Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 June 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Parental Questionnaires as a Reliable Instrument for the Assessment of Child Language Development View all 16 articles

Effect of biomedical complications on very and extremely preterm children's language

\r\nVirginia Varela-Moraga
Virginia Varela-Moraga1*Benjamín Diethelm-VarelaBenjamín Diethelm-Varela2Miguel Prez-PereiraMiguel Pérez-Pereira3
  • 1Departamento de Fonoaudiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
  • 2Department of Molecular Genetics and Microbiology, School of Biological Sciences, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
  • 3Departamento de Psicoloxía Evolutiva e da Educación, Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Introduction: Very and extremely preterm children have been found to show delays in the development of language in early years. In some investigations, however, a rigorous control of biomedical complications, such as Periventricular Leukomalacia (PVL), Intraventricular Hemorrhage (IVH) or Bronchopulmonary Dysplasia (BPD), does not always exist. For that reason, a confounding effect of low gestational age and biomedical complications may lead to erroneous conclusions about the effect of gestational age.

Methods: In this investigation we compare language development [use of words, sentence complexity and mean length of the three longest utterances (MLU3)] of three groups of Chilean children at 24 months of age (corrected age for preterm children). The first group was composed of 42 healthy full-term children (Full term group: FT), the second group of 60 preterm children born below 32 gestational weeks without medical complications (low risk preterm group: LRPT), and the third group was composed of 64 children below 32 gestational weeks who had medical complications (High risk preterm group: HRPT). The three groups were similar in terms of gender distribution, maternal education, and socio-economic environment. The instrument used to assess language was the Communicative Development Inventories (CDI). In addition, the Ages and Stages Questionnaire-3 (ASQ-3) was also used to assess other developmental dimensions.

Results: The results indicate that HRPT and LRPT children obtained significantly lower results than the FT group in the three language measures obtained through the CDI. No significant differences were observed between the HRPT and the LRPT groups, although the HRPT obtained the lowest results in the three CDI measures. The results obtained through the administration of the ASQ-3 confirm the delay of both preterm groups in communicative development when compared to the FT group. No significant differences between the FT and the PT groups were observed in gross motor, fine motor and problem solving dimensions of the ASQ-3. The LRPT group obtained results that were significantly higher than those of the FT group and the HRPT group in gross motor development.

Discussion: These results seem to indicate that the area of language development is particularly influenced by very or extremely low gestational age.

Introduction

Premature children are considered a vulnerable population due to their immaturity as a result of their early birth. Very premature and extremely premature children, who were born with < 32 and 28 weeks of gestation, respectively (Goldenberg et al., 2008; Blencowe et al., 2019), present high morbidity and are exposed to greater biological risk. The lower the gestational age and the lower the birth weight, the more likely the presence of associated complications, chronic pathologies, and developmental delays (Bhutta et al., 2002). These biomedical complications could eventually have an impact on cognitive, linguistic, and behavioral performance during childhood. The probability of suffering cerebral palsy and neurosensory disorders increases as the gestational age decreases (Synnes et al., 1994). Children with gestational age (GA) between 31 and 23 weeks are those who have a higher probability of suffering neurodevelopmental disorders (Kilbride et al., 2004; Thorngren-Jerneck and Herbst, 2006; GuangXi Cooperative Research Group for Extremely Preterm Infants et al., 2019). GuangXi Cooperative Research Group for Extremely Preterm Infants et al. (2019) evaluated a sample of children born before 28 weeks of gestation and found that GA is a predictive factor for neurodevelopmental disorders, which means that as GA decreases, neurosensory disorders increase. Similarly, Baron et al. (2014) found that GA was the most important factor that determined differences in neuropsychological, intellectual, and behavioral functioning, and Anderson and Doyle (2008) found that 40% of the children under 26 weeks of gestation had cognitive delays.

Among the most frequent early biomedical complications found in preterm infants, bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) stands out for its association with the risk of delay in cognitive and language development (Singer et al., 2001; Rvachew et al., 2005; Anderson and Doyle, 2006; Short et al., 2007; Sansavini et al., 2011; Gallini et al., 2021; Katz et al., 2022). The complications and sequelae caused by intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) on cognitive, linguistic, and educational achievements are related to its severity, in such a way that the higher the grade of IVH, the worse its effects, with IVH of grades III and IV being particularly serious (Sherlock et al., 2005; Luu et al. al., 2009; Srinivasakumar et al., 2013; Vohr, 2022). A similar situation occurs with the presence of periventricular leukomalacia (PVL), which has a negative effect on cognitive and language development (Ohgi et al., 2005; Resic et al., 2008). Low Apgar scores in premature infants who manage to survive are related to the appearance of HIV, PVL, necrotizing enterocolitis, and retinopathy of prematurity, in addition to a long hospital stay (Phalen et al., 2012). Soares et al. (2017) found that the risk for language development in preterm children under 32 weeks of gestation was highly associated with the presence of intraventricular hemorrhage, bronchopulmonary dysplasia, maternal age of < 18 years, birth weight of < 1,000 g, and prolonged hospitalization (15–30 days minimum).

In addition to the aforementioned biomedical complications, there are environmental factors that provide additional influence (Reidy et al., 2013). Although neonatal medical risk consistently displays a negative impact on early childhood outcomes, socioeconomic and demographic risks (such as mothers who are single, of young age, or with less than a high school education) may affect cognitive, language, and motor delays, and the opposite, good socioeconomic status positively impacts on development (Mangin et al., 2016; Nyman et al., 2017; Kilbride et al., 2022).

