Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 14 February 2023
Sec. Educational Psychology

Piano practice with emphasis on left hand for right handers: Developing pedagogical strategies based on motor control perspectives

\nJinrui Pang&#x;Jinrui Pang1Shan Zhao&#x;Shan Zhao2Yilin WangYilin Wang3Qian Wang
Qian Wang1*Qun Fang
Qun Fang2*
  • 1School of Music, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China
  • 2School of Physical Education, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China
  • 3College of Arts, Beijing Language and Culture University, Beijing, China

Introduction

Piano play involves a series of bimanual movements, thus utilizing both hands fluently is fundamental for a pianist (Furuya and Altenmüller, 2013). Skillful performance requires a high level of precise control over right and left hands. After years of intensive practice, pianists are able to move both hands with great automaticity (Kilincer et al., 2019).

Over the past few decades, piano play has raised an increasing interest among neuroscientists. Pianists provide an ideal model to investigate plastic changes at both behavioral and neural levels (Münte et al., 2002). With the wide application of neuroimaging techniques, researchers are able to study in the neural mechanisms underlying bimanual coordination. Research on piano performance has contributed insights into brain functioning (Globerson and Nelken, 2013). However, efforts of enhancing piano education based on neuroscience evidence remain limited. In fact, the same issue has been brought to notice in the field of physical education. In a recent study, researchers called for attention to asymmetry of interlimb transfer, a typical phenomenon in motor learning and control, which could be a promising pedagogical strategy to enhance sport skill acquisition (Fang et al., 2022).

Based on neural evidence and findings of bimanual control, the current study proposed an opinion that particular attention should be given to the left-hand practice to enhance piano learning for right handers. The improved piano performance and bimanual coordination imply behavioral changes and neural adaptations associated with long-term training, which may provide rationales of highlighting the left-hand practice. Further discussions focus on pedagogical suggestions from a motor control perspective. It is our purpose to provide evidence-based insights into effective approaches to facilitate piano learning and teaching.

Behavioral changes associated with piano learning

One of the noticeable features of proficient piano performance is the increased independence between left and right hands. A study compared pianists with amateurs when playing two excerpts. Although the excerpts consisted of same notes which should be played by both hands at the same time, the pianist group performed with distinct differences between both hands in dexterous control over timing and force of keystrokes (Kim et al., 2021). The greater hand independence is also displayed as less interference between hands. Research that investigated muscular activities in piano play identified smaller co-activations of hand and finger muscles in experts than amateurs (Furuya et al., 2010, 2011). The findings can be substantiated by subsequent study in mirror movements of pianists and non-piano controls during manual tasks (Chieffo et al., 2016). Mirror movement refers to unintended movements of homologous muscles contralateral to the voluntarily active ones (Schott and Wyke, 1981). The occurrence of mirror movements in healthy individuals suggests challenges in motor control due to task complexity and fatigue (Arányi and Rösler, 2002). While the participants performing finger tapping task as fast as they could, electromyography (EMG) recorded mirror movements in the control group but not in the pianist group. The evidence suggests that the long-term piano experience may induce a strong inhibitory control over involuntary hand movements (Chieffo et al., 2016).

Another prominent change associated with piano skill proficiency is the reduced motor laterality between dominant and non-dominant hands. Hand difference is effective in differentiating pianists from amateurs (Kim et al., 2021). Compared with the controls, musicians are characterized by symmetric motor performance and left-hand superiority (Gärtner et al., 2013). In a reaching accuracy task, piano experts outperformed non-piano controls in terms of left-hand performance, whereas no significant difference was identified between the experts and the controls in task performance by right hand. Thus, a smaller between-hand difference was identified in the experts compared with the controls (Kilincer et al., 2019). Consistent findings were also reported in a study which included a pegboard test and a finger tapping test (Chieffo et al., 2016). Pianists showed similar performance of left and right hands, whereas novices completed the tasks with superior performance on the right hand. A lower asymmetry index (a measure of performance difference between left and right hands) was found in the pianist group than that in the control group, suggesting the tendency of less motor asymmetry associated with increased piano proficiency.

The reduced motor laterality is also evident in hand preference. Pianists tend to use left hand more often than controls when reaching targets in their frontal space, indicating altered pattern of hand selection associated with piano experience (Kilincer et al., 2019). According to the evidence from both piano play and bimanual tasks, pianists are characterized by balanced hand preference and reduced manual asymmetry due to enhanced left-hand performance.

