Firstly, it enhances the existing theoretical framework concerning the connection between parenting style and children’s emotion management skills, shedding light on the underlying mechanisms by which parenting style impacts the occurrence of children’s emotional well-being. Secondly, this study integrates environmental and individual perspectives in the exploration of potential factors that can ameliorate children’s emotion management skills, which will provide a more comprehensive picture of the development of emotion management in Chinese children between the ages of 3 and 6 years. Lastly, the results of this study provide practical implications for parents seeking the best parenting style to develop good emotion management skills in children and contribute to family education.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee Review Board of Liaoning Normal University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

DL: writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. WL: conceptualization, methodology, software, formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. XZ: writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Foundation of Liaoning Province Education Administration‘s Key Research Project for Universities, grant number LJKZR20220114.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Acar, I. H., Uçuş, Ş., and Yıldız, S. (2019). Parenting and Turkish children’s behaviour problems: the moderating role of qualities of parent–child relationship. Early Child Dev. Care 189, 1072–1085. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1365362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmetoglu, E., Acar, I. H., and Ozturk, M. A. (2022). Parental involvement and children’s peer interactions. Curr. Psychol. 41, 4447–4456. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00965-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Vol. 5, Washington, DC: American psychiatric association.

Google Scholar

Astington, J. W., and Barriault, T. (2001). Children's theory of mind: how young children come to understand that people have thoughts and feelings. Infant. Young. Child. 13, 1–12. doi: 10.1097/00001163-200113030-00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avenevoli, S., Baio, J., Bitsko, R. H., Blumberg, S. J., Brody, D. J., Crosby, A., et al. (2013). Mental health surveillance among children–United States, 2005–2011. MMWR Suppl. 62, 1–35. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azimi, A. L., Vaziri, S., and Kashani, F. L. (2012). Relationship between maternal parenting style and child's aggressive behavior. Procedia. Soc. Behav. Sci. 69, 1276–1281. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.12.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badenes, L. V., Clemente Estevan, R. A., and García Bacete, F. J. (2000). Theory of mind and peer rejection at school. Soc. Dev. 9, 271–283. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baesu, C. (2019). Leadership based on emotional intelligence in modern organizations. USV Ann. Econ. Public Admin. 18, 73–78.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1976). Social learning analysis of aggression.

Google Scholar

Bang, K. S., Kim, S., Kang, K., and Song, M. (2018). Physical symptoms, depression, and related factors of late school-age children in Seoul, Korea: the mediating role of peer relationships. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 43, e120–e125. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2018.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1968). Authoritarian vs authoritative parental demandingness. Adolesc 3, 255–272.

Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Dev. Psychol. 4, 1–103. doi: 10.1037/h0030372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1991). “Effective parenting during the early adolescent transition” in Family transitions. eds. P. A. Cowan and E. M. Hetherington (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 111–163.

Google Scholar

Beazidou, E., and Botsoglou, K. (2016). Peer acceptance and friendship in early childhood: the conceptual distinctions between them. Ear. Child Dev. Care. 186, 1615–1631. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2015.1117077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, M. A., and Deater-Deckard, K. (2007). Biological systems and the development of self-regulation: integrating behavior, genetics, and psychophysiology. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 28, 409–420. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181131fc7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. Am. Psychol. 57:111. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, L., and Bornstein, M. H. (2007). Parenting styles and child social development. Encycl. Early. Child Dev. 4, 1–4.

Google Scholar

Bosacki, S. L. (2015). Children's theory of mind, self-perceptions, and peer relations: a longitudinal study. Infant Child Dev. 24, 175–188. doi: 10.1002/icd.1878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowen, M. (1993). Family therapy in clinical practice. New York: Jason Aronson.

Google Scholar

Briki, W. (2020). Active exercisers with a higher orientation toward task-approach goal might experience higher happiness: the mediating role of dispositional self-control. Inter. J. Sport. Exe. Psychol. 18, 581–591. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2019.1570534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U., and Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature - nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychol. Rev. 101, 568–586. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U., and Morris, P. A. (2007). “The bioecological model of human development,” in Handbook of child psychology. Eds. W. Damon and R. Lerner (New York: Wiley), 793–828.

Google Scholar

Buhs, E. S., and Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as antecedent of young children's school adjustment: an examination of mediating processes. Dev. Psychol. 37:550. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.4.550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnes, B., and Bargal, D. (2017). Kurt Lewin: 70 years on. J. Chg. Mgmt. 17, 91–100. doi: 10.1080/14697017.2017.1299371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camisasca, E., Miragoli, S., Di Blasio, P., and Feinberg, M. (2022). Pathways among negative co-parenting, parenting stress, authoritarian parenting style, and Child adjustment: the emotional dysregulation driven model. J. Child Fam. Stud. 31, 3085–3096. doi: 10.1007/s10826-022-02408-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camodeca, M., and Nava, E. (2022). The long-term effects of bullying, victimization, and bystander behavior on emotion regulation and its physiological correlates. J. Interpers. Violence 37:NP2056-NP2075. doi: 10.1177/0886260520934438

