Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 June 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Education and Development in Early Years From Cultural-historical Theory View all 17 articles

Parental burnout, depression and emotional development of the preschoolers

  • Faculty of Psychology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia

Introduction: Parental burnout is becoming more and more prevalent in the world, mainly incultures with high demands towards parents. Parental burnout is distinctive from depression and might have its unique influence on child development, which isunder current international research. This work contributes to the understanding of parental burnout, maternal depression and child emotional development(specifically emotion comprehension) interrelations. Additionally, we explored whether there are differences in the effects of parental burnout and depressionon boys and girls.

Methods: To analyse the emotional development of the preschoolers, the Russian version of the Test of Emotional Comprehension (TEC) was used. We used the Russian version of the Parental Burnout Inventory (PBI) to analyse the level of PB and the Russian version of Beck depression Inventory (BDI) to assess participants’ depression level.

Results: Parental burnout positively correlates with child emotional comprehension skills, specifically understanding of external causes (B = 0.20, CI: 0.03; 0.37) and mental causes of emotions (B = 0.22, CI: 0.05; 0.40). This effect is gender dependent and is significantlyhigher for girls (B = 0.54, CI: 0.09; 0.98). The effect of maternal depression on emotion comprehension skills is also gender dependent: total scores on emotioncomprehension tasks are significantly higher for daughters of mothers with depression (B = 0.59, CI: 0.001; 1.18).

Discussion: Maternal depression and parental burnout might provoke development of extra sensitivity and self-regulation strategies in girls.

1. Introduction

Parenting can be one of the most challenging jobs, with demands for high emotional involvement, multitasking, and many physical and financial resources. This job does not include sick leave, opportunity to quit, or even weekends. At the same time, societal demands to the standards of parenting have increased significantly in the past decades (Roskam et al., 2021), which may lead to parental burnout (PB). PB is commonly defined as a state of intense emotional exhaustion related to one’s parental role, which includes emotional detachment from one’s children and doubts in one’s capacity to be a good parent (Roskam et al., 2017).

Risk factors for PB include child chronic disease, lack of social and family support, high social demands toward parents, conflicts with the spouse and co-parenting problems, emotional dysregulation of a parent and child-oriented perfectionism (Roskam et al., 2021; Favez et al., 2022; Lin et al., 2022a,b; Wauters et al., 2022). The cross-cultural study across 42 countries showed that the main risk factors for PB were lack of support from extended family and high societal demands towards parents, namely, the expectation that parents could control physical, emotional, intellectual and social development of the child as well as minimise any risks for it (Roskam et al., 2021).The authors further showed that PB is common in the majority of countries around the world, with particularly high prevalence in the countries with individualistic cultural values and the trend for intensive parenting (Roskam et al., 2021). However, PB is mainly discussed in relation to parents of children with chronic diseases and/or special needs (Lindström et al., 2011; Gerber et al., 2021; Wauters et al., 2022), while cohort and population-based studies on the topic remain scarce.

This may be partially due to the fact that some symptoms of PB overlap with depression, e.g., sleep problems, escape ideation, and emotional exhaustion (Lebert-Charron et al., 2018; Mikolajczak et al., 2019). However, there is convincing evidence that PB is distinctive from depressive symptomatology and it might have unique consequences for the parents and their children (Sánchez-Rodríguez et al., 2019; Mikolajczak et al., 2020). The surge of studies of the association between PB and child mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic, when there was an increased pressure on parents, revealed that PB was positively associated with children’s stress behaviours, and negatively associated with children’s positive behaviours (Kerr et al., 2021; Koerber et al., 2023).

Nevertheless, to date this topic remains understudied and the existing literature has mainly focused on maternal depression and its consequences for child development. The meta-analysis of 191 studies showed that maternal perinatal depression and anxiety were associated with poorer child’s social–emotional, language and cognitive development in infancy, childhood and adolescence (Rogers et al., 2020). In the Norwegian longitudinal study child social–emotional problems at age 2 were strongly associated both with maternal depression during pregnancy and 8 weeks postpartum (Junge et al., 2017). The study of Abdollahi et al. (2017) indicated that developmental disabilities in communication and gross motor skills in 4 years-old children were associated with concurrent and chronic maternal depressive symptoms, rather than with the perinatal maternal depression. The study of 1,992 mother–child dyads also showed the significance of the current maternal depressive symptoms for the child’s emotional development and communication skills (Hentges et al., 2020).