Neural adaptations associated with piano learning

Applications of neuroimaging techniques provide insights into neural mechanisms underlying bimanual control. In a positron emission tomography (PET) study, stronger activations were identified in premotor cortex and supplementary motor area during bimanual finger movements, suggesting an active role of the cortical regions in bimanual coordination (Sadato et al., 1997). Premotor cortex is responsible for planning and organizing voluntary movements (Sira and Mateer, 2014). Research has shown particular involvement of premotor cortex in the early phase of motor learning to set up cognitive strategies and motor program of complex movements (Filippi et al., 2010; Gryga et al., 2012). Once the motor sequence is established, premotor cortex becomes less activated in motor control (Andres et al., 1999; Lafleur et al., 2002). Corpus callosum consists of white matter tracts that connect left and right hemispheres. Due to the primary function of interhemispheric communication, corpus callosum is important for bimanual performance (Serrien et al., 2001). Based on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) technique, researchers found a connection between structural organization of the corpus callosum and capacity of bimanual coordination (Muetzel et al., 2008). The higher factorial anisotropy, a measure of structural properties of the corpus callosum, suggests increased interhemispheric connectivity which contributes to skilled performance in bimanual tasks. Johansen-Berg et al. (2007) examined integrity of white matter in corpus callosum by diffusions MRI. Variability in white matter integrity in callosal pathways was found to correlate with individual differences in bimanual performance.

Piano practice induces structural and functional changes in the brain. Hyde et al. (2009) investigated influences of piano training on brain structure by comparing young children (Age: 6.32 ± 0.82 years) who regularly attended piano lessons with their counterparts (Age: 5.90 ± 0.54 years) who did not receive any instrumental training. While no difference between the groups was found in the beginning of the study, significant differences were identified in primary motor cortex, primary auditory cortex, and corpus callosum after 15 months, indicating the differential brain development due to the piano training in early childhood.

Hemispheric lateralization is considered an outcome of evolution, which allows superior cognitive capacity and motor performance in more lateralized individuals (Rogers, 2000, 2002). For example, as a representation of lateralized hemispheric function, handedness enables individuals to complete a difficult task with the stronger and more dexterous hand (Corballis, 2019). However, neural evidence indicates reduced hemispheric asymmetry associated with piano training. In an MRI study, pianists were characterized by symmetrical size of the primary motor cortex while the non-pianist controls indicated left-larger-than-right asymmetry (Amunts et al., 1997). Houdayer et al. (2016) identified balanced interhemispheric interactions along with improved piano skill proficiency. Specifically, ipsilateral silent period (ISP), a measure of callosal transmission, was asymmetric at the baseline, with the left hemisphere characterized by longer duration than the right hemisphere. Increased ISP duration was identified in the right hemisphere after 10 days of piano training, leading to symmetrical ISP between hemispheres in the post-test. The findings were consistent with the research which identified more symmetrical motor cortical representation in pianists (Chieffo et al., 2016). For the right-handed controls with no musical experience, cortical representation of the dominant hand muscles was larger than that of the non-dominant hand muscles. Pianists, however, indicated larger cortical representation of the non-dominant hand muscles, leading to the reduced asymmetry in motor representation. Hemispheric asymmetry has been considered to favor the dominant hand performance in unimanual tasks. In contrast, bimanual performance such as piano play demands a comparable level of skills between hands. The long-term piano experience induces symmetrical neural functioning and brain structure which, in turn, may facilitate piano learning and performance.

Pedagogical implications based on motor control perspectives

For right handers, left hand is considered weaker and slower in functional performance (Kim et al., 2021). Left-hand skills need more attention in practice because optimal performance in a challenging bimanual task can be achieved only if hand differences are minimal (Kopiez et al., 2006). To familiarize learners with left-hand play, a commonly used strategy is the part-whole approach with initial practice on single hand before training both hands together. However, effectiveness of the teaching approach may be limited in that the mechanisms between unimanual and bimanual control are similar but not identical (Van Vugt and Altenmüller, 2019). Unimanual training on each hand may hardly result in complete transfer of learning to bimanual performance. Therefore, bimanual practice with particular emphasis on the left hand is worth further notice in piano education.

Given the rationales of highlighting left-hand practice in piano education, pedagogical strategies can be developed by taking advantage of bimanual coordination. Research has shown a greater influence of the dominant left hemisphere on the non-dominant left hand than that of the non-dominant right hemisphere on the dominant right hand (Kagerer, 2016). The asymmetrical influence implies greater interference on the left-hand performance. Indeed, when task demands on bimanual coordination are high, right handers tend to prioritize right-hand performance at the cost of left-hand performance (Kopiez et al., 2006).