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantekin, D., and Akduman, G. G. (2020). Annelerin çocuk yetiştirme tutumları ile çocuklarının duygularını yönetme becerileri arasındaki ilişkinin incelenmesi. Anadolu. Psikiyatr. De. 21, 87–92. doi: 10.5455/apd.47631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlo, G., Crockett, L. J., Wolff, J. M., and Beal, S. J. (2012). The role of emotional reactivity, self-regulation, and puberty in adolescents' prosocial behaviors. Soc. Dev. 21, 667–685. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00660.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Tur, A., and Armenta, B. E. (2011). The longitudinal relations among dimensions of parenting styles, sympathy, prosocial moral reasoning, and prosocial behaviors. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 35, 116–124. doi: 10.1177/0165025410375921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University (2011). Building the brain’s “air traffic control” system: how early experiences shape the development of executive function. Working Paper No. 11 Available at: https://www.developingchild.harvard.edu

Google Scholar

Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Dev. 65, 1111–1119. doi: 10.2307/1131308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Dev. 72, 1832–1843. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Wakschlag, L. S., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (1995). A psychological perspective on the development of caring in children and youth: the role of the family. J. Adolesc. 18, 515–556. doi: 10.1006/jado.1995.1037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Huang, X., Wang, L., and Chang, L. (2012). Aggression, peer relationships, and depression in Chinese children: a multiwave longitudinal study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 1233–1241. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02576.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, J. T., Hui, C. M., and Lau, S. (2015). A depleted mind feels inefficacious: Ego-depletion reduces self-efficacy to exert further self-control. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 45, 754–768. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chui, W. H., and Chan, H. C. O. (2015). Self-control, school bullying perpetration, and victimization among Macanese adolescents. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 1751–1761. doi: 10.1007/s10826-014-9979-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Climent-Galarza, S., Alcaide, M., Garcia, O. F., Chen, F., and Garcia, F. (2022). Parental socialization, delinquency during adolescence and adjustment in adolescents and adult children. Behav. Sci. 12, 1–21. doi: 10.3390/bs12110448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleman, P. K. (2003). Perceptions of parent-child attachment, social self-efficacy, and peer relationships in middle childhood. Infant. Child Dev: Int. J. Res. Pra. 12, 351–368. doi: 10.1002/icd.316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbett, B. A., Qualls, L. R., Valencia, B., Fecteau, S. M., and Swain, D. M. (2014). Peer-mediated theatrical engagement for improving reciprocal social interaction in autism spectrum disorder. Front. Pediatr. 2:110. doi: 10.3389/fped.2014.00110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crandall, A., Deater-Deckard, K., and Riley, A. W. (2015). Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: a conceptual framework. Dev. Rev. 36, 105–126. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, M., Darling, C. A., Coccia, C., Fincham, F. D., and May, R. W. (2019). Indulgent parenting, helicopter parenting, and well-being of parents and emerging adults. J. Child Fam. Stud. 28, 860–871. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-01314-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darling, N., and Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: an integrative model. Psychol. Bull. 113, 487–496. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M., Bilms, J., and Suveg, C. (2017). In sync and in control: a meta-analysis of parent–child positive behavioral synchrony and youth self-regulation. Fam. Pro. 56, 962–980. doi: 10.1111/famp.12259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Minzi, M. R. (2007). Parental styles and attachment in relation with self-control, social skills and coping in children at risk for poverty. New Dev. Parent Child. Rel. 4, 87–110.

Google Scholar

Denham, S. A., and Couchoud, E. A. (1990). Young preschoolers’ understanding of emotions. Child. Stud. J. 20, 171–192.

Google Scholar

Di Maggio, R., Zappulla, C., Pace, U., and Izard, C. E. (2017). Adopting the emotions course in the Italian context: a pilot study to test effects on social-emotional competence in preschool children. Child Indic. Res. 10, 571–590. doi: 10.1007/s12187-016-9387-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinkha, J., Sharma, N. S., and Al Enezi, N. (2023). Parents, children, teens, and psychological viewpoints on parenting practices in Kuwait and Greece. Rowan & Littlefield Publishing Group, Inc.

Google Scholar

Domberg, A., Köymen, B., and Tomasello, M. (2018). Children's reasoning with peers in cooperative and competitive contexts. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 36, 64–77. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G. (2005). A study on the structural validation factors and developmental characteristics of self-control in 3-5year-old (doctoral dissertation). LNNU. Dalian [In Chinese].

Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., and Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. J. Res. Person. 45, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, L. G., Coatsworth, J. D., and Greenberg, M. T. (2009). A model of mindful parenting: implications for parent–child relationships and prevention research. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 12, 255–270. doi: 10.1007/s10567-009-0046-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Reiser, M., et al. (2001a). The relations of regulation and emotionality to children's externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Dev. 72, 1112–1134. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Pidada, S., and Liew, J. (2001b). The relations of regulation and negative emotionality to Indonesian children's social functioning. Child Dev. 72, 1747–1763. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erkan, N. S., and Sop, A. (2018). Analyzing the relationship between parenting styles, behavioural problems and school readiness through the mediating role of self-regulation. Edu. Sci. 43, 27–47. doi: 10.15390/EB.2018.7474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farzadi, F., Behroozi, N., and Shehni Yailagh, M. (2021). The causal relationship between parenting stylesâ attachment styles and indiscipline mediated by the emotional self-regulating mediation, social skills and theory of mind. J. Fam. Psychol. 3, 3–16.