When these biomedical complications occur in preterm newborns, the presence of negative consequences later in development is highly likely. Preterm children who have presented evident and demonstrable damage through medical examinations and procedures are considered at high risk for possible delays or disorders in their development. On the contrary, those who present few or no associated alterations are considered a low-risk group (da Ribeiro et al., 2017). In any case, there is so much heterogeneity among preterm children that even low-risk preterm infants show mixed outcomes in their language development (Casiro et al., 1990; Menyuk et al., 1995; Stolt et al., 2007; Cattani et al., 2010; Pérez-Pereira et al., 2014; Pérez-Pereira and Cruz, 2018; Suttora et al., 2020; Pérez-Pereira, 2021).

What appears repeatedly in research on very preterm and extremely preterm children with low birth weight and the presence of biomedical complications are low scores in language assessments. Most assessments of language development of children approximately 24 months of age have been carried out using the MacArthur-Bates scales or Communicative Development Inventories (CDIs) (Fenson et al., 2006). The spread of adaptations of this parental report instrument to many different languages has permitted the assessment of a large number of very young children. It would have otherwise been very difficult to assess these children through individual administration of conventional tests.

Most studies using the CDI have shown that very and extremely preterm children present lower scores and an evident delay in early lexical and morphosyntactic development or in vocabulary composition when compared to their term-born peers (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2004; Sansavini et al., 2006, 2011; Foster-Cohen et al., 2007; Gayraud and Kern, 2007; Stolt et al., 2007, 2009, 2012, 2017; Schults et al., 2013; Sentenac et al., 2020; Tulviste et al., 2020). The performance of preterm children is worse the lower the weight and gestational age, especially with < 32 weeks of gestational age (Foster-Cohen et al., 2007; Gayraud and Kern, 2007).

The Ages and Stages Questionnaire-3rd edition (ASQ-3) (Squires et al., 2009), another parental questionnaire, has also been used as a screening test of infant and child development in 5 areas: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal social. This questionnaire has been proposed as a useful screening instrument for the following up of preterm children as a population at risk of developmental delay (Skellern et al., 2001; Marks et al., 2009; Simard et al., 2012; Agarwal et al., 2016; Ballantyne et al., 2016; Al-Hindi et al., 2021). It has been found that the scores obtained by preterm children approximately 4 years of age in the five areas explored by the ASQ-3 decrease as the gestational age of the children decreases and, coherently, that the risk of developmental delay of preterm children between 25 and 40 weeks of gestation ascends as their gestational age descends. The percentage of 4-year-old children with rates of abnormal total problems scale in the ASQ ranges from 4.2% among term-born children to 37.5% among children born at 24–25 weeks' gestation (p < 0.001) in a large Dutch study with 1,439 preterm children and 544 FT children (Kerstjens et al., 2012). A similar pattern has been observed in all underlying ASQ domains. Coincident results were found by Hua et al. (2021) in a study carried out with a huge sample of 137,530 Chinese preschoolers between 3 and 5 years of age who ranged from very and moderately preterm (< 34 gestational weeks) to post-term (< 41 gestational weeks). The authors found that the mean scores obtained in the five domains assessed with the ASQ-3 increased as the GA of the children grew. There was, however, a decrease in the scores obtained by the children born post-term when compared to the term-born children (Hua et al., 2021). Coherently, the percentage of children at risk of developmental delay in the five ASQ-3 domains increased as gestational age decreased, with, again, the exception of post-term children who presented an increase in relation to the FT children. The adjusted risks of GAs (very and moderately preterm, late preterm, early term, and post-term groups) on suspected developmental delays were observed in communication (odds ratios (ORs) were 1.83, 1.28, 1.13, and 1.21, respectively, each p < 0.05), gross motor skill (ORs were 1.67, 1.38, 1.10, and 1.05, respectively, each p < 0.05), and personal-social behavior (ORs were 1.01, 1.36, 1.12, and 1.18, respectively, each p < 0.05) (Hua et al., 2021).

In a study with 52 infants, 12-month-old late preterm (GA of 34–36 weeks) and 156 full-term infants matched for sex, Ballantyne et al. (2016) observed a trend for late preterm infants to be at risk of communication and gross motor delays as measured through the ASQ-3 at 12 months of chronological age. Neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) admission has been found to increase the risk of developmental delay. In another comparative study with 44 late preterm and 44 full-term children, Gutiérrez Cruz et al. (2019) also observed that the late preterm infants had significantly lower scores (p < 0.005) in the dimension of communication of the ASQ-3.

Al-Hindi et al. (2021) administered the Saudi Arabian version of the ASQ-3 to a sample of 61 very preterm children (below 32 weeks) between 18 and 24 months of age. Twenty-six infants (42.6%) had at least one neurodevelopmental delay. The percentages found of children with developmental delays in the different dimensions were the following: communication skills (11.5%), gross motor (11.5%), fine motor (19.7%), problem-solving skills (19.7%), and personal-social skills (23%). Therefore, the domains of personal-social, problem-solving, and fine motor skills seem to be the most affected in Saudi Arabian infants.

Early age evaluations in preterm children are of great importance to identify children at risk of developmental delays and to implement intervention programs (Schults et al., 2013; Vohr, 2016). Parental report instruments such as the CDI or the ASQ-3 may be of great help in this regard.

The primary objective of this study was to compare the language development of three groups of Chilean children at 24 months of age. Three different measures of language development were obtained through the administration of the MacArthur-Bates scales: (1) use of words, (2) sentence complexity, and (3) mean length of the three longest utterances (MLU3). The three groups of children differed in terms of gestational age and in terms of the presence or not of additional biomedical complications: (1) healthy full-term children, (2) very and extremely preterm children with biomedical complications, and (3) very and extremely preterm children without serious biomedical complications. The research questions are as follows: (1) Do full-term children show better language development than the two groups of very and extremely preterm children? (2) Do the extremely and very preterm children with biomedical complications have worse language results than the very and extremely preterm children without biomedical complications?