Findings on gender differences and maternal depression consequences are contradictory. The US longitudinal study showed that maternal depression was associated with poorer social-emotional child development, particularly among girls (Urizar and Muñoz, 2022). A recent meta-analysis indicates that maternal depression affects cognitive development in boys, but not girls (Ahun et al., 2021). The Norwegian longitudinal study showed that while maternal depression affected both infant boys and girls’ cognitive development, girls’ scores tended to increase during the time of observation (Azak, 2012). The study conducted on the adolescent sample revealed that the relationship between maternal depression and child internalising symptoms was significantly larger for daughters compared to sons (Livings, 2021). Such data inconsistency on gender differences in child outcomes could be largely explained by cultural differences and measurement diversity (Ahun et al., 2021) and demonstrates a clear knowledge gap in this area.

Cumulatively, the abovementioned findings indicate that the areas of child development most vulnerable for the effects of parental depression are those related to emotional development. While the consequences of PB for the child’s emotional development remain unknown, there is data showing that PB is associated with parental neglect and violence more often than depression (Mikolajczak et al., 2020; Griffith, 2022), which, in turn, may be damaging for the child emotional development. Furthermore, there is evidence of association between PB, perceived parental hostility and adolescent later externalising problems (Chen et al., 2022). Although a number of studies have also found the association between PB and adolescent behavioural problems (Yuan et al., 2022), student’s problematic behaviour, and academic outcomes (Hong et al., 2022), the direction of these associations remains unclear. To date there is no evidence on gender differences in the PB effects on child emotional development.

Some data suggests that parents of younger age children are at higher risk for PB (Favez et al., 2022). Early childhood is a critical time for social and emotional development: approximately by age 4 children are able to recognize and name emotions and begin to understand how external causes can affect emotions of other people (Pons et al., 2004). High comprehension of emotions performance predicts academic success in preschool children (Da Glória Franco et al., 2017; Garrett-Peters et al., 2017; Józsa and Barrett, 2018; Cavadini et al., 2021). Cultural-historical theory considers understanding of emotions as an important component of any successful child activity (Zaporozhets et al., 1985, p. 26–28). Emotion comprehension plays a significant role in prosocial skills development (Liao et al., 2014; Da Glória Franco et al., 2017). Good understanding of emotions skills are also associated with lower risk of mental health problems (Herba and Phillips, 2004; Martinsen et al., 2019).

Emotional development during the first 3 years of life is strongly associated with social–emotional development at 5 years old (Wang et al., 2022). Maternal depression and anxiety during toddlerhood are also strongly associated with child outcomes at age 5 (Hentges et al., 2020). As there are more tools available to accurately measure emotional development in preschoolers, than in children of 0–3 years old, hence this age group was selected as a cohort for the present study.

Therefore, the aim of this work is to examine the association between PB, depression and child emotional development. We are going to focus on children of preschool age, as it is a sensitive period for emotional development (Pons et al., 2004). Moreover, we are going to compare the effects of maternal concurrent depression and PB on 4 dimensions of the emotion comprehension ability in preschoolers: understanding of emotions caused by external causes, understanding of mental emotions, understanding of the ability to regulate emotions, and the general ability to understand emotions. Finally, we are going to explore whether there are differences in the effects of parental PB and depression on boys and girls.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Procedure and participants

The study included two stages: (1) a survey for parents that assessed parental depression and PB and (2) observational diagnostics of the preschoolers’ emotional development. The data was collected during the period of April–June 2022 on the basis of kindergartens in 4 regions of Russia (Yakutia, Tatarstan, Perm, Moscow). The findings are based on the responses of a sample of 251 dyads: parents (M = 33.74, SD = 5.08) and children of preschool age (M = 4.92, SD = 0.44). The participants received an invitation to take part in the study through the kindergarten’s administration. Families were included in the study if the participating parent was living together with the child and was her legal representative, and the parent was able to speak and read in Russian.