Google Scholar

Feng, L., Zhang, L., and Zhong, H. (2021). Perceived parenting styles and mental health: the multiple mediation effect of perfectionism and altruistic behavior. Psychol. Res. Behav. Mgmt. 14, 1157–1170. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S318446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fooladvand, M., Nadi, M. A., Abedi, A., and Sajjadian, I. (2021). Parenting styles for children with oppositional defiant disorder: scope review. J. Educ. Health Promote :21:10. doi: 10.4103/jehp.jehp_566_19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes, M. C., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Garcia-Ros, R., and Garcia, F. (2022). Analyzing when parental warmth but without parental strictness leads to more adolescent empathy and self-concept: evidence from Spanish homes. Front. Psychol. 13, 1–15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1060821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagne, J. R. (2017). Self-control in childhood: a synthesis of perspectives and focus on early development. Child Dev. Perspect. 11, 127–132. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagnon, S. G., Huelsman, T. J., Reichard, A. E., Kidder-Ashley, P., Griggs, M. S., Struby, J., et al. (2014). Help me play! Parental behaviors, child temperament, and preschool peer play. J. Child Fam. Stud. 23, 872–884. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9743-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajos, J. M., and Beaver, K. M. (2016). The development of self-control from kindergarten to fifth grade: the effects of neuropsychological functioning and adversity. Early Child Dev. Care 186, 1571–1583. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2015.1111881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, W., Hou, Y., Hao, S., and Yu, A. (2023). Helicopter parenting and emotional problems in Chinese emerging adults: are there cross-lagged effects and the mediations of autonomy? J. Youth Adolesc. 52, 393–405. doi: 10.1007/s10964-022-01702-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez-Escudero, J. A., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Bochons, I., and Garcia, F. (2023). Parental socialization and adjustment components in adolescents and middle-aged adults: how are they related? Psychol. Res. Behav. Mgmt. 16, 1127–1139. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S394557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia, F., Serra, E., Garcia, O. F., Martinez, I., and Cruise, E. (2019). A third emerging stage for the current digital society? Optimal parenting styles in Spain, the United States, Germany, and Brazil. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16, 1–20. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16132333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garthe, R. C., Sullivan, T., and Kliewer, W. (2015). Longitudinal relations between adolescent and parental behaviors, parental knowledge, and internalizing behaviors among urban adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 819–832. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0112-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghandour, R. M., Sherman, L. J., Vladutiu, C. J., Ali, M. M., Lynch, S. E., Bitsko, R. H., et al. (2019). Prevalence and treatment of depression, anxiety, and conduct problems in US children. J. Pediatr. 206, 256–267. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, J. J., Giever, D., and Martin, J. S. (1998). Parental management and self-control: an empirical test of Gottfredson and Hirschi's general theory. J. Res. Crime Delinq. 35, 40–70. doi: 10.1177/0022427898035001002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, C. L., Sullivan, C. J., Jones, S., and Piquero, A. R. (2010). “Does it take a village?” assessing neighborhood influences on children’s self-control. J. Res. Crime Delinq. 47, 31–62. doi: 10.1177/0022427809348903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gimenez-Serrano, S., Garcia, F., and Garcia, O. F. (2022). Parenting styles and its relations with personal and social adjustment beyond adolescence: Is the current evidence enough? Eur. J. Develop. Psychol. 19, 749–769. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2021.1952863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., and Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: theoretical models and preliminary data. J. Fam. Psychol. 10:243. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guan, Y., Wang, Z., Gong, Q., Cai, Z., Xu, S. L., Xiang, Q., et al. (2018). Parents’ career values, adaptability, career-specific parenting behaviors, and undergraduates’ career adaptability. Couns. Psychol. 46, 922–946. doi: 10.1177/0011000018808215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guina, J. (2016). The talking cure of avoidant personality disorder: remission through earned-secure attachment. Am. J. Psychother. 70, 233–250. doi: 10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.2016.70.3.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, Y., Hu, B. Y., Pan, Y., and Vitiello, G. (2023). The bidirectional relationship between supportive parenting and social skills: a longitudinal study among Chinese preschoolers. J. Child Fam. Stud. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10826-023-02592-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, J., Tang, X., Marsh, H. W., Parker, P., Basarkod, G., Sahdra, B., et al. (2023). The roles of social–emotional skills in students’ academic and life success: a multi-informant and multicohort perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 124:1079. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrist, A. W., and Waugh, R. M. (2002). Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function in children’s development. Develop. Rev. 22, 555–592. doi: 10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00500-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haslam, D., Poniman, C., Filus, A., Sumargi, A., and Boediman, L. (2020). Parenting style, child emotion regulation and behavioral problems: the moderating role of cultural values in Australia and Indonesia. Mar. Fam. Rev. 56, 320–342. doi: 10.1080/01494929.2020.1712573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrich, L. M., and Gullone, E. (2006). The clinical significance of loneliness: a literature review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 695–718. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, D. A., Betancur Cortés, L., and Votruba-Drzal, E. (2020). Black–white achievement gaps differ by family socioeconomic status from early childhood through early adolescence. J. Educ. Psychol. 112:1471. doi: 10.1037/edu0000439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, Y. J., Hong, J. C., Ye, J. H., Chen, P. H., Ma, L. P., and Chang Lee, Y. J. (2022). Effects of helicopter parenting on tutoring engagement and continued attendance at cram schools. Front. Psychol. 13:1535. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.880894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Friese, M., and Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 162–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01116.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollenstein, T., and Lewis, M. D. (2006). A state space analysis of emotion and flexibility in parent-child interactions. Emotion 6:656. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.4.656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, J. S., Kim, D. H., deLara, E. W., Wei, H. S., Prisner, A., and Alexander, N. B. (2021). Parenting style and bullying and victimization: comparing foreign-born Asian, US-born Asian, and white American adolescents. J. Fam. Violence 36, 799–811. doi: 10.1007/s10896-020-00176-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Walker, J. M., Sandler, H. M., Whetsel, D., Green, C. L., Wilkins, A. S., et al. (2005). Why do parents become involved? Research findings and implications. Element. School J. 106, 105–130. doi: 10.1086/499194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, W., Jung, E., Kim, S., and Hadi, N. (2023). Can helicopter parenting be beneficial for parent–child relationships? A person-centered approach in the United States and South Korea. Front. Psychol. :1097348:14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1097348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C., Stark, K., Trentacosta, C., and Schultz, D. (2008). Beyond emotion regulation: emotion utilization and adaptive functioning. Child Dev. Perspect. 2, 156–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00058.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jespersen, J. E., Hardy, N. R., and Morris, A. S. (2021). Parent and peer emotion responsivity styles: an extension of Gottman’s emotion socialization parenting typologies. Child 8:319. doi: 10.3390/children8050319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., Li, Y. J., and Huang, C. H. (2015). A model of factors influencing peer interaction skills of 3- to 6-year-olds. Pres. Edu. Res. (5), 45–54 [In Chinese].

