Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 16 June 2023
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Emotional Antecedents and Consequences of Social Rejection View all 10 articles

How does disgust regulate social rejection? a mini-review

John A. Terrizzi
John A. Terrizzi1*Richard S. PondRichard S. Pond2Trevor C. J. ShannonTrevor C. J. Shannon2Zachary K. KoopmanZachary K. Koopman2Jessica C. ReichJessica C. Reich2
  • 1Department of Psychology, Texas Woman’s University, Denton, TX, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, Wilmington, NC, United States

The need to belong is a fundamental aspect of human nature. Over the past two decades, researchers have uncovered many harmful effects of social rejection. However, less work has examined the emotional antecedents to rejection. The purpose of the present article was to explore how disgust––an emotion linked to avoidance and social withdrawal––serves as an important antecedent to social rejection. We argue that disgust affects social rejection through three routes. First, disgust encourages stigmatization, especially of those who exhibit cues of infectious disease. Second, disgust and disease-avoidance give rise to cultural variants (e.g., socially conservative values and assortative sociality), which mitigate social interaction. Third, when the self is perceived as a source of contamination, it promotes shame, which, subsequently, encourages withdrawal from social interaction. Directions for future research are also discussed.

Introduction

Humans are intensely social. People enjoy immense benefits, both psychologically and physically, from their participation in and maintenance of positive relationships (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). Consequently, social rejection serves as a serious threat to our mental and physical well-being. Although much is known about the consequences of rejection, including poorer self-control, increased self-defeating and aggressive behavior, and even physiological responses associated with pain (e.g., Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010; DeWall et al., 2011; Eisenberger, 2011; Leary, 2015), less is well-known about its antecedents. In the present article, we review empirical evidence that suggests disgust––an emotion linked to avoidance and social withdrawal––is an important precursor to rejection. We argue that disgust is directly and indirectly linked to social rejection through a few key pathways.

We obtained articles for this review by searching the PsychINFO database and Google Scholar for published articles, using the terms: “disgust OR parasite stress OR disease threat OR behavioral immune system” AND “rejection OR exclusion OR ostracism OR avoidance.” These broad terms allowed us to cast a wide net to identify relevant literature regarding the influence of disgust on rejection.

The adaptive value of social connection

Long before dating apps and Facebook, our early ancestors faced a world much harsher than our own, in which pursuing a solitary existence posed grave danger (Buss, 2008). To offset the risks of solitude, early humans formed small communities wherein members helped each other to survive (e.g., hunting, foraging, building shelter, defense from physical threats, childcare duties; Trivers, 1971; Eastwick, 2009). Given how critical group-living was for our ancestors, they required psychological mechanisms for detecting and responding to social threats (Leary and Downs, 1995; Kurzban and Leary, 2001; Leary, 2001; Chester et al., 2012; Wesselmann et al., 2012). For instance, social pain likely evolved concurrently with early societies to promote survival (MacDonald and Leary, 2005). Studies examining neural responses to socially painful events have largely supported a model of common neural substrates for detecting physical and social pain (see Eisenberger, 2011 for a comprehensive review). Psychological mechanisms for detecting social threats are even sensitive to non-verbal cues (e.g., averted eye gaze) that may warn of potential rejection (Wirth et al., 2010).

Rejection as a social strategy likely developed alongside early group living and has been molded by selective pressures to satisfy certain adaptive needs (Williams, 1966; Cosmides and Tooby, 1994; Kurzban and Leary, 2001). Of course, our brains would likely require certain mechanisms through which to address the specific issues emerging from sociality (Wesselmann et al., 2012). Disgust is potentially one mechanism for strategically triggering social rejection to avoid costly group members (or trigger social withdrawal when the costly group member is oneself).