Cognitive related approach was often used to reduce interference on the left hand in bimanual performance (Peters and Schwartz, 1989). Researchers have shown that more attention is paid to the hand playing melody (Parncutt et al., 1999). Accordingly, to raise attention to the left hand during piano play, it is reasonable to practice the musical score in which melody is performed by left hand. This pedagogical suggestion can be particularly useful for novices who often face difficulty in controlling both hands at the same time. With left hand playing the melody part, novices may reduce right-hand interference by giving more attention to the left-hand performance.

Movement tempo could be another critical consideration in piano education. Given that more attention would be paid to the limb moving at a higher frequency (Panzer et al., 2021), the faster moving limb has a significant impact on the slower moving limb (Kennedy et al., 2016, 2017). Improved stability and accuracy have been identified with the faster moving hand regardless of handedness (Summers et al., 1993; Peper et al., 1995; Panzer et al., 2021). In order to improve left-hand performance of the right handers, a reasonable strategy is to choose the musical score with left hand playing at a higher tempo than right hand.

Attention plays an essential role in learning and performance. Increases in attentional effort, mediated by top-down mechanisms, is necessary to maintain or recover performance under challenging circumstances (Sarter et al., 2006). Top-down attentional modulation primarily involves prefrontal, parietal, and limbic areas which represent essential cognitive activities in task performance such as selective attention, divided attention, working memory, and motivation (Mcdowd, 2007). Additionally, bottom-up mechanism can be activated when an individual becomes highly focused from a less focused state. The ascending reticular activating system, involving reticulothalamocortical pathway and extrathlamic pathways, regulates the switch in mental states (Mcdowd, 2007). In the early stage of learning to play a musical score, multiple factors have to be attended, with more attentional resources recruited in learning and performance.

It is also important to raise awareness of higher neural efficiency associated with enhanced proficiency. Compared with initial stage of learning, autonomous stage after intensive repetitions and practice is characterized by reduced activation of the attention system without compromising performance. An fMRI study identified smaller activation of cortical areas in pianists than controls when performing a complex finger movement task, which provided evidence for the reduced cortical activations in pianists (Krings et al., 2000). The sequenced movement suggests programmed performance during piano play. Motor program refers to a set of stored muscle commands ready for action at any time (Schmidt, 1975). Once the program has been established, performance can be generated in an efficient manner. The features of programmed performance are in line with Hebbian mechanisms which account for strengthened synaptic connections through mere repetitions to achieve effortless performance (Makino et al., 2016). This process is classified as unsupervised learning which is dominant at the later stages of motor learning. The strengthened connections between coactive neurons build neural circuits which generate a stable, reproducible activity pattern in support of autonomous performance (Fiete et al., 2010). Therefore, pedagogical strategies that direct attentional efforts to the non-dominant left hand are necessary to reduce interference by the dominant right hand in the initial stage of learning a novel musical score. With practice and improved proficiency, attention is expected to decline as performance becomes autonomous.

Conclusions

The current study proposed an opinion as to enhancing piano education based on the understandings of bimanual control. Piano experience over a long period of time induces plastic changes in behavioral performance as well as brain structure and functioning. Compared with novices and non-piano controls, pianists are characterized by reduced motor laterality and increased hand independence. Neural adaptations associated with the piano experience indicate less hemispheric asymmetry which facilitates bimanual coordination in piano play. The changes at behavioral and neural levels provide insights into pedagogical strategies. Left-hand practice should be highlighted in piano teaching and learning by (i) practicing the musical scores with left hand playing the melody parts, and (ii) practicing the musical scores with left hand playing at the higher tempo. The pedagogical suggestions were reached by means of qualitative analysis on the existing literature. Empirical evidence is needed in future research to testify efficacy of the pedagogical suggestions in practice.

Author contributions

QW and QF: conceptualization. JP and SZ: draft preparation. YW, QW, and QF: validation. YW: review and editing. All authors collaborated in preparing the manuscript and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Steinmetz, H., Schleicher, A., Dabringhaus, A., et al. (1997). Motor cortex and hand motor skills: structural compliance in the human brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 206–215. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:3<206::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andres, F. G., Mima, T., Schulman, A. E., Dichgans, J., Hallett, M., and Gerloff, C. (1999). Functional coupling of human cortical sensorimotor areas during bimanual skill acquisition. Brain 122 (Pt 5), 855–870. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.5.855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arányi, Z., and Rösler, K. M. (2002). Effort-induced mirror movements. Exp. Brain Res. 145, 76–82. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1101-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chieffo, R., Straffi, L., Inuggi, A., Gonzalez-Rosa, J. J., Spagnolo, F., Coppi, E., et al. (2016). Motor cortical plasticity to training started in childhood: the example of piano players. PLoS ONE 11, e0157952. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corballis, M. C. (2019). Evolution of cerebral asymmetry. Prog. Brain Res. 250, 153–178. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.04.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, Q., Xia, Y., Zhang, X., and Huang, F. (2022). Asymmetry of interlimb transfer: pedagogical innovations in physical education. Front. Psychol. 13, 1029888. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1029888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiete, I. R., Senn, W., Wang, C. Z. H., and Hahnloser, R. H. R. (2010). Spike-time-dependent plasticity and heterosynaptic competition organize networks to produce long scale-free sequences of neural activity. Neuron 65, 563–576. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippi, M., Ceccarelli, A., Pagani, E., Gatti, R., Rossi, A., Stefanelli, L., et al. (2010). Motor learning in healthy humans is associated to gray matter changes: a tensor-based morphometry study. PLoS ONE 5, e10198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., and Altenmüller, E. (2013). Flexibility of movement organization in piano performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 173. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., Altenmüller, E., Katayose, H., and Kinoshita, H. (2010). Control of multi-joint arm movements for the manipulation of touch in keystroke by expert pianists. BMC Neurosci. 11, 82. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-82