2.2. Ethical considerations

Both stages of the study were approved by the Ethical Committee of the Russian Psychological Society, Lomonosov Moscow State University. All participants were asked to sign an informed consent using the online form before starting the completion of the parental survey. The participants signed the agreement for their children to participate in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All women participated voluntarily in the study. The assessments were carried out with the Testograph online platform.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. The demographic questionnaire

The survey included questions regarding the participants’ age at the time of testing, education (primary/secondary/tertiary) and marital status (married/cohabiting with partner/single). We also collected the data about the child’s age, sex and chronic diseases (reported by the parents).

2.3.2. Beck depression inventory

We used the Russian version of the Beck depression inventory (BDI) to assess the levels of parental depression (Yakupova, 2018). BDI is a 21-item, self-report questionnaire measuring symptoms of depression (Beck et al., 1961). Items are scored on a scale from 0 to 3, e.g., 0: “I do not feel guilty,” 1: “I often feel guilty” 2: “I feel guilty most of the time,” 3: “I feel guilty all the time” The depression score is obtained by summing the 21 item scores. For the Russian version Cronbach’s coefficient is α = 0.866 (Yakupova, 2018).

2.3.3. Parental burnout

We used the Russian version of the parental burnout inventory (PBI) (Starchenkova, 2019) to analyse the level of PB in mothers. This is a 23-item questionnaire assessing the four core symptoms of parental burnout: emotional exhaustion (9 items; e.g., I feel completely run down by my role as a parent), contrast with previous parental self (6 items; e.g., I tell myself I’m no longer the parent I used to be), loss of pleasure in one’s parental role (5 items; e.g., I do not enjoy being with my children), and emotional distancing from one’s children (3 items; e.g., I am no longer able to show my children that I love them) (Roskam et al., 2018). The participants were asked to assess each statement using a seven-point frequency scale from 0 to 6 (never, a few times a year, once a month or less, a few times a month, once a week, a few times a week, every day). The parental burnout score is computed by summing the item scores: higher scores reflect higher parental burnout levels. The internal consistency of the Russian version of the scale was Cronbach’s alpha α = 0.97 (Roskam et al., 2021).

2.3.4. Child emotional development

To analyse the emotional development of the preschoolers, the Russian version of the test of emotional comprehension (TEC) was used (Pons and Harris, 2000; Veraksa et al., 2021). This test is designed to assess the emotion comprehension skills in children of 3–11 years old. The child is presented with 23 simple plot pictures about the emotional experience of children of the same sex with him. Each plot includes four possible emotional responses. The child needs to evaluate what the protagonist of the story felt in the situation presented. For example: “This boy (girl) is getting a birthday present. How is this boy (girl) feeling? Is he (she) happy, sad, alright or scared?” The test captures information about the different components of emotion comprehension: understanding of emotions caused by external causes, understanding of mental emotions, understanding of the ability to regulate emotions, as well as the general ability to understand emotions. Each answer is scored “0,” if it is wrong, “1” if it is correct. The internal consistency of the Russian version of the test is α = 0.97.

2.3.5. Covariates

Two statistical models were used to test our hypotheses. Model 1 was adjusted for child age at testing and sex. Model 2 was further adjusted for maternal age at testing, level of education, family status, socioeconomic status, and parity as covariates. When testing the hypothesis of differential effects of parental burnout and depression on boys and girls, child sex was removed from the list of covariates, as in this analysis the sample was already stratified by sex.

2.4. Statistical analysis

BDI and PBI scores were log-transformed to attain normality.

Spearman’s correlation coefficient was used to estimate the relationship between BDI and PBI scores.

Pearson chi-square test and independent t-test were performed to compare the sample characteristics between boys and girls. t-tests exploring the differences in emotional development were weighted by age.

Multiple linear regression analysis examined the association between parental depression and PB and child emotional development.

All analyses were performed using SPSS 28 software (IBM).

3. Results

Demographic characteristics for the parents and their children are presented in Table 1. It shows that the majority of the dyads come from middle-income families (69.7%), where mothers were married (81.6%) and had higher education (61.8%). Furthermore, shows that when comparing boys (n = 106) and girls (n = 144), boys were slightly older, scored statistically significantly higher on TEC External scale and lower on TEC Mental scale. There were no other significant differences between the subsamples divided according to child sex (p-values for all >0.029) (Table 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of the sample according to child’s sex.