Disgust as an antecedent to rejection

Here, we discuss three routes by which disgust affects social rejection. First, disgust encourages stigmatization (see Oaten et al., 2011). It encourages avoidance and rejection of others (Park et al., 2003; Faulkner et al., 2004; Navarrete and Fessler, 2006; Park et al., 2006; Terrizzi et al., 2010), especially those who exhibit cues of infectious disease (Van Leeuwen and Petersen, 2018). Second, disgust and disease-avoidance (i.e., parasite stress) give rise to cultural variants (e.g., socially conservative values and assortative sociality; see Fincher and Thornhill, 2012; Terrizzi et al., 2013; Thornhill and Fincher, 2014), which mitigate social interaction. Third, disgust can promote self-isolation. When the self is perceived as a source of contamination (i.e., self-disgust; see Overton et al., 2008), it promotes shame (Terrizzi and Shook, 2020), which encourages withdrawal from social interaction (Tangney et al., 1996). We preface our discussion of these routes by outlining the adaptive challenge of infectious disease and its evolutionary solutions (i.e., the physiological immune system and the behavioral immune system).

The adaptive challenge of infectious disease

Infectious diseases present an adaptive challenge for humans. Like all living organisms, infectious agents (e.g., viruses, bacteria) are in the business of survival and reproduction. However, their reproductive success can make us sick or even die. Consequently, humans and pathogens are locked in an evolutionary arms race (see Nesse and Williams, 1994). Pathogens are evolving new methods of decoding our security system, and we are evolving new tactics for fending them off. One of our lines of defense is the physiological immune system (PIS). When an infectious agent enters the human body, the PIS produces antibodies that recognize specific portions of the pathogens, bind to them, and, hopefully, inactivate them (Delves and Roitt, 2000). Although the PIS is an effective tool for combating pathogens, it can be costly, sometimes resulting in friendly fire (i.e., attacking the very body that it is designed to protect). Fortunately, the PIS is not the only solution to the problem of infectious disease. We are also equipped with psychological mechanisms that help promote disease-avoidance.

The behavioral immune system

The behavioral immune system (BIS) is the first line of defense against infectious disease (Schaller, 2006). It is a suite of psychological mechanisms that promote disease-avoidance. The goal of the BIS is to limit exposure to infectious agents. It helps us avoid situations, people, and objects that would increase the likelihood of infection exposure. The BIS accomplishes this by triggering adaptive cognitive (e.g., infection specific thoughts), affective (e.g., disgust), and behavioral responses (e.g., avoidance, repulsion), which collaborate to produce prophylactic responses to cues of infectious disease (Schaller and Duncan, 2007).

The embodied cognitive nature of disgust

Disgust is a key component of the BIS. It is a cross-cultural human emotion (Ekman, 1970) that is believed to have originated in our ancestral past as a means of distinguishing healthy and edible items from those that may be dangerous (Rozin and Fallon, 1987). As such, the facial expression accompanying disgust results in flaring nostrils, squinted eyes, protruding tongue, and a gaping mouth, which aid in minimizing disease exposure (Rozin et al., 1994). Though there is cross-cultural variability in the triggers of disgust, some triggers like bodily by-products (e.g., blood, feces, vomit) seem to be cross-culturally universal (Curtis and Biran, 2001; Curtis et al., 2011). This is not all surprising, given how such substances often spread diseases. Indeed, pictures that are disease-relevant (e.g., resembling bodily fluids) are more cross-culturally evocative of disgust than those that are not disease-relevant (e.g., blue slime; Curtis et al., 2004).

From an evolutionary perspective, psychological systems that are designed to solve adaptive challenges are not always accurate. Rather, they promote the avoidance of errors that are the most reproductively costly (see Haselton and Buss, 2000). In the case of the BIS, this means that individuals will be more vulnerable to Type I errors (i.e., perceiving an object as a disease threat when it is not; Oaten et al., 2009). As a result, people are prone to “magical contagion” (Rozin et al., 1992). For example, participants will avoid eating fudge that is shaped like dog feces (Rozin et al., 1986).

Conceptually, disgust is a system that is turned on and off by environmental triggers (i.e., cues of infectious disease). When individuals are exposed to a disgusting object (e.g., rotten meat), it elicits disgust, which encourages disease-avoidant behavior. The salience of bodily by-products as a universal disgust elicitor is indicative of the role that human-to-human contact plays in the transmission of infectious disease. Contagious diseases are often spread by incidental contact with bodily by-products (e.g., respiratory droplets). Given the prevalence of this route of infectious exposure, it follows that disgust will have implications for human social interaction. In the next few sections, we will discuss how disgust and disease-avoidance cause stigmatization and avoidance of others.

