Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 October 2023
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Technology For the Greater Good? The Influence of (Ir)responsible Systems on Human Emotions, Thinking and Behavior View all 11 articles

#Blessed: the moderating effect of dispositional gratitude on the relationship between social comparison and envy on Instagram

  • 1Faculty of Psychology, Institute for Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, Institute of Department of Occupational, Economic, and Social Psychology, Motivation Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 3Division of Psychotherapy and Gerontopsychology, Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria

Introduction: The role of dispositional gratitude as a positive psychological resource and prosocial personality trait in real life interactions militates in favor of its introduction to the research field of social media.

Methods: Based on a literature review of the previously studied relationship of dispositional gratitude with social comparison and envy in offline settings, a twofold moderation model was proposed and quantitatively tested in a cross-sectional sample of N = 268 Instagram users aged between 18 and 40 years. Additionally, the dual conceptualization of benign and malicious envy was scrutinized by validating its respective connections with affective outcomes and inspiration on Instagram.

Results and discussion: Dispositional gratitude serves as a protective factor when using Instagram by significantly mitigating the relationship of social comparison and malicious as well as general envy on Instagram. Furthermore, the results support the more nuanced understanding of envy as a dual construct in the face of social media use.

1. Introduction

Blog posts with titles like “#Blessed: Why it’s not gratitude if you need to advertise it on social media” (Theresa Christine, 2014), “How to practice gratitude without feeling like an Instagram cliché” (Hoff, 2019), and “How gratitude fixed my relationship with Instagram” (Coyne, 2018) indicate the relevance of positive-psychological research for the area of social media. As noted in previous work, social media use can exhibit both positive and negative characteristics with potentially favorable as well as harmful effects on the user’s well-being (Fox and Moreland, 2015; Radovic et al., 2017; Verduyn et al., 2017). Therefore, it is essential to explore the circumstances and mechanisms of both outcomes to promote future beneficial usage behavior. Especially, regarding the increasing number of social media users and the related psychological costs, variables from Positive Psychology show the potential to function as protective factors when using social media (Pantic, 2014; Hansen, 2015; Winata and Andangsari, 2017). For this purpose, the current paper proposes the implementation of gratitude into the research field of social media by exploring its association with social comparison and envy.

1.1. Gratitude and social media

Whereas state gratitude refers to the emotional response to a blessing received, trait gratitude considers a habitual and constant characteristic and tendency to experience gratitude with higher frequency and intensity (Wood et al., 2008). Significant to a broader conception of dispositional gratitude is the propensity to appreciate consciously the positive aspects of life and to consider one’s own abilities and possessions as valuable gifts (McCullough et al., 2002; Wood et al., 2010; Winata and Andangsari, 2017). Regarding the aim of integrating gratitude into the research context of social media, the social component of gratitude is especially relevant. To experience as well as to express gratitude strengthens interpersonal relationships and functions as a social engine and amplifier of prosocial behavior (McCullough et al., 2008; Blickhan, 2018). Inherently, gratitude is considered an intrinsic value and the counterpart of extrinsic demands such as materialism, fame, and possession (Froh et al., 2011). There is a growing body of research on the moderating effect of dispositional gratitude in the context of social variables. In this light, dispositional gratitude was found to downsize negative health consequences associated with a lower socioeconomic status (Hartanto et al., 2018). Dispositional gratitude further serves as a moderator when looking at affective needs and victimization frequency in romantic interpersonal relationships (Griffin et al., 2016). From a meta-analytic point of view, dispositional gratitude is described as an especially meaningful trait in predicting well-being (Portocarrero et al., 2020). Despite the positive effects of gratitude in a variety of domains, its implementation in the research context of social media so far is rather sparse. The existing evidence indicates that the potential of dispositional gratitude as a resource in offline-contexts can be transferred to online-interactions. The reciprocity of gratitude, for instance, applies to an online-context too and is present toward contacts that already existed offline (Godawa et al., 2019). Social media users with higher dispositional gratitude undergo less social comparison and exhibit an overall lower tendency to compare with others socially (Winata and Andangsari, 2017). Moreover, a gratitude intervention on Instagram was found to enhance the users’ dispositional gratitude. Due to its interactive features and the social mechanisms behind sharing pictures, Instagram is considered a platform potentially reinforcing gratitude (Koay et al., 2020). On the downside, the platform focuses on aesthetic enhancement by encouraging the use of filters, which in turn favors a strong positivity norm and impels users to mainly share positive and optimized aspects of their lives. As a result, Instagram promotes passive usage habits and almost continuous social comparison processes (Lup et al., 2015). By fostering social comparison, social media use further relates to feelings of envy and decreased well-being (Krasnova et al., 2013; Lange and Crusius, 2015; Verduyn et al., 2015).