Parental depression and PB scores were moderately correlated (Pearson correlation coefficient = 0.49, p < 0.001).

Table 3 shows that, when adjusted for child age and sex, TEC scales of understanding of external causes of emotions and mental emotions scores were significantly associated with PB. Additionally, we discovered an association of general understanding of emotions with PB in Model 2. While the trends were largely the same, there were no statistically significant associations between parental depression and child emotional development.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association of parental depression and burnout with child emotional development.

Table 4 further shows that when we performed the multiple regression analyses separately for boys and girls, the associations remained significant only for fully adjusted models for total and mental scales and only in girls. Furthermore, we discovered a borderline significant association between parental depression and total emotional development in girls, when controlling for child sex, maternal age, education, family status, socio-economic status (SES), and parity (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Gender dependent association of parental depression and burnout with child emotional development.

However, the interaction analyses revealed that the differences between the subsamples divided by sex were not statistically significant due to low statistical power (p-value for all >0.31; observed power <0.17 for all).

4. Discussion

The aim of our research was to examine the association between PB, depression and preschoolers’ emotional development.

The obtained data indicate significant correlation between PB and child emotional development. In the present study the association with PB was stronger, than with maternal depression. To date there is a lack of evidence of the PB effects on child emotional development, and our results contribute to this topic. Higher PB scores are associated with higher child outcomes on understanding of the following components of emotion comprehension: external and mental. It is easier for the child to recognize emotions based on facial expressions, to understand external causes of emotions and how memories, desires and beliefs influence emotions. These results contradict our hypothesis of direct adverse effects of PB on child emotional development. PB is associated with parental neglect and violence more often than depression (Mikolajczak et al., 2020; Griffith, 2022). Neglect presumes emotional deprivation, which may be damaging for the child’s emotional development and contribute to internalising problems (Christ et al., 2017; Hunt et al., 2017).

Mothers who are suffering from emotional exhaustion (the dominant PB symptom) report having difficulties in providing clear and consistent emotional responses and have depleted facial expressions of emotions (Roskam et al., 2017). The child has to make an effort to interpret the parent’s state as her safety depends on it and, thus, might develop extra sensitivity. PB is associated with lower parent emotional intelligence and regulation skills (Lin et al., 2022a,b), therefore, the child might develop its own regulation skills to compensate for a parent’s lack of self-regulation. There is data showing that negative effects of PB on child’s mental health can be minimised when parent’s emotional regulation skills are improved (Yang et al., 2021; Lin et al., 2022a,b). In the present study we did not assess parent’s emotional regulation skills, which could play a mediating role and decrease adverse PB effects on children.

According to the cultural-historical framework, parenting is a social construct and parenting behaviour depends on the demands and norms of the society during the historical period (Zakharova, 2012). The social image of the parenting role serves as an orientation system for the style of everyday childcare and parent self-efficiency assessment (Zakharova, 2012; Burmenskaya, 2022). The results obtained in the study may be attributed to the intensive parenting trend and high social demands towards parents and their positive association with PB risks (Roskam et al., 2021). Parents with PB might have invested much effort in the child’s development, which could have led to their exhaustion. Their children, in turn, might have higher TEC scores because of the parent’s past investments. There is also evidence on the role of attachment style in the emotion comprehension development (Cooke et al., 2016; Psychogiou et al., 2018). The parent with PB could manage to develop the secure attachment style, which is associated with better emotion comprehension skills. Moreover, there is data on the significant role of father-child attachment style in the emotion comprehension development (Psychogiou et al., 2018). In our study only mothers participated, and data on fathers’ PB and mental health could enrich the results.

The insignificant association between depression and emotional development contradicts the research accumulated to date (Hentges et al., 2020). This finding might be explained by the characteristics of the sample—there is low awareness of depressive symptoms in Russian society and depression is stigmatised (Dubitskaya, 2009; Beshanova and Kashirskikh, 2020). The study was conducted in the kindergartens, and it might be important for the participants to show the researchers and kindergarten administration that they are competent parents, despite the assurance in the confidentiality of their responses. Therefore, the data may be limited by the social desirability bias. PB is far less stigmatised, so the questionnaire might not elicit socially desirable answers. However, it is important to note that in the present study PB and depression moderately correlate.