Disgust and stigmatization

Disgust is an avoidant emotion (Cottrell and Neuberg, 2005). Consequently, its influence on social behavior should be indicative of social conservatism, rejection, and avoidance of others (especially those who are different). Evidence of the avoidant nature of disgust and its impact on social behavior can be seen in the aggression literature. Disgust is negatively associated with physical aggression (i.e., approach-oriented aggression; Pond et al., 2012) but positively associated with relational aggression (e.g., rejection; Molho et al., 2017).

One of the ways disgust encourages avoidance of others is through the mechanism of stigma. Stigmatization is the process of categorizing groups or individuals based on undesirable characteristics, both physical (e.g., morphological differences) and moral (e.g., norm violations), as a means of segregation and avoidance (see Goffman, 1963). From a disease-avoidance perspective, stigma can be conceptualized as a strategy for decreasing the probability of exposure to infectious disease by limiting contact with “contaminated” groups (see Oaten et al., 2011). As disgust makes us prone to Type I errors, the effect that disgust has on stigmatization will default toward false positives. Thus, groups or individuals that pose no disease-threat will be avoided.

The disease-avoidant nature of stigma impacts perceptions of disease-threat. For instance, stigmatized others (e.g., out-group members) are often blamed for the onset of epidemics (Oaten et al., 2011). Stigma also has vicious long-lasting downstream effects on social identity. Stigma is often placed on individuals or groups because of strong feelings of disgust and avoidance (Major and O’brien, 2005; Oaten et al., 2011). Once a particular group has been stigmatized, the mere label (i.e., social categorization) of that group can confer contamination concerns. Thus, the label itself can metaphorically contaminate those to whom it is applied.

Interestingly, although stigma leads to social rejection by out-group members, stigmas can also drive a stronger sense of association with in-group members (Major and O’brien, 2005; Oaten et al., 2011). When our sense of self is threatened and we are rejected by others, we seek to repair that by finding support from other members of our own, stigmatized group and relying more on the group identity to depersonalize the offense (Crocker et al., 1991).

Disgust and in-group/out-group bias

Because other people are a significant source of contamination, humans attend to morphological differences that could signal disease-threat (disease cues: runny nose, swelling; Duncan et al., 2009). Some evidence suggests that attention to such cues can even trigger immunological responses that help prepare the body for disease (Schaller et al., 2010). Individuals who are particularly concerned with infectious disease show an over-perception of disease threat (i.e., perceiving and recalling disease cues where there were none; Miller and Maner, 2012).

As disgust is believed to be a disease-avoidance mechanism, it follows that it would trigger avoidance and rejection of those who exhibit cues of infectious disease (Van Leeuwen and Petersen, 2018). However, its effect on social interaction is not limited to those who display cues of infectious disease. Disgust seems to cause an in-group/out-group bias, such that it encourages avoidance of out-group members and, reciprocally, a greater affinity for in-group members. Disgust and disease-avoidant concerns are associated with prejudice and avoidance of a wide variety of out-group members, including foreigners, sexual minorities, and obese individuals (Park et al., 2003; Faulkner et al., 2004; Navarrete and Fessler, 2006; Terrizzi et al., 2010). In addition to its impact on interpersonal prejudice and avoidance, disgust seems to have a large impact on cultural values.

Cultural quarantining

Culture plays an important role in the defense against infectious disease. Parasite stress theory suggests that historic exposure to infectious disease affects the evolution of cultural value systems (see Thornhill and Fincher, 2014). In areas of the world in which there are higher rates of infectious disease and more life lost due to infectious disease, there should be more orderliness and strict adherence to social norms. Indeed, regions with higher rates of infectious disease exhibit more constraints on high-risk behaviors (e.g., sexual behaviors, drug use; Fincher et al., 2008; Schaller and Murray, 2008), tend to be more collectivistic (i.e., a cultural orientation that encourages in-group cohesion and adherence to social norms; Guernier et al., 2004; Fincher et al., 2008), and experience more religiosity and assortative sociality (i.e., preference for similar others; Fincher and Thornhill, 2012).

The potential prophylactic value of tight cultures was also observed during the COVID-19 pandemic. Tight cultures (i.e., those that are more orderly and have less crime) exhibited lower mortality rates and less prevalence of COVID-19 (Gelfand et al., 2021). Likewise, power distance (i.e., the extent to which subordinates accept the power of authority figures) and institutional collectivism (which are both associated with norm adherence) were predictive of lower rates of COVID-19 morbidity and mortality (Kumar, 2021).