A secondary aim was to compare other dimensions of psychological development (communication, social interaction, fine motor development, gross motor development, and problem-solving) among the three groups of participants. The research questions are as follows: (1) Do the full-term children show better performance in the five dimensions of psychological development than the two groups of extremely and very preterm children? (2) Do the preterm children with biomedical complications have worse results in the five dimensions of psychological development than the preterm children without biomedical complications?

The hypotheses of the study are as follows:

• Full-term (FT) children will outperform the two groups of very and extremely preterm children in the three language measures.

• High-risk very preterm and extremely preterm infants (HRPT) will perform worse and have a higher incidence of delay in the three measures of language development than the low-risk very and extremely preterm (LRPT) children as well as the full-term children.

• Full-term children will outperform the two groups of preterm children in the other developmental measures obtained through the ASQ-3 (communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social development).

• High-risk very preterm and extremely preterm infants (HRPT) will perform worse and have a higher incidence of delay in the ASQ-3 five developmental measures than the other two groups (low-risk very and extremely preterm (LRPT) and full-term children).

Method

Participants

Three groups of Chilean children were studied at 24 months of age (corrected age for preterm children).

The first group was composed of 42 healthy full-term children (Full-term group: FT), born between 37 and 40 weeks of gestation, and without medical problems. The children were recruited in different preschool centers located in Santiago de Chile, and they were of similar gender distribution and socioeconomic and maternal education levels as the children from the two preterm groups. The FT children obtained an Apgar score at 1 min of 7 points or higher.

The second group consisted of 60 preterm children born below 32 gestational weeks without serious medical complications (low-risk preterm group: LRPT). The children attended a follow-up program for preterm children in the Hospital Roberto del Río and the health center Cordillera Oriente, both of them located in Santiago de Chile. The criteria to include the children in this group were that they had no serious neurological impairment (IVH of grades III or IV, PVL) or lung disease (BPD), that they had an Apgar score in the 1st min of 7 or above, and that they stayed in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) of the hospital for < 30 days after being born.

The third group was composed of 64 children below 32 gestational weeks who had medical complications (high-risk preterm group: HRPT). The children attended a follow-up program for preterm children in the Hospital Roberto del Río and the Health Center Cordillera Oriente, both located in Santiago de Chile. The children of this group were included if they had suffered any of the following biomedical conditions: serious neurological impairment (IVH of grades III or IV, PVL), bronchopulmonary dysplasia, Apgar score in the 1st min below 7 points, or a stay in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) of the hospital of 30 days or longer after being born.

All the children were assessed at 24 months of age (corrected age for the preterm children).

Table 1 displays descriptive data of the three groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive data of the three groups: Mean and (SD)/frequency (%).

Table 2 shows the educational level of the mothers per group. The following categorization was used:

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mothers' educational level per group (frequency).

Level 0: no formal education; Level 1: basic or incomplete primary education; Level 2: complete basic or primary education; Level 3: middle or incomplete secondary education; Level 4: middle or complete secondary education; Level 5: incomplete technical studies; Level 6: complete technical studies; Level 7: incomplete university studies; and Level 8: complete university studies.

The X2 test revealed that there were no significant differences between groups regarding gender distribution (X2 = 1.015; df = 2.2; p = 0.602).

The X2 test indicates that there were no significant differences between groups regarding the educational level of the mothers (X2 = 20.012, df = 16.1, p = 0.220).

Therefore, the three groups could be considered comparable in relation to gender distribution and mother's educational level.

No children with cerebral palsy, metabolic or genetic syndromes, serious motor or sensorial problems, or pervasive developmental delay were admitted to the study.

Instruments

The Mexican Spanish version of the Communicative Development Inventories (CDIs) (Inventario para el Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas: IDHC) (Jackson-Maldonado et al., 2003) was used to assess the linguistic development of the participants. Given the age of the children, the form Palabras y Enunciados (Words and Sentences) for children between 16 and 30 months of age was given to the mothers. The CDI is a parental report instrument that has two parts. The first part, Uso de las Palabras, has two sections: (A) List of words, with a list of 680 words, organized into 23 categories, from which the parents must mark those that are produced by their child, with one point given for each marked word (out of the 680 words that make up the Mexican Spanish version, 666 (98%) have exactly the same form in Chilean Spanish, and 14 words have variations; the person in charge of the application explained their meanings to the parents when necessary); and (B) Cómo usa y comprende el lenguaje el niño (How children use and understand language), which contains five items that assess whether the child talks about situations to refer to the past, present, and future or with respect to absent objects or people and their search. The maximum score for this section is 5 points.

The second part of the Mexican Spanish CDI, Oraciones y Gramática (grammar and sentences), has three sections. The first one, A. Formas de verbos (Verb forms), explores the capacity of the child to produce forms of verbs in present (12 items), past (6 items), and imperative (6 items) for the three verb conjugations existing in Spanish. The maximum possible score is 24. The second section, B. Combinación de palabras (Word combination), asks the parents whether their child already combines words or not. If the answer is yes, the parents must give three examples of the longest utterances their child has recently produced. The mean length of these three longest utterances (MLU3) in words is obtained. If the child does not yet produce word combinations, the parents should stop filling out the inventory. The third part, C. Complejidad de frases (Sentence complexity), consists of 37 pairs of phrases. Both phrases express the same idea, although the second is always more complex (and evolved) from a morphosyntactic point of view. The parents are asked to mark the form that is more similar to that used by their child. One point is given if the parents mark the second option. The maximum possible score is 37 points.

For the purposes of the present research, only the scores obtained in the list of words produced (named use of words), MLU3, and sentence complexity were considered.

The Mexican Spanish CDI has good validity and reliability values (Thal et al., 2000; Jackson-Maldonado et al., 2003).