Google Scholar

Kashyap, A., Singh, T. K., and Arun, P. (2020). Parenting style and emotional regulation in children with borderline and average intellectual functioning. J. Ind. Assoc. Child. Adolesc. Mental Health 16, 82–96. doi: 10.1177/0973134220200406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawabata, Y., Alink, L. R., Tseng, W. L., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., and Crick, N. R. (2011). Maternal and paternal parenting styles associated with relational aggression in children and adolescents: a conceptual analysis and meta-analytic review. Dev. Rev. 31, 240–278. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2011.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendall, P. C., and Wilcox, L. E. (1979). Self-control in children: development of a rating scale. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 47:1020. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.47.6.1020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khaleque, A. (2013). Perceived parental warmth, and children’s psychological adjustment, and personality dispositions: a meta-analysis. J. Child Fam. Stud. 22, 297–306. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9579-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y., Calzada, E. J., Barajas-Gonzalez, R. G., Huang, K. Y., Brotman, L. M., Castro, A., et al. (2018). The role of authoritative and authoritarian parenting in the early academic achievement of Latino students. J. Educ. Psychol. 110:119. doi: 10.1037/edu0000192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., and Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderate effects of parent–child mutuality on self-regulation: a relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Dev. 83, 1275–1289. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01778.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kriger, S. F., and Kroes, W. H. (1972). Child-rearing attitudes of Chinese, Jewish, and Protestant mothers. J. Soc. Psychol. 86, 205–210. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1972.9918618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladd, G. W., and Kochenderfer-Ladd, B. (2019). Parents and children’s peer relationships. Handb. Par. 38, 278–315. doi: 10.4324/9780429401695-10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., and Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Dev. 62, 1049–1065. doi: 10.2307/1131151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, B., Keller, H., Teiser, J., Gudi, H., Yovsi, R. D., Freitag, C., et al. (2018). Waiting for the second treat: developing culture-specific modes of self-regulation. Child Dev. 89, e261–e277. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanjekar, P. D., Joshi, S. H., Lanjekar, P. D., and Wagh, V. (2022). The effect of parenting and the parent-Child relationship on a Child's cognitive development: a literature review. Cureus 14:e30574. doi: 10.7759/cureus.30574

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, E. Y. H., and Power, T. G. (2020). Coparenting, parenting stress, and authoritative parenting among Hong Kong Chinese mothers and fathers. Parenting 20, 167–176. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2019.1694831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le, B. M., and Impett, E. A. (2019). Parenting goal pursuit is linked to emotional well-being, relationship quality, and responsiveness. J. Soc. Person Rel. 36, 879–904. doi: 10.1177/0265407517747417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. S. T., and Wong, D. S. W. (2009). School, parents, and peer factors in relation to Hong Kong students' bullying. Inter. J. Adolesc. Youth. 15, 217–233. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2009.9748030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lereya, S. T., Samara, M., and Wolke, D. (2013). Parenting behavior and the risk of becoming a victim and a bully/victim: a meta-analysis study. Child Abuse. Neg. 37, 1091–1108. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, C., Leung, J. T., Kwok, S. Y., Hui, A., Lo, H., Tam, H. L., et al. (2021). Predictors to happiness in primary students: positive relationships or academic achievement. App. Res. Qua. Life. 16, 2335–2349. doi: 10.1007/s11482-021-09928-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewin, K. (1948). Resolving social conflicts; selected papers on group dynamics. New York: Harper & Row.