1.2. The concept of envy

Generally, envy refers to the painful experience of the perceived superiority of others and is considered a frequent cause of dissatisfaction and decreased well-being, as it results from aligning one’s self-worth with the comparison to others (Smith, 2007). Theoretically, two conceptualizations of envy can be differentiated: in the unitary approach, envy genuinely entails feelings of pain and hostility and yet possibly triggers positive as well as negative reactions. To do justice to this twofold characteristic of envy, the dual approach distinguishes two different forms of envy. Accordingly, benign envy aims at reducing the superiority of the envied person by motivating them to catch up, whereas malicious envy does so by downgrading, disparaging, and harming the envied (Crusius et al., 2020). Therefore, benign envy is accompanied by active emulation and malicious envy comes along with aggression (Lange et al., 2018). Empirical research as well as factor analyses reveal two inherent factors with weak intercorrelations (rs = −0.07 to 0.32; Lange and Crusius, 2015), hinting at the existence of two forms of envy (Crusius et al., 2020).

To come to terms with this recent debate, we define envy here in a general form as well as in its dual specification for a more differentiated perspective. As previously noted, and generally implemented in social media research, the consideration of the envy subtypes permits an in-depth analysis and reflects the different social media usage outcomes as potentially harmful or beneficial for the users (van de Ven, 2016; Wu and Srite, 2021). Benign envy induces positive outcomes such as inspiration (Meier and Schäfer, 2018), elevated goal setting, better performance (Lange and Crusius, 2015) and positive affect (van de Ven et al., 2009; Braun et al., 2018; Meier and Schäfer, 2018). Malicious envy is associated with “schadenfreude” (Lange et al., 2018), counterproductive work methods (Braun et al., 2018), hostility (Crusius et al., 2020), and negative emotions (Braun et al., 2018). These connections are not applicable to the other form of envy, respectively (Crusius et al., 2020).

When transferring the dual understanding of envy to the research area of gratitude, the findings emphasize the complexity of envy as a psychological phenomenon: Gratitude is defined as a counterpart of and an incompatible characteristic to envy (McCullough et al., 2002). The dual conception of envy allows a more detailed view, with gratitude being a positive predictor of benign and a negative predictor of malicious envy (Xiang et al., 2018).

1.3. The current study

Considering that social media use contributes on the one hand positively and on the other hand negatively to users’ well-being, it is especially relevant to expand our knowledge on traits and behaviors inhibiting negative usage habits and promoting positive outcomes (Hansen, 2015). Dispositional gratitude might help to gain new insights into individual usage styles. The relationship between gratitude and social comparison as well as envy suggests that dispositional gratitude might be decisive in whether social media use leads to positive rather than negative consequences. To test this assumption empirically, a twofold moderation model is proposed.

Current research shows that both types of envy are closely associated with social comparison processes (Meier and Schäfer, 2018). Dispositional gratitude is related negatively to the social comparison orientation of social media users (Winata and Andangsari, 2017) and in addition, serves as a positive predictor of benign and as a negative one of malicious envy (Xiang et al., 2018). Taken together, these findings strongly support the assumptions of our moderator model. We therefore assume that dispositional gratitude positively moderates the relationship of social comparison and benign envy and negatively that of social comparison and malicious envy. As a complementary post hoc analysis, we explore the moderating effect of dispositional gratitude on the relationship of social comparison and general envy on Instagram. In the context of our model, social comparison processes can operate in two ways, as a general and a specific phenomenon. The first one targets a common trait and the second one relates to Instagram use in specific. For validating the dual conception of envy, we look at their relationships with inspiration on Instagram and positive and negative emotions, supposing that benign envy positively predicts inspiration and positive affect and malicious envy shows no relationship with inspiration and a positive one with negative affect.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Sample

