Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 27 September 2023
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Educational Neuroscience: Key Processes and Approaches to Measurement View all 9 articles

Measuring the building blocks of everyday cognition: executive functions and relational reasoning

  • School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, United States

Measurement of the building blocks of everyday thought must capture the range of different ways that humans may train, develop, and use their cognitive resources in real world tasks. Executive function as a construct has been enthusiastically adopted by cognitive and education sciences due to its theorized role as an underpinning of, and constraint on, humans’ accomplishment of complex cognitively demanding tasks in the world, such as identifying problems, reasoning about and executing multi-step solutions while inhibiting prepotent responses or competing desires. As EF measures have been continually refined for increased precision; however, they have also become increasingly dissociated from those everyday accomplishments. We posit three implications of this insight: (1) extant measures of EFs that reduce context actually add an implicit requirement that children reason using abstract rules that are not accomplishing a function in the world, meaning that EF scores may in part reflect experience with formal schooling and Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, Democratic (WEIRD) socialization norms, limiting their ability to predict success in everyday life across contexts, (2) measurement of relational attention and relational reasoning have not received adequate consideration in this context but are highly aligned with the key aims for measuring EFs, and may be more aligned with humans’ everyday cognitive practices, but (3) relational attention and reasoning should be considered alongside rather than as an additional EF as has been suggested, for measurement clarity.

1. Introduction

Executive function (EF) is a construct that has taken on great attention in cognitive science as well as in educational and psychological literatures aiming to train and improve children’s developmental trajectories, due to its theorized centrality to human cognition as a building block, and accordingly as a capacity limiter, in higher cognitive function. As such, EFs are theorized to predict individual differences in human reasoning, problem solving, and learning, and there is much data to support this inference, though the specific relationships between individual EFs and these key processes are somewhat variable (Bull and Scerif, 2001; Bull and Lee, 2014; Simms et al., 2018). Indeed the literature on EFs is highly variable, and it is clear that task-specific constraints and affordances are impactful on measurements, in part due to task impurity such that most tasks involve multiple types of EF demands (Burgess, 1997; Phillips, 1997), and in part because of potential lack of clarity about the nature of composite EF skills (Doebel, 2020).

At the same time, there are often wide disparities between the ways that EFs are measured, the everyday skills they are intended to explain, and the ways they are used and invoked by educators invested in improving knowledge and skills. This is important theoretically for measurement but also for guiding recommendations for training EFs. When measurements are misalignments to the everyday skills EFs are designed to explain and constrain, training recommendations stem from these measures rather than usage in the world. One consequence is the potential low likelihood that trained gains would thus transfer to everyday practices.

A second, less well considered consequence is that cultural norms and expectations that are embedded in the creation of EF tasks may be particularly misaligned with the human reasoning and problem solving performed by individuals in non-Western, Industrialized, Educated, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD; Henrich et al., 2010), contexts. This may lead to measurement of skills that are not predictive of these individuals’ everyday performance and may suggest training practices that could be inefficient or counter to extant practices that are indeed predictive of success in real world contexts.

This manuscript focuses on elaborating these concerns, and poses approaches to responding to this challenge. In particular we focus on illuminating culturally valanced assumptions that are embedded in many EF tasks, and suggest that relational reasoning and relational attention are cognitive measures that incorporate but do not seek to reduce EFs into their base cognitive units, may in fact be closer to meeting the second two goals highlighted above – explaining everyday cognitive behaviors and limits, and supporting training for regulating one’s behavior to best make use of one’s limited cognitive resources.

2. Defining executive functions

EFs are commonly defined as the limited capacity cognitive processing system that deploys resources to perform cognitive tasks and regulate the dynamics of human cognition (see Diamond, 2006; Miyake and Friedman, 2012). Within EF, the dominant model centers on three primary subsystems that include Working Memory (WM), the resources for holding information active within attention and manipulating that information (Engle and Kane, 2004), attentional control, or inhibitory control, the processes of controlling attention away from irrelevant information and inhibiting prepotent responses (Diamond, 2002), and task switching, the ability to regulate attention and execution of task rules when moving between two or more tasks (Miyake et al., 2000; Zelazo et al., 2004).

