Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 28 September 2023
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

When guilt works: a comprehensive meta-analysis of guilt appeals

\r\nWei Peng
Wei Peng1*Qian Huang
Qian Huang2*Bingjing MaoBingjing Mao3Di LunDi Lun4Ekaterina MalovaEkaterina Malova5Jazmyne V. SimmonsJazmyne V. Simmons6Nick CarcioppoloNick Carcioppolo7
  • 1Edward R. Murrow College of Communication, Washington State University, Pullman, WA, United States
  • 2Department of Communication, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks, ND, United States
  • 3TSET Health Promotion and Research Center, Stephenson Cancer Center, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma, OK, United States
  • 4Department of Health Disparities Research, University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, TX, United States
  • 5Simon Business School, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, United States
  • 6Division of Health Science, School of Allied Health Sciences, Florida Agricultural and Mechanical University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
  • 7School of Communication, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States

Introduction: Guilt appeals are widely used as a persuasive approach in various areas of practice. However, the strength and direction of the persuasive effects of guilt appeals are mixed, which could be influenced by theoretical and methodological factors.

Method: The present study is a comprehensive meta-analysis of 26 studies using a random-effects model to assess the persuasive effects of guilt appeals. In total, 127 effect sizes from seven types of persuasive outcomes (i.e., guilt, attitude, behavior, behavioral intention, non-guilt emotions, motivation, and cognition) were calculated based on 7,512 participants.

Results: The analysis showed a small effect size of guilt appeals [g = 0.19, 95% CI (0.10, 0.28)]. The effect of guilt appeals was moderated by the theoretical factors related to appraisal and coping of guilt arousal, including attributed responsibility, controllability and stability of the causal factors, the proximity of perceiver-victim relationship, recommendation of reparative behaviors, and different outcome types. The effect was also associated with methods used in different studies.

Discussion: Overall, the findings demonstrated the persuasive effects of guilt appeals, but theoretical and methodological factors should be considered in the design and testing of guilt appeals. We also discussed the practical implications of the findings.

Introduction

Guilt appeals are a type of persuasive communication that evokes feelings of guilt to influence persuasive outcomes (O'Keefe, 2000). In essence, guilt appeals arouse guilt by activating a self-evaluative process through which the perceiver finds their behavior deviating from their moral and social standards (Tangney et al., 2007; Greenbaum et al., 2020). Aroused guilt elicits reparative behaviors toward the victim, such as apologies, compensation, and cooperative behaviors, to undo the harm and reduce conflicts (de Hooge et al., 2011; Chrdileli and Kasser, 2018). For this reason, guilt has been used to promote compliance in the forms of advertisements, health or educational materials, electronic information, and interpersonal conversations (Antonetti et al., 2018; Xu and Guo, 2018).

Empirical research testing the effects of guilt appeals has shown mixed results (Shehu et al., 2013; Noble et al., 2014; Antonetti et al., 2018). The inconsistent findings draw attention to several prominent issues in guilt appeal research. First, guilt induction involves multiple processes, meaning that the antecedents of guilt arousal may vary across different studies (Tangney et al., 2007; Greenbaum et al., 2020). One factor is the attribution of the cause of other's suffering (Tracy and Robins, 2006). Previous studies elicited guilt by focusing on the victim's suffering only (Chang, 2012; Lee, 2013) or by additionally attributing the responsibility to the perceiver (Antonetti et al., 2018). In addition, the relational distance between the perceiver and the victim could affect guilty feelings (Ghorbani et al., 2013). Second, guilt appeals seek to motivate reparative behaviors, but including such information often makes the perceiver feel coerced, resulting in weaker effectiveness (Graton et al., 2016). Third, a survey of studies testing guilt appeals has found a mixture of research methods and interests that vary in topic contexts, induction approaches, and methods of measurement.

Overall, distinct theoretical and methodological factors from past studies increase the uncertainty of guilt appeal research, calling for meta-analytical studies. Previous meta-analyses have demonstrated the effects of guilt appeals (O'Keefe, 2000; Boster et al., 2016; Xu and Guo, 2018; Turner and Rains, 2021) but had several limitations. Not all meta-analyses systematically accounted for various processes involved in guilt appeals under a consistent theoretical framework. Abbreviated investigations of non-theoretical moderators have been conducted so far. As a result, the variations of guilt induction and coping were not examined. Moreover, heterogeneity in methodological approaches, including but not limited to contexts, induction methods, and measurement in extant studies on guilt appeals, had not been fully evaluated.

Therefore, the present meta-analysis aimed to achieve three goals. First, this study sampled a larger body of guilt appeal studies to investigate the overall effect size. Second, the study centered on the emotional mechanism of guilt appeals to evaluate relevant theoretical moderators that had not been examined before. Third, it investigated the moderating effects of methodological differences. The following section is organized into two parts. The first part examines the theoretical framework and mechanism of guilt appeals, which guides the review of theoretical moderators related to the processes of guilt induction and coping. The second part is a review of key methodological moderators—contexts, induction methods, narratives, modalities, and time points of measurement. A conceptual model of the present meta-analysis is available in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The conceptual model of the meta-analysis of guilt appeals with theoretical and methodological moderators.

Theoretical framework and moderators of guilt appeals

Guilt appeals arouse guilt as a discrete emotion to persuade (O'Keefe, 2000). There is substantial agreement that human emotions are adaptive responses of an organism to stimuli in the external environment (Mulligan and Scherer, 2012; Roseman, 2013). Each discrete emotion is aroused by a unique pattern of appraisals that evaluates the external stimulus with regard to different dimensions, such as motivational relevance, goal congruence, expectedness, control, and agency (Smith and Ellsworth, 1985; Moors et al., 2013; Moors, 2020). The perceiver also appraises the coping ability to reduce the demands from emotions arising from this event (Roseman, 2013; Scherer and Moors, 2019). This coping appraisal prompts mental and behavioral strategies to meet the needs of the personal goal and regulate the aroused emotions (Roseman, 2013; Scherer and Moors, 2019). Because the adaptive responses of emotions can change motivational priorities to address the causal factors, theorists reason that emotions can cause biased estimation of the possibility and desirability of an object, leading to changes in decision-making processes (Nabi, 2003; DeSteno et al., 2004; So et al., 2015).

Based on the appraisal theories, guilt appeals should be designed with two pivotal components: guilt induction and coping (Nabi, 1999; O'Keefe, 2002). In the first step, guilt appeals induce guilt in the perceiver by guiding their cognitive appraisals to evaluate specific dimensions of the stimulus event (O'Keefe, 2002). Once feeling guilty, a perceiver evaluates and adopts possible strategies to control the emotion (Dearing et al., 2005; de Hooge et al., 2011; Behrendt and Ben-Ari, 2012). Thus, in the second step, guilt appeals can recommend specific actions of the perceiver to regulate guilty feelings (O'Keefe, 2002). Based on the framework of discrete emotions, variations in theoretical factors related to guilt induction and coping can influence the effects of guilt appeals.

Guilt induction as theoretical moderators

Guilt induction is the first component of guilt appeals. Theoretical models conceptualize guilt as a self-conscious emotion elicited by the inconsistency between one's self-representation and moral or social standards (Tracy and Robins, 2006; Tangney et al., 2007). Guilt is associated with complex concepts of self and socialization needs, such as maintaining a friendship, as opposed to survival instincts (de Hooge et al., 2007; Cryder et al., 2012). We review several key factors of guilt induction, including attribution dimensions associated with responsibility, controllability, stability, and the proximity of the perceiver–victim relationship (Tracy and Robins, 2006; Tangney et al., 2007; Ghorbani et al., 2013).

Attribution

Attribution is an essential part of the appraisal in guilt elicitation (Weiner et al., 1982). In the attribution process, the perceiver evaluates the cause of the victim's suffering presented in guilt appeals regarding its responsibility, controllability, and stability (Tracy and Robins, 2004). From the perspective of the persuader, how the cause is presented and perceived concerning these three attribution dimensions is critical to eliciting guilt. The first dimension of attribution is responsibility. Emotion research shows that guilt arises from the sense of responsibility for inflicting harm on others (Tangney et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2014; Miceli and Castelfranchi, 2018). Responsibility plays an essential role in the social and moral function of guilt as it motivates people to acknowledge their mistakes and take reparative actions in the aftermath of transgressions (Tangney et al., 2007). To activate the appraisal of responsibility, guilt appeals blame the perceiver as the cause of others' suffering in a specific social or moral context (Harth et al., 2013; Antonetti et al., 2018).

However, responsibility is not always a necessary condition of guilt arousal. Existential guilt is associated with a person's broader perception of their meaning and expectations in a human society (Binder, 2022). This type of guilt is often evoked by moral responsibility or social obligation, which does not result from an actual transgression one commits but from one's perceived failure to fulfill their meaning and duties in society (Zimmermann et al., 2011). Thus, existential guilt extends beyond mere compensation and punishment as described by Tangney et al. (2007) and could motivate people to engage in broader issues. Others contend that existential guilt is possibly driven by empathy, which links one with others in a social relation, to reduce others' suffering (Baumeister et al., 1994; Roberts et al., 2014). Thus, people would feel guilty about inequality impacting others, even though they did not cause its occurrence (O'Connor et al., 2000; Harth et al., 2013; Dull et al., 2021). For example, guilty feelings are experienced by the survivors of natural disasters (Hugelius et al., 2017; Dang et al., 2022), workplace layoffs (Karfakis and Kokkinidis, 2018; Mujtaba and Senathip, 2020), or cancer (Perloff et al., 2019). For this reason, without assigning responsibility, guilt appeals can still be persuasive by evoking the perceivers' role in a social system and their behaviors to address broad social issues (e.g., climate change, equality, and poverty) (Chang, 2011).

The other two attributional dimensions are stability and controllability (Tangney et al., 2007). The stability dimension is crucial to distinguish guilt from other moral emotions, such as shame (Tracy and Robins, 2004). Compared with shame, guilt arises from one's attention to the unstable aspect of the self, such as behaviors, as opposed to one's personality, character, and other stable aspects (Greenbaum et al., 2020). To influence the appraisal of stability, guilt appeals provide evaluation information showing that the nature of a causal event is changeable (Xu and Guo, 2018). The attribution to unstable causes is often achieved by focusing on a specific incident or behavior of the perceiver as the cause of the victim's suffering (Roberts et al., 2014).