Google Scholar

Lewis, M. (1995). Shame: The exposed self. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Li, D., Li, X., Wang, Y., Zhao, L., Bao, Z., and Wen, F. (2013). School connectedness and problematic internet use in adolescents: a moderated mediation model of deviant peer affiliation and self-control. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 1231–1242. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9761-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, D., Zhang, W., Li, D., Wang, Y., and Yan, S. (2012). The effects of parenting style and temperament on adolescent aggression: a test of unique, differential, and mediating effects. J. Psychol. (44), 211–225. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2012.00211 [In Chinese].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, D., Zhang, W., and Wang, Y. (2015). Parental behavioral control, psychological control and Chinese adolescents’ peer victimization: the mediating role of self-control. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 628–637. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9873-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. B., Willems, Y. E., Stok, F. M., Deković, M., Bartels, M., and Finkenauer, C. (2019). Parenting and self-control across early to late adolescence: a three-level meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 967–1005. doi: 10.1177/1745691619863046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., Tang, Y., and Zheng, Y. (2023). How the home learning environment contributes to children’s social–emotional competence: a moderated mediation model. Front. Psychol. 14:491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1065978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Cui, N., Kok, H. T., Deatrick, J., and Liu, J. (2019). The relationship between parenting styles practiced by grandparents and children’s emotional and behavioral problems. J. Child Fam. Stud. 28, 1899–1913. doi: 10.1007/s10826-019-01415-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. Y. C., and Fu, V. R. (1990). A comparison of child-rearing practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. Child Dev. 61, 429–433. doi: 10.2307/1131104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, X., Zhang, Y., Liao, Y., and Xie, W. (2023). Socioeconomic status and problem behaviors in young Chinese children: a moderated mediation model of parenting styles and only children. Front. Psychol. 14:1029408. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1029408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindahl, K. M., Bregman, H. R., and Malik, N. M. (2012). Family boundary structures and child adjustment: the indirect role of emotional reactivity. J. Fam. Psychol. 26:839. doi: 10.1037/a0030444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, F., Chui, H., and Chung, M. C. (2020). The effect of parent–adolescent relationship quality on deviant peer affiliation: the mediating role of self-control and friendship quality. J. Soc. Person. Rel. 37, 2714–2736. doi: 10.1177/0265407520937358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Peng, P., and Luo, L. (2020). The relation between family socioeconomic status and academic achievement in China: a meta-analysis. Edu. Psychol. Rev. 32, 49–76. doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09494-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Xiao, B., Hipson, W. E., Coplan, R. J., Li, D., and Chen, X. (2017). Self-control, peer preference, and loneliness in Chinese children: a three-year longitudinal study. Soc. Dev. 26, 876–890. doi: 10.1111/sode.12224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llorca-Mestre, A., Samper-García, P., Malonda-Vidal, E., and Cortés-Tomás, M. T. (2017). Parenting style and peer attachment as predictors of emotional instability in children. Soc. Behav. Person. Int. J. 45, 677–694. doi: 10.2224/sbp.5363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luby, J. L., Gilbert, K., Whalen, D., Tillman, R., and Barch, D. M. (2020). The differential contribution of the components of parent-child interaction therapy emotion development for treatment of preschool depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 59, 868–879. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.07.937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, S., Liu, Y., and Zhang, D. (2019). Socioeconomic status and young children’s problem behaviours–mediating effects of parenting style and psychological suzhi. Early Child Dev. Care 191, 148–158. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2019.1608196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maccoby, E. E., and Martin, J. A. (1983). “Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction” in Handbook of Child Psychology. ed. P. H. Mussen, vol. 4 (New York: Wiley), 1–101.

Google Scholar

Malonda, E., Llorca, A., Mesurado, B., Samper, P., and Mestre, M. V. (2019). Parents or peers? Predictors of prosocial behavior and aggression: a longitudinal study. Front. Psychol. 10:2379. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcone, R., Borrone, A., and Caputo, A. (2021). Peer interaction and social competence in childhood and early adolescence: the affected of parental behaviour. J. Fam. Stud. 27, 178–195. doi: 10.1080/13229400.2018.1526702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marginson, S., and Yang, L. (2020). “China meets Anglo-America on the new silk road: A comparison of state, society, self, and higher education” in China and Europe on the New Silk Road: Connecting Universities Across Eurasia (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 255.