EFs are theorized to be integral to intelligent behavior (Carpenter et al., 1990; Little et al., 2014), as well as school-based achievement skills (Best et al., 2011) including mathematics (Bull and Lee, 2014) and reading (van der Sluis et al., 2007; Kim, 2020). Importantly to broad everyday impact, EFs indicated to be integral to human higher cognitive functions such as reasoning and problem solving (Morrison et al., 2004; Richland et al., 2006; Krawczyk et al., 2008; Richland and Burchinal, 2013).

At the same time, measurement of EF skills is not straightforward, and complications have arisen because measures that are ostensibly of the same process do not always correlate, and at the same time, many EF tasks involve shared skills, which is difficult to disentangle (Miyake et al., 2000; Snyder et al., 2015).

In the aim to resolve this challenge and produce tasks that have removed the interference of other EFs as well as everyday knowledge and experiences; however, the field has also shaped these tasks in ways that may not reliably reflect all children’s skills.

3. EFs across cultural populations

Growing evidence has documented that cultural context and socialization practices profoundly impact cognitive development, but even so, models of key psychological constructs such as EFs continue to be primarily developed and refined on samples of children from WEIRD societies, which represent only a small portion of the world’s population (12%; Henrich et al., 2010). This sampling bias may be particularly consequential in a theoretical domain such as EFs, where socialization practices across communities may have direct implications for children’s opportunities for displaying their ability to enact problem solving, holding information in mind, managing and switching tasks, and inhibiting prepotent responses.

We posit that the tendency for most standardized, field accepted measures of EF to require children to manipulate arbitrary rules to solve non-consequential tasks may have led them to be broadly aligned with many skills taught within WEIRD formal educational and socialization routines, and misaligned with socialization routines identified in other communities. For example, in rural and indigenous Latine communities, children are highly autonomous and are not expected to follow verbally articulated arbitrary rules without a clear rationale or consequence (see Gaskins, 2000; Correa-Chávez and Rogoff, 2009; Ochs and Izquierdo, 2009; Alcalá et al., 2018; Kulis et al., 2019). In another example building on measurement of delayed gratification abilities, Japanese children were found to wait longer than American children for food, but not for gifts. Such different patterns of self-control could be due to cultural differences (Yanaoka et al., 2022). In Japan, mealtime is often considered as a communal and social event. It is customary to wait until everyone is seated and ready before starting a meal. Waiting for everyone to be present before eating is considered polite and demonstrates consideration for others. However, in many communities within American society these values are less associated with mealtime. Instead, many U.S. children may be more used to waiting to open gifts, for instance when everyone is present at a holiday gathering such as Christmas. This practice allows for the family or group to share and celebrate joy and excitement as gifts are opened together. These examples provide evidence that cultural routines and socialization can play an important role in influencing attentional control behaviors and must be considered when conceptualizing and measuring EF. Recognition of this problem is important to the field.

Theoretically, the under-considered role of arbitrary rules in EF tasks and cultural context could have led to models of reasoning and EF that are culturally specific and could explain some lack of shared variance across many EF tasks, as well as the low performance among lower wealth and less educated participants. The literature linking poverty to EFs is robust (Dahlman et al., 2013; Pluck et al., 2018), yet at the same time, Dahlman et al. (2013) find instead that unhoused children in Bolivia scored significantly higher on an EF flexibility and planning tasks than children with more stable homes (Dahlman et al., 2013), so SES may be confounded with participants’ alignment with cultural routines implicit within EF task measurement.

Building strong and effective EF skills in the service of strong problem solving and reasoning has been posited to be one of the most crucial 21st century skills, meaning better understanding how to capitalize on children’s assets to support their development has the potential for powerful and broad impacts on children’s cognitive development. Rather than pushing first/s generation Latine children’s routines away from their everyday practices, for example, it could theoretically instead be important to support and enhance children’s participation and autonomy in daily tasks.