The Spanish version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire-3 (ASQ-3) (Squires et al., 2009) was used to assess the psychological development of the participants. The form for age 24 months was used. The ASQ-3 is a parental questionnaire that assesses five areas of development: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal social. Each area contains six items assessing different abilities in each domain, which can be scored as 0 (not yet), 5 (sometimes), or 10 (yes). The maximum score for each dimension is 60 points. The score is interpreted according to a normative chart that is included in the questionnaire. According to this chart and the User's Guide, each child can be classified in one of three areas: (1) above the cutoff, when the child's development appears to be on schedule; (2) close to the cutoff, when it is necessary to provide learning activities and monitor; or (3) below the cutoff, when further assessment with a professional may be needed. The validity and reliability of the ASQ-3 reach good values (validity is 0.82 to 0.88, test–retest reliability is 0.91, and inter-rater reliability is 0.92) (Squires et al., 2015). The results obtained through the ASQ-3 are coincident with those obtained through the Bayley III scales (Agarwal et al., 2016; Mackin et al., 2017).

Structured interview. An interview adapted from that used by Pérez-Pereira et al. (2014) explored aspects such as socioeconomic indicators, parental educational level, family composition, health issues of the child and the parents, daily routines, and family history of language problems.

In addition, information on biomedical problems, gestational history, and delivery was also obtained from the records at the hospitals.

Procedure

Approval of the Comité de Ética para Investigaciones en Seres Humanos (Ethics Committee for Research with Human Beings) of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Chile was obtained to carry out this research (resolution No. 1026). Prior informed consent was also given by the participants' parents.

The preterm children were selected from the two previously reported medical centers: Hospital Roberto del Río and the Health Center Cordillera Oriente in Santiago de Chile, where the preterm children were included in a follow-up program. Information about the eligible children was obtained from these centers, and those who fulfilled the age requirements and the rest of the inclusion criteria previously specified were included.

The full-term participants that presented the age requirements and inclusion criteria were chosen from preschool centers in the north area of Santiago de Chile.

During the second semester of 2019, the instruments were administered in person. From the year 2020, because of the COVID-19 pandemic, the instruments were administered remotely (through email, SMS, WhatsApp, or video call). Previously, the mother (in most cases) or the father of each child was called by phone to arrange the modality of contact. In this first call, information on the study and instructions on how to proceed were given to the parents.

The first author carried out the assessment. The assessment took place when the children were 24 months old (±15 days), using the chronological age for the full-term children and the corrected age for the preterm children.

The parents filled out the two questionnaires (Mexican Spanish CDI and ASQ-3) and the structured interview.

Analysis performed

Analyses of variance (ANOVA) were performed to compare the scores obtained by the three groups of children (FT, LRPT, and HRPT) in the three measures taken from the CDI—that is, use of words or word production, MLU3, and sentence complexity—and the five measures obtained through the ASQ-3. The SPSS-28 was used for the analyses.

In addition, and solely for discussion purposes, Pearson's correlations between the three language measures (IDHC) and the five measures of psychological development (ASQ-3) were obtained.

Results

Table 3 shows the results obtained in the three measures of the IDHC taken under consideration.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. ANOVA comparisons among groups in the scores of the CDI [mean and (SD)].

As can be observed, the ANOVA results indicate that there are significant differences among the groups in the use of words, MLU3, or sentence complexity. A post hoc Bonferroni test (p < 0.05) reveals that those differences are due to the significantly higher results obtained by the FT children in relation to those obtained by the HRPT and the LRPT groups; there are no significant differences, however, between the HRPT and the LRPT groups in the use of words, MLU3, or sentence complexity. The relatively higher results obtained by the LRPT children in comparison with the HRPT children do not reach significance.

The results obtained in the ASQ-3 are shown in Table 4, together with the results of the ANOVA.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. ANOVA comparisons among groups in the scores of the ASQ-3 [mean and (SD)].

The area of communication is where the differences between preterm children and full-term children are the highest. The difference between the FT group, on the one hand, and the HRPT and the LRPT groups, on the other hand, is highly significant, and the effect size (η2) reaches nearly 19%. The Bonferroni post hoc test confirms that the difference found in the ANOVA is due to the significantly higher result obtained by the FT children in relation to both the HRPT and the LRFT children (p < 0.001).

In the areas of fine motor development and problem-solving capacity, there are no significant differences among the three groups, although, in this case, the group with the best results is the LRPT and not the FT group.

In the area of gross motor development, the LRPT group obtains the highest results, but now the ANOVA value reaches significance. The Bonferroni post hoc test indicates that there are significant differences between the LRPT and the HRPT groups and between the LRPT and FT groups (p < 0.001).

Finally, the ANOVA results indicate that there are significant differences between the groups in the area of personal-social development. The post hoc Bonferroni test confirms that the FT group obtains significantly higher results than the HRPT (p < 0.001) and that the LRPT group obtains significantly higher results than the HRPT group. The difference between the FT and the LRPT groups does not reach significance.

The two groups of PT children also differed in birth weight and gestational age. In order to check whether these differences would have affected the results, we have introduced these variables as covariates in univariate general linear models. Dependent variables use of words, MLU3, and sentence complexity (obtained through the CDI) and communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal social (obtained through the ASQ-3) were successively introduced. Belonging to the LRPT or HRPT group has been used as an independent variable (fixed factor). Birth weight and gestational age were introduced as covariates. The results show that gestational age does not affect any of the results, while birth weight only affects gross motor skills and personal-social development. The pattern of results regarding the independent variable (LRPT vs. HRPT group) does not change in relation to the results of the ANOVA test reported in Tables 3, 4, with the sole exception of personal-social development, which when introducing the covariates does not show a significant difference between the LRPT and HRPT groups (F = 1.532, p = 0.218).

Considering the interpretation of the mean scores of each group in the different ASQ-3 areas, the area of communication is clear, that is, in which the two groups of preterm children obtain the worse results. In relation to the limit score in this area (which is 25.17), the FT children are clearly above the cutoff, while the HRPT and the LRPT children are close to the cutoff and would need monitoring and additional learning activities to help their development. In the four remaining areas, the scores obtained by the three groups are above the cutoff, with the children showing age-appropriate performance.