Not only are regional differences in parasite stress correlated with conservative cultural values (e.g., collectivism, adherence to social norms), but so too are individual differences in disgust sensitivity and concern about infectious disease (Terrizzi et al., 2013). Those who are more sensitive to disgust and chronically concerned about disease-threat are more likely to report higher levels of socially conservative values (e.g., right-wing authoritarianism, xenophobia, religious fundamentalism). Additionally, these value systems seem to function as a means of discouraging interaction with out-groups by promoting in-group assortative sociality (Terrizzi et al., 2010, 2012, 2014).

Shame as self-directed disgust

Other people are not the only object of our disgust. Humans are self-conscious beings and, just as we make evaluations of others, we make evaluations of ourselves (i.e., self-esteem). Therefore, we can experience self-disgust, which has severe socioemotional consequences (e.g., depression and anxiety; Overton et al., 2008). Here we demonstrate how internalized disgust (i.e., self-disgust) can lead to self-stigmatization and self-rejection.

One of the consequences of self-reflected disgust is shame. Shame is a negatively valenced self-conscious emotion that results in global self-condemnation (Tangney, 1991; Niedenthal et al., 1994). In the case of self-disgust, global condemnation is perceiving the self as a source of contamination.

Though little research has explored the relation between disgust and shame, there is some preliminary evidence for their association. Evidence suggests that perceiving facial expressions of disgust can trigger increased shame (Giner-Sorolla and Espinosa, 2011). Specifically, across two cultures (i.e., the United Kingdom and Spain), participants primed with pictures depicting facial expressions of disgust reported more shame than guilt, and participants who saw angry faces reported more guilt than shame.

Not only does perceiving facial expressions of disgust induce shame, but those more sensitive to disgust and have a greater fear of contamination are more vulnerable to shame. In a series of studies, disgust sensitivity and fear of contamination were associated with shame but not guilt, and priming individuals with disgust increased shame but not guilt in individuals who were sensitive to disgust (Terrizzi and Shook, 2020).

Just as disgust stymies social interaction as a means of limiting exposure to infectious disease, so too may shame. Shame and disgust have similar behavioral features. They both encourage avoidance and social withdrawal. Just as disgust and disease-threat encourage behavioral avoidance (Faulkner et al., 2004; Navarrete and Fessler, 2006), shame that results from moral transgressions encourages avoidance of social interaction (Orth et al., 2006; Schmader and Lickel, 2006). Likewise, both shame and disgust seem to be involved in the maintenance of social norms. They are both described as moral emotions, which encourage moral behavior and adherence to social norms (Haidt, 2003; Tangney et al., 2007), and they are both associated with moral decision-making (Tangney et al., 2007; Schnall et al., 2008). Furthermore, deficiencies in both shame and disgust are associated with psychopathy (i.e., an antisocial disregard for social norms; Morrison and Gilbert, 2001; Tangney et al., 2003; Tybur et al., 2009).

Because disgust and shame both encourage social withdrawal (Faulkner et al., 2004; Navarrete and Fessler, 2006; Orth et al., 2006; Schmader and Lickel, 2006), it is likely that intense feelings of both emotions will precede and coincide with feeling lonely, rejected, and socially disconnected. That is, disgust should promote feelings of shame, which, in turn, increase perceptions of rejection.

Discussion

Humans are tremendously social. Yet, the ironic consequence of this sociality is that social rejection is ubiquitous. It occurs everyday, and no one is immune to its harmful influence. To neatly encapsulate all the reasons for which rejection occurs is an ambitious endeavor. Humans evolved to avoid poor social exchange partners, favor their in-group (and exclude or exploit out-group members), and avoid contact with those who may be differentially likely to carry communicable pathogens. In each case, the tendency to exclude others confers survival advantages. The present review supports the idea that disgust plays an important role in the social rejection experience. Not only does this emotion trigger the avoidance of costly group members, but, when directed inward, it can result in shame, self-condemnation, and social withdrawal.