Moreover, to influence the appraisal of controllability, guilt appeals attribute the victim's suffering to a cause over which the perceiver has control (Antonetti and Maklan, 2014). As a result, the perceiver can develop a stronger sense of agency over the process and consequences of the causal event (Roberts et al., 2014). A theoretical review explains that the action taken by the self is an integral part of morality, which is linked to guilt as a moral emotion (Jennings et al., 2015), that is, in addition to the person's identity and environment, the actions a person takes to influence society and self-shape the characteristics of their morality (Jennings et al., 2015). On the other hand, controllability should be distinguished from stability attribution in the appraisal of guilt (Tracy and Robins, 2004). While a cause is attributed to be unstable, a person might not feel guilty if the locus of control is external (Antonetti and Maklan, 2014). Thus, the effects of guilt appeals might depend on whether the guilt appeals attribute a negative event to a cause that is controlled by the perceiver.

The proximity of perceiver–victim relationships

The elicitation of guilt can vary by the proximity of the relationships between the perceiver and the victim (Baumeister et al., 1994). Transgressors usually have a stronger feeling of guilt when they are interpersonally close to the victims, such as friends or family (Ghorbani et al., 2013). Because guilt involves one's self-representation in a moral or social context, people can more easily appraise the effects of their misdeeds on others and empathetically feel their suffering if there is a close connection (Tangney et al., 2007; Ruckstaetter et al., 2017). In addition, transgressors can be motivated to reconcile their transgressions due to the concern about alienating those in close relationships (Ghorbani et al., 2013).

Additionally, a personal relationship is not always required for guilt induction. Guilt can be elicited by the wrongdoings committed by in-group members onto others (Iyer et al., 2007; Zimmermann et al., 2011). For example, people felt guilty about their country's actions toward disadvantaged groups in the Dutch colonization of Indonesia (Doosje et al., 1998) and the Israeli–Palestinian conflicts (Roccas et al., 2006). In response to the finding, Doosje et al. (1998) explained that transgressions committed by in-group members might threaten the perceiver's social identity and arouse collective guilt. In addition, individuals may develop a sense of community toward workplaces or other institutions (Nowell and Boyd, 2014). Guilty feelings might result from the perception that one had not responded to the misfortune of their community (Zimmermann et al., 2011).

Thus, we seek to understand whether the proximity of the relationship between a perceiver and a depicted victim moderates the effectiveness of guilt appeals. When appraising a negative event, a perceiver may react differently after assessing whether the victim is in a close relationship. Hence, the proximity of the perceiver–victim relationship will be tested as a theoretical moderator.

Guilt coping as a theoretical moderator

There is a clear consensus among emotion theories that negative human emotions must be regulated by changing the causal event directly or releasing the mental state from its influence (Lazarus, 1991; Ellsworth and Scherer, 2003; Roseman, 2013). Once feeling guilty, the perceiver seeks and evaluates possible strategies to cope with the emotion (Tracy and Robins, 2006; Tangney et al., 2007). In this process, following the guilt induction process, guilt appeals provide the perceiver with a certain action or counterfactual thinking to remedy guilt-arousing causes (O'Keefe, 2002; Antonetti et al., 2015). Thus, the meta-analysis will test the following two moderators to understand how the effects of guilt appeals are associated with the regulation of aroused guilt.

Recommendation of reparative behaviors

Guilt appeals in various contexts provide immediately accessible information to help the perceiver take measures directly against the source of the negative feelings (Agrawal and Duhachek, 2010; Antonetti et al., 2015). Extensive research has shown that guilt motivates reparative behaviors to amend wrongdoings and undo the harm to others (Ketelaar and Tung Au, 2003; Tangney et al., 2007; Shen, 2018; Vaish, 2018). The reparation process helps transgressors control their guilty feelings (Tangney et al., 2007). Confession, apology, cooperative, and prosocial behaviors are among the most common reparative behaviors in social interactions (Tangney et al., 2007; de Hooge et al., 2011; Behrendt and Ben-Ari, 2012). For example, Ilies et al. (2013) found that in the workplace, employees who felt guilty about their counterproductive work behaviors were more likely to offer helping behaviors. In interpersonal conflicts, guilty feelings prompt people to engage in cooperative approaches (Behrendt and Ben-Ari, 2012). Other studies suggest that apologies were related to guilt, but not to shame (Ruckstaetter et al., 2017; Chrdileli and Kasser, 2018).

For this reason, guilt appeals might be more persuasive if recommending reparative actions can immediately satisfy the perceiver's need to make amends (Graton and Mailliez, 2019). In addition, guilt appeals with reparation suggestions convey the repairable nature of the events for which people feel guilty (Tangney et al., 2007), that is, the negative consequences of someone's wrongdoings can be counteracted by the recommended actions in guilt appeals (Graton et al., 2016). If no actions or remedies are provided in guilt appeals, the perceiver might feel that the negative consequences cannot be offset, and their recovery from guilt is inhibited (LeBlanc et al., 2020). In turn, people would seek to deal with the negative feelings directly without changing the cause (Antonetti and Baines, 2015). As a result, guilt appeals may be avoided or rejected (Graton et al., 2016).

However, explicit information about reparative behaviors does not necessarily strengthen the effectiveness of guilt appeals. In a series of experiments, Graton and Ric (2017) found that guilty feelings have increased attention to and positive attitudes toward implicit cues of making amends. Additional demands for reparative behaviors, such as asking for charitable donations, could make people feel coerced. These findings bring uncertainty into whether recommending reparative behaviors in guilt appeals can influence persuasive effectiveness.

Outcomes as a theoretical moderator

The process of guilt appeals may vary outcomes, resulting in variations in the effect size of guilt appeals. The first outcome is guilt, which often serves as a validity indicator of guilt appeals (O'Keefe, 2000). Guilt is a mediating process of guilt appeals before more distal persuasion outcomes. Through the elicitation of guilt, guilt appeals engender persuasion resulting from the perceiver's motivation to control the guilty feelings (Tangney et al., 2007; Allard and White, 2015). Guilt elicited by guilt appeals is thus expected to be consistent and strong. However, if the stimulus event framed in a guilt appeal is relatively more important to the perceiver's goals other than maintaining the social or moral dimension of self-identity, more ambiguous or other emotions may arise, reducing the intensity of guilty feelings (Tracy and Robins, 2006; Tangney et al., 2007).

Attitude is another essential outcome of guilt appeals. Attitude is generated from an appraisal process in which the perceiver evaluates the attributes of the persuasion object (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975). While early theoretical work primarily focused on cognitive antecedents of attitudinal assessments, more recent studies show that emotional information is an essential dimension of the evaluations for attitude (Fishbein and Middlestadt, 1995; Zanna and Rempel, 2008). Guilt appeals influence attitudes by providing information that can cause biased estimation of the value and probability of specific attributes associated with a persuasion object (DeSteno et al., 2004). Variations of attitude depend on whether certain attributes of the object are appraised as potentially helpful to cope with the aroused guilty feelings and attain one's goal related to social and moral issues (Allen et al., 2005; Antonetti and Baines, 2015). The desirability of the persuasion object can also be lower if the guilt appeals elicit other emotions or conflict with incidental emotions (DeSteno et al., 2004).

Behaviors and behavioral intentions are considered focal outcomes of guilt appeals. Behavior is at the center of guilt experiences. Guilt is elicited by specific wrongdoings and the regulation process is often achieved by reparative behaviors (Tangney et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2014). As an emotion regulation mechanism, behavioral actions are taken by individuals to address the problems that cause emotions (Scherer and Moors, 2019). In this sense, guilt appeals suggest specific behavioral actions that can undo the harm to the victim and reduce guilty feelings (Tangney et al., 2007; Antonetti and Baines, 2015). Thus, guilt appeals are often used in persuasion to gain behavioral compliance (Agrawal and Duhachek, 2010; Chang, 2012). If behavioral outcomes provide little help to control negative feelings, maladaptive guilt experiences can result in rejection or avoidance of the entire message and recommended behaviors (Kim et al., 2011; Antonetti and Baines, 2015).

Lastly, guilt is expected to change motivation and cognition (Bradley et al., 2001; Pessoa, 2009). Aroused guilt disrupts thoughts and directs cognitive resources to fix eliciting events and control aversive feelings (Tangney et al., 2007; Scherer and Moors, 2019). Motivation is an intermediate process between emotional arousal and cognitive or behavioral responses. Emotion is aroused when a person's motivation-related goal is satisfied or at risk (Roseman, 2013). In response, motivation prioritizes specific cognitive resources (e.g., attention) and executive functions (e.g., behaviors) to act toward the goal (Pessoa, 2009). Thus, guilt can motivate a person to engage in reparation behaviors that help maintain the goal related to one's self-representation or interpersonal relationships (Tangney et al., 2007; Greenbaum et al., 2020).

Methodological moderators of guilt appeals

Methodological differences should be assessed to understand the varying effects of guilt appeals. The effects of guilt appeals can be influenced by different contexts (Massi Lindsey, 2005; Chang, 2012), induction methods (e.g., Allard and White, 2015; La Ferle et al., 2019), or outcome variables (Noble et al., 2014). Below, we review methodological factors related to the effectiveness of guilt appeals.

Contexts of persuasion objects

Different contexts of persuasion objects can account for variations in effects in that each context may result in different appraisal patterns (e.g., causal attribution, reparative behaviors, and personal relevance). Commonly seen in advertising and marketing, guilt appeals are used to promote a product or service by provoking the perceiver's examination of self-representation against a person's goal and claiming how the persuasion object (e.g., a product) helps them reach the goal (Antonetti and Baines, 2015). Guilt appeals can also change self-efficacy and behaviors in health and medical contexts (Xu and Guo, 2018). Guilt appeals in a health-related context seek to portray certain health behaviors as wrongdoing that violates socially acceptable rules and generates negative impacts on people in social relationships, such as the harmful effects of human papillomavirus infection on a sexual partner (Babin, 2009). Another important context is social issues with broad impacts, such as environmental protection (Baek and Yoon, 2017; Schneider et al., 2017) and charity giving (Chang, 2011; Xu, 2022). Guilt feelings arise when people consider themselves not fulfilling their moral and social responsibility and these causes (Zimmermann et al., 2011; Urbonavicius et al., 2019). Guilt appeals in this context motivate prosocial and altruistic behaviors to alleviate guilty feelings (de Hooge et al., 2011; Urbonavicius et al., 2019).