The required sample size for detecting a small to medium effect (f2 = 0.1) with a given power of 0.8 was computed using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2009), which recommended a minimum number of 114 individuals. In view of range restriction in moderation analyses and in orientation toward existing studies, we collected ad hoc data of 303 Instagram users, which resulted in a sample size of 268 Instagram users after the exclusion of incomplete datasets and four cases which completed the survey in less than 4 min. The observed power for each model was computed and can be found on OSF.1 Following the inclusion criteria of young and emerging adulthood (18–40 years; Berk, 2011), our sample shows a mean age of 22.84 years (SD = 3.85, min. = 18, max. = 39). Addressing participants in this younger age segment ensured a representation of Instagram users that are especially concerned with self-branding and impression-management, which in turn makes social comparison processes more prevalent (Alfasi, 2019). Further recruitment criteria included people at least occasionally using Instagram and exhibiting sufficient German language skills for questionnaire handling. The majority of the resulting sample identified as female (63.05%) followed by male (36.19%) and two participants identifying as diverse (non-binary; 0.74%). We did not distinguish between gender and sex and did not measure the alignment between the two in this study. Future studies, aiming to target sex and/or gender differences regarding our hypothesis should include the respective measures to allow respective comparisons. Additional descriptive data on the sample is presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive details on sample (N = 268).

2.2. Measures

To test our hypotheses, we collected data from each participant with six psychometric scales. Gratitude was measured using the Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ-6; McCullough et al., 2002) in its German adaptation (Samson et al., 2011), using a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). It showed acceptable reliabilities in earlier studies (McCullough et al., 2002: α = 0.82; Samson et al., 2011: α = 0.73).

To record individuals’ general tendency for social comparison, we used the 11-item Iowa-Netherlands Comparison Orientation Measure (INCOM; Gibbons and Buunk, 1999) in its German version by Schneider and Schupp (2011). Social comparison tendency was assessed on a five-point Likert Scale (1 = I disagree strongly, 5 = I agree strongly), with a reliability (Cronbach’s α) ranging from 0.78 to 0.85.

For registering social media specific comparison processes we added the six-item Social Comparison Tendency on Facebook Scale (COM-F; Steers et al., 2014), translated into German and modified for Instagram by Meier and Schäfer (2018). It is referenced here as COM-I (Cronbach’s α = 0.85; Steers et al., 2014), using a five-point Likert scale as well.

Additionally, participants completed the Benign and Malicious Envy Scale (BeMaS; Lange and Crusius, 2015) for benign (α = 0.84–0.90) and malicious (α = 0.84–0.91) envy subtypes and general envy by calculating the total score. The envy scales were scored using a five-point Likert Scale (1 = I disagree strongly, 5 = I agree strongly). To gather Instagram specific envy, we applied the respective German adaptation by Meier and Schäfer (2018).

For registering inspiration, we used the Inspiration Scale (Thrash and Elliot, 2003) in its German adaptation (Meier and Schäfer, 2018) on a five-point Likert scale, showing Cronbach’s α’s between 0.90 and 0.95 (Thrash and Elliot, 2003).

As our final psychometric measure, we applied the widely used Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988; German version by Breyer and Bluemke, 2016) with α = 0.86 for both subscales. Again, a five-point Likert scale was used (1 = very slightly or not at all, 5 = extremely) to assess positive and negative affect with ten items each.

2.3. Procedure

Mainly, participants were recruited via social media, where an invitation and the link to the online-study were spread. No compensation for participation was offered. First of all, participants were asked to give informed consent for participation. Only then could they continue with filling out the respective questionnaires. The measures were presented in a fixed order to prevent differences in mutual interference of the measures for each participant. Contact details were presented in case any questions arose.

2.4. Data analysis

We excluded all participants with incomplete data or overly fast completion time (4 min). From 303 initially sampled individuals, 268 remained in the final sample. We initially calculated descriptive statistics and Cronbach’s α reliability coefficients for all scales and subscales used. To deliver a general oversight on the relationship between measurements, we obtained all correlations (see text footnote 1). To conduct the planned analysis, we tested the predictability of social orientation on envy and its subscales (benign and malicious) as well as the moderating role of gratitude on the relationship. This was done, using the general social orientation measure (INCOM) as well as the social media specified measure (COM-I). This procedure resulted in six models, which are described in Tables 4, 5. Wherever significant, the moderation is plotted, using simple slope analysis (Figures 1 (3). Last, we tested the consequences of benign and malicious envy on inspiration as well as positive and negative affect. Note that the term prediction is used non-causal in this article as the data does not allow for immediate causal conclusions. Rather, the term is used to describe explained variance. All reported regression coefficients are z-standardized.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Simple slope analysis of gratitude on general social comparison. Full line (–1 SD) and fine-dotted line (+1 SD).