Spearman's correlations are shown in Table 5. The results clearly show that the correlations between the ASQ-3 communication scores and the IDHC scores (use of words r = 0.735, MLU3 r = 0.800, and sentence complexity = 0.511) are the highest (p < 0.001).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Pearson's correlations (bilateral) between IDHC and ASQ-3 measures.

Discussion

In relation to the first objective of the study, the FT group obtained significantly higher scores in the use of words, MLU3, and sentence complexity than the other two PT groups: HRPT and LRPT, as Table 3 shows. These results agree with those found in other studies comparing FT and very and extremely preterm children through the CDI (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2004; Sansavini et al., 2006, 2011; Foster-Cohen et al., 2007; Gayraud and Kern, 2007; Stolt et al., 2007, 2009, 2012, 2017; Schults et al., 2013; Sentenac et al., 2020; Tulviste et al., 2020). Therefore, we can say that hypothesis 1 is confirmed. The effect size (η2) found in the use of words (0.354) was higher than the effect size obtained in the two morphosyntactic measures: MLU3 (0.212) and sentence complexity (0.260). This is probably because morphosyntactic development is barely developed at the age of 24 months, and, logically, the differences found are lower than in lexical development.

The second hypothesis is only partially confirmed; however, because even though the differences between the HRPT group and the FT group are clearly significant in the three language measures, the differences between the HRPT and the LRPT groups do not reach significance. These results do not match those found in other studies, which point to a negative effect of biomedical complications on language development (Singer et al., 2001; Ohgi et al., 2005; Rvachew et al., 2005; Sherlock et al., 2005; Anderson and Doyle, 2006; Short et al., 2007; Resic et al., 2008; Luu et al., 2009; Sansavini et al., 2011; Phalen et al., 2012; Srinivasakumar et al., 2013; Soares et al., 2017; Gallini et al., 2021; Katz et al., 2022; Vohr, 2022). Few exceptions to this widespread pattern are available. Although the children from the LRPT group obtained higher mean scores than the children from the HRPT group in the use of words (71.93 vs. 57.27), MLU3 (1.69 vs. 1.51), and sentence complexity (0.92 vs. 0.81, respectively), these differences are not significant. Probably, the reduced number of participants in the sample makes it difficult to find significant differences. Furthermore, we have used the criteria of counting the number of risk circumstances (from 1 to 5), and the combinations of these biomedical risks could vary. We have not analyzed the effect of singular risk conditions (such as having BPD or IVH of grades III and IV) because the number of children who suffered from them was rather limited. In addition, many children from the HRPT group presented comorbidities, and two or more biomedical risks were present.

The comparison of the results obtained by the three groups in the five areas of psychological development assessed by the ASQ-3 indicates that hypothesis 3 is only partially confirmed. The area of communication is the only one in which the FT group obtains significantly higher results than the two groups of very and extremely PT children (see Table 4). This result agrees with those found by other studies (Ballantyne et al., 2016; Gutiérrez Cruz et al., 2019; Hua et al., 2021), which point to a higher risk of delay of PT children in communication. In this area, the effect size (η2) is higher (0.187).

Along the same line, the results obtained in personal-social development seem to support the hypothesis that FT children should have higher results than the LRPT and the HRPT groups. The FT group obtained the highest results, followed by the LRPT group, and finally the HRPT group. In this case, significant differences were found between the FT and the HRPT groups but not between the FT and the LRPT groups. This result is, for the most part, coincident with those found by other studies (Al-Hindi et al., 2021; Hua et al., 2021), although the mean scores obtained by all the groups are above the cutoff.

In the area of fine motor development, however, there were no significant differences among the groups, and hypothesis 3 has not been supported. The group of LRPT children even obtained the highest scores in this dimension, although no significant differences were found. This result does not agree with that found by Al-Hindi et al. (2021), who found a relatively high percentage (19.7%) of very preterm children who showed developmental delays in fine motor development. In any case, the mean scores of all the groups are appropriate to their age.

Similar results were found for problem-solving. Although the group of LRPT children obtained the highest results, no significant differences were found among the groups. This result is not in agreement with what has been found in other studies (Kerstjens et al., 2012; Al-Hindi et al., 2021). Again, the mean scores of the three groups were above the cutoff, and no developmental risk has been found.

Finally, the results observed in gross motor development are in contradiction with hypothesis 3 because the LRPT group obtained results that were significantly higher than those of the FT group and, even more so, than those of the HRPT group. In any case, no group seemed to be at risk of developmental delay in this area, since the mean scores of all the groups were in the normal range, according to ASQ-3 norms. The results obtained in gross motor development in our study are dissonant with those studies that found that PT children obtained significantly lower results than FT children (Ballantyne et al., 2016; Hua et al., 2021).

In relation to hypothesis 4, as expected, the HRPT group obtained lower results than the FT group in the five areas of development explored by the ASQ-3, although these differences reach significance in only communicative and personal-social development. The HRPT group also obtained lower results than the LRPT group in all the areas explored, although those differences reach significance only in gross motor development.

Clearly, communicative development seems to be the area most affected by prematurity. The development of both the HRPT and LRPT children in the remaining areas seems to unfold according to expectations. Curiously, LRPT children have even higher results than the FT children in gross motor, fine motor, and problem-solving development, although only in gross motor development do differences reach significance.

Therefore, the results obtained with the ASQ-3 do not support the idea that very and extremely preterm children show delays in all areas of development at 24 months of age, as suggested by other studies carried out with 4-year-old children (Kerstjens et al., 2012; Hua et al., 2021). Their development seems to follow normal patterns in gross motor, fine motor, problems solving, and personal-social development, particularly if these PT children do not have biomedical complications. The improvement in the care provided to PT children in the hospitals in the last years might be also related to the results found.