Although there is strong theoretical and empirical evidence that suggests that both disgust and shame play a critical role in human social rejection, there is room for further research. For instance, one limitation is that there is a dearth of experimental work demonstrating the extent to which disgust induces feelings of shame, and less still that identifies shame as a precursor to self-rejection and avoidance of others. Future work would benefit from manipulating disgust (both generally as well as self-disgust) and shame in the laboratory and then measuring their impact across multiple measures of rejection (both toward others and oneself).

Furthermore, we conceptualized disgust and shame as important antecedents of rejection; however, the bidirectionality of the relations is unclear. Some theoretical and empirical work has identified shame as a potential consequence of rejection (Leary, 2015; Wang et al., 2020). However, it appears that rejection does not modulate the disgust experience (Antico et al., 2018). Moreover, no work that we are aware of has identified disgust as a consequence of rejection, but rather a trigger (Park et al., 2003; Faulkner et al., 2004; Navarrete and Fessler, 2006; Terrizzi et al., 2010). Yet, the social rejection experience is complicated. The rejection literature is replete with experimental studies that focus on between-person differences within a single laboratory session. Consequently, only a few studies have examined how the experience of rejection develops within individuals over time (e.g., Nezlek et al., 2012). This is another significant limitation of the extant literature. Future work would benefit from exploring the day-to-day emotional experiences that unfold and coincide with perceived rejection and related phenomena (e.g., feelings of loneliness and disconnection, discrimination, ostracism).

Finally, the literature is dominated by Western samples of college students that are predominately White and female. Thus, it is difficult to know the extent to which results can be generalized to other populations. Future work would benefit from obtaining more diverse community samples, as well as greater cross-cultural representation.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Antico, L., Guyon, A., Mohamed, Z. K., and Corradi-Dell'Acqua, C. (2018). Beyond unpleasantness. Social exclusion affects the experience of pain, but not of equally-unpleasant disgust. Cognition 181, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R., and Leary, M. (1995). The need to belong. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (2008). “Human nature and individual differences: evolution of human personality” in Handbook of personality: Theory and research. eds. O. P. John, R. W. Robins, and L. A. Pervin (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 29–60.

Google Scholar

Chester, D. S., Pond, R. S. Jr., Richman, S. B., and DeWall, C. N. (2012). The optimal calibration hypothesis: how life history modulates the sensitivity of the brain's social pain network. Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (1994). “Origins of domain specificity: the evolution of functional organization” in Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture. eds. L. Hirschfeld and S. German (New York: Cambridge University Press), 85–116.

Google Scholar

Cottrell, C., and Neuberg, S. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 770–789. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., and Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: the affective consequences of attributional ambiguity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 218–228. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, V., Aunger, R., and Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 271, 131–133. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, V., and Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease. Perspect. Biol. Med. 44, 17–31. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2001.0001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, V., De Barra, M., and Aunger, R. (2011). Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 389–401. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delves, P. J., and Roitt, I. M. (2000). The immune system. N. Engl. J. Med. 343, 37–49. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200007063430107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeWall, C. N., Deckman, T., Pond, R. S. Jr., and Bonser, I. (2011). Belongingness as a core personality trait: how social exclusion influences social functioning and personality expression. J. Pers. 79, 1281–1314. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00695.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., and Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personal. Individ. Differ. 47, 541–546. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eastwick, P. W. (2009). Beyond the pleistocene: using phylogeny and constraint to inform the evolutionary psychology of human mating. Psychol. Bull. 135, 794–821. doi: 10.1037/a0016845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). “The neural basis of social pain: findings and implications” in Social pain: Neuropsychological and health implications of loss and exclusion. eds. G. MacDonald and L. A. Jensen-Campbell (American Psychological Association), 53–78.

Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1970). Universal facial expressions of emotion. California Mental Health Research Digest 8, 151–158.

Google Scholar

Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J., and Duncan, L. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 7, 333–353. doi: 10.1177/1368430204046142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fincher, C., and Thornhill, R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: the cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behav. Brain Sci. 35, 61–79. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11000021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., and Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 275, 1279–1285. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, M. J., Jackson, J. C., Pan, X., Nau, D., Pieper, D., Denison, E., et al. (2021). The relationship between cultural tightness–looseness and COVID-19 cases and deaths: a global analysis. Lancet Planetary Health 5, e135–e144. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30301-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giner-Sorolla, R., and Espinosa, P. (2011). Social cuing of guilt by anger and of shame by disgust. Psychol. Sci. 22, 49–53. doi: 10.1177/0956797610392925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goffman, I. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall Inc.