4. Relationship between EFs and relational reasoning

While EFs have gained attention due to their role as a building block of higher cognition and as being crucial to the skills and practices defined as central to success in the modern world of technology, innovation, and flexible problem solving, relational reasoning has long been studied as a building block foundation to these same skills (see Gentner and Holyoak, 1997; Markman and Wood, 2009; Richland and Simms, 2015). Relational reasoning is the process of drawing relational correspondences across representations, enabling reasoners broad opportunities including to make inferences from known information to novel problems or contexts, to recognize opportunities to transfer solutions from one problem to another, to build understanding of concepts or abstractions. These are underpinnings of innovation, problem solving, educational learning and expertise, higher order thinking and inferences about everyday phenomena (Markman and Wood, 2009; Richland and Simms, 2015; Zhao et al., 2021).

Starr et al. (2023) have argued that relational reasoning should be considered one of the EFs. They provide a compelling analysis of canonical EF measures and relational reasoning, finding a high correlation between relational reasoning and most of the EF measures, but also that this task better explained variance in math fluency and fraction comparison task performance than the EF measures.

We concur that measuring relational reasoning is crucial to understanding the building blocks of human cognitive activity, with relational reasoning being a core component of expertise in many educational domains (Richland and Simms, 2015; Alexander, 2019; Bunge and Leib, 2020; Zhao et al., 2021), and theorize that relational reasoning measures may be more likely to capture children and adults’ skills at managing attention and information in the world to accomplish tasks than traditional EF tasks. They may be also more likely than many EF measures to generalize across cultural contexts when in problem solving form, thereby being closer to characterizing what makes humans successful in varied contexts including non-WEIRD environments.

At the same time, we suggest that adding relational reasoning to the characterization of EFs will have the effect of perpetuating and expanding the challenges in developing precision in measurements that should correlate highly across EF tasks. Relational reasoning has by its nature levels of difficulty that may not function as linear, and in that way functions differently than other EFs. One type of difficulty in relational reasoning is the need to focus one’s attention on relational, rather than other types of similarity, including object correspondences, association, or perceptual similarity (see Rattermann and Gentner, 1998; Figure 1), where the D term of a matrix could be filled by relational or perceptual similarity (Simms and Richland, 2019). Relational attention may shift with a reasoner’s expertise in the relevant knowledge-base, which changes the nature of reasoners’ attention to the relational content of a task (Chi et al., 1981). As knowledge increases, reasoners may shift from attention to surface features and object-level correspondences to relational correspondences (see Gentner, 1988; Starr et al., 2018; Thibaut et al., 2022).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A sample item from relational reasoning measures.

Relational attention can also be manipulated by task goals and recent reasoning experience (Holyoak and Thagard, 1997; Vendetti et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2018; Simms and Richland, 2019), and may be in itself an individual difference that predicts learning and task ability (Zhao et al., under review). Relational attention, described by Vendetti et al. (2014) as a relational mindset, refers to the likelihood of noticing relational correspondences versus perceptual or featural similarity (see Vendetti et al., 2014; Simms and Richland, 2019), when there is not a specific cue to direct attention to object or relational correspondences.

Secondly, relational reasoning tasks vary by relational complexity, which again may not function as linear difficulty on an individual basis, but rather change in relation to individual differences in other EF capacities. These seem to function with a baseline, such that with adequate EFs for a task, reasoners’ performance will vary minimally across levels of relational complexity, while with not adequate EFs, reasoners may make relational errors, or may reason in qualitatively different ways, focusing on perceptual similarity rather than relational similarity (see Gentner and Rattermann, 1991; Richland et al., 2006; Krawczyk et al., 2008; Thibaut et al., 2010; Simms et al., 2018).

Thus, the cognitive resources of relational reasoning and EFs are distinct, and relational reasoning should be considered by researchers aiming to investigate the cognitive building blocks underlying individual differences in complex thought and intelligent behavior, but they are not independent and is productive to measure alongside EFs (see also Richland and Morrison, 2010).

There are also variations in the capacities involved in relational reasoning measured by different tasks. Verbal and non-verbal relational reasoning relate differently to verbal skills, and scores measured by relational tasks themselves may vary based on the form of the comparisons themselves (TORR, TORRJr: Zhao et al., 2021).