Google Scholar

Martinez-Escudero, J. A., Villarejo, S., Garcia, O. F., and Garcia, F. (2020). Parental socialization and its impact across the lifespan. Behav. Sci. 10:101. doi: 10.3390/bs10060101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2004). TARGET ARTICLES:" emotional intelligence: theory, findings, and implications". Psychol. Inq. 15, 197–215. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCurdy, K., and Daro, D. (2001). Parent involvement in family support programs: an integrated theory. Fam. Rel. 50, 113–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2001.00113.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehroof, M., and Griffiths, M. D. (2010). Online gaming addiction: the role of sensation seeking, self-control, neuroticism, aggression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. Cyber. Behav. Soc. Net. 13, 313–316. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meng, H., Ma, J., Shen, Y., and Chang, R. (2020). The influences of father absence on primary school children's pedestrian behaviors: the mediating effect of self-control. Trans. Res. Int. Perspect. 7:100202. doi: 10.1016/j.trip.2020.100202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mestre, V., Samper, P., Tur-Porcar, A. M., Richaud de Minzi, M. C., and Mesurado, B. (2012). Emociones, estilos de afrontamiento y agresividad en la adolescencia. Uni. Psychol. 11, 1263–1275. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy11-4.eeaa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischel, W., and Ayduk, O. (2002). Self-regulation in a cognitive--affective personality system: Attentional control in the Service of the Self. Self Ident. 1, 113–120.

Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 108, 2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, A. S., Criss, M. M., Silk, J. S., and Houltberg, B. J. (2017). The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Dev. Perspect. 11, 233–238. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, A. S., Ratliff, E. L., Cosgrove, K. T., and Steinberg, L. (2021). We know even more things: a decade review of parenting research. J. Res. Adolesc. 31, 870–888. doi: 10.1111/jora.12641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., and Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc. Dev. 16, 361–388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Na, E., Lee, H., Choi, I., and Kim, D. J. (2017). Comorbidity of internet gaming disorder and alcohol use disorder: a focus on clinical characteristics and gaming patterns. Am. J. Addict. 26, 326–334. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng-Knight, T., Shelton, K. H., Riglin, L., McManus, I. C., Frederickson, N., and Rice, F. (2016). A longitudinal study of self-control at the transition to secondary school: considering the role of pubertal status and parenting. J. Adolesc. 50, 44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2004). Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in the toddler years: antecedents and consequences. Dev. Psychol. 16, 43–68. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404044402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigra, B., Xia, Y. Y., and Li, D. P. (2015). Direct and indirect effects of parental control on adolescent depression. Chi. J. Clin. Psychol. 23, 494–497. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2015.03.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ong, M. Y., Eilander, J., Saw, S. M., Xie, Y., Meaney, M. J., and Broekman, B. F. (2018). The influence of perceived parenting styles on socio-emotional development from pre-puberty into puberty. Eur. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 27, 37–46. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1016-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Özdemir, Y., Kuzucu, Y., and Ak, Ş. (2014). Depression, loneliness and internet addiction: how important is low self-control? Com. Human. Behav. 34, 284–290. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Özdemir, Y., Vazsonyi, A. T., and Çok, F. (2013). Parenting processes and aggression: the role of self-control among Turkish adolescents. J. Adolesc. 36, 65–77. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palacios, I., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., and Garcia, F. (2022). Positive parenting style and positive health beyond the authoritative: self, universalism values and protection against emotional vulnerability from Spanish adolescents and adult children. Front. Psychol. 13, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1066282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, W., Gao, B., Long, Y., Teng, Y., and Yue, T. (2021). Effect of caregivers’ parenting styles on the emotional and behavioral problems of left-behind children: the parallel mediating role of self-control. Inter. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 18:12714. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182312714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, Y., and Shang, Z. (2023). Linking culture and family travel behaviour from generativity theory perspective: A case of confucian culture and Chinese family travel behaviour. J. Hospitality Tourism Manage. 54, 212–220. doi: 10.1016/j.jhtm.2022.12.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papadopoulos, D. (2021). Parenting the exceptional social-emotional needs of gifted and talented children: what do we know? Child 8:953. doi: 10.3390/children8110953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M., and Kauser, R. (2018). Do the associations of parenting styles with behavior problems and academic achievement vary by culture? Results from a meta-analysis. Cultur. Divers. Ethnic Minor. Psychol. 24, 75–100. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plexousakis, S. S., Kourkoutas, E., Giovazolias, T., Chatira, K., and Nikolopoulos, D. (2019). School bullying and post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms: the role of parental bonding. Front. Public Health 7:75. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2019.00075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, T. C., and Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi's general theory of crime: a meta-analysis. Crim. 38, 931–964. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2000.tb00911.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, L. (2022). Influence of family environment on peer interaction skills of 4- and 6-year-old children. Advance. Edu. 12:2008. [In Chinese]

Google Scholar

Qin, L., and Caizhen, Y. (2022). The relationship between peer interactions and social withdrawal in young children. Advance. Soc. Sci. 11:356. [In Chinese]

Google Scholar

Qiu, J. I. N., Hui, H., and Yaohui, C. (2021). Relationship between aggressive behavior and emotion management of middle school students in Bengbu. Chin. J. School Health 42, 1340–1343. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2021.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., Boelen, P. A., Van der Schoot, M., and Telch, M. J. (2011). Prospective linkages between peer victimization and externalizing problems in children: a meta-analysis. Agg. Behav. 37, 215–222. doi: 10.1002/ab.20374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., and Telch, M. J. (2010). Peer victimization and internalizing problems in children: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Child. Abuse. Neg. 34, 244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitz, A. K., Zimmermann, J., Hutteman, R., Specht, J., and Neyer, F. J. (2014). How peers make a difference: the role of peer groups and peer relationships in personality development. Eur. J. Person. 28, 279–288. doi: 10.1002/per.1965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, C. C., Hart, C. H., Mandleco, B. L., and Olsen, S. F. (1996). Psychometric support for a new measure of authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: Cross-cultural connections.