Google Scholar

Guernier, V., Hochberg, M. E., Guégan, J. F., and Harvey, P. (2004). Ecology drives the worldwide distribution of human diseases. PLoS Biol. 2, 0740–0746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haidt, J. (2003). “The moral emotions” in Handbook of affective sciences. eds. R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, and H. H. Goldsmith (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 852–870.

Google Scholar

Haselton, M., and Buss, D. (2000). Error management theory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 81–91. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., and Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 7:e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, R. (2021). Impact of societal culture on COVID-19 morbidity and mortality across countries. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 52, 643–662. doi: 10.1177/00220221211025100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurzban, R., and Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: the functions of social exclusion. Psychol. Bull. 127, 187–208. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leary, M. R. (2001). “Towards a conceptualization of interpersonal rejection” in Interpersonal rejection. ed. M. R. Leary (Oxford, England: Oxford University Press), 4–20.

Google Scholar

Leary, M. R. (2015). Emotional responses to interpersonal rejection. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 17, 435–441. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2015.17.4/mleary

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leary, M. R., and Downs, D. L. (1995). “Interpersonal functions of the self-esteem motive: the self-esteem system as a sociometer” in Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem. eds. M. R. Leary and D. L. Downs (New York: Plenum Press), 123–144.

Google Scholar

MacDonald, G., and Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social rejection hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychol. Bull. 131, 202–223. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Major, B., and O’brien, L. T. (2005). The social psychology of stigma. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 393–421. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, S., and Maner, J. (2012). Overperceiving disease cues: the basic cognition of the behavioral immune system. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 1198–1213. doi: 10.1037/a0027198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molho, C., Tybur, J., Güler, E., Balliet, D., and Hofmann, W. (2017). Disgust and anger relate to different aggressive responses to moral violations. Psychol. Sci. 28, 609–619. doi: 10.1177/0956797617692000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, D., and Gilbert, P. (2001). Social rank, shame and anger in primary and secondary psychopaths. J. Forensic Psychiatry 12, 330–356. doi: 10.1080/09585180110056867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, C. D., and Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: the effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 270–282. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesse, R. M., and Williams, G. C. (1994). Why we get sick: the new science of Darwinian medicine. New York: Vintage.

Google Scholar

Nezlek, J. B., Wesselmann, E. D., Wheeler, L., and Williams, K. D. (2012). Ostracism in everyday life. Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 16, 91–104. doi: 10.1037/a0028029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niedenthal, P. M., Tangney, J. P., and Gavanski, I. (1994). “If only I weren’t” versus “if only I hadn’t”: distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 585–595. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychol. Bull. 135, 303–321. doi: 10.1037/a0014823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I. (2011). Disease avoidance as a functional basis for stigmatization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 3433–3452. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orth, U., Berking, M., and Burkardt, S. (2006). Self-conscious emotions and depression: rumination explains why shame but not guilt is maladaptive. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 1608–1619. doi: 10.1177/0146167206292958

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overton, P. G., Markland, F. E., Taggart, H. S., Bagshaw, G. L., and Simpson, J. (2008). Self-disgust mediates the relationship between dysfunctional cognitions and depressive symptomatology. Emotion 8, 379–385. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J. H., Faulkner, J., and Schaller, M. (2003). Evolved disease-avoidance processes and contemporary anti-social behavior: prejudicial attitudes and avoidance of people with physical disabilities. J. Nonverbal Behav. 27, 65–87. doi: 10.1023/A:1023910408854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J. H., Schaller, M., and Crandall, C. S. (2006). Psychological disease-avoidance mechanisms and stigmatization of fat people. Unpublished manuscript, University of Groningen: The Netherlands.

