Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 28 March 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Parental Mentalization: New Frontiers View all 8 articles

Editorial: Parental mentalization: New frontiers

  • 1Faculty of Educational Sciences, Department of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
  • 2Yale School of Medicine, Child Study Center, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 3Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Turku, Turku, Finland

Editorial on the Research Topic
Parental mentalization: New frontiers

Central to navigating social interactions is the ability to interpret and predict behaviors through understanding the underlying thoughts, feelings, and emotions of the self and others—referred to as mentalization (Fonagy et al., 1991). Early attachment relationships are thought to lay the foundation for mentalization (Fonagy et al., 2002), and disruptions in mentalization have been associated with psychopathology (Fonagy and Luyten, 2009). Importantly, mentalization may be dynamic and relationally specific. Parental mentalization is thought to play a critical role in child development, scaffolding the child's own ability to regulate their emotions to support the later emergence of their own ability to mentalize (Slade, 2005). Infants are limited in their non-verbal communicative bids. Consequently, parents may need to flexibly adapt their mentalizing capacity to understand infant affective cues to sensitively respond to their child, supporting the emerging security of the early attachment relationship. Importantly, intervention research has shown that mentalization-based work with at-risk parents have a greater potential to enhance parent-child interactions and greater attachment security in comparison with psycho-educational approaches (Suchman et al., 2017).

Despite parental mentalization being a core theme of clinical work, it remains a relatively new construct in the scientific literature, with many exciting avenues for research. Due to demanding and time-consuming methodology in the measurement of parental mentalization, sample sizes in empirical research have been limited. Therefore, there is great need to develop more feasible, but accurate, measures of parental mentalization that can be scaled to larger samples. There is also significant heterogeneity in measures of parental mentalization that have primarily focused on mothers, and not fathers, limiting the generalization of this work. With respect to intervention studies, more use of controlled research designs, especially randomized clinical trials designs is needed alongside longitudinal approaches. It is also important to broaden the focus of child developmental outcomes, including empathy and the child's own capacity to mentalize. Our goal in this Research Topic was to showcase the most essential advances and research projects currently going on in the field of parental mentalization.

Parental mentalizing: Research presented in this Research Topic

The Research Topic consists of seven papers, with 36 authors included: four original research articles, one systematic review, and two brief research reports. One of the emergent themes of the papers presented are the varying approaches to measuring parental mentalization, which has often been operationalized as parental reflective functioning: an overt manifestation of the mental processes that underpin the capacity to mentalize at an individual level as a parent as well as when interacting with the child (Nijssens et al., 2020). Historically, the assessment of parental mentalization has been captured by interview-based approaches that are then scored by reliable coders for the level of reflective functioning that parents express through the course of the interview. The two main interview approaches have been the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI, George et al., 1984) and the Parent Development Interview (PDI, Slade et al., 2004). Both the AAI and PDI have been designed to assess parental capacity of mentalizing via a transcribed, semi-structured interview. However, while PDI questions focus on current parenting of the child, the AAI focuses on the childhood experiences. Traditionally, researchers implement either the AAI or the PDI; however, in the current issue, Flykt et al., employed both measures to examine parental reflective functioning. Their central finding was that reflective functioning as measured by the AAI and reflective functioning as measured by the PDI seem to be partly distinct constructs of a broader parental mentalizing capacity, and therefore could be uniquely targeted in interventions. These findings are valuable, as research has not yet fully differentiated the interconnections and different outcomes of reflective functioning in the context of these two interview approaches.

Two further papers in the Research Topic separately examined the AAI and PDI in clinical samples. In the paper presented by Rosso, parental RF was assessed using the AAI among parents who were known to have maltreated their children. These parents were found to have severe impairments in their ability to mentalize upon their own childhood experiences. In most cases, their reflective capacity was not only absent, but they systematically resisted taking a reflective stance. Suboptimal mentalizing capacity assessed by the PDI was also found among mothers using substances who had a childhood history of their own mother's substance abuse and mental illness (Lowell et al.). Somewhat surprisingly, mothers with childhood experiences of their own mother's mental illness and substance use had higher levels of PRF in comparison to mothers with a background of maternal substance use alone.