One more possible explanation is based on the findings of Nonnenmacher et al. (2021), which indicated no significant associations between theory of mind development and mother’s depression in preschoolers. The authors discovered a mediating role of self-comforting behaviour developed by children, which was a protective factor against an insensitive parenting environment. Children of depressed mothers can develop their own emotion regulation strategies that can decrease the adverse effects of maternal depression.

Our results show moderate gender differences in emotional development: in our study boys scored statistically significantly higher on TEC External scale and lower on TEC Mental scale. Data on gender differences in emotion comprehension is contradictory. There is evidence on girls’ better performance (Bosacki and Moore, 2004; Garner and Waajid, 2008), some studies reveal boys’ better performance in emotion comprehension tasks (Laible and Thompson, 2000), while others indicate no gender differences in emotion understanding (Pons et al., 2004; Denham et al., 2012). Although Aznar and Tenenbaum (2013) found no gender differences in overall TEC scores, boys scored higher than girls in understanding the situational causes of emotion, whereas girls scored higher on understanding reflective emotions. Fidalgo et al. (2018) revealed no gender differences in 8 of 9 TEC components, with the exception of the False belief task, where girls performed better. Thus, our findings support the hypothesis that there are gender disparities in different types of emotion comprehension, but no such trend in total emotional development.

Finally, our study indicated the gender difference in PB and depression effects on emotional development as well: they positively correlate with girls’ emotional development, specifically total TEC scores. PB is also positively related with understanding of mental causes of emotions in girls. The possible explanation of this finding could be related to the characteristics of women’s gender socialisation, as girls are expected to be more sensitive, show empathy and high self-regulation skills (Chaplin and Aldao, 2013). Thereby girls might be more likely to develop understanding of a parent’s emotions as a strategy, compensating for parent’s dysregulation. The 13 years longitudinal study by Murray et al. (2006) also showed that daughters of depressed mothers tend to develop higher sensitivity. Similarly, they demonstrated elevated social maturity. Extra sensitivity was associated with adolescent depression, therefore, the influence of maternal depression can be considered as adverse.

5. Conclusion

Our findings support the evidence that PB is distinctive from depression and can have its unique consequences for child emotional development. PB positively correlates with child emotional comprehension skills, specifically understanding of external causes and mental causes of emotions. This effect is gender dependent and is significantly higher for girls. Maternal depression and parental burnout might provoke development of extra sensitivity and self-regulation strategies in girls.

6. Limitations

The data obtained in the study might be contaminated by a social desirability bias, because it was collected via kindergartens. Despite the confidentiality guarantees, parents could be anxious about revealing their depressive symptoms, as they are socially stigmatised.

Data on PB in fathers could enrich the results and, therefore, further studies that include paternal mental health measures are warranted.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Russian Psychological Society. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AS and VY contributed to conception and design of the study. VY organized the database and wrote sections of the manuscript. AS performed the statistical analysis and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The research project was supported by the grant of the Russian Science Foundation number 22-18-00356.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all the women who took part in this study and to Victoria Yureva for her help with the data.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abdollahi, F., Rezai Abhari, F., and Zarghami, M. (2017). Post-partum depression effect on child health and development. Acta Med. Iran. 55, 109–114.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ahun, M. N., Gapare, C., Gariépy, G., and Côté, S. M. (2021). Sex differences in the association between maternal depression and child and adolescent cognitive development: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 51, 1431–1440. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azak, S. (2012). Maternal depression and sex differences shape the infants’ trajectories of cognitive development. Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 803–814. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aznar, A., and Tenenbaum, H. (2013). Spanish parents’ emotion talk and their children’s understanding of emotion. Front. Psychol. 4:670. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., CH, W., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beshanova, E. V., and Kashirskikh, O. N. (2020) Media effects in the Russian mediascope on the example of depression and anxiety narratives. Master’s Thesis. Moscow (Russia): Higher School of Ecomonocs. Available at: (Accessed: April 17, 2023). (in Russian) https://www.hse.ru/en/edu/vkr/366242220