2.5. Open science statement

The current study was not pre-registered, yet we follow the open science recommendations of open data and open code, which can be obtained from see text footnote 1.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive statistics, reliabilities, and social media usage

Table 2 summarizes the descriptive statistics of the applied scales as well as their coefficient alphas. Apart from the GQ-6, all scales showed satisfactory reliability. The alpha of the scale states a limitation to the presented analysis.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive details on applied scales.

Analysis of broad items regarding social media usage showed that 47.01% indicated to use it to gather information, 62.68% to interact with others, 88.06% for entertainment, and 49.63% for inspiration or motivation. Further items included distraction (54.47%), self-presentation (19.40%), keeping up-to-date (41.41%) as well as being creative (22.38%). The wording of items on additional information of Instagram usage style was chosen following Meier and Schäfer (2018).

Participants have been asked how much time they spend on Instagram per day (subjective estimation) which was answered by 265 participants. A total of 10.18% indicated 10 min or less, 24.53% reported 11–30 min, 33.96% said 31–60 min, 21.50% 1–2 h, 9.81% 2–3 h, and 0.02% more than 3 h per day.

As summarized in Table 3, participants were most likely to post self-related content (people and travel). The category people included selfies, group pictures, and portraits.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Posted content categories by frequency in percentage (%).

3.2. Part I: predictability of envy and its facets

Following the planned analysis, overall envy as well as its subscales was predicted by the general tendency for social comparison. This prediction was significant and positive in all three models, indicating that social comparison tendencies relate toward higher experiences of envy (Table 4). For general and malicious envy, gratitude is negatively related to envy experiences, indicating a reversed relationship against social comparison tendencies. For malicious envy, social comparison tendencies and gratitude interacted on their effect on envy experience.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Prediction of envy by general social comparison tendencies (INCOM).

The simple slope analysis in Figure 1 indicates that individuals with higher mean gratitude (full-line) experience less malicious envy than individuals with less gratitude (fine-dotted line).

When using the social media specific measure of social comparison (COM-I), we observed a very similar effect in directionality but higher explained variance across the models (Table 5). Again, social comparison predicted all facets of envy positively, yet meaningfully stronger. Gratitude moderated the relationship for general as well as malicious envy in the same directionality as before, where individuals with higher trait gratitude experience less envy (Figures 2, 3). It is noteworthy that there was no observed direct effect of gratitude but solely through the moderation of the effect of social comparison tendencies on envy.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Prediction of envy by social media specific social comparison processes (COM-I).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simple slope analysis of gratitude on social media specific social comparison and general envy. Full line (–1SD) and fine-dotted line (+1SD).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Simple slope analysis of gratitude on social media specific social comparison and malicious envy. Full line (–1SD) and fine-dotted line (+1SD).

3.3. Part II: consequences of envy facets on inspiration and affect

To test the consequences of envy experience, we investigated the predictive value of benign and malicious envy on scales for inspiration as well as positive and negative affect (Table 6). The estimates underline the divergence of the envy dimensions. While inspiration and positive affect are negatively predicted by malicious envy, it is positively related to negative affect, indicating individuals who experience malicious envy tend to also experience less inspiration, less positive affect and more negative affect. On the other side, benign envy positively predicts inspiration and positive affect and was neutral to negative affect.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Prediction of inspiration and affect by envy facets.

4. Discussion

4.1. Summary

In the present study, we investigated the relationship between social comparison tendencies and envy as well as the moderating role of dispositional gratitude. We observed that comparison tendencies and envy are related, on the overall level as well as by envy-facets (malicious and benign). Dispositional gratitude moderated this relationship, depending on the perspective on social comparison tendencies and envy conceptualization. Following, we explored the consequences of benign and malicious envy on subjective experiences of inspiration, positive and negative affect. This illustrates that malicious envy specifically holds potential for negative consequences.

4.2. Dispositional gratitude as a resource for social media use

When faced with the destructive aspects of Instagram use such as social comparison and envy, our findings highlight the potential of dispositional gratitude to serve as a buffer variable in this context. According to our results, dispositional gratitude significantly alters the relationship between social comparison and general/malicious envy on Instagram. Thus, gratitude can be considered a desirable trait for social media users. Therefore, the importance of promoting dispositional gratitude by interventions is no longer limited to real-life interactions, but transferrable to online-contacts too. Our work paves the way for further studies in this field, as it is crucial to shed light on the circumstances and individual characteristics that determine whether social media use results in favorable or else harmful outcomes. For this purpose, the moderation model proposed can be extended to complementary research questions.