The high correlations found between the results of communication in the ASQ-3 and the use of words MLU3 and sentence complexity in the CDI reinforce the idea that language and communication are particularly affected in very and extremely preterm children regardless of whether they present medical complications or not.

Conclusion

The extremely and very preterm children studied obtained significantly lower results than the full-term children in all language and communicative measures taken at 24 months of age. Contrary to expectations, the biomedical risk factors have not shown any significant effect on the development of language and communication, although HRPT children obtained the lowest results in all measures.

The development of other areas is not so much affected by preterm birth and LRPT, and HRPT children seem to develop according to expectations. LRPT children obtain higher results than the HRPT and even the FT children in certain areas of development. This indicates that biomedical complications seem to have a detrimental effect on development, particularly gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social development.

One limitation of the present research is the relatively reduced number of participants in the study. It is possible that differences between the HRPT and the LRPT groups could appear, particularly in language and communication, if the number of participants was larger. In the same vein, the singular effect of different biomedical complications (including necrotizing enterocolitis) could be studied with a larger number of participants.

One strength of the present research is that two groups of very and extremely preterm children, with and without biomedical complications, were studied.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité de Ética de Investigación en Seres Humanos (CEISH) Facultad de Medicina Universidad de Chile. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

VV-M contributed to conception and design of the study and performed assessments of all study participants. BD-V organized the database and performed the statistical analysis. MP-P contributed to the conception and design of the study and the analysis and interpretation of the results by critically reviewing it and wrote the draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript review, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Agarwal, P. K., Shi, L., Daniel, L. M., Yang, P. H., Khoo, P. C., Quek, B. H., et al. (2016). Prospective evaluation of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire 3rd Edition in very-low-birthweight infants. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. 59, 484–489. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Hindi, M. Y., Almahdi, B. H., Alasmari, D. A., Alwagdani, R. K., Hunjur, W. M., Khalel, A. F., et al. (2021). Screening for neurodevelopmental delay for preterm very low birth weight infants at tertiary care center in Saudi Arabia. Cureus 13, e20092. doi: 10.7759/cureus.20092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P., and Doyle, L. (2008). Cognitive and educational deficits in children born extremely preterm. Semin. Perinatol. 32, 51–58. doi: 10.1053/j.semperi.2007.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P. J., and Doyle, L. W. (2006). Neurodevelopmental outcome of bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Semin. Perinatol. 30, 227–232. doi: 10.1053/j.semperi.2006.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballantyne, M., Benzies, K. M., McDonald, S., Magill-Evans, J., and Tough, S. (2016). Risk of developmental delay: Comparison of late preterm and full-term Canadian infants at age 12 months. Early Human Develop. 101, 27–32. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, I. S., Weiss, B. A., Litman, F. R., Ahronovich, M. D., and Baker, R. (2014). Latent mean differences in executive function in at-risk preterm children: The delay-deficit dilemma. Neuropsychology 28, 541–551. doi: 10.1037/neu0000076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhutta, A. T., Cleves, M. A., Casey, P. H., Cradock, M. M., and Anand, K. J. S. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 288, 728–737. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.6.728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blencowe, H., Krasevec, J., de Onis, M., Black, R. E., An, X., Stevens, G. A., et al. (2019). National, regional, and worldwide estimates of low birthweight in 2015, with trends from 2000: a systematic analysis. Lancet Global Health 7, e849–e860. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30565-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casiro, O. G., Moddemann, D. M., Stanwick, R. S., Panikkar-Thiessen, V. K., Cowan, H., and Cheang, M. S. (1990). Language development of very low birth weight infants and fullterm controls at 12 months of age. Early Hum. Dev. 24, 65–77. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(90)90007-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattani, A., Bonifacio, S., Fertz, M., Iverson, J. M., Zocconi, E., and Caselli, M. C. (2010). Communicative and linguistic development in preterm children: a longitudinal study from 12 to 24 months. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disor. 45, 162–173. doi: 10.3109/13682820902818870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

da Ribeiro, C., de Pachelli, M. R. O., de Amaral, N. C. O., and Lamônica, D. A. C. (2017). “Habilidades do desenvolvimento de crianças prematuras de baixo peso e muito baixo peso,” in CoDAS (Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia) 29. doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20162016058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., and Bates, E. (2006). The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical Manual. Second Edition. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. doi: 10.1037/t11538-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster-Cohen, S., Edgin, J. O., Champion, P. R., and Woodward, L. J. (2007). Early delayed language development in very preterm infants: Evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. J. Child Lang. 34, 655–675. doi: 10.1017/S0305000907008070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallini, F., Coppola, M., De Rose, D. U., Maggio, L., Arena, R., Romano, V., et al. (2021). Neurodevelopmental outcomes in very preterm infants: The role of severity of Bronchopulmonary Dysplasia. Early Human Develop. 152, 105275. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gayraud, F., and Kern, S. (2007). Influence of preterm birth on early lexical and grammatical acquisition. First Langu. 27, 159–173. doi: 10.1177/0142723706075790

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldenberg, R. L., Culhane, J. F., Iams, J. D., and Romero, R. (2008). Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth. The Lancet 371, 75–84. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60074-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