Google Scholar

Bosacki, S. L., and Moore, C. (2004). Preschoolers’ understanding of simple and complex emotions: links with gender and language. Sex Roles J. Res. 50, 659–675. doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.0000027568.26966.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burmenskaya, G. V. (2022). Orienting activity of the subject as a mechanism for instruction learning and development. Psychol Russ 15, 36–48. doi: 10.11621/pir.2022.0403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavadini, T., Richard, S., Dalla-Libera, N., and Gentaz, E. (2021). Emotion knowledge, social behaviour and locomotor activity predict the mathematic performance in 706 preschool children. Sci. Rep. 11:14399. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93706-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaplin, T. M., and Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 139, 735–765. doi: 10.1037/a0030737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, B.-B., Qu, Y., Yang, B., and Chen, X. (2022). Chinese mothers’ parental burnout and adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems: the mediating role of maternal hostility. Dev. Psychol. 58, 768–777. doi: 10.1037/dev0001311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christ, S. L., Kwak, Y. Y., and Lu, T. (2017). The joint impact of parental psychological neglect and peer isolation on adolescents’ depression. Child Abuse Negl. 69, 151–162. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooke, J. E., Stuart-Parrigon, K. L., Movahed-Abtahi, M., Koehn, A. J., and Kerns, K. A. (2016). Children’s emotion understanding and mother-child attachment: a meta-analysis. Emotion 16, 1102–1106. doi: 10.1037/emo0000221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Da Glória Franco, M., Beja, M. J., Candeias, A., and Santos, N. (2017). Emotion understanding, social competence and school achievement in children from primary School in Portugal. Front. Psychol. 8:1376. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., and Zinsser, K. (2012). Early childhood teachers as socializers of young children’s emotional competence. Early Childhood Educ. J. 40, 137–143. doi: 10.1007/s10643-012-0504-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubitskaya, E. A. (2009) Stigmatization of depression. Doctoral Thesis. Moscow (Russia): Samara State Medical University. (in Russian).