Induction methods

Two methods of guilt induction commonly used in previous research could influence the effects of guilt appeals. One method is based on persuasive messages, which provide a brief piece of information that matches the core appraisal theme of guilt to arouse guilty feelings in the perceiver (Nabi, 1999, 2003). For instance, a guilt appeal message portrays the victim's suffering and frames the perceiver's controllable behavior as the cause. The other method uses recall and imagination, commonly through verbal instructions in interpersonal communications (Tracy and Robins, 2006). The perceiver is asked by the persuader to recall a salient incident in their lives that once aroused guilt and then to re-experience it in the current setting (Allard and White, 2015; Graton et al., 2016).

The influence of induction methods on guilt appeals might center on the relevance of information. According to the self-reference effect, information about oneself is clearly organized in memory and easily accessible (Block, 2005). Despite its popularity, message-based guilt appeals can be perceived as less relevant to the perceiver when describing the suffering of an unknown party (Ghorbani et al., 2013). As a result, the effects of guilt appeals may be hampered (Chang, 2012; Ghorbani et al., 2013). In this sense, recall might be advantageous to elicit self-conscious emotions like guilt (Tracy and Robins, 2006; Siedlecka and Denson, 2019). Through recall, the perceiver can reproduce the emotional experience and examine whether certain aspects of themselves have caused negative impacts on others (Tracy and Robins, 2006). Thus, this study will test whether induction methods moderate the effects of guilt appeals.

Narrative

A growing body of literature points to emotional experiences as the foundation of narrative persuasion (Green and Brock, 2000; Nabi and Green, 2015; Liu and Yang, 2020). Using narrative in guilt appeals has also been tested in the literature. Past studies have narratives to evoke feelings of guilt by providing the victim's stories or by engaging the perceiver in summoning personal experiences (Graton et al., 2016; Antonetti et al., 2018, study 1b). In contrast, non-narrative guilt appeals typically use reasoning, logical arguments, and statistical information (Chang, 2012; Antonetti et al., 2018, study 1a). However, the overall effects of narrative vs. non-narrative persuasion are mixed (Shen et al., 2015; Freling et al., 2020). We seek to test whether using narratives in guilt appeals is associated with stronger persuasiveness.

Modalities

Guilt appeals can benefit from different modalities (e.g., text, audio, video, or mixed modes), but the effects may vary. According to multimedia learning theory (Mayer and Moreno, 2003; Mayer, 2014), information in multiple modalities increases attention to essential content, adds visual cues to exemplify the central theme, and reduces the cognitive burden through redundant information. Visual content may build mental experiences through which a person can experience the if-then scenario in guilt arousal (Chien and Chang, 2012; Antonetti et al., 2015). Thus, it is reasonable to argue that these benefits of multimedia information could support the processing of guilt appeals to achieve the intended effects. However, multimedia information with a poor design might also distract a perceiver from scrutinizing the claims in guilt appeals (Mayer, 2014). For example, irrelevant visual information might cause incidental processing and unintended effects (Sung and Mayer, 2012). Given limited evidence to inform the most effective modality for guilt appeal, the present meta-analysis tests whether the effectiveness of guilt appeals varies by different modalities.

Time points of measurement

The effects of guilt appeals may depend on different time points of measurement after persuasion occurs. Most existing studies measured the effects of guilt appeals immediately after exposure (Peloza et al., 2013; Graton et al., 2016) because emotional responses are often transient (Brosch, 2021; Ogunfowora et al., 2023). Nevertheless, recent research suggests that immediate measurement might not capture the desired effects of guilt appeals as people might delay their decisions to adopt the suggested behavior in persuasion (Antonetti et al., 2018; Brosch, 2021; Kemp, 2023). Thus, the present meta-analysis will understand how different measurement time points can cause variation in the observed persuasive effects of guilt appeals.

The present meta-analysis

The persuasive effects of guilt appeals have been synthesized by past meta-analyses, but several gaps remain to be addressed. Both O'Keefe (2000, p. 74) and Boster et al. (2016) included guilt appeal studies based on the transgression-compliance mechanism and found medium effect sizes (rs = 0.28 and 0.26, respectively). However, both studies presumed based on a priori knowledge that guilt would drive behavioral compliance of transgressors. Thus, their findings should not be interpretable as the effects of guilt appeal because transgressors' behavioral compliance is not necessarily explained by guilt (Cialdini et al., 1973; Baumeister et al., 1994). The other three meta-analyses studied guilt appeals that intentionally aroused guilt emotion for persuasive outcomes but suffered from multiple limitations. First, O'Keefe (2000, p. 80) compared five guilt appeal studies with varying explicitness and found that more explicit guilt appeals were associated with less persuasive effects (r = −0.26). However, this meta-analysis did not explicate the processes related to the intensity of guilt induction. Second, Xu and Guo (2018) demonstrated moderate effects of guilt-based persuasive messages but their findings were limited to health-related outcomes. In addition, several included studies manipulated persuasive constructs that were not intended to evoke guilt (e.g., norm perception, Lee and Paek, 2013). Furthermore, their analysis only examined a limited number of moderators, including message modality, self/other focus, and health behaviors. In a more recent meta-analysis, Turner and Rains (2021) found a small effect size of guilt appeals on attitudes or behavioral intentions and medium effects on guilt and anger. However, similar to previous meta-analyses, moderators associated with the appraisal dimensions of guilt arousal, coping process, and outcomes were not of primary interest. Their study did not account for heterogeneity in outcomes of guilt appeals.

The present study hence aimed to conduct a comprehensive synthesis of guilt appeals. There are four strengths of this study. First, we examined the studies that intentionally evoked guilt as an emotional mechanism of persuasion. Persuasion studies that manipulated the constructs not intended to evoke guilt were excluded. Thus, this meta-analysis provided a more robust and accurate estimate of the effectiveness of guilt appeals. Second, guided by appraisal theories of emotion, we systematically accounted for key factors associated with guilt induction and coping. The absence of such inquiries in previous studies could contribute to inconsistent results (O'Keefe, 2002; Tangney and Dearing, 2002). Thus, examining these factors would offer important insights into variations in guilt arousal and persuasive outcomes. Third, previous meta-analyses primarily assessed the effects on attitude and behaviors. In contrast, our study evaluated additional dependent variables to assess the overall effects of guilt appeals thoroughly. Fourth, we evaluated various methodological characteristics of past guilt appeal studies. As the body of guilt appeals expands, this analytic approach can critically address the limitation of previous meta-analyses in which methodological differences were not of focus.

The first goal of this meta-analysis is to understand the overall effectiveness of guilt appeals. To this end, we proposed the following research question.

RQ1: Compared with equivalent neutral true control conditions, do guilt appeals generate greater persuasive effects?

Also, as reviewed, theoretical factors involved in guilt induction and coping could influence the effectiveness of guilt appeals. The following hypotheses were proposed to test whether these factors moderated the effects of guilt appeals.

H1: The effects of guilt appeals are moderated by the presence of attribution dimensions related to (a) responsibility, (b) controllability, or (c) stability in guilt induction.

H2: The effects of guilt appeals are moderated by the proximity of the relationship between the perceiver and the victim in guilt induction.

H3: The effects of guilt appeals are moderated by the recommendation of reparative behaviors to cope with aroused guilt.

H4: The effects of guilt appeals are moderated by different types of outcomes.

Across a large body of past research, various method approaches to guilt appeals may influence the effectiveness of guilt appeals. The following research question is proposed to understand which methodological factors moderate the effects of guilt appeals.

RQ2: Are the effects of guilt appeals moderated by (a) context of persuasion objects, (b) induction methods, (c) narrative, (d) modalities, and (e) time point of measurement?

Methods

Search process

We used the Population, Intervention, Comparator, Outcomes, and Study (PICOS) design model in accordance with the PRISMA guideline (Moher et al., 2009) to develop the search strategy. The details of our PICOS criteria are provided in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The PICOS (populations, interventions, comparators, outcomes, study design) inclusion criteria and initial article screening results.

An initial literature search was conducted in 2020 through the following databases: PsycINFO, ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global, ERIC, MEDLINE, and Communication and Mass Media Complete via EBSCO. The search terms “Guilt*” AND [“appeal” OR “campaign” OR “adverti*” OR “message” OR “intervention”] were used. Publicly available doctoral and master's degree theses were also retrieved. A total of 1,240 citations were exported to a citation manager. The initial sample included studies published from 1929 to 2020. To avoid file drawer issues, authors and experts in the field were consulted using the listserve “Communication Research and Theory Network's (CRTNET)” to solicit unpublished studies in their personal libraries, and responses were accepted throughout 2020. This procedure yielded no additional articles. After removing duplicates (n = 72), 1,168 articles remained in the initial screening phase.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Figure 2 provides a PRISMA flowchart indicating the overall inclusion and exclusion procedure. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were developed and verified based on PICOS. The screening results are summarized in Table 1. In the first round, two coders screened the abstracts of 1,168 articles (n = 1,168) and excluded (a) five articles (n = 5) not written in English (n = 5); (b) the comparator criterion: 1,014 articles reported studies that compared the effects of guilt with other emotions, mixed emotions, or variants of guilt without a true control condition (n = 1,014); (c) the outcomes criterion: four articles reported studies that measured guilt as the only outcome variable (n = 4); and (d) the study design criterion: 92 studies reporting non-quantitative studies only (n = 92). A total of 53 articles remained after the first round of screening.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PRISMA flow chart.

In the second round of screening, the coders further assessed the full text of the remaining articles based on the PICOS criteria and removed the following articles: (1) the population criterion: no articles were excluded (n = 0) due to no criterion placed on demographic characteristics; (2) the interventions criterion: (a) four articles reported no studies that manipulated guilt appeals as an independent variable (n = 4) and (b) seven articles reported studies that manipulated different levels or subtypes of guilt (e.g., strong or naturalistic guilt) (n = 7); (3) the comparators criterion: (a) no articles were excluded due to not reporting at least one guilt appeal condition and one emotion-neutral control condition (n = 0); (4) the outcomes criterion: one article with studies that only reported the outcomes based on indirect rating or observation results by non-participant judges (n = 1); and (5) the study design criterion: (a) one article reported studies that did not use a between-subject design with a guilt appeal and a neutral control condition (n = 1).

In addition, the articles that reported no usable statistical results for effect size calculation were excluded, including (a) 20 articles with studies that provided no statistical results used to calculate effect sizes (i.e., mean and standard deviation, F-test values, t-test values, Cohen's d, and Pearson's r values) (n = 20), and (b) two articles (n = 2) with studies that only reported omnibus F-tests for the comparison of multiple conditions (e.g., F-tests of guilt, shame, hope, and control conditions) without reporting mean and standard deviation of guilt and neutral control conditions. Studies reported only unstandardized coefficients from multiple regression were excluded because effect sizes computed from these coefficients would be partial and under the influence of other predictors in the multiple regression (Hunter and Schmidt, 1990). Thus, coefficients did not reflect the meaningful effects unless corresponding correlation coefficients were obtained (Peterson and Brown, 2005).