Google Scholar

Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., and Hart, C. H. (2001). The parenting styles and dimensions questionnaire (PSDQ). Handb. Fam. Measur. Techni. 3, 319–321.

Google Scholar

Romeo, R., Knapp, M., and Scott, S. (2006). Economic cost of severe antisocial behaviour in children-and who pays it. Br. J. Psychiatry 188, 547–553. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.104.007625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rongeven, R. L., Gao, C., Huang, Y., He, H., Peng, C., et al. (2022). Association of mothers' adverse childhood experiences and parenting styles with emotional behavior problems in preschool children. Sch. Health China 43, 1134–1138. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2022.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Dev. Neuro. 28, 573–594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, B. S., Londhe, R., and Britner, P. A. (2013). Parental contributions to the delay of gratification in preschool-aged children. J. Child Fam. Stud. 22, 471–478. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9599-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, M. J., and Smith, D. J. (eds.). (1995). “Youth crime and conduct disorders: trends patterns and causal explanations,” in Psychosocial disorders in young people: time trends and their causes. (Chichester: Wiley).

Google Scholar

Safdar, S., and Zahrah, S. M. (2016). Impact of parenting styles on the intensity of parental and peer attachment: exploring the gender differences in adolescents. Am. J. App. Psychol. 4, 23–30. doi: 10.12691/ajap-4-2-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahithya, B. R., and Raman, V. (2021). Parenting style, parental personality, and child temperament in children with anxiety disorders—a clinical study from India. Ind. J. Psychol. Med. 43, 382–391. doi: 10.1177/0253717620973376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satir, V., Stachowiak, J., and Taschman, H. A. (1994). Helping families to change. Jason Aro. Incorp., Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc.

Google Scholar

Scharff, D. E. (2022). Changing family and marital structure in China: emotional strain at cultural and individual levels. Psychoanal. Psychother. China 5, 34–40. doi: 10.33212/ppc.v5n1.2022.34

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmeichel, B. J., and Baumeister, R. F. (2010). “Effortful attention control,” in Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action. Ed. B. Bruya, 29–49.

Google Scholar

Scholtes, C. M., Lyons, E. R., and Skowron, E. A. (2021). Dyadic synchrony and repair processes are related to preschool children's risk exposure and self-control. Dev. Psychol. 33, 1072–1084. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420000358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, S., Knapp, M., Henderson, J., and Maughan, B. (2001). Financial cost of social exclusion: follow up study of antisocial children into adulthood. BMJ 323:191. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7306.191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, Y., Yang, L. Z., and Fang, L. L. (2015). Self-control developmental characteristics of 3-6-year-old children and their educational suggestions. Edu. Sci. 1, 68–74. doi: 10.1037/aap0000099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T., Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A., et al. (2007). Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers' effortful control to children's adjustment and social competence. Dev. Psychol. 43:1170. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, E. H., and McKinney, C. (2019). Emerging adult psychological problems and parenting style: moderation by parent-child relationship quality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 146, 201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., and Dornbusch, S. M. (1994). Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Dev. 65, 754–770. doi: 10.2307/1131416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, Y., Li, J. B., Oktaufik, M. P. M., and Vazsonyi, A. T. (2022). Parental attachment and externalizing behaviors among Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of self-control. J. Child Fam. Stud. 31, 923–933. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-02071-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tao, Y., Bi, X. Y., and Deng, M. (2020). The impact of parent–child attachment on self-injury behavior: negative emotion and emotional coping style as serial mediators. Front. Psychol. 11:1477. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tao, A., Zhou, Q., and Wang, Y. (2010). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: prospective relations to Chinese children’s psychological adjustment. J. Fam. Psychol. 24:135. doi: 10.1037/a0018974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tibshirani, R. J., and Efron, B. (1993). An introduction to the bootstrap. Mon. Sta. App. Pro. 57, 20–25.

Google Scholar

Tudge, J. R., Merçon-Vargas, E. A., Liang, Y., and Payir, A. (2022). “The importance of Urie Bronfenbrenner's bioecological theory for early childhood education” in Theories of Early Childhood Education. (Routledge), 50–61.