Google Scholar

Favez, N., Max, A., Bader, M., and Tissot, H. (2022). When not teaming up puts parents at risk: coparenting and parental burnout in dual-parent heterosexual families in Switzerland. Fam. Process 62, 272–286. doi: 10.1111/famp.12777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fidalgo, A. M., Tenenbaum, H. R., and Aznar, A. (2018). Are there gender differences in emotion comprehension? Analysis of the test of emotion comprehension. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27, 1065–1074. doi: 10.1007/s10826-017-0956-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, P. W., and Waajid, B. (2008). The associations of emotion knowledge and teacher-child relationships to preschool children’s school-related developmental competence. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 29, 89–100. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2007.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett-Peters, P. T., Castro, V. L., and Halberstadt, A. G. (2017). Parents’ beliefs about children’s emotions, children’s emotion understanding, and classroom adjustment in middle childhood. Soc. Dev. 26, 575–590. doi: 10.1111/sode.12222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, Z., Davidovics, Z., and Anaki, D. (2021). The relationship between self-compassion, concern for others, and parental burnout in child’s chronic care management. Mindfulness 12, 2920–2928. doi: 10.1007/s12671-021-01752-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffith, A. K. (2022). Parental burnout and child maltreatment during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Fam. Violence 37, 725–731. doi: 10.1007/s10896-020-00172-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hentges, R. F., Graham, S. A., Fearon, P., Tough, S., and Madigan, S. (2020). The chronicity and timing of prenatal and antenatal maternal depression and anxiety on child outcomes at age 5. Depress. Anxiety 37, 576–586. doi: 10.1002/da.23039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herba, C., and Phillips, M. (2004). Annotation: development of facial expression recognition from childhood to adolescence: behavioural and neurological perspectives. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1185–1198. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00316.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, V. T. P., An, N. H., Thao, T. T. P., Thao, L. N., and Thanh, N. M. (2022). Behavior problems reduce academic outcomes among primary students: a moderated mediation of parental burnout and parents’ self-compassion. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2022, 27–42. doi: 10.1002/cad.20482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, T. K. A., Slack, K. S., and Berger, L. M. (2017). Adverse childhood experiences and behavioral problems in middle childhood. Child Abuse Negl. 67, 391–402. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Józsa, K., and Barrett, K. C. (2018). Affective and social mastery motivation in preschool as predictors of early school success: a longitudinal study. Early Child. Res. Q. 45, 81–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Junge, C., Garthus-Niegel, S., Slinning, K., Polte, C., Simonsen, T. B., and Eberhard-Gran, M. (2017). The impact of perinatal depression on children’s social-emotional development: a longitudinal study. Matern. Child Health J. 21, 607–615. doi: 10.1007/s10995-016-2146-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, M. L., Fanning, K. A., Huynh, T., Botto, I., and Kim, C. N. (2021). Parents’ self-reported psychological impacts of COVID-19: associations with parental burnout, child behavior, and income. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 46, 1162–1171. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsab089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koerber, M. I., Mack, J. T., Seefeld, L., Kopp, M., Weise, V., Starke, K. R., et al. (2023). Psychosocial work stress and parent-child bonding during the COVID-19 pandemic: clarifying the role of parental symptoms of depression and aggressiveness. BMC Public Health 23:113. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-14759-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laible, D. J., and Thompson, R. A. (2000). Mother-child discourse, attachment security, shared positive affect, and early conscience development. Child Dev. 71, 1424–1440. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebert-Charron, A., Dorard, G., Boujut, E., and Wendland, J. (2018). Maternal burnout syndrome: contextual and psychological associated factors. Front. Psychol. 9:885. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, Z., Li, Y., and Su, Y. (2014). Emotion understanding and reconciliation in overt and relational conflict scenarios among preschoolers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 38, 111–117. doi: 10.1177/0165025413512064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, G.-X., Goldenberg, A., Arikan, G., Brytek-Matera, A., Czepczor-Bernat, K., Manrique-Millones, D., et al. (2022a). Reappraisal, social support, and parental burnout. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 61, 1089–1102. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, G.-X., Szczygieł, D., and Piotrowski, K. (2022b). Child-oriented perfectionism and parental burnout: the moderating role of parents’ emotional intelligence. Personal. Individ. Differ. 198:111805. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindström, C., Aman, J., and Norberg, A. L. (2011). Parental burnout in relation to sociodemographic, psychosocial and personality factors as well as disease duration and glycaemic control in children with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Acta Paediatr 100, 1011–1017. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02198.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livings, M. S. (2021). The gendered relationship between maternal depression and adolescent internalizing symptoms. Soc. Sci. Med. 291:114464. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinsen, K. D., Rasmussen, L. M. P., Wentzel-Larsen, T., Holen, S., Sund, A. M., Løvaas, M. E. S., et al. (2019). Prevention of anxiety and depression in school children: effectiveness of the transdiagnostic EMOTION program. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 87, 212–219. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikolajczak, M., Gross, J. J., and Roskam, I. (2019). Parental burnout: what is it, and why does it matter? Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7, 1319–1329. doi: 10.1177/2167702619858430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikolajczak, M., Gross, J. J., Stinglhamber, F., Lindahl Norberg, A., and Roskam, I. (2020). Is parental burnout distinct from job burnout and depressive symptoms? Clin. Psychol. Sci. 8, 673–689. doi: 10.1177/2167702620917447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L., Halligan, S. L., Adams, G., Patterson, P., and Goodyer, I. M. (2006). Socioemotional development in adolescents at risk for depression: the role of maternal depression and attachment style. Dev. Psychopathol. 18, 489–516. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nonnenmacher, N., Müller, M., Taczkowski, J., Zietlow, A. L., Sodian, B., and Reck, C. (2021). Theory of mind in pre-school aged children: influence of maternal depression and infants’ self-comforting behavior. Front. Psychol. 12:741786. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.741786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pons, F., and Harris, P. L. (2000) Test of emotion comprehension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Pons, F., Harris, P. L., and de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: developmental periods and hierarchical organization. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 1, 127–152. doi: 10.1080/17405620344000022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Psychogiou, L., Nath, S., Kallitsoglou, A., Dimatis, K., Parry, E., Russell, A. E., et al. (2018). Children’s emotion understanding in relation to attachment to mother and father. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 36, 557–572. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, A., Obst, S., Teague, S. J., Rossen, L., Spry, E. A., Macdonald, J. A., et al. (2020). Association between maternal perinatal depression and anxiety and child and adolescent development: a meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 174, 1082–1092. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.2910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roskam, I., Aguiar, J., Akgun, E., Arikan, G., Artavia, M., Avalosse, H., et al. (2021). Parental burnout around the globe: a 42-country study. Affect. Sci. 2, 58–79. doi: 10.1007/s42761-020-00028-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roskam, I., Brianda, M.-E., and Mikolajczak, M. (2018). A step forward in the conceptualization and measurement of parental burnout: the parental burnout assessment (PBA). Front. Psychol. 9:758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roskam, I., Raes, M.-E., and Mikolajczak, M. (2017). Exhausted parents: development and preliminary validation of the parental burnout inventory. Front. Psychol. 8:163. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Rodríguez, R., Orsini, É., Laflaquière, E., Callahan, S., and Séjourné, N. (2019). Depression, anxiety, and guilt in mothers with burnout of preschool and school-aged children: insight from a cluster analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 259, 244–250. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starchenkova, E. S. (2019) ‘Social-psychological factors of parental burnout’ Psychology of Stress and Coping Behavior: Challenges, Resources, Well-Being. Kostroma, Russia, September 26–28, 2019Kostroma: Kostroma State University 109–113. (in Russian)