4.2.1. Specificity of social media directed comparison behavior

To our surprise, we observed differing results between the measures for social comparison. The predictive power (see β-values) underlines the stronger effect of social media specific comparison. In addition, only the effect of COM-I on general envy and malicious envy was moderated by individual gratitude. This might illustrate the relevance of context specific measures. As the INCOM measures the general tendency to socially compare oneself in the sense of an underlying traitlike variable, the scale was found to be a subject to socially desirable response behavior (Schneider and Schupp, 2011). The instructions and wording of the COM-I on the other hand refer to more state like reactions to opportunities for social comparison on Instagram specifically. Therefore, our findings form an interesting starting point for further investigations on media-specific measures as well as on the coherence of scales targeting the trait- vs. state-dimension.

4.2.2. Differing effects of envy expression and corresponding effects on other resources

Concerning the continuing debate on how to conceptualize envy, we decided for an interim solution by differentiating between benign and malicious envy subtypes, but also investigating envy as a whole. Still, in the second part of our analysis we aimed at contributing to the discussion by validating the differing envy subtypes via their affective consequences and inspiration on Instagram. Benign envy positively predicted positive affect as well as inspiration on Instagram, while malicious envy positively predicted negative affect and negatively predicted inspiration. In combination with the findings of our moderation models, these results indicate the relevance of the dual envy conception. As our results show, different forms of envy are associated with opposing outcomes in terms of affect and inspiration. Omitting the analysis of envy facets would have resulted in a loss of information. Hence, our study suggests that the dual specification of envy allows an in-depth perspective on the data and enables the derivation of accurate conclusions on the harmful or else beneficial potential of envy. On that basis, not all manifestations of envy are to be considered detrimental ultimately.

4.3. Limitations and prospect

To capture the diverse realities of social media users, future research should focus on broader recruiting modalities, extending our approach to a more inclusive sample in terms of gender, age, and education. Generally, self-assessing online-surveys are prone to social desirability responding bias (van de Mortel, 2008). A more experience-based assessment method using online gratitude interventions could potentially lead to less biased responses. Another limitation is the low reliability of the GQ-6 in its German translation. This calls for supplementary validation studies on the GQ-6 in German samples.

Moreover, it has yet to be clarified how envy should effectively be conceptualized and measured. Based on the missing consensus, we see our method of analyzing subscales and the entire scale separately as adequate. Yet, we see that the construction as a two-factor latent construct or other measurement approaches could be valid as well.

To allow for a generalization of our findings, including other social media platforms is of further interest. Future studies in this field should supplement existing research by comparing different social network sites. Furthermore, the statistical model of moderation is inherently correlational and bidirectional, but theoretically deemed causal. Hence, it would be highly desirable for future research to implement an experimental or longitudinal design, as the moderating effect of gratitude is so far exclusively rooted in thorough theoretical derivation of presumed causality (Baron and Kenny, 1986; Wu and Zumbo, 2008).

5. Conclusion

Dispositional gratitude is a key trait variable in the context of social media use and a worthwhile research extension to that field. The high variance explained by the proposed moderation model emphasizes the role of gratitude as a protective factor for Instagram users. Future research should compare the moderating effect of gratitude on social media relevant variables before and after conducting online gratitude interventions. Additionally, the inclusion of clinically relevant variables such as depression or media addiction could be of interest for the extension of our model. By mitigating the relationship of social comparison and envy on Instagram, dispositional gratitude is a valuable resource for social media users. The enhancement of trait-gratitude through social-media-based intervention studies is therefore highly encouraged.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/supplementary material.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Departmental Review Board, University of Vienna. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SK conceptualized the study, chose the methodology, collected and analyzed the data, and wrote the original draft as well as the revisions. LR curated and analyzed the data, and wrote and reviewed the manuscript. A-RL supervised the project and reviewed and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article, going through multiple feedback loops and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research received no grants or funding for the implementation and realization of the study. The open access publication fee was covered by the University of Vienna.

Acknowledgments

The content of the manuscript is based upon SK’s thesis at the University of Vienna.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://osf.io/qm9g5?view_only=d7596b60e9f648438612474233a97f78..