5. Training EFs: building on everyday assets and use of EFs in context

The developmental trajectory of children’s EF skills suggests these grow and shift over the lifespan (see Anderson, 2002; Zelazo et al., 2004), yet the mechanisms driving changes are not well understood, which has implications for policies and protections for encouraging its growth. The vast majority of explicit EF training programs involve repeated experiences with cognitively demanding training programs such as repeated practice on the dual n-back task (see Jaeggi et al., 2020), and many such studies find gains on the same EF task trained, but inconsistent or sometimes no transfer to new formal EF tasks (Doebel, 2020; Niebaum and Munakata, 2023). This suggests that if the ultimate goal of building EF skills is to support youth’s ability to perform tasks such as handling complexity in reasoning, inhibiting misleading pre-potent responses, and switching between taxing everyday tasks, perhaps EF training should take place by engaging in these types of tasks.

Some studies provide evidence that there may be productive gains for EFs as measured in traditional tasks by engaging in everyday activities such as sports or certain types of preschool curricula (Diamond, 2006; Niebaum and Munakata, 2023). Importantly, other seemingly mundane everyday practices that are not extra-curricular (and thus tied to available SES resources) but rather are tied to home work have not been investigated but seem to involve the same types of cognitive resource work, such as remembering long lists of groceries while going to and purchasing at the market, planning multi-step sequences while cooking or fixing equipment. Children’s involvement in these practices varies dramatically across cultural communities (see Alcalá et al., 2021), and thus may be underrecognized but potent means for training EF skills. At the same time, individual differences in children who display strong skills on activities such as shopping as noted above, may not be scored accordingly by a working memory measure requiring children to perform a task while retaining long lists of arbitrary letters, due to factors discussed here that may artificially limit performance, most notably because these children may treat the importance and goals of these tasks differently.

There is some evidence to support the role of cultural practices as unrecognized assets for EF training. Previous research on EF skills suggests that everyday bilingualism can lead to gains in EF skills, particularly on tasks requiring cognitive flexibility and managing conflicting attentional demands (Carlson and Meltzoff, 2008; Bialystok, 2011). However, there might be other cultural factors, in addition to bilingualism, that contribute to their EF skills. For example, Chinese-American immigrant children’s performance on some measures of executive control was predicted only by proficiency in Chinese, suggesting that perhaps higher fluency in Chinese could be related to greater experience with traditional Chinese values of obedience, behavioral control, and self-restraint, which would be causal to developing the higher EF skills (Chen et al., 2014).

5.1. Cultural variability in autonomy

A developmental mechanism that has not yet been considered broadly to play a role in the measurement and development of EFs is the known variability in children’s level of autonomy and management of household tasks across cultural communities (see Gaskins, 2000; Alcalá et al., 2014). Management of household tasks often requires decision-making about key goals and tasks that are necessary to accomplish and execute these tasks. Mechanistically, this often involves holding high amounts of information in mind while solving problems, inhibiting prepotent responses to one stimulus in favor of persisting on another task, or fluidly switching between tasks that must be completed. These are processes that seem to require both relational reasoning/ problem solving and high levels of EFs, and thus may be a potent training regime that has not yet been considered as such.

Cross-cultural research has documented a wide range in ideologies about the level of autonomy and control that parents expect children to maintain, visible as the level of work and initiative that children contribute to household work and other community activities (Gaskins, 1996, 2000; Gaskins and Paradise, 2010; Alcalá et al., 2014). For example, a study of first and second generation Latine children in California found higher levels of help at home on their own initiative than observed in European American families, while keeping in mind the needs of the group and help when needed (Alcalá et al., 2018). A study of indigenous children revealed more time spent time in household work and engaging in free play, setting their own agenda, while European-American children had less access to work and were more likely to participate in activities organized and managed by adults (Ochs and Izquierdo, 2009; Cervera, 2016). In a recent study on the impact of COVID on child development across cultures found clear cultural differences in how families organized children’s level of autonomy and participation in the household (Alcalá et al., 2021).

Additionally, having the experience of making consequential decisions and solving real problems raises children’s expectation that they can and should make real decisions about when and whether to engage one’s reasoning and EF resources in any given task. The implication for standard psychometric measures of EF is that these children may be less likely to do so when the task rules are arbitrary, and any actual gain is not recognizable. In homes where children’s lives are organized and guided by adults, children may become highly skilled at following instructions, while in homes where children take initiative and manage tasks, children may become highly skilled at making their own decisions about how to manage complex tasks, determining goal directed behavior and holding constraints in mind while acting to perform other tasks. These different modes of engaging with the world may differently affect performance on psychometric tasks regardless of EF capacities (Barker et al., 2014).