Google Scholar

Vandell, D. L. (2000). Parents, peer groups, and other socializing influences. Dev. Psychol. 36:699. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.6.699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veiga, F. H., Festas, I., García, Ó. F., Oliveira, Í. M., Veiga, C. M., Martins, C., et al. (2021). Do students with immigrant and native parents perceive themselves as equally engaged in school during adolescence? Curr. Psychol. 42, 11902–11916. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02480-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villarejo, S., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Villarreal, M. E., and Garcia, F. (2023). Early family experiences, drug use and psychosocial adjustment across the life span: is parental strictness always a protective factor? Psychos. Int. doi: 10.5093/pi2023a16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, S. C., Perry, R. E., Brandes-Aitken, A., Braren, S., and Blair, C. (2021). Deprivation and threat as developmental mediators in the relation between early life socioeconomic status and executive functioning outcomes in early childhood. Dev. Cog. Neur. 47:100907. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M., and Tice, D. M. (2014). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94, 883–898. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.883

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wahl, K., and Metzner, C. (2012). Parental influences on the prevalence and development of child aggressiveness. J. Child Fam. Stud. 21, 344–355. doi: 10.1007/s10826-011-9484-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Li, Z., and Zhu, L. (2019). Emotional intelligence of 3-to 6-year-olds and parenting style: peer communication ability as a mediator. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 47, 1–12. doi: 10.2224/sbp.8636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. F., Liu, L., and Kim, Y. D. (2015). The relationship between parental psychological aggression and elementary school children's anxiety: the mediating role of children's self-esteem. Chi. J. Clin. Psychol. 23, 273–276. [In Chinese]

Google Scholar

Wang, L., Tao, T., Tang, Y., Fan, C., and Gao, W. (2016). The relationship between children's self-control and parenting style. Chi. J. Behav. Med. Brain Sci. 4, 542–546.

Google Scholar

Wang, M., Wang, J., Deng, X., and Chen, W. (2019). Why are empathic children more liked by peers? The mediating roles of prosocial and aggressive behaviors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 144, 19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, C.-Y. (2013). A study on the current situation of children's emotion management ability from 3 to 6 years old (doctoral dissertation). Guangzhou University. [In Chinese].

Google Scholar

Wu, D. Y. (1996). “Parental control: Psychocultural interpretations of Chinese patterns of socialization,” in Growing up the Chinese way: Chinese child and adolescent development. 1–28.

Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Zhou, Y., Zhao, J., Xuan, Z., Li, W., Han, L., et al. (2021). The relationship between shyness and aggression in late childhood: the multiple mediation effects of parent-child conflict and self-control. Personal. Individ. Differ. 182:111058. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, P., Li, X., Li, X., Li, D., Xu, X., and Zhao, C. (2023). Parent-grandparent co-parenting and preschoolers’ self-control in China: the roles of maternal and grandparental parenting styles. Early. Edu. Dev. 6, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2023.2186087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yen, C. F., Chou, W. J., Liu, T. L., Yang, P., and Hu, H. F. (2014). The association of internet addiction symptoms with anxiety, depression and self-esteem among adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 55, 1601–1608. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, F., Zhao, J., and Zhang, W. (2013). Development of future planning in early and middle adolescents and its relationship with parenting behavior: the mediating effect of behavioral autonomy. J. Psychol. 45:658. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.00658, [In Chinese]

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y. (2002). Development of a peer interaction skills scale for 4-6year olds. J. J. Edu. Col. Soc. Sci. Edit. 18, 42–44. [In Chinese]

Google Scholar

Zhang, H., and Deng, C. (2022). The impact of parent–Child attachment on school adjustment in left-behind children due to transnational parenting: the mediating role of peer relationships. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:6989. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19126989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, R., Zhang, H., Guo, X., Wang, J., Zhao, Z., and Feng, L. (2022). Relationship between helicopter parenting and Chinese elementary school Child procrastination: a mediated moderation model. Inter. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 19:14892. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192214892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, R. P., Zhang, X., Xiao, N., Ren, P., and Li, X. Y. (2023). Parenting practices and rural Chinese Children’s self-control and problem behaviors: a comparison of left-behind and non-left-behind children. J. Child Fam. Stud. 32, 704–715. doi: 10.1007/s10826-022-02422-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, Q., Hu, N., and Ding, X. (2019). Factors influencing rejection sensitivity - based on the "individual × environment model". App. Psychol. 26, 83–96. [In Chinese]

Google Scholar

Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Wang, Y., and Reiser, M. (2004). Chinese children's effortful control and dispositional anger/frustration: relations to parenting styles and children's social functioning. Dev. Psychol. 40:352. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, H., and Long, L. R. (2004). A statistical test and control method for common method bias. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 12, 942–950. [In Chinese]

Google Scholar

Zhu, X., Liu, J., and Liang, Z. (2020). Are left-behind children more likely to be bullied? Empirical evidence from rural schools in Shaanxi province. Chin. Sociol. Rev. 52, 411–437. doi: 10.1080/21620555.2020.1776603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: authoritarian parenting style, authoritative parenting style, emotion management skills, self-control, peer interactions, chain mediating effects, Chinese children

Citation: Li D, Li W and Zhu X (2023) Parenting style and children emotion management skills among Chinese children aged 3–6: the chain mediation effect of self-control and peer interactions. Front. Psychol. 14:1231920. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1231920

Received: 01 June 2023; Accepted: 30 August 2023;
Published: 18 September 2023.

Edited by:

Xiaoqin Zhu, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong SAR, China

Reviewed by:

Antonio Luque, University of Almeria, Spain
Andreja Brajsa Zganec, Institute of Social Sciences Ivo Pilar (IPI), Croatia
Limin Zhang, Guangzhou University, China
Oscar F. Garcia, University of Valencia, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Li, Li and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xingchen Zhu, zhuxingchenlnnu@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.