References

Alfasi, Y. (2019). The grass is greener on my friends’ profiles: The effect of Facebook social comparison on state self-esteem and depression. Pers. Individ. Differ. 147, 111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berk, L. E. (2011). Entwicklungspsychologie. München: Pearson Studium.

Google Scholar

Blickhan, D. (2018). Positive Psychologie: Ein Handbuch für die Praxis. Paderborn: Junfermann Verlag.

Google Scholar

Braun, S., Aydin, N., Frey, D., and Peus, C. (2018). Leader narcissism predicts malicious envy and supervisor-targeted counterproductive work behavior: Evidence from field and experimental research. J. Bus. Ethics 151, 725–741. doi: 10.1007/s10551-016-3224-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breyer, B., and Bluemke, M. (2016). Deutsche version der positive and negative affect schedule PANAS (GESIS Panel). Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher items und skalen. Leibniz: Leibniz-Institut für Sozialwissenschaften. doi: 10.6102/zis242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coyne, M. (2018). How gratitude fixed my relationship with Instagram. Verily. Available online at: https://verilymag.com/2018/09/how-gratitude-fixed-my-relationship-with-instagram (accessed January 17, 2023).

Google Scholar

Crusius, J., Gonzalez, M. F., Lange, J., and Cohen-Charash, Y. (2020). Envy: An adversarial review and comparison of two competing views. Emot. Rev. 12, 3–21. doi: 10.1177/1754073919873131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, J., and Moreland, J. J. (2015). The dark side of social networking sites: An exploration of the relational and psychological stressors associated with Facebook use and affordances. Comput. Hum. Behav. 45, 168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froh, J. J., Emmons, R. A., Card, N. A., Bono, G., and Wilson, J. A. (2011). Gratitude and the reduced costs of materialism in adolescents. J. Happiness Stud. 12, 289–302. doi: 10.1007/s10902-010-9195-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbons, F. X., and Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: Development of a scale of social comparison orientation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 129–142. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godawa, G., Gurba, E., Rzewucka, P., Tabor, M., and Gurba, K. (2019). “The use of social media and the youth’s experience of gratitude,” in Proceedings of the 13th International Technology, Education and Development Conference, Valencia. doi: 10.21125/inted.2019.0890

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffin, B. J., Garthe, R. C., Worthington, E. L. Jr., Sullivan, T. N., Larsen, R., Lavelock, C. R., et al. (2016). How positive processes function in negative relationships: Dispositional gratitude moderates the association between affective need and frequency of dating violence victimization. J. Posit. Psychol. 11:4.

Google Scholar

Hansen, C. (2015). Gender differences in gratitude, rumination, and social comparison through Facebook (Publication No. 3713944) [dissertation]. Scottsdale: Northcentral University.

Google Scholar

Hartanto, A., Lee, S. T. H., and Yong, J. C. (2018). Dispositional gratitude moderates the association between socioeconomic status and interleukin-6. Sci. Rep. 9:802. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37109-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, V. (2019). How to practice gratitude without feeling like an Instagram cliché. Yahoo! life. Available online at: https://www.yahoo.com/lifestyle/practice-gratitude-without-feeling-instagram-060000727.html (accessed January 17, 2023).

Google Scholar

Koay, S.-H., Ng, A.-T., Tham, S.-K., and Tan, C.-S. (2020). Gratitude intervention on Instagram: An experimental study. Psychol. Stud. 65, 168–173.

Google Scholar

Krasnova, H., Wenninger, H., Widjaja, T., and Buxmann, P. (2013). “Envy on Facebook: A hidden threat to users’ life satisfaction?,” in Proceedings of the International Conference on Wirtschaftsinformatik, Leipzig, Germany, Leipzig.