GuangXi Cooperative Research Group for Extremely Preterm Infants, Li, Y., Meng, D. H., Wei, Q. F., Pan, X. N., Liang, W. H., et al. (2019). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of extremely preterm infants in southern China: A multicenter study. Early Hum. Dev. 133, 5–10. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2019.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez Cruz, N., Torres Mohedas, J., Carrasco Marina, M. L., Olabarrieta Arnal, I., Martín del Valle, F., and García García, M. L. (2019). Desarrollo psicomotor en prematuros tardíos a los dos años de edad: comparación con recién nacidos a término mediante dos herramientas diferentes. Rev. de Neurol. 68, 503–509. doi: 10.33588/rn.6812.2018360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hua, J., Barnett, A. L., Williams, G. J., Dai, X., Sun, Y., Li, H., et al. (2021). Association of gestational age at birth with subsequent suspected developmental coordination disorder in early childhood in China. JAMA Netw. Open 4, e2137581. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.37581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D. J., Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Newton, T., and Conboy, B. (2003). MacArthur Inventarios Del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas. User‘s guide and technical manual. Baltimore, EEUU: Paul H. Brookes.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jansson-Verkasalo, E., Valkama, M., Vainionp,ää, L., Pääkk,ö, E., Ilkko, E., and Lehtihalmes, M. (2004). Language development in very low birth weight preterm children: a follow-up study. Folia Phoniatrica Logopaedica 56, 108–119. doi: 10.1159/000076062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, T. A., Vliegenthart, R. J. S., Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S. H., Leemhuis, A. G., Beuger, S., Blok, G. J., et al. (2022). Severity of bronchopulmonary dysplasia and neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 and 5 years corrected age. J. Pediatrics 243, 40–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2021.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerstjens, J. M., De Winter, A. F., Bocca-Tjerrrtes, I. F., Bos, A. F., and Reijneveld, S. A. (2012). Risk of developmental delay increases exponentially as gestational age of preterm infants decreases: a cohort study at age 4 years. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 54, 1096–1101. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04423.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilbride, H. W., Thorstad, K., and Daily, D. K. (2004). Preschool outcome of less than 801-gram preterm infants compared with full-term siblings. Pediatrics 113, 742–747. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.4.742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilbride, H. W., Vohr, B. R., McGowan, E. M., Peralta-Carcelen, M., Stringer, K., Das, A., et al. (2022). Early neurodevelopmental follow-up in the NICHD neonatal research network: Advancing neonatal care and outcomes, opportunities for the future. Semin. Perinatol. 46, 151642. doi: 10.1016/j.semperi.2022.151642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luu, T., Vohr, B., Schneider, K., Katz, K., Tucker, R., Allan, W., et al. (2009). Trajectories of receptive language development from 3 to 12 years of age for very preterm children. Pediatrics, 124, 333–341. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackin, R., Ben Fadel, N., Feberova, J., Murray, L., Nair, A., Kuehn, S., et al. (2017). ASQ3 and/or the Bayley-III to support clinicians' decision making. PLoS ONE 12, e0170171. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mangin, K. S., Horwood, L. J., and Woodward, L. J. (2016). Cognitive development trajectories of very preterm and typically developing children. Child Dev. 88, 282–298. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, K., Hix-Small, H., Clark, K., and Newman, J. (2009). Lowering developmental screening thresholds and raising quality improvement for preterm children. Pediatrics 123, 1516–1523. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menyuk, P., Liebergott, J. W., and Schultz, M. C. (1995). Early Language Development in Full-Term and Premature Infants (1era ed.). New York: Taylor and Francis Group.c

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nyman, A., Korhonen, T., Munck, P., Parkkola, R., Lehtonen, L., and Haataja, L. (2017). Factors affecting the cognitive profile of 11-year-old children born very preterm. Pediatr. Res. 82, 324–332. doi: 10.1038/pr.2017.64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohgi, S., Akiyama, T., and Fukuda, M. (2005). Neurobehavioural profile of low-birthweight infants with cystic periventricular leukomalacia. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47, 221–228. doi: 10.1017/S0012162205000447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez-Pereira, M. (2021). Prevalence of language delay among healthy preterm children, language outcomes and predictive factors. Children 8, 282. doi: 10.3390/children8040282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez-Pereira, M., and Cruz, R. (2018). A longitudinal study of vocabulary size and composition in low-risk preterm children. First Langu. 38, 72–94. doi: 10.1177/0142723717730484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez-Pereira, M., Fernandez, P., Luisa Gomez-Taibo, M., and Resches, M. (2014). Language development of low-risk preterm infants up to the age of 30 months. Early Hum. Dev. 90, 649–656. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phalen, A. G., Kirkby, S., and Dysart, K. (2012). The 5-minute Apgar score: survival and short-term outcomes in extremely low-birth-weight infants. J. Perinatal Neonatal Nurs. 26, 166–171. doi: 10.1097/JPN.0b013e31825277e9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reidy, N., Morgan, A., Thompson, D. K., Inder, T. E., Doyle, L. W., and Anderson, P. J. (2013). Impaired language abilities and white matter abnormalities in children born very preterm and/or very low birth weight. J. Pediatr. 162, 719–724. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resic, B., Tomasovic, M., Kuzmanic, R., Lozic, M., Resic, J., and Solak, M. (2008). Neurodevelopmental outcome in children with periventricular leukomalacia. Collegium Antropol. 32, 143–147.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rvachew, S., Creighton, D., Feldman, N., and Sauve, R. (2005). Vocal development of infants with very low birth weight. Clin. Lingu. Phonet. 19, 275–294. doi: 10.1080/02699200410001703457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., Alessandroni, R., Faldella, G., Giovanelli, G., and Salvioli, G. (2006). Early relations between lexical and grammatical development in very immature Italian preterms. J. Child Lang. 33, 199–216. doi: 10.1017/S0305000905007208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., Savini, S., Broccoli, S., Justice, L., Alessandroni, R., et al. (2011). Longitudinal trajectories of gestural and linguistic abilities in very preterm infants in the second year of life. Neuropsychologia 49, 3677–3688. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schults, A., Tulviste, T., and Haan, E. (2013). Early vocabulary in full term and preterm Estonian children. Early Hum. Dev. 89, 721–726. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sentenac, M., Johnson, S., Charkaluk, M. L., Seppanen, A. V., Aden, U., Cuttini, M., et al. (2020). Maternal education and language development at 2 years corrected age in children born very preterm: results from a European population-based cohort study. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 74, 346–353. doi: 10.1136/jech-2019-213564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherlock, R. L., Anderson, P. J., and Doyle, L. W. (2005). Neurodevelopmental sequelae of intraventricular haemorrhage at 8 years of age in a regional cohort of ELBW/very preterm infants. Early Hum. Dev. 81, 909–916. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Short, E. J., Kirchner, H. L., Asaad, G. R., Fulton, S. E., Lewis, B. A., Klein, N., et al. (2007). Developmental sequelae in preterm infants having a diagnosis of bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Arch. Pediatr. Adoles. Med. 161, 1082. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.11.1082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simard, M.-N., Luu, T. M., and Gosselin, J. (2012). Concurrent validity of ages and stages questionnaires in preterm infants. Pediatrics 130, e108–e114. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, L. T., Siegel, A. C., Lewis, B., Hawkins, S., Yamashita, T., and Baley, J. (2001). Preschool language outcomes of children with history of bronchopulmonary dysplasia and very low birth weight. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 22, 19–26. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200102000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skellern, C., Rogers, Y., and O'Callaghan, M. (2001). A parent-completed developmental questionnaire: Follow up of ex-premature infants. J. Paediatr. Child Health 37, 125–129. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1754.2001.00604.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, A. C. C., Silva, K., and da Zuanetti, P. A. (2017). Variáveis de risco para o desenvolvimento da linguagem associadas à prematuridade. Audiol. Commun. Res. 22, 1745. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2016-1745

