Skip to main content

CORRECTION article

Front. Psychol., 30 June 2023
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Corrigendum: Minority influence and degrowth-oriented pro-environmental conflict: when emotions betray our attachment to the social dominant paradigm

  • Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland

In the published article, there was an error in Table 3 as published. In the column “Men”, row “High control-oriented emotions” the values were reported as “89, 79.00” but should be “89, 79.5”. In the column “Men”, row “Low control-oriented emotions” values were reported as “82, 92” but should be “70, 79.5”. In the column “Women”, row “High control-oriented emotions” the values were reported as “75, 85.00” but should be “75, 84.5. In the column Women, row “Low control-oriented emotions” the values were reported as “109, 99” but should be “94, 84.5”. The corrected Table 3, including the updated totals, appears below.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Frequency table of men and women who selected more high than low control-oriented emotions vs. more low than high control-oriented emotions—Raw data.

In the published article, there was also an error in Supplementary Table S2 as published. In the column “Men”, row “High control-oriented emotions” the values were reported as “16, 14.47” but should be “16, 15.02”. In the column “Men”, row “Low control-oriented emotions” values were reported as “11, 12.53” but should be “10, 10.98”. In the column “Women”, row “High control-oriented emotions” the values were reported as “51, 52.53” but should be “51, 51.98”. In the column Women, row “Low control-oriented emotions” the values were reported as “47, 45.47” but should be “39, 38.02”. The corrected Supplementary Table S2, including the updated totals, appears below.

SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE S2
www.frontiersin.org

Supplementary Table S2. Frequency Table of Men and Women who Selected more High than Low Control-oriented Emotions vs. more Low than High Control-oriented Emotions—Raw Data.

In the published article, there was also an error in Pilot Study, Results, Overall frequencies. The original text stated that the analyses were run on the total emotion count when, in fact, they were conducted on the total number of participants reporting higher than lower control-oriented emotions vs. lower than higher control-oriented emotions. The corrected paragraph appears below:

“When faced with degrowth-oriented pro-environmental policies, expectedly although non significantly, there was a greater count of participants selecting more high control-oriented emotions than low control-oriented emotions (67) than participants selecting more low control-oriented emotions than high control-oriented emotions (49) on the Geneva Emotion Wheel, χ2(1, 116) = 2.79, p = 0.09 Cohen's w = 0.16. Furthermore, the gender differences were in the expected direction, with men selecting more high than low control-oriented emotions to a higher extent than women, although the difference was not significant, Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Test, χ2(1, 116) = 0.05, p = 0.82, Cohen's w = 0.04 (see Supplementary Table S2).”

In the published article, there was also an error in Pilot Study, Results, Proportions, Paragraph 1. The original standard deviation for the personal high control-oriented emotions was incorrectly provided as “0.97” but should be “0.193”. The corrected paragraph appears below:

“When observing the weighted data8 for the proportional dependent variable, it appeared that participants did indeed select a higher personal proportion of high control-oriented emotions (M = 0.56, SD = 0.193), with the test against equal proportion level (0.50) being significant, t(137) = 3.59, p < .001, Cohen's d = 0.319.”

In the published article, there was an error in Pilot Study, Results, Proportions, Paragraph 2. The original Cohen's d effect size for the difference in men's and women's individual proportion of high and low control-oriented emotions was incorrectly provided as “0.04” but should be “0.069”. The corrected paragraph appears below:

“Overall, men (M = 0.57, SD = 0.21) descriptively selected a higher individual proportion of high control-oriented emotions than women (M = 0.56, SD = 0.19), but the difference was not significant, t(136) = 0.35, p = 0.73, Cohen's d = 0.069 (as can be seen in Supplementary Figure S1).”

In the published article, there was an error in Study 2, Results, Overall Frequencies.

Similarly to the pilot study, the original text for study 2 stated that the analyses were run on the total emotion count when, in fact, they were conducted on the total number of participants reporting higher than lower control-oriented emotions vs. lower than higher control-oriented emotions. The corrected paragraph appears below:

“The overall frequency of participants who selected more high than low control-oriented emotions was not significantly greater than the frequency of participants who selected more low than high control-oriented emotions, due to the asymmetry across genders. Indeed, and as hypothesised, more men (than women) selected more high than low control-oriented emotions, Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Test, χ2(1, 328) = 3.95, p = 0.046, Cohen's w = 0.12 (Table 3).”

In the published article, there was an error in Study 2, Results, Proportions. The reported t value for the overall difference in the weighted proportion of personal high and low control-oriented emotions was incorrectly provided as “1.84” but should be “4.39”. The corrected paragraph appears below:

As predicted, participants selected a higher proportion of high control-oriented emotions overall, as for the weighted11 data analysis (M = 0.55, SD = 0.23), t(389) = 4.39, p < 0.0001, Cohen's d = 0.22. Moreover, men (M = 0.59, SD = 0.23) selected a higher proportion of high control-oriented emotions than women (M = 0.52, SD = 0.23), t(388) = 3.20, p = 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.32 (Figure 3).12

The authors apologize for these errors and state that this does not change the scientific conclusions of the article in any way. The original article has been updated.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Keywords: minority influence, emotions, degrowth, change, conflict, pro-environmental action

Citation: Avery RAT and Butera F (2023) Corrigendum: Minority influence and degrowth-oriented pro-environmental conflict: when emotions betray our attachment to the social dominant paradigm. Front. Psychol. 14:1228184. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1228184

Received: 24 May 2023; Accepted: 06 June 2023;
Published: 30 June 2023.

Approved by:

Stamos Papastamou, Panteion University, Greece

Copyright © 2023 Avery and Butera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fabrizio Butera, ZmFicml6aW8uYnV0ZXJhJiN4MDAwNDA7dW5pbC5jaA==

ORCID: Robert A. T. Avery orcid.org/0000-0002-8432-5536
Fabrizio Butera orcid.org/0000-0002-8856-4374

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.