6. Conclusion

We face a pressing need to understand the building blocks of everyday thinking and learning, to better know how to prepare youth to succeed in a complex and changing world (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2018). Children learn as they engage in culturally meaningful activities (Rogoff, 2014), supported by a set of dynamic processes that need to be coordinated for learning to occur, including attention, emotional regulation, and inhibition of incorrect or inadequate responses. Measurement of the building blocks of everyday cognition must capture the range of different ways that humans may build and use attentional control in real world tasks, and relational attention and reasoning is an underdeveloped field for measuring these individual differences. EF measurements must also be better aligned with the range of cultural practices humans use them for, with one key aspect being to recognize the cultural constraints present in the use of tasks that require manipulating arbitrary rules – a hallmark of contemporary EF measurement.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

LR took lead on formulating the manuscript focus and text. HZ contributed to all study ideas and manuscript writing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Institute of Education Science, U.S. Department of Education (R305A170488 and R305A190467), and the National Science Foundation (2141411).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alcalá, L., Gaskins, S., and Richland, L. E. (2021). A cultural lens on Yucatec Maya families’ COVID-19 experiences. Child Dev. 92, 851–865. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcalá, L., Rogoff, B., and Fraire, A. L. (2018). Sophisticated collaboration is common among Mexican-heritage US children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, 11377–11384. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1805707115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcalá, L., Rogoff, B., Mejía-Arauz, R., Coppens, A. D., and Dexter, A. L. (2014). Children's initiative in contributing to family work in indigenous-heritage and cosmopolitan communities in Mexico. Hum. Dev. 57, 96–115. doi: 10.1159/000356763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, P. A. (2019). Individual differences in college-age learners: the importance of relational reasoning for learning and assessment in higher education. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 89, 416–428. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 8, 71–82. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, J. E., Semenov, A. D., Michaelson, L., Provan, L. S., Snyder, H. R., and Munakata, Y. (2014). Less-structured time in children's daily lives predicts self-directed executive functioning. Front. Psychol. 5:593. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., and Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E. (2011). Reshaping the mind: the benefits of bilingualism. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 65:229. doi: 10.1037/a0025406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, R., and Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 36–41. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, R., and Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev. Neuropsychol. 19, 273–293. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunge, S. A., and Leib, E. R. (2020). How does education hone reasoning ability? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 29, 167–173. doi: 10.1177/0963721419898818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, P. W. (1997). “Theory and methodology in executive function research” in Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function. ed. P. Rabbit (London: Psychology Press Publishers), 81–116.

Google Scholar

Carlson, S. M., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Dev. Sci. 11, 282–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., and Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: a theoretical account of the processing in the raven progressive matrices test. Psychol. Rev. 97, 404–431. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cervera, M. D. (2016). Studying Yucatec Maya children through the eyes of their mothers. In V. Pache, C.-E. Suremain de, and E. Guillermet (Eds.), Production Institutionnelle de L’enfance. Déclinaisons Locales et Pratiques D'acteurs (Amérique Latine et Europe). (Belgium: Presses Universitaires de Liège), 161–177.

Google Scholar

Chen, S. H., Zhou, Q., Uchikoshi, Y., and Bunge, S. A. (2014). Variations on the bilingual advantage? Links of Chinese and English proficiency to Chinese American children's self-regulation. Front. Psychol. 5:1069. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P., and Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cogn. Sci. 5, 121–152. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0502_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correa-Chávez, M., and Rogoff, B. (2009). Children’s attention to interactions directed to others: Guatemalan Mayan and European American patterns. Dev. Psychol. 45:630. doi: 10.1037/a0014144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahlman, S., Bäckström, P., Bohlin, G., and Frans, Ö. (2013). Cognitive abilities of street children: low-SES Bolivian boys with and without experience of living in the street. Child Neuropsychol. 19, 540–556. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.731499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2002). “Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. eds. D. T. Stuss and R. T. Knight (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 466–503.

Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2006). “The early development of executive functions” in Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change. eds. E. Bialystok and F. I. M. Craik (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 70–95.

Google Scholar

Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 942–956. doi: 10.1177/1745691620904771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engle, R. W., and Kane, M. J. (2004). “Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control” in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. ed. S. Ross (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 145–199.

Google Scholar

Gaskins, S. (1996). “How Mayan parental theories come into play” in Parents' Cultural Belief Systems: Their Origins, Expressions, and Consequences. eds. C. M. Super and S. Harkness (New York: Guilford), 345–363.

Google Scholar

Gaskins, S. (2000). Children's daily activities in a Mayan village: a culturally grounded description. Cross-Cult. Res. 34, 375–389. doi: 10.1177/106939710003400405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaskins, S., and Paradise, R. (2010). “Learning through observation” in The Anthropology of Learning in Childhood. eds. D. F. Lancy, J. Bock, and S. Gaskins (Lanham: Alta Mira Press), 85–117.

Google Scholar

Gentner, D. (1988). Metaphor as structure mapping: the relational shift. Child Dev. 59, 47–59. doi: 10.2307/1130388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentner, D., and Holyoak, K. J. (1997). Reasoning and learning by analogy: introduction. Am. Psychol. 52, 32–34. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.1.32

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentner, D., and Rattermann, M. J. (1991). “Language and the career of similarity” in Perspectives on Thought and Language: Interrelations in Development. eds. S. A. Gelman and J. P. Byrnes (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 225–277.

Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holyoak, K. J., and Thagard, P. (1997). The analogical mind. Am. Psychol. 52, 35–44. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.1.35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Parlett-Pelleriti, C. M., Moon, S. M., Evans, M., Kritzmacher, A., et al. (2020). Investigating the effects of spacing on working memory training outcome: a randomized, controlled, multisite trial in older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser B 75, 1181–1192. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y.-S. G. (2020). Hierarchical and dynamic relations of language and cognitive skills to reading comprehension: testing the direct and indirect effects model of reading (DIER). J. Educ. Psychol. 112, 667–684. doi: 10.1037/edu0000407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krawczyk, D. C., Morrison, R. G., Viskontas, I., Holyoak, K. J., Chow, T. W., Mendez, M. F., et al. (2008). Distraction during relational reasoning: the role of prefrontal cortex in interference control. Neuropsychologia 46, 2020–2032. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulis, S. S., Tsethlikai, M., Harthun, M. L., Hibbeler, P. K., Ayers, S. L., and Deschine Parkhurst, N. (2019). Parenting in 2 worlds: effects of a culturally grounded parenting intervention for urban American Indians on participant cultural engagement. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 26, 437–446. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, D. R., Lewandowsky, S., and Craig, S. (2014). Working memory capacity and fluid abilities: the more difficult the item, the more more is better. Front. Psychol. 5:239. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markman, A. B., and Wood, K. L. (2009). “The cognitive science of innovation tools” in Tools for Innovation: The Science Behind the Practical Methods that Drive New Ideas. eds. A. B. Markman and K. L. Wood (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 3–22.

Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and Organization of Individual Differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, R. G., Krawczyk, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., Hummel, J. E., Chow, T. W., Miller, B. L., et al. (2004). A neurocomputational model of analogical reasoning and its breakdown in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 260–271. doi: 10.1162/089892904322984553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine (2018). How People Learn II: Learners, Contexts, and Cultures. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

Google Scholar

Niebaum, J. C., and Munakata, Y. (2023). Why doesn’t executive function training improve academic achievement? Rethinking individual differences, relevance, and engagement from a contextual framework. J. Cogn. Dev. 24, 241–259. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2022.2160723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochs, E., and Izquierdo, C. (2009). Responsibility in childhood: three developmental trajectories. Ethos 37, 391–413. doi: 10.1111/j.1548-1352.2009.01066.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, L. (1997). “Do “frontal tests” measure executive function? Issues of assessment and evidence from fluency tests” in Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function. ed. P. Rabbitt (London: Psychology Press), 191–213.