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squires, J., Bricker, D., and Twombly, E. (2015). ASQ: SE-2 Ages and Stages Questionnaires® Socio-Emotional. [Internet]. Brookes Publishing Co. Available online at: https://agesandstages.com (acccessed March 18, 2021).

Google Scholar

Squires, J., Bricker, D. D., and Twombly, E. (2009). Ages and Stages Questionnaires® Third Edition (ASQ3). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. doi: 10.1037/t11523-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasakumar, P., Limbrick, D., Munro, R., Mercer, D., Rao, R., Inder, T., et al. (2013). Posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation-impact on early neurodevelopmental outcome. Am. J. Perinatol. 30, 207–214. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1323581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., and Lehtonen, L. (2009). The early lexical development and its predictive value to language skills at 2 years in very-low-birth-weight children. J. Commun. Disor. 42, 107–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolt, S., Klippi, A., Launonen, K., Munck, P., Lehtonen, L., Lapinleimu, H., et al. (2007). Size and composition of the lexicon in prematurely born very-low-birthweight and full-term Finnish children at two years of age. J Child Lang. 34, 283–310. doi: 10.1017/S0305000906007902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolt, S., Matomaki, J., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., Lehtonen, L., and Pipari Study, G. (2012). The emergence of grammar in very-low-birth-weight Finnish children at two years of age. J Child Lang. 40, 336–357. doi: 10.1017/S0305000911000456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolt, S., Savini, S., Guarini, A., Caselli, M. C., Matomäki, J., Lapinleimu, H., et al. (2017). Does the native language influence lexical composition in very preterm children at the age of two years? A cross-linguistic comparison study of Italian and Finnish children. First Lang. 37, 368–390. doi: 10.1177/0142723717698006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suttora, C., Guarini, A., Zuccarini, M., Aceti, A., Corvaglia, L., and Sansavini, A. (2020). Speech and language skills of low-risk preterm and full-term late talkers: the role of child factors and parent input. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 7684. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17207684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Synnes, A. R., Ling, E. W., Whitfield, M. F., Mackinnon, M., Lopes, L., Wong, G., et al. (1994). Perinatal outcomes of a large cohort of extremely low gestational age infants (twenty-three to twenty-eight completed weeks of gestation). J. Pediatr. 125, 952–960. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(05)82015-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thal, D., Jackson-Maldonado, D., and Acosta, D. (2000). Validity of a parent-report measure of vocabulary and grammar for Spanish-speaking toddlers. J. Speech, Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 1087–1100. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4305.1087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorngren-Jerneck, K., and Herbst, A. (2006). Perinatal Factors Associated with Cerebral Palsy in Children Born in Sweden. Obstetr. Gynecol. 108, 1499–1505. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG.0000247174.27979.6b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulviste, T., Toome, L., Männamaa, M., and Varendi, H. (2020). Language skills at corrected age 2;0 is poorer in extremely and very preterm boys but not girls compared with their full-term peers. Early Hum. Dev. 151, 105164. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vohr, B. (2016). Language and hearing outcomes of preterm infants. Semin. Perinatol. 40, 510–519. doi: 10.1053/j.semperi.2016.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vohr, B. R. (2022). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of premature infants with intraventricular hemorrhage across a lifespan. Semin. Perinatol. 46, 151594. doi: 10.1016/j.semperi.2022.151594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: biomedical complication, preterm children, language acquisition, parental questionnaires, psychological development

Citation: Varela-Moraga V, Diethelm-Varela B and Pérez-Pereira M (2023) Effect of biomedical complications on very and extremely preterm children's language. Front. Psychol. 14:1163252. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1163252

Received: 10 February 2023; Accepted: 05 June 2023;
Published: 28 June 2023.

Edited by:

Maria-José Ezeizabarrena, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Spain

Reviewed by:

Suvi Stolt, University of Helsinki, Finland
Pasquale Rinaldi, National Research Council (CNR), Italy

Copyright © 2023 Varela-Moraga, Diethelm-Varela and Pérez-Pereira. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Virginia Varela-Moraga, vvarela@uchile.cl

Editor's note: Maria-José Ezeizabarrena edited the article in collaboration with Melita Kovacevic, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.