Google Scholar

Lange, J., and Crusius, J. (2015). Dispositional envy revisited: Unraveling the motivational dynamics of benign and malicious envy. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 284–294. doi: 10.1177/0146167214564959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, J., Weidman, A. C., and Crusius, J. (2018). The painful duality of envy: Evidence for an integrative theory and a meta-analysis on the relation of envy and schadenfreude. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 114, 572–598. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lup, K., Trup, L., and Rosenthal, L. (2015). Instagram #instasad?: Exploring associations among Instagram use, depressive symptoms, negative social comparison, and strangers followed. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 18, 247–252. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., and Tsang, J. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 112–127. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M. E., Kimeldorf, M. B., and Cohen, A. D. (2008). An adaptation for altruism: The social causes, social effects, and social evolution of gratitude. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 281–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00590.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, A., and Schäfer, S. (2018). The positive side of social comparison on social network sites: How envy can drive inspiration on Instagram. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 21, 411–417. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantic, I. (2014). Online social networking and mental health. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 652–657. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portocarrero, F. F., Gonzalez, K., and Ekema-Agbaw, M. (2020). A meta-analytic review on the relationship between dispositional gratitude and well-being. Pers. Individ. Differ. 164:110101. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radovic, A., Gmelin, T., Stein, B. D., and Miller, E. (2017). Depressed adolescents’ positive and negative use of social media. J. Adolesc. 55, 5–15. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samson, A. C., Proyer, R. T., Ceschi, G., Pedrini, P. P., and Ruch, W. (2011). The fear of being laughed at in Switzerland: Regional differences and the role of positive psychology. Swiss J. Psychol. 70, 53–62. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, S., and Schupp, J. (2011). The social comparison scale: Testing the validity, reliability, and applicability of the Iowa-Netherlands comparison orientation measure (INCOM) on the German population. SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research. Berlin: DIW.

Google Scholar

Smith, R. H. (2007). “Envy,” in Encyclopedia of social psychology, eds R. F. Baumeister and K. D. Vohs (Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications), 306–307.

Google Scholar

Steers, M.-L. N., Wickham, R. E., and Acitelli, L. K. (2014). Seeing everyone else’s highlight reels: How Facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 33, 701–731. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2014.33.8.701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theresa Christine (2014). #Blessed: Why it’s not gratitude if you need to advertise it on social media. Elite Daily. Available online at: https://www.elitedaily.com/life/culture/why-its-not-gratitude-if-you-need-to-advertise-it-on-social-media/595070 (accessed January 17, 2023).

Google Scholar

Thrash, T. M., and Elliot, A. J. (2003). Inspiration as a psychological construct. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 871–889. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van de Mortel, T. F. (2008). Faking it: Social desirability response bias in self-report research. Aust. J. Adv. Nurs. 25:4.

Google Scholar

van de Ven, N. (2016). Envy and its consequences: Why it is useful to distinguish between benign and malicious envy. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 10, 337–349. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van de Ven, N., Zeelenberg, M., and Pieters, R. (2009). Leveling up and down: The experiences of benign and malicious envy. Emotion. 9, 419–429. doi: 10.1037/a0015669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Brayer, J., et al. (2015). Passive facebook usage undermindes affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. J. Exp. Psy. Gen. 144, 480–488. doi: 10.1037/xge0000057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., and Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 11:1. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 54, 1063–1070.

Google Scholar

Winata, C., and Andangsari, W. W. (2017). Dispositional gratitude and social comparison orientation among social media users. Humaniora. 8:3. doi: 10.21512/humaniora.v8i3.3620

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. M., Froh, J. J., and Geraghty, A. W. A. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 890–905. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. M., Maltby, J., Stewart, N., Linley, P. A., and Joseph, S. (2008). A social-cognitive model of trait and state levels of gratitude. Emotion. 8, 281–290. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, A. D., and Zumbo, B. D. (2008). Understanding and using mediators and moderators. Soc. Indic. Res. 87, 367–392. doi: 10.1007/s11205-007-9143-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, J., and Srite, M. (2021). Envy on social media: The good, the bad and the ugly. Int. J. Inf. Manage 56:102255. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2020.102255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiang, Y., Chao, X., and Ye, Y. (2018). Effect of gratitude on benign and malicious envy: The mediating role of social support. Front. Psychiatry 9: 139.

Google Scholar

Keywords: dispositional gratitude, social comparison, benign envy, malicious envy, social media, Instagram

Citation: Kaminger S, Roth LHO and Laireiter A-R (2023) #Blessed: the moderating effect of dispositional gratitude on the relationship between social comparison and envy on Instagram. Front. Psychol. 14:1159999. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1159999

Received: 06 February 2023; Accepted: 02 October 2023;
Published: 20 October 2023.

Edited by:

Andrea Szymkowiak, Abertay University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Danielle Sulikowski, Charles Sturt University, Australia
Andree Hartanto, Singapore Management University, Singapore

Copyright © 2023 Kaminger, Roth and Laireiter. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Leopold Helmut Otto Roth, leopold.roth@univie.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.