Google Scholar

Pluck, G., Banda-Cruz, D. R., Andrade-Guimaraes, M. V., and Trueba, A. F. (2018). Socioeconomic deprivation and the development of neuropsychological functions: a study with “street children” in Ecuador. Child Neuropsychol. 24, 510–523. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2017.1294150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rattermann, M. J., and Gentner, D. (1998). More evidence for a relational shift in the development of analogy: Children's performance on a causal-mapping task. Cogn. Dev. 13, 453–478. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90003-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richland, L. E., and Burchinal, M. (2013). Early executive function predicts reasoning development. Psychol. Sci. 24, 87–92. doi: 10.1177/0956797612450883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richland, L. E., and Morrison, R. G. (2010). Is analogical reasoning just another measure of executive functioning? General commentary. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, 1–2. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richland, L. E., and Simms, N. (2015). Analogy, higher order thinking, and education. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 6, 177–192.

Google Scholar

Richland, L. E., Morrison, R. G., and Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children’s development of analogical reasoning: insights from scene analogy problems. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 94, 249–273. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogoff, B. (2014). Learning by observing and pitching in to family and community endeavors: an orientation. Hum. Dev. 57, 69–81. doi: 10.1159/000356757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simms, N. K., Frausel, R. R., and Richland, L. E. (2018). Working memory predicts children’s analogical reasoning. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 166, 160–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simms, N. K., and Richland, L. E. (2019). Generating relations elicits a relational mindset in children. Cogn. Sci. 43:e12795. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., and Hankin, B. L. (2015). Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Front. Psychol. 6:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starr, A., Leib, E. R., Younger, J. W., Project iLead Consortium, Uncapher, M. R., and Bunge, S. A. (2023). Relational thinking: an overlooked component of executive functioning. Dev. Sci. 26:e13320. doi: 10.1111/desc.13340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starr, A., Vendetti, M. S., and Bunge, S. A. (2018). Eye movements provide insight into individual differences in children's analogical reasoning strategies. Acta Psychol. 186, 18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibaut, J. P., French, R., and Vezneva, M. (2010). The development of analogy making in children: Cognitive load and executive functions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 106, 1–19.

Google Scholar

Thibaut, J. P., Glady, Y., and French, R. M. (2022). Understanding the what and when of analogical reasoning across analogy formats: an eye-tracking and machine learning approach. Cogn. Sci. 46:e13208. doi: 10.1111/cogs.13208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., and van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence 35, 427–449. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vendetti, M. S., Wu, A., and Holyoak, K. J. (2014). Far out thinking: generating solutions to distant analogies promotes relational thinking. Psychol. Sci. 25, 928–933. doi: 10.1177/0956797613518079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, C. M., Hubachek, S. Q., and Vendetti, M. S. (2018). Achieving abstraction: generating far analogies promotes relational reasoning in children. Dev. Psychol. 54, 1833–1841. doi: 10.1037/dev0000581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yanaoka, K., Michaelson, L. E., Guild, R. M., Dostart, G., Yonehiro, J., Saito, S., et al. (2022). Cultures crossing: the power of habit in delaying gratification. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1172–1181. doi: 10.1177/09567976221074650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I. M., and Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the life. Acta Psychol. 115, 167–183. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, H., Alexander, P. A., and Sun, Y. (2021). Relational reasoning’s contributions to mathematical thinking and performance in Chinese elementary and middle-school students. J. Educ. Psychol. 113, 279–303. doi: 10.1037/edu0000595

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, H., Richland, L. E., Vollman, E., Lerner, B., Yeung, N., and Wong, J. (under review). Individual Differences in Attention to Analogical Relations.

Google Scholar

Keywords: executive function, relational reasoning, cultural context, problem solving, WEIRD samples

Citation: Richland LE and Zhao H (2023) Measuring the building blocks of everyday cognition: executive functions and relational reasoning. Front. Psychol. 14:1219414. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1219414

Received: 09 May 2023; Accepted: 13 July 2023;
Published: 27 September 2023.

Edited by:

Andrew Tolmie, University College London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Vanessa R. Simmering, University of Kansas, United States

Copyright © 2023 Richland and Zhao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lindsey Engle Richland, bC5yaWNobGFuZEB1Y2kuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.