Although largely considered gold-started assessments of parental mentalization, interviews designed to assess PRF are time consuming to administer and interpret, and therefore attempts have been made to develop “off-line” self-report instruments, such as the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ, Luyten et al., 2017), to make it possible to evaluate parental RF in large samples. However, self-report questionnaires are limited in their ability to catch the rich and multilevel concept of PRF and may be limited by responder bias.

Questionnaire measures also need to be carefully evaluated in different cultural contexts. With this in mind, work by Ye et al. in this issue successfully implemented the PRFQ in Chinese parents. The confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the Chinese version of the PRFQ with 12 items is psychometrically sound and can be applied to populations in China. Furthermore, this issue also includes the application the PRFQ self-report questionnaire in assessing the early unfolding of maternal mentalization for the first time in a large Finnish cohort (Lindblom et al.). Importantly, it was found that the “high” maternal mentalization profile in early parenthood was associated with high child socio-emotional competence at the age of two years, including the child's capacity for empathy.

A more recent advance in the measurement of parental mentalization that examines non-verbal behavior, instead of relying on parents' verbal expressions, is presented in this issue. Shai evidenced the positive impact of parental embodied mentalization on children's cognitive and language development in a longitudinal study. Such findings are important as they indicate the predictive value of both verbal and non-verbal approaches in the assessment parental mentalization.

Finally, in their systematic review included in this issue, Stuhrmann et al. explored how PRF is associated with parenting behaviors incorporating only studies using the PDI. Based on their review they concluded that most of the associations indicated a positive effect of PRF on parenting quality further emphasizing parental mentalization as a critical construct for parent and child development. However, the complex interaction between PRF and contextual factors emphasizes the need for differentiation of PRF dimensions.

Perspectives on future research

In conclusion, this Research Topic provides more insight to the ongoing research activity in the field of parental mentalization, highlighting its importance, as well as its methodological development. In the future it will be important to better understand and identify the different forms and aspects of parental mentalizing, as well as different and overlapping dimensions between different measurement approaches. This will be especially valuable for designing and interpreting the results of intervention studies that are designed to enhance parental mentalization.

Author contributions

MK and HR wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MP commented and added to sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

Editor MP, who contributed to this editorial paper, has contributed to two of the original research papers in this special issue. Thus she has not been involved in the review processes.

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., and Target, M. (2002). Affect Regulation, Mentalization and the Development of the Self. New York, NY: Other Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Luyten, P. (2009). A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 1355–1381. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., and Higgitt, A. C. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: the reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Ment. Health J. 12, 201–218.

Google Scholar

George, C., Kaplan, N., and Main, M. (1984). The Adult Attachment Interview. Berkeley, CA: University of California at Berkeley. doi: 10.1037/t02879-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luyten, P., Mayes, L. C., Nijssens, L., and Fonagy, P. (2017). The parental reflective functioning questionnaire: development and preliminary validation. PLoS ONE 12, e0176218. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijssens, L., Vliegen, N., and Luyten, P. (2020). The mediating role of parental reflective functioning in child social–emotional development. J. Child Fam. Stud. 29, 2342–2354. doi: 10.1007/s10826-020-01767-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: an introduction. Attachment Hum. Dev. 7, 269–281. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slade, A., Aber, J. L., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., and Kaplan, M. (2004). The Parent Development Interview—Revised. New York, NY: The City University of New York.

Suchman, N. E., DeCoste, C. L., McMahon, T. J., Dalton, R., Mayes, L. C., and Borelli, J. (2017). Mothering from the inside out: results of a second randomized clinical trial testing a mentalization-based intervention for mothers in addiction treatment. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 617–636. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: parental mentalization, reflective functioning, child development, maltreatment, embodied mentalization, treatment outcome

Citation: Kalland M, Rutherford H and Pajulo M (2023) Editorial: Parental mentalization: New frontiers. Front. Psychol. 14:1154250. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1154250

Received: 30 January 2023; Accepted: 10 March 2023;
Published: 28 March 2023.

Edited and reviewed by: Peter Klaver, Interkantonale Hochschule für Heilpädagogik (HfH), Switzerland

Copyright © 2023 Kalland, Rutherford and Pajulo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mirjam Kalland, bWlyamFtLmthbGxhbmQmI3gwMDA0MDtoZWxzaW5raS5maQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.