Google Scholar

Pond, R. S. Jr., DeWall, C. N., Lambert, N. M., Deckman, T., Bonser, I., and Fincham, F. D. (2012). Repulsed by violence: disgust sensitivity buffers trait, behavioral, and daily aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 175–188. doi: 10.1037/a0024296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozin, P., and Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychol. Rev. 94, 23–41. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozin, P., Lowery, L., and Ebert, R. (1994). Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of disgust. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 870–881. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozin, P., Markwith, M., and Nemeroff, C. (1992). Magical contagion beliefs and fear of AIDS. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 22, 1081–1092. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00943.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozin, P., Millman, L., and Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the Laws of sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 703–712. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.4.703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, M. (2006). Parasites, behavioral defenses, and the social psychological mechanisms through which cultures are evoked. Psychol. Inq. 17, 96–101. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1702_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, M., and Duncan, L. A. (2007). The behavioral immune system: its evolution and social psychological implications. In J. P. Forgas, M. G. Haselton, and W. Hippelvon (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognitions 293–307). New York: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Schmader, T., and Lickel, B. (2006). The approach and avoidance function of guilt and shame emotions: comparing reactions to self-caused and other-caused wrongdoing. Motiv. Emot. 30, 42–55. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9006-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, M., Miller, G. E., Gervais, W. M., Yager, S., and Chen, E. (2010). Mere visual perception of other people’s disease symptoms facilitates a more aggressive immune response. Psychological Science, 21, 649–652. doi: 10.1177/0956797610368064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, M., and Murray, D. R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 212–221. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G., and Jordan, A. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1096–1109. doi: 10.1177/0146167208317771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral affect: the good, the bad, and the ugly. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 598–607. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J., Miller, R., Flicker, L., and Barlow, D. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 1256–1269. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., and Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 345–372. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., Mashek, D., Kendall, S., Goodman, K., and Taylor, C. (2003) Moral emotions and psychopathy: inmates’ shame, guilt, and empathy. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada.

Google Scholar

Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., and Shook, N. J. (2020). On the origin of shame: does shame emerge from an evolved disease-avoidance architecture? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Clay, W. R., and Shook, N. J. (2014). Does the behavioral immune system prepare females to be religiously conservative and collectivistic? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 189–202. doi: 10.1177/0146167213508792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., and Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: a predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personal. Individ. Differ. 49, 587–592. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., and Ventis, W. L. (2012). Religious conservatism: an evolutionarily evoked disease-avoidance strategy. Religion Brain Behav. 2, 105–120. doi: 10.1080/2153599X.2012.695514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrizzi, J., Shook, N., and McDaniel, M. (2013). The behavioral immune system and social conservatism: a meta-analysis. Evol. Hum. Behav. 34, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornhill, R., and Fincher, C. L. (2014). The parasite-stress theory of values and sociality: infectious disease, history and human values worldwide. Cham: Springer International Publishing

Google Scholar

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q. Rev. Biol. 46, 35–57. doi: 10.1086/406755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., and Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 103–122. doi: 10.1037/a0015474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Leeuwen, F., and Petersen, M. (2018). The behavioral immune system is designed to avoid infected individuals, not outgroups. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 226–234. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y. L., Chen, H. L., and Yuan, Y. (2020). Effect of social exclusion on adolescents’ self-injury: the mediation effect of shame and the moderating effect of cognitive reappraisal. J. Psychol. Sci. 43, 333–339.

Google Scholar

Wesselmann, E. D., Nairne, J. S., and Williams, K. D. (2012). An evolutionary social psychological approach to studying the effects of ostracism. J. Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 6, 309–328. doi: 10.1037/h0099249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: a critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Wirth, J. H., Sacco, D. F., Hugenberg, K., and Williams, K. D. (2010). Eye gaze as relational evaluation: averted eye gaze leads to feelings of ostracism and relational devaluation. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 869–882. doi: 10.1177/0146167210370032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: disgust, shame, behavioral immune system, social rejection, social exclusion, ostracism

Citation: Terrizzi JA, Pond RS, Shannon TCJ, Koopman ZK and Reich JC (2023) How does disgust regulate social rejection? a mini-review. Front. Psychol. 14:1141100. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1141100

Received: 10 January 2023; Accepted: 01 June 2023;
Published: 16 June 2023.

Edited by:

Corrado Corradi-Dell'Acqua, University of Geneva, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Hamdi Muluk, University of Indonesia, Indonesia
Lia Antico, Brown University, United States

Copyright © 2023 Terrizzi, Pond, Shannon, Koopman and Reich. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: John A. Terrizzi Jr., johh.a.terrizzi.jr@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.