A final sample of 18 articles yielding 26 studies was included in further analyses. The references of all included articles are listed in Appendix C.

Coding procedure

Appendix A lists the coded variables and their operational definitions. In the present meta-analysis, we also coded demographic and descriptive information as situational moderators of the included studies (i.e., publication type, sampling frame, funding source, study location, and year of publication). The studies were coded independently by two coders. The coders were graduate students who had completed courses and conducted relevant research projects related to persuasion and emotions. Each coder was responsible for half of the studies. Seven studies (27% of all selected studies) were sampled to compute the intercoder reliability. Intercoder reliability was computed with Krippendorff (2004) alpha (α) for categorical variables and intraclass correlation (ICC) for continuous variables (Shrout and Fleiss, 1979). Intercoder reliability of coded variables ranged from 0.74 to 1.00 (α bar = 0.95, ICC = 0.87) within the category of agreement (Landis and Koch, 1977). Intercoder reliability was also assessed during the coding of the full sample (Lacy and Riffe, 1996; Neuendorf, 2017). All rating disagreements were resolved through discussion until 100% agreement was achieved for each variable.

Calculation of effect size

Appendix B explains how effect sizes were calculated. Because studies of relatively small sample sizes were included, Hedges' g was applied to indicate effect size, correcting for small sample sizes (Hedges, 1981; Hedges and Olkin, 1985). A positive Hedges' g indicated an increase of the effect on study outcomes was elicited by guilt appeals compared to an equivalent non-guilt or true control condition; and a negative Hedges' g indicated a decrease of the effect was elicited by guilt appeals compared to an equivalent non-guilt or true control condition. For studies where the guilt appeal was linked to a reduction in outcome variables for which a greater score indicated negative effects, the computed sign of the effect size was reversed, so all positive differences reflected an increase in the dependent variables.

Overall analysis and outliers

Outliers are frequently present in meta-analyses and can distort pooled effect estimates of final effects; therefore, outliers were detected and removed from analyses (Hedges and Olkin, 1985; Schmidt et al., 1993; Viechtbauer and Cheung, 2010). We determined an effect size as an outlier if its confidence interval did not overlap with the confidence interval of the pooled effects. Such an effect size was less likely to be part of the population of pooled effects by the present meta-analysis (Viechtbauer and Cheung, 2010). Using the R package “altmeta” (Lin and Chu, 2016), we identified two outliers (i.e., 1.87 and 2.21), which were from Allard and White (2015; Studies 2 and 5).

The removal of these outliers yielded 127 effect sizes from 26 studies that examined the effects of guilt appeals with multiple measurements in the current meta-analysis. For most studies included, we extracted more than one effect size. Considering potential interdependency among effect sizes from the same study, we conducted all moderation analyses using the Robust Variance Estimation (RVE; Gleser and Olkin, 2009; Hedges et al., 2010; Pustejovsky and Tipton, 2022) for adjusting for effect size tendencies while retaining appropriate Type I error rates. All analyses were performed in R (version 4.2.1). We used the “robumeta” package (Fisher and Tipton, 2015) to perform the overall and moderator analyses, including meta-regression modeling. All test statistics (i.e., Q values) were conducted using the “metafor” package (Viechtbauer, 2010). Pairwise comparisons were performed using the “ClubSandwich” package (Pustejovsky and Tipton, 2018, 2022; Pustejovsky, 2020) and “multicomp” package (Bretz et al., 2010). Appendix D of the Supplemental material lists the acronyms in this article.

Results

Description of studies

A total of 127 effect sizes from 26 studies (N = 7,512) were included in the meta-analysis. Sample sizes ranged from 61 to 1,054 (M = 288.90, SD = 261.22). Participants' mean age ranged from 19.34 to 54 years (M = 28.68, SD = 11.65).

Publication bias

The rank correlation test (Kendall's tau) of Begg and Mazumdar (1994) suggested a non-significant relationship between standard error and effect size (Kendall's tau = 0.08, p = 0.17), indicating no publication biases. In addition, Egger's regression (Sutton, 2009) test showed a non-significant relationship between standard error and effect size (z = 0.24, p = 0.81), which indicated no evidence for publication bias. The trim and fill method of Duval and Tweedie (2000) suggested that zero studies were missing on the right side of the funnel plot (SE = 6.43). The graphical evaluation of the funnel plot (Figure 3) shows a symmetrical inverted shape, indicating that the effects of the included studies were variable and scattered to both sides of the point estimate of effect. Thus, the appearance of the funnel plot suggests no missing studies, either due to publication bias or sampling errors. Rosenthal's (1979) fail-safe N was 6,035 at the observed significance level of 0.0001 and the perceived significance level of 0.05. We thus concluded that it is unlikely that this meta-analysis missed a substantial number of studies. Given these results, we concluded that potential publication biases were absent in extant literature examining the effects of guilt appeals. A funnel plot with trim and fill and Egger's regression is provided in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Funnel plot with trim and fill and Egger's regression.

Overall analysis

Excluding outliers, the overall effect size of guilt appeal produced on all types of outcomes was significant, Q(126) = 902.31, t(24.2) = 3.11, I-squared = 91%, tau-square = 0.24 (SE = 0.04), with g = 0.19, SE = 0.05, rho = 0.8, 95% CI (0.10, 0.28), p < 0.001. The results indicate the heterogeneity in true effect sizes of guilt appeal (Higgins et al., 2003). Thus, we used the random-effects model in the analysis. Providing supportive evidence to address RQ1, this finding suggested a small effect size of guilt appeals. In contrast, the overall effect size of guilt appeal, including outliers, was 0.24, p < 0.001, suggesting that having outliers in subsequent analyses would inflate the overall effect size. The post-hoc statistical power of the overall analysis was 80%. Thus, without additional studies, the sample size was large enough to detect the true effect sufficiently.

Analysis of moderators

All moderator analyses were conducted for studies with the removal of outlier estimations. We reported the results of individual moderators. In addition, a meta-regression model with theoretical moderators as the predictors was also specified to explain the effects of guilt appeals (see details in Tables 13).

Analysis of theoretical moderators

Responsibility attribution

Responsibility attribution moderated the effects of guilt appeals. H1a was supported. Guilt appeals that attributed responsibilities to the perceiver produced smaller effects compared to those not attributing responsibilities (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Effect sizes by theoretical moderators (sa = 26, kb = 127).

Controllability attribution

The meta-analysis supported H1b, indicating that controllability attribution moderated persuasive effects on outcomes. Guilt appeals depicting controllable causes generated smaller effects than those not depicting controllable causes.

Stability attribution

Stability attribution moderated the effects of guilt appeals, supporting H1c. Guilt appeals attributing the victim's suffering to an unstable cause produced greater effects on outcomes than did those not depicting such causes.

The proximity of perceiver–victim relationship

The proximity of the perceiver–victim relationship moderated the persuasive effects of guilt appeals, supporting H2. Guilt appeals featuring the victim in a personal relationship with the perceiver or being a non-human subject generated greater persuasive effects than those featuring victims in other relationships.

Recommendation of reparative behaviors

The results supported H3. When recommending reparative behaviors, guilt appeals produced significantly stronger effects than those without recommending reparative behaviors.

Outcome types

H4 was supported, indicating varied effects of guilt appeals by different types of outcomes. We found that guilt appeals were more effective in eliciting guilt, attitude, behavior, behavioral intention, and motivation.

Meta-regression of theoretical moderators

We conducted meta-regression for theoretical moderators. All theoretical moderators were included in the same meta-regression model because the influence of these moderators could occur simultaneously (Roseman, 2013). After adjustment for other theoretical moderators, the effects of guilt appeals were significantly moderated by controllability attribution and measured outcomes but marginally lowered by responsibility. Other theoretical moderators were not significant. After adjustment for other theoretical moderators, guilt appeals that attributed the suffering to the responsibility of a perceiver and to controllable causes produced smaller effects than those without these attributional factors. The effect size of guilt appeals depended on outcome types as stronger effects were mostly produced on behavioral intention and behavior.

Analysis of methodological moderators

The meta-analysis evaluated the following methodological moderators of guilt appeals to answer RQ2 (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Effect sizes by methodological moderators (sa = 26, kb = 127).

Contexts of persuasion objects

Effects of guilt appeals varied by contexts of persuasion objects. Guilt appeals were more effective in education and environment-related contexts compared to advertising or marketing contexts. On the other hand, pairwise comparisons showed that guilt appeals did not differ between advertising or marketing and health contexts.

Induction methods

Effects of guilt appeals varied by induction methods. Guilt appeals using imagination or recall through interpersonal communication instigated greater effects than persuasive guilt messages.

Narrative

The use of narrative or not influenced the effects of guilt appeals. Guilt appeals using narrative elicited more effects than those using non-narrative guilt appeals.

Message modalities

Message modalities moderated effects of guilt appeals. Guilt appeals using text elicited stronger effects than text and image-based guilt appeals.

Time point of measurement

Different time points of measurement moderated the effects of guilt appeals. The effects of guilt appeals were stronger if measured immediately after the exposure than those measured later within a week or after a week of exposure.

Analysis of situational moderators

The meta-analysis examined five situational moderators related to the characteristics of published studies. The result showed that published studies with university panels after 2011 were associated with more effects (all p < 0.10; Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effect sizes by situational moderators (sa = 26, kb = 127).

Discussion

The present meta-analysis aimed to conduct a comprehensive synthesis of guilt appeals. Compared with previous meta-analyses (O'Keefe, 2000; Boster et al., 2016; Xu and Guo, 2018; Turner and Rains, 2021), the present study centered on the emotional mechanism of guilt appeals and accounted for multiple theoretical moderators associated with guilt elicitation and coping. Overall, our findings showed a small effect of guilt appeals. The effect of guilt appeals could be influenced by guilt induction and coping processes, which echoed empirical evidence that guilt appeals may not be effective without considering multiple elicitation conditions (e.g., Chang, 2012) or approaches to regulating emotions (e.g., Graton et al., 2016). In addition, we identified various methodological approaches in past research that could moderate the effectiveness. The findings brought forth theoretical and practical implications of designing, testing, and implementing guilt appeals.