Google Scholar

Urizar, G. G., and Muñoz, R. F. (2022). Role of maternal depression on child development: a prospective analysis from pregnancy to early childhood. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 53, 502–514. doi: 10.1007/s10578-021-01138-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veraksa, N., Veraksa, A., Gavrilova, M., Bukhalenkova, D., and Tarasova, K. (2021). The Russian version of the test of emotion comprehension: adaptation and validation for use in preschool children. Psychol. J. High. Sch. Econ. 18, 56–70. doi: 10.17323/1813-8918-2021-1-56-70, (in Russian).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Chen, Y., Zhang, S., and Rozelle, S. (2022). Paths of social-emotional development before 3 years old and child development after 5 years old: evidence from rural China. Early Hum. Dev. 165:105539. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2022.105539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wauters, A., Vervoort, T., Dhondt, K., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Morbée, S., et al. (2022). Mental health outcomes among parents of children with a chronic disease during the COVID-19 pandemic: the role of parental burn-out. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 47, 420–431. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsab129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yakupova, V. A. (2018). The impact of psychological and physiological conditions of motherhood on postnatal depression. Russ. Psychol. J. 15, 8–23. doi: 10.21702/rpj.2018.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, B., Chen, B. B., Qu, Y., and Zhu, Y. (2021). Impacts of parental burnout on Chinese Youth’s mental health: the role of parents autonomy support and emotion regulation. J Youth Adolesc 50, 1679–1692. doi: 10.1007/s10964-021-01450-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, Y., Wang, W., Song, T., and Li, Y. (2022). The mechanisms of parental burnout affecting adolescents problem behavior. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:15139. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192215139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakharova, E. I. (2012). Individual notions of social role characteristics as means of orientation. Cult.-Histor. Psychol. 8, 38–41. (in Russian).

Google Scholar

Zaporozhets, A. V., Neverovich, Ya. Z., Kosheleva, A. D., Strelkova, L. P., and Abramyan, L. A. (1985). Emotional development of a preschooler: a guide for kindergarten teachers. Moscow: Education. (in Russian).

Google Scholar

Keywords: parental burnout, depression, maternal depression, parenthood, emotion comprehension, emotional development

Citation: Yakupova V and Suarez A (2023) Parental burnout, depression and emotional development of the preschoolers. Front. Psychol. 14:1207569. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1207569

Received: 17 April 2023; Accepted: 05 June 2023;
Published: 20 June 2023.

Edited by:

Yulia Solovieva, Meritorious Autonomous University of Puebla, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Ana Baltazar, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico
María Alejandra Morales González, Instituto de Neuropsicología y Psicopedagogía de Puebla, A.C., Mexico
Daniel Rosas, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Copyright © 2023 Yakupova and Suarez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vera Yakupova, vera.a.romanova@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.