Theoretical moderators of guilt appeals

Responsibility attribution

The results showed that guilt appeals without attributing the responsibility to the perceiver produced greater effects compared to those with responsibility. This finding appeared contrary to past research which identified an essential role of responsibility in guilt arousal (Tangney et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2014; Miceli and Castelfranchi, 2018). We considered two possible explanations. First, without blaming one's behavior as the direct culprit of a problem, guilt appeals could be effective by focusing on the perceiver's social obligations in important social issues and their contribution to potential solutions (Urbonavicius et al., 2019). Some cases of this persuasive approach direct the perceiver to examine their advantages over others and take on social obligations (Chang, 2011; Harth et al., 2013; Dull et al., 2021). Such persuasive effects could be possibly related to existential guilt (Zimmermann et al., 2011; Binder, 2022). Negative feelings might result from a motivational mechanism of guilt intended to promote empathetic concern and altruistic helping behaviors toward other's suffering linked to problems in a social system (Baumeister et al., 1994; Roberts et al., 2014). In this persuasion process, the sense of responsibility was not imposed on the perceiver by the persuader but potentially developed by the perceiver once guilt was aroused (Berndsen and Manstead, 2007).

Second, attributed causes by guilt appeals might not be consistent with how responsibility was appraised by the perceiver, which could hinder the persuasive effects. The theoretical review shows that responsibility ambiguously involves appraisals of causality, intentionality, and capacity (Malle et al., 2014). One might accept themselves as the causal agent of a negative event but could still avoid the blame by justifying their actions or denying the nature of such events (Malle et al., 2014; Gerstenberg et al., 2018). Thus, attributed responsibilities in guilt appeals might oversimplify how people evaluate the cause of a specific event and whether they accept the blame (Malle et al., 2014).

Because responsibility is difficult to pinpoint in many situations (Tangney et al., 2007), assigning responsibility in guilt appeals should warrant careful consideration. While guilt appeals could emphasize perceived responsibility based on the moral values in a social system, personal traits and internalization of such values can also change how much responsibility one would accept (Ho et al., 2004; Tangney et al., 2007; Malle et al., 2014). Thus, the main takeaway is that the suffering of the victim should be the focus of guilt appeals as explicit attribution of responsibility is not necessarily accepted by the perceiver. Future research can also emphasize potential connections between the perceiver and the victims to stimulate empathy and promote a sense of equity (Harth et al., 2013). It may also be beneficial to test whether the evaluative value underlying a guilt-arousing event is consistent with the moral system held by the target population, especially when the contexts of guilt appeals are familiar to the perceiver (Jennings et al., 2015).

Stability and controllability attributions

Guilt appeals that attribute the causal event to unstable factors were more effective than those without such attributional information. This finding confirmed unstable factors as a key distinguishing dimension between guilt and other moral-related emotions, such as shame (Tangney et al., 2007; Greenbaum et al., 2020). Guilt appeals are designed to modify the perceiver's changeable behaviors or beliefs, rather than more stable characteristics, such as personality (Xu and Guo, 2018). Without specifically attributing the causes to be changeable, guilt appeals could not sufficiently stimulate the perceiver's adaptive response, such as reparation and change (Tangney et al., 2007; Shen, 2018; Graton and Mailliez, 2019).

Nonetheless, guilt appeals emphasizing the causes to be controllable by the perceiver might decrease persuasive effects. This finding could be explained by the relatedness of different attributional dimensions in guilt appeals. Theoretical reviews indicate that the perceiver's behavior, an internal, unstable aspect of the self, is a necessary condition of guilt induction (Tracy and Robins, 2006; Tangney et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2014). Guilt appeals also seek reparative behaviors to undo transgressions (Antonetti et al., 2018; Graton and Mailliez, 2019). Thus, the controllability of the cause may have been suggested by the induction- and coping-related information in a guilt appeal, including attributions of responsibility and stability as well as reparative behaviors (Tracy and Robins, 2006). For example, a guilt appeal directs the perceiver to appraise their consumer behaviors of traditional printers as the cause of environmental pollution and advocates them to use environment-friendly printers as the reparation (Chang, 2012). This case of guilt appeals has suggested that the cause of a negative event can be controlled by the action taken by the perceiver. As such, arguments reinforcing controllability may be redundant and further raise psychological reactance among the perceivers (Graton et al., 2016). On the other hand, controllability might not be appraised until the perceiver has evaluated and accepted other attributions in a guilt appeal (Malle et al., 2014). Given these potential explanations, future research is warranted to confirm the effects of controllability in guilt appeals.

The proximity of perceiver–victim relationship

Contrary to previous literature (Ghorbani et al., 2013), our analysis did not suggest that guilt appeals would be necessarily more effective by portraying a victim in a closer, more personal relationship with the perceiver. One explanation is that personal relationships do not necessarily reflect the psychological distance between the perceiver and another person or object (Trope and Liberman, 2010). In addition, the perceiver might maintain a broader sense of duty and personal meaning in a human society that exists beyond common personal relationships (Binder, 2022). The perceived relatedness on a more expansive level could evoke guilty feelings when personally unrelated individuals or the natural environment are impacted by a negative event, motivating helping and prosocial behaviors (Zimmermann et al., 2011). Thus, additional research is needed to evaluate and incorporate perceived closeness to the victim or an object by the perceiver in guilt appeals. Concrete information about the victims may also improve the perceived proximity (Ghorbani et al., 2013).

Recommendation of reparative behaviors

Recommending reparative behaviors increased the effects of guilt appeals. This result supported the importance of reparative actions as a coping process in which guilty people control negative feelings by making amends (Ketelaar and Tung Au, 2003; Tangney et al., 2007; Shen, 2018; Vaish, 2018). Our interpretation is that guilty perceivers were more likely to seek readily available information in the stimuli, rather than from alternative sources, that can help them quickly assuage the feeling (Tangney et al., 2007; Graton and Mailliez, 2019). Recommending reparative behaviors might also emphasize the repairable nature of the cause (Tangney et al., 2007), which increases the efficacy of making restitution. In contrast, non-repairable outcomes can cause maladaptive responses, lowering persuasive effectiveness (Kubany and Manke, 1995; LeBlanc et al., 2020). Based on current findings, practitioners using guilt appeals should provide a solution to rectify negative situations, especially when a behavior is not commonly considered wrong.

Outcome type

Expanding previous meta-analyses, our study tested the effectiveness of guilt appeals on various types of outcome variables. We found significant effects of guilt appeals on guilt, attitudes, and behavioral outcomes in support of broad influences of guilt as a persuasive approach. The strong effects on guilt reflect the theoretical foundation of guilt appeals, which centers on guilt as an emotional mechanism of persuasion (O'Keefe, 2002; Antonetti et al., 2018; Graton and Mailliez, 2019). Attitudinal and behavioral outcomes changed by guilt appeals showed that the influence on how one thinks and behaves could function as a key mechanism to regulate guilt and elicit persuasive outcomes (Tangney et al., 2007; Scherer and Moors, 2019).

Meta-regression of theoretical moderators

Overall, the meta-regression finding suggests that most of the factors could not consistently influence the effects of guilt appeals. After controlling for other theoretical moderators, stability attribution, proximity of perceiver–victim relationship, and recommendation of reparative behaviors did not significantly moderate the effects of guilt appeals. It is possible that the persuasive effects of guilt appeals were potentially generated through the appraisals of various factors in a correlated and sequential manner (Tracy and Robins, 2006; Tangney et al., 2007; Malle et al., 2014). Thus, one of these factors could be a necessary but insufficient condition of guilt arousal and persuasive outcomes. However, after adjustment for other theoretical covariates, guilt appeals emphasizing responsibility and controllability lowered persuasive effects, which further confirmed our previous finding. The perceiver might feel guilty about the other's suffering and engage in reparative behaviors based on holistic descriptions of the event but explicit information about the responsibility and controllability of the cause could backfire.

Moreover, insignificant theoretical moderators in the meta-regression also suggested that cognitive appraisals are subject to individual differences. The cognitive system that activates guilt depends on situational and individual differences, which increases uncertainty in the appraisal and regulation process (Muir et al., 2017; Kobylińska and Kusev, 2019). Future research should address whether personal preferences and certain persuasion contexts can influence how different attributions are weighed and prioritized in the appraisal of guilt appeals (Mantler et al., 2003). The finding also challenges the universal standard of guilt appeals that would fit all situations and individuals. How to design guilt appeals that involve different attribution dimensions for various contexts is an important question to answer.

Methodological moderators

Contexts

This study is the first meta-analysis to compile published evidence across several different contexts where guilt appeals are commonly tested. Guilt appeals were most effective when employed in environmental advocacy and education. This finding further demonstrates that guilty feelings are linked to one's moral and social responsibility, which motivate caring and prosocial behaviors for the wellbeing of others and the entire society (Zimmermann et al., 2011). However, guilt appeals used in health-related contexts were less effective than the control condition, which conflicts with Xu and Guo's (2018) meta-analysis. The discrepancy was possibly due to the fact that our analysis examined more outcomes influenced by guilt appeals in health-related contexts (e.g., efficacy and perceived threat) than only attitude and behavioral intentions assessed by Xu and Guo's study. In addition, this meta-analysis investigated the persuasive effects of guilt appeals on health behaviors benefiting both oneself and others (e.g., blood donation).

Induction methods

We found that compared with persuasive messages, imagination and recall as an induction method increased the effects of guilt appeals. The finding was consistent with a theoretical argument that emotion enhances experiential memory (Gomes et al., 2013). People can better remember an emotionally valenced event relative to neutral ones, which is encoded in memory to reconstruct an episode of past experience (Finn and Roediger, 2011). Thus, guilt-arousing events can be effectively consolidated in memory and easily retrieved once recalled, which may generate a consistent influence on metacognitive judgment. Through recall, people would examine the role of themselves in a negative event. This process could help trigger self-conscious emotions such as guilt (Tracy and Robins, 2006). This finding further supported earlier calls for using recall or imagination to elicit guilt and reach the persuasive goal in interpersonal settings (Baumeister et al., 1995; O'Keefe, 2002). Future studies should replicate the effectiveness of inducing guilt through recall in real-world settings. On the other hand, message designers should find the connection and relevance of the present object to the perceiver's emotional experience. Practitioners may also elicit guilt by encouraging the perceiver to explore personal experiences and examine their own transgression.

Narrative

Guilt appeals involving narrative were found relatively more effective. This result suggested the advantage of narrative in elevating emotional experiences (Nabi and Green, 2015). The practice of storytelling could engage the perceiver to reflect upon their own experience and increase understanding of the victim's suffering (Hamby et al., 2017). In addition, narrative can help the perceiver identify with the victim and find similarities between themselves and others (Hoeken et al., 2016). These processes could heighten one's self-representation in a social or moral context, which facilitates guilt arousal (Tracy and Robins, 2006).

Because guilt appeals are sometimes perceived to be manipulative (Cotte et al., 2005; Graton et al., 2016), narratives can also implicitly express persuasive purposes without increasing reactance (Reynolds-Tylus, 2019; Hasell et al., 2020). However, narrative persuasion might be less effective if persuasive arguments are incongruent with the perceiver's existing values because narrative engagement and enjoyment could be interrupted by counterarguments (Slater and Rouner, 1996; Krakow et al., 2018). Thus, non-narrative guilt appeals for mainstream social or moral causes might still be advantageous. Future research and practice can test the effects and limitations of narrative vs. non-narrative guilt appeals, especially when promoting controversial or less accepted topics.

Modalities

Text-based guilt appeals generated the strongest effects, which could be due to in-depth cognitive processing enabled by textual information (Mayer, 2014). As a result, the perceiver receiving textual information could inspect the claims of guilt appeals closely before accepting the request for compliance. This finding lent support to the continuing use of textual information as the major approach to developing guilt appeals. Using both text and image in guilt appeals produced lower but consistent effects. While effective images should attract attention and provide resemblances to real-life experiences, some images used in guilt appeals may not be congruent with textual information, hampering effective understanding (van Rompay et al., 2010; Sung and Mayer, 2012). Researchers should also assess whether new modalities facilitate the cognitive or emotional processing of guilt appeals that can elevate attention or guilt arousal (Geise and Baden, 2015; Kandaurova and Lee, 2019).

Time points of measurement

The meta-analysis showed the effects of guilt appeals waned after time passed. Despite recent literature (Antonetti et al., 2018; Kemp, 2023), the delayed effects of guilt appeals were found to be lower across all studies. Considering the transient nature of guilty feelings after misdeed (Brosch, 2021; Ogunfowora et al., 2023), the effects of guilt appeals should be sought immediately after persuasion. Nonetheless, we could not rule out the possibility that delay effects still exist (Brosch, 2021) but could not be observed by existing measurements. Thus, future research should explore alternative mechanisms and measurement approaches for enduring persuasive effects.

Limitations and future research

This meta-analysis has three limitations. First, the sampling part of the meta-analysis was conducted in 2020, and no studies published since then were sampled. In addition, meta-analyses need to maintain a balance between the study design and the scope of sampling because the validity of the study findings depends on whether we synthesized comparative studies in the literature (Cortina, 2003). Thus, we carefully developed the inclusion/exclusion criteria based on the research purpose and research questions and strictly enforced these criteria in the coding process. As a result, many studies, including recent ones, that failed to meet the inclusion and exclusion criteria were excluded from the final sample. For instance, studies that used mixed emotions in persuasive appeals were excluded because the effects of guilt appeals would not be comparable to their combination with another emotion due to distinct appraisal antecedents. As shown in the results, the present meta-analysis had reached sufficient power to examine the true effects of guilt appeals. The findings based on more rigorous conceptual and operational processes could help address the gap in the previous meta-analyses. Nonetheless, future research should sample more recent published and unpublished studies to replicate the findings of this meta-analysis. Second, because guilt is rooted in one's connection with other people and social relationships (Tangney et al., 2007), people may feel guilty about different issues and adopt new coping approaches as society and cultures evolve. These changes introduce new ways to design guilt appeals and test their effects. Thus, future studies can examine additional moderators involved in guilt induction, coping, and methodological approaches to advance this area of research. Third, through a synthesis of past studies, this meta-analysis provided important insights into the true effects of guilt appeals and their moderators. Compared with individual studies, meta-analyses make a unique contribution to the literature by examining and synthesizing a complex, even contradictory body of literature in an effect (Sutton and Higgins, 2008; Haidich, 2010). Nonetheless, researchers can also conduct empirical studies to confirm whether the significant moderators in this study could improve the effects of guilt appeals.

Conclusion

The present meta-analysis was a comprehensive evaluation of the effectiveness of guilt appeals. The findings of the present meta-analysis showed an overall small effect of guilt appeals, and this effect could be stronger on guilt emotions, attitudes, behaviors, and behavioral intentions. Guilt appeals were particularly more effective in the contexts of environmental advocacy and education. The study contributes to the understanding of the specific theoretical (i.e., responsibility attribution, controllability attribution, stability attribution, and recommendation of reparative behaviors and outcome types) and methodological factors (i.e., contexts, induction methods, narrative, message modalities, and time point of measurement) that could facilitate or impede the persuasive effects.

From a practical perspective, the findings could improve the confidence of practitioners in using and developing guilt appeals by offering valuable insights into different parts and processes of this persuasive means. To achieve better effectiveness, practitioners might design guilt appeals that portray the victim's suffering, focus on the unstable aspects of the cause, and offer reparative recommendations. Conversely, it is advisable to avoid arguments that attribute the victim's suffering to the perceiver's responsibility or overly emphasize the controllability of the cause as these approaches might be perceived as manipulative and adversely affect persuasion outcomes. Moreover, specific methods should be considered in practice to improve the effectiveness of guilt appeals, including invoking imagination and recall to elicit guilt, employing narratives, and incorporating textual information. Immediate persuasive effects should also be sought in practice.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

WP: conceptualization, methodology, writing—original draft, and writing—review and editing. QH: methodology, formal analysis, software, writing—original draft, and visualization. BM and DL: methodology and investigation. EM and JS: conceptualization and writing—original draft. NC: conceptualization, writing—review and editing, and supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1201631/full#supplementary-material

References

Agrawal, N., and Duhachek, A. (2010). Emotional compatibility and the effectiveness of antidrinking messages: a defensive processing perspective on shame and guilt. J. Mark. Res. 47, 263–273. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.47.2.263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allard, T., and White, K. (2015). Cross-domain effects of guilt on desire for self-improvement products. J. Consum. Res. 42, 401–419. doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucv024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, C. T., Machleit, K. A., Kleine, S. S., and Notani, A. S. (2005). A place for emotion in attitude models. J. Bus. Res. 58, 494–499. doi: 10.1016/S0148-2963(03)00139-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonetti, P., and Baines, P. (2015). Guilt in marketing research: an elicitation–consumption perspective and research agenda. Int. J. Manag. Rev. 17, 333–355. doi: 10.1111/ijmr.12043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonetti, P., Baines, P., and Jain, S. (2018). The persuasiveness of guilt appeals over time: pathways to delayed compliance. J. Bus. Res. 90, 14–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.03.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonetti, P., Baines, P., and Walker, L. (2015). From elicitation to consumption: assessing the longitudinal effectiveness of negative emotional appeals in social marketing. J. Mark. Manage. 31, 940–969. doi: 10.1080/0267257X.2015.1031266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonetti, P., and Maklan, S. (2014). Feelings that make a difference: how guilt and pride convince consumers of the effectiveness of sustainable consumption choices. J. Bus. Ethics 124, 117–134. doi: 10.1007/s10551-013-1841-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babin, E. A. (2009). Men's Intentions to Reduce Female Partners' Risk of HPV and Cervical Cancer. Available online at: https://search.proquest.com/docview/304844959?pq-origsite=gscholar&fromopenview=true (accessed February, 2020).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Baek, T. H., and Yoon, S. (2017). Guilt and shame: environmental message framing effects. J. Advert. 46, 440–453. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2017.1321069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Reis, H. T., and Delespaul, P. A. E. G. (1995). Subjective and experiential correlates of guilt in daily life. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21, 1256–1268. doi: 10.1177/01461672952112002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., and Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: an interpersonal approach. Psychol. Bull. 115, 243–267. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begg, C. B., and Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics 50, 1088–1101. doi: 10.2307/2533446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrendt, H., and Ben-Ari, R. (2012). The positive side of negative emotion: the role of guilt and shame in coping with interpersonal conflict. J. Conflict Resolut. 56, 1116–1138. doi: 10.1177/0022002712445746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berndsen, M., and Manstead, A. S. (2007). On the relationship between responsibility and guilt: antecedent appraisal or elaborated appraisal?. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 774–792. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, P.-E. (2022). The call of the unlived life: on the psychology of existential guilt. Front. Psychol. 13, 991325. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.991325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, L. G. (2005). Self-referenced fear and guilt appeals: the moderating role of self-construal. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 35, 2290–2309. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02103.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boster, F. J., Cruz, S., Manata, B., DeAngelis, B. N., and Zhuang, J. (2016). A meta-analytic review of the effect of guilt on compliance. Soc. Inf. 11, 54–67. doi: 10.1080/15534510.2016.1142892

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., and Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion 1, 276–298. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bretz, F., Hothorn, T., and Westfall, P. (2010). Multiple Comparisons Using R. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Brosch, T. (2021). Affect and emotions as drivers of climate change perception and action: a review. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 42, 15–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C.-T. (2011). Guilt appeals in cause-related marketing: The subversive roles of product type and donation magnitude. Int. J. Advert. 30, 587–616. doi: 10.2501/IJA-30-4-587-616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C.-T. (2012). Are guilt appeals a panacea in green advertising? The right formula of issue proximity and environmental consciousness. Int. J. Advert. 31, 741–771. doi: 10.2501/IJA-31-4-741-771

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chien, Y.-T., and Chang, C.-Y. (2012). Exploring the impact of animation-based questionnaire on conducting a web-based educational survey and its association with vividness of respondents' visual images. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 43, E81–E85. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8535.2012.01287.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrdileli, M., and Kasser, T. (2018). Guilt, shame, and apologizing behavior: a laboratory study. Pers. Individ. Dif. 135, 304–306. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.07.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cialdini, R. B., Darby, B. L., and Vincent, J. E. (1973). Transgression and altruism: a case for hedonism. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 9, 502–516. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(73)90031-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortina, J. M. (2003). Apples and oranges (and pears, oh my!): the search for moderators in meta-analysis. Organ. Res. Methods 6, 415–439. doi: 10.1177/1094428103257358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotte, J., Coulter, R. A., and Moore, M. (2005). Enhancing or disrupting guilt: the role of ad credibility and perceived manipulative intent. J. Bus. Res. 58, 361–368. doi: 10.1016/S0148-2963(03)00102-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cryder, C. E., Springer, S., and Morewedge, C. K. (2012). Guilty feelings, targeted actions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 607–618. doi: 10.1177/0146167211435796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dang, C. M., Lee, M. H., Nguyen, A. M., Diduck, A., Villareal, A., Simic, Z., et al. (2022). Survivor narratives of the Oklahoma City bombing: the story over time. J. Conting. Crisis Manag. 30, 102–111. doi: 10.1111/1468-5973.12357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Hooge, I. E., Nelissen, R. M. A., Breugelmans, S. M., and Zeelenberg, M. (2011). What is moral about guilt? Acting “prosocially” at the disadvantage of others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 462–473. doi: 10.1037/a0021459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., and Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: differential influences of shame and guilt. Cogn. Emot. 21, 1025–1042. doi: 10.1080/02699930600980874

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dearing, R. L., Stuewig, J., and Tangney, J. P. (2005). On the importance of distinguishing shame from guilt: relations to problematic alcohol and drug use. Addict. Behav. 30, 1392–1404. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Rucker, D. D., Wegener, D. T., and Braverman, J. (2004). Discrete emotions and persuasion: the role of emotion-induced expectancies. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 43–56. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doosje, B., Branscombe, N. R., Spears, R., and Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Guilty by association: when one's group has a negative history. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 872–886. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dull, B. D., Hoyt, L. T., Grzanka, P. R., and Zeiders, K. H. (2021). Can white guilt motivate action? The role of civic beliefs. J. Youth Adolesc. 50, 1081–1097. doi: 10.1007/s10964-021-01401-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duval, S., and Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56, 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellsworth, P. C., and Scherer, K. R. (2003). “Appraisal processes in emotion,” in Handbook of Affective Sciences, eds R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, and H. H. Goldsmith (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 572–595.

Google Scholar

Finn, B., and Roediger, H. L. III. (2011). Enhancing retention through reconsolidation: negative emotional arousal following retrieval enhances later recall. Psychol. Sci. 22, 781–786. doi: 10.1177/0956797611407932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. trid.trb.org. Available online at: https://trid.trb.org/view/1150648

Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., and Middlestadt, S. (1995). noncognitive effects on attitude formation and change: fact or artifact? J. Consum. Psychol. 4, 181–202. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp0402_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, Z., and Tipton, E. (2015). robumeta: an R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis. arXiv preprint arXiv:1503.02220.

Google Scholar

Freling, T. H., Yang, Z., Saini, R., Itani, O. S., and Rashad Abualsamh, R. (2020). When poignant stories outweigh cold hard facts: a meta-analysis of the anecdotal bias. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 160, 51–67. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2020.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geise, S., and Baden, C. (2015). Putting the image back into the frame: modeling the linkage between visual communication and frame-processing theory. Commun. Theory 25, 46–69. doi: 10.1111/comt.12048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerstenberg, T., Ullman, T. D., Nagel, J., Kleiman-Weiner, M., Lagnado, D. A., and Tenenbaum, J. B. (2018). Lucky or clever? From expectations to responsibility judgments. Cognition 177, 122–141. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghorbani, M., Liao, Y., Çayköylü, S., and Chand, M. (2013). Guilt, shame, and reparative behavior: the effect of psychological proximity. J. Bus. Ethics 114, 311–323. doi: 10.1007/s10551-012-1350-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gleser, L., and Olkin, I. (2009). “Stochastically dependent effect sizes,” in The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis, eds H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges, and J. C. Valentine (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 357–376.

Google Scholar

Gomes, C. F. A., Brainerd, C. J., and Stein, L. M. (2013). Effects of emotional valence and arousal on recollective and nonrecollective recall. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 39, 663–677. doi: 10.1037/a0028578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graton, A., and Mailliez, M. (2019). A theory of guilt appeals: a review showing the importance of investigating cognitive processes as mediators between emotion and behavior. Behav. Sci. 9, 117. doi: 10.3390/bs9120117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graton, A., and Ric, F. (2017). How guilt leads to reparation? Exploring the processes underlying the effects of guilt. Motiv. Emot. 41, 343–352. doi: 10.1007/s11031-017-9612-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graton, A., Ric, F., and Gonzalez, E. (2016). Reparation or reactance? The influence of guilt on reaction to persuasive communication. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 62, 40–49. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. C., and Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 701–721. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenbaum, R., Bonner, J., Gray, T., and Mawritz, M. (2020). Moral emotions: a review and research agenda for management scholarship. J. Organ. Behav. 41, 95–114. doi: 10.1002/job.2367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haidich, A. B. (2010). Meta-analysis in medical research. Hippokratia 14(Suppl. 1), 29–37.

Google Scholar

Hamby, A., Brinberg, D., and Daniloski, K. (2017). Reflecting on the journey: mechanisms in narrative persuasion. J. Consum. Psychol. 27, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2016.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harth, N. S., Leach, C. W., and Kessler, T. (2013). Guilt, anger, and pride about in-group environmental behaviour: different emotions predict distinct intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 34, 18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasell, A., Lyons, B. A., Tallapragada, M., and Jamieson, K. H. (2020). Improving GM consensus acceptance through reduced reactance and climate change-based message targeting. Environ. Commun. 14, 987–1003. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2020.1746377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 6, 107–128. doi: 10.3102/10769986006002107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V., and Olkin, I. (1985). “Random effects models for effect sizes,” in Statistical Methods for Meta Analysis, eds L. V. Hedges, and I. Olkin (Orlando, FL: Academic Press), 189–203.

Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., and Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Res. Synth. Methods 1, 39–65. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, D. Y.-F., Fu, W., and Ng, S. M. (2004). Guilt, shame and embarrassment: revelations of face and self. Cult. Psychol. 10, 64–84. doi: 10.1177/1354067X04044166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeken, H., Kolthoff, M., and Sanders, J. (2016). Story perspective and character similarity as drivers of identification and narrative persuasion. Hum. Commun. Res. 42, 292–311. doi: 10.1111/hcre.12076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hugelius, K., Adolfsson, A., Örtenwall, P., and Gifford, M. (2017). Being both helpers and victims: health professionals' experiences of working during a natural disaster. Prehosp. Disaster Med. 32, 117–123. doi: 10.1017/S1049023X16001412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, J. E., and Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Dichotomization of continuous variables: the implications for meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 75, 334–349. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.75.3.334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilies, R., Peng, A. C., Savani, K., and Dimotakis, N. (2013). Guilty and helpful: an emotion-based reparatory model of voluntary work behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 98, 1051–1059. doi: 10.1037/a0034162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iyer, A., Schmader, T., and Lickel, B. (2007). Why individuals protest the perceived transgressions of their country: the role of anger, shame, and guilt. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 572–587. doi: 10.1177/0146167206297402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jennings, P. L., Mitchell, M. S., and Hannah, S. T. (2015). The moral self: a review and integration of the literature. J. Organ. Behav. 36, S104–S168. doi: 10.1002/job.1919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kandaurova, M., and Lee, S. H. (2019). The effects of Virtual Reality (VR) on charitable giving: the role of empathy, guilt, responsibility, and social exclusion. J. Bus. Res. 100, 571–580. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.10.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karfakis, N., and Kokkinidis, G. (2018). On guilt and the depoliticization of downsizing practices. Int. J. Sociol. Soc. Policy 39, 156–180. doi: 10.1108/IJSSP-06-2018-0100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, D. (2023). Empathy or escape? Examining alternative mechanisms underlying responses to disgust-evoking charity appeals over time. J. Curr. Issues Res. Advert. 44, 88–104. doi: 10.1080/10641734.2022.2130483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ketelaar, T., and Tung Au, W. (2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: an affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cogn. Emot. 17, 429–453. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., Thibodeau, R., and Jorgensen, R. S. (2011). Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 137, 68–96. doi: 10.1037/a0021466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobylińska, D., and Kusev, P. (2019). Flexible emotion regulation: how situational demands and individual differences influence the effectiveness of regulatory strategies. Front. Psychol. 10, 72. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krakow, M. M., Yale, R. N., Jensen, J. D., Carcioppolo, N., and Ratcliff, C. L. (2018). Comparing mediational pathways for narrative- and argument-based messages: believability, counterarguing, and emotional reaction. Hum. Commun. Res. 44, 299–321. doi: 10.1093/hcr/hqy002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krippendorff, K. (2004). Reliability in content analysis. Hum. Commun. Res. 30, 411–433. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2004.tb00738.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubany, E. S., and Manke, F. P. (1995). Cognitive therapy for trauma-related guilt: conceptual bases and treatment outlines. Cogn. Behav. Pract. 2, 27–61. doi: 10.1016/S1077-7229(05)80004-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

La Ferle, C., Muralidharan, S., and Kim, E. (2019). Using guilt and shame appeals from an eastern perspective to promote bystander intervention: a study of mitigating domestic violence in India. J. Advert. 48, 555–568. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2019.1668893

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacy, S., and Riffe, D. (1996). Sampling error and selecting intercoder reliability samples for nominal content categories. J. Mass. Commun. Q. 73, 963–973. doi: 10.1177/107769909607300414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landis, J. R., and Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. doi: 10.2307/2529310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

LeBlanc, N. J., Toner, E. R., O'Day, E. B., Moore, C. W., Marques, L., Robinaugh, D. J., et al. (2020). Shame, guilt, and pride after loss: exploring the relationship between moral emotions and psychopathology in bereaved adults. J. Affect. Disord. 263, 405–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., and Paek, H.-J. (2013). Impact of norm perceptions and guilt on audience response to anti-smoking norm PSAs: the case of Korean male smokers. Health Educ. J. 72, 503–511. doi: 10.1177/0017896912450249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y.-K. (2013). The influence of message appeal, environmental hyperopia, and environmental locus of control on green policy communication. Soc. Behav. Person. 41, 731–738. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2013.41.5.731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, L., and Chu, H. (2016). altmeta: Alternative Meta-Analysis Methods. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/altmeta/index.html

Google Scholar

Liu, S., and Yang, J. Z. (2020). Incorporating message framing into narrative persuasion to curb E-cigarette use among college students. Risk Anal. 40, 1677–1690. doi: 10.1111/risa.13502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., and Monroe, A. E. (2014). A theory of blame. Psychol. Inq. 25, 147–186. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.877340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantler, J., Schellenberg, E. G., and Page, J. S. (2003). Attributions for serious illness: are controllability, responsibility and blame different constructs? Can. J. Behav. Sci. 35, 142–152. doi: 10.1037/h0087196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massi Lindsey, L. L. (2005). Anticipated guilt as behavioral motivation: an examination of appeals to help unknown others through bone marrow donation. Hum. Commun. Res. 31, 453–481. doi: 10.1093/hcr/31.4.453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E. (2014). “Cognitive theory of multimedia learning,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, ed R. Mayer (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 43–71.

Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E., and Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educ. Psychol. 38, 43–52. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miceli, M., and Castelfranchi, C. (2018). Reconsidering the differences between shame and guilt. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 14, 710–733. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v14i3.1564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., and PRISM Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 62, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. (2020). “Appraisal theory of emotion,” in Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences, eds V. Zeigler-Hill, and T. K. Shackelford (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 232–240.

Google Scholar

Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., and Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: state of the art and future development. Emot. Rev. 5, 119–124. doi: 10.1177/1754073912468165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muir, K., Madill, A., and Brown, C. (2017). Individual differences in emotional processing and autobiographical memory: interoceptive awareness and alexithymia in the fading affect bias. Cogn. Emot. 31, 1392–1404. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1225005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mujtaba, B. G., and Senathip, T. (2020). Layoffs and downsizing implications for the leadership role of human resources. Int. J. Biodivers. Sci. Eco. Serv. Mgmt. 13, 209–228. doi: 10.4236/jssm.2020.132014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulligan, K., and Scherer, K. R. (2012). Toward a working definition of emotion. Emot. Rev. 4, 345–357. doi: 10.1177/1754073912445818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nabi, R. L. (1999). A cognitive-functional model for the effects of discrete negative emotions on information processing, attitude change, and recall. Commun. Theory 9, 292–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.1999.tb00172.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nabi, R. L. (2003). Exploring the framing effects of emotion: do discrete emotions differentially influence information accessibility, information seeking, and policy preference? Communic. Res. 30, 224–247. doi: 10.1177/0093650202250881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nabi, R. L., and Green, M. C. (2015). The role of a narrative's emotional flow in promoting persuasive outcomes. Media Psychol. 18, 137–162. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2014.912585

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuendorf, K. A. (2017). The Content Analysis Guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Noble, G., Pomering, A., and Johnson Lester, W. (2014). Gender and message appeal: their influence in a pro-environmental social advertising context. J. Soc. Market. 4, 4–21. doi: 10.1108/JSOCM-12-2012-0049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowell, B., and Boyd, N. M. (2014). Sense of community responsibility in community collaboratives: advancing a theory of community as resource and responsibility. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 54, 229–242. doi: 10.1007/s10464-014-9667-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, L. E., Berry, J. W., Weiss, J., Schweitzer, D., and Sevier, M. (2000). Survivor guilt, submissive behaviour and evolutionary theory: the down-side of winning in social comparison. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 73 (Pt 4), 519–530. doi: 10.1348/000711200160705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogunfowora, B., Nguyen, V. Q., Lee, C. S., Babalola, M. T., and Ren, S. (2023). Do moral disengagers experience guilt following workplace misconduct? Consequences for emotional exhaustion and task performance. J. Org. Behav. 44, 476–494. doi: 10.1002/job.2668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Keefe, D. J. (2000). Guilt and Social Influence. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 23, 67–101. doi: 10.1080/23808985.2000.11678970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Keefe, D. J. (2002). “Guilt as a mechanism of persuasion,” in The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice, eds J. P. Dillard, and M. Pfau (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 329–344.

Google Scholar

Peloza, J., White, K., and Shang, J. (2013). Good and guilt-free: the role of self-accountability in influencing preferences for products with ethical attributes. J. Mark. 77, 104–119. doi: 10.1509/jm.11.0454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perloff, T., King, J. C., Rigney, M., Ostroff, J. S., and Johnson Shen, M. (2019). Survivor guilt: the secret burden of lung cancer survivorship. J. Psychosoc. Oncol. 37, 573–585. doi: 10.1080/07347332.2019.1569191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 160–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. A., and Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 175–181. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pustejovsky, J. E. (2020). clubSandwich: Cluster-Robust (Sandwich) Variance Estimators With Small-Sample Corrections. Github. Available online at: https://github.com/jepusto/clubSandwich (accessed September 12, 2022).

Google Scholar

Pustejovsky, J. E., and Tipton, E. (2018). Small-sample methods for cluster-robust variance estimation and hypothesis testing in fixed effects models. J. Bus. Econ. Stat. 36, 672–683. doi: 10.1080/07350015.2016.1247004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pustejovsky, J. E., and Tipton, E. (2022). Meta-analysis with robust variance estimation: expanding the range of working models. Prev. Sci. 23, 425–438. doi: 10.1007/s11121-021-01246-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds-Tylus, T. (2019). Psychological reactance and persuasive health communication: a review of the literature. Front. Commun. 4, 56. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2019.00056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, W., Strayer, J., and Denham, S. (2014). Empathy, anger, guilt: emotions and prosocial behaviour. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 46, 465–474. doi: 10.1037/a0035057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roccas, S., Klar, Y., and Liviatan, I. (2006). The paradox of group-based guilt: modes of national identification, conflict vehemence, and reactions to the in-group's moral violations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 698–711. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roseman, I. J. (2013). Appraisal in the emotion system: coherence in strategies for coping. Emot. Rev. 5, 141–149. doi: 10.1177/1754073912469591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull. 86, 638. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruckstaetter, J., Sells, J., Newmeyer, M. D., and Zink, D. (2017). Parental apologies, empathy, shame, guilt, and attachment: a path analysis. J. Couns. Dev. 95, 389–400. doi: 10.1002/jcad.12154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R., and Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: event appraisal and component differentiation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 719–745. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, F. L., Law, K., Hunter, J. E., Rothstein, H. R., Pearlman, K., and McDaniel, M. (1993). Refinements in validity generalization methods: implications for the situational specificity hypothesis. J. Appl. Psychol. 78, 3–12. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, C. R., Zaval, L., Weber, E. U., and Markowitz, E. M. (2017). The influence of anticipated pride and guilt on pro-environmental decision making. PLoS ONE 12, e0188781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shehu, E., Langmaack, A.-C., and Clement, M. (2013). The dark side of using emergencies in direct marketing campaigns for blood donation services. Serv. Sci. 5, 163–178. doi: 10.1287/serv.1120.0041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, F., Sheer, V. C., and Li, R. (2015). Impact of narratives on persuasion in health communication: a meta-analysis. J. Advert. 44, 105–113. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2015.1018467

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, L. (2018). The evolution of shame and guilt. PLoS ONE 13, e0199448. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P. E., and Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol. Bull. 86, 420–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siedlecka, E., and Denson, T. F. (2019). Experimental methods for inducing basic emotions: a qualitative review. Emot. Rev. 11, 87–97. doi: 10.1177/1754073917749016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slater, M. D., and Rouner, D. (1996). Value-affirmative and value-protective processing of alcohol education messages that include statistical evidence or anecdotes. Commun. Res. 23, 210–235. doi: 10.1177/009365096023002003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, C. A., and Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 813–838. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

So, J., Achar, C., Han, D., Agrawal, N., Duhachek, A., and Maheswaran, D. (2015). The psychology of appraisal: specific emotions and decision-making. J. Consum. Psychol. 25, 359–371. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2015.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sung, E., and Mayer, R. E. (2012). When graphics improve liking but not learning from online lessons. Comput. Human Behav. 28, 1618–1625. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.03.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, A. J. (2009). “Publication bias,” in The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis, eds H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges, and J. C. Valentine (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 435–452.

Google Scholar

Sutton, A. J., and Higgins, J. P. T. (2008). Recent developments in meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 27, 625–650. doi: 10.1002/sim.2934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., and Dearing, R. L., (eds.) (2002). Shame and Guilt. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., and Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 345–372. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracy, J. L., and Robins, R. W. (2004). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: a theoretical model. Psychol. Inq. 15, 103–125. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracy, J. L., and Robins, R. W. (2006). Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: support for a theoretical model. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 1339–1351. doi: 10.1177/0146167206290212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 117, 440–463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, M., and Rains, S. (2021). Guilt appeals in persuasive communication: a meta-analytic review. Commun. Stud. 72, 684–700. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2021.1953094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urbonavicius, S., Adomaviciute, K., Urbutyte, I., and Cherian, J. (2019). Donation to charity and purchase of cause-related products: the influence of existential guilt and experience. J. Consum. Behav. 18, 89–96. doi: 10.1002/cb.1749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaish, A. (2018). The prosocial functions of early social emotions: the case of guilt. Curr Opin. Psychol. 20, 25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Rompay, T. J. L., de Vries, P. W., and van Venrooij, X. G. (2010). More than words: on the importance of picture–text congruence in the online environment. J. Interact. Market. 24, 22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.intmar.2009.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viechtbauer, W., and Cheung, M. W.-L. (2010). Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 1, 112–125. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B., Graham, S., and Chandler, C. (1982). Pity, anger, and guilt: an attributional analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 8, 226–232. doi: 10.1177/0146167282082007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J. (2022). The impact of guilt and shame in charity advertising: the role of self-construal. J. Philanthr. Market. 27, e1709. doi: 10.1002/nvsm.1709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Z., and Guo, H. (2018). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of guilt on health-related attitudes and intentions. Health Commun. 33, 519–525. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2017.1278633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanna, M. P., and Rempel, J. K. (2008). “Attitudes: a new look at an old concept,” in Attitudes: Their Structure, Function, and Consequences, eds R. H. Fazio, and R. E. Petty (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 7–15.

Google Scholar

Zimmermann, A., Abrams, D., Doosje, B., and Manstead, A. S. R. (2011). Causal and moral responsibility: antecedents and consequences of group-based guilt. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41, 825–839. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: guilt, emotion, persuasion, attitudes, behaviors, meta-analysis

Citation: Peng W, Huang Q, Mao B, Lun D, Malova E, Simmons JV and Carcioppolo N (2023) When guilt works: a comprehensive meta-analysis of guilt appeals. Front. Psychol. 14:1201631. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1201631

Received: 11 April 2023; Accepted: 04 September 2023;
Published: 28 September 2023.

Edited by:

Keith Markman, Ohio University, United States

Reviewed by:

Tejaswi Patil, BMS College of Engineering, India
Eduardo Moraes Sarmento, University of Lisbon, Portugal
Belem Barbosa, University of Porto, Portugal

Copyright © 2023 Peng, Huang, Mao, Lun, Malova, Simmons and Carcioppolo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wei Peng, d2VpLnBlbmcmI3gwMDA0MDt3c3UuZWR1; Qian Huang, cWlhbi5odWFuZyYjeDAwMDQwO3VuZC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.