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furuya, S., Goda, T., Katayose, H., Miwa, H., and Nagata, N. (2011). Distinct inter-joint coordination during fast alternate keystrokes in pianists with superior skill. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5, 50. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gärtner, H., Minnerop, M., Pieperhoff, P., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Altenmüller, E., et al. (2013). Brain morphometry shows effects of long-term musical practice in middle-aged keyboard players. Front. Psychol. 4, 636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Globerson, E., and Nelken, I. (2013). The neuro-pianist. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7, 35. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gryga, M., Taubert, M., Dukart, J., Vollmann, H., Conde, V., Sehm, B., et al. (2012). Bidirectional gray matter changes after complex motor skill learning. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6, 37. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houdayer, E., Cursi, M., Nuara, A., Zanini, S., Gatti, R., Comi, G., et al. (2016). Cortical motor circuits after piano training in adulthood: neurophysiologic evidence. PLoS ONE 11, e0157526. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., et al. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29, 3019–3025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansen-Berg, H., Della-Maggiore, V., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S. M., and Paus, T. (2007). Integrity of white matter in the corpus callosum correlates with bimanual co-ordination skills. Neuroimage 36(Suppl. 2), T16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kagerer, F. A. (2016). Asymmetric interference in left-handers during bimanual movements reflects switch in lateralized control characteristics. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 1545–1553. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4556-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. M., Rhee, J., Jimenez, J., and Shea, C. H. (2017). The influence of asymmetric force requirements on a multi-frequency bimanual coordination task. Hum. Mov. Sci. 51, 125–137. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2016.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. M., Rhee, J., and Shea, C. H. (2016). Symmetrical and asymmetrical influences on force production in 1:2 and 2:1 bimanual force coordination tasks. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 287–300. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4460-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilincer, O., Ustun, E., Akpinar, S., and Kaya, E. E. (2019). Motor lateralization may be influenced by long-term piano playing practice. Percept. Mot. Skills 126, 25–39. doi: 10.1177/0031512518807769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., Park, J. M., Rhyu, S., Nam, J., and Lee, K. (2021). Quantitative analysis of piano performance proficiency focusing on difference between hands. PLoS ONE 16, e0250299. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopiez, R., Galley, N., and Lee, J. I. (2006). The advantage of a decreasing right-hand superiority: the influence of laterality on a selected musical skill (sight reading achievement). Neuropsychologia 44, 1079–1087. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krings, T., Töpper, R., Foltys, H., Erberich, S., Sparing, R., Willmes, K., et al. (2000). Cortical activation patterns during complex motor tasks in piano players and control subjects. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 278, 189–193. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00930-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafleur, M. F., Jackson, P. L., Malouin, F., Richards, C. L., Evans, A. C., and Doyon, J. (2002). Motor learning produces parallel dynamic functional changes during the execution and imagination of sequential foot movements. Neuroimage 16, 142–157. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makino, H., Hwang, E. J., Hedrick, N. G., and Komiyama, T. (2016). Circuit mechanisms of sensorimotor learning. Neuron 92, 705–721. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcdowd, J. M. (2007). An overview of attention: behavior and brain. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 31, 98–103. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0b013e31814d7874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muetzel, R. L., Collins, P. F., Mueller, B. A., A, M. S., Lim, K. O., and Luciana, M. (2008). The development of corpus callosum microstructure and associations with bimanual task performance in healthy adolescents. Neuroimage 39, 1918–1925. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Münte, T. F., Altenmüller, E., and Jäncke, L. (2002). The musician's brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 473–478. doi: 10.1038/nrn843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panzer, S., Kennedy, D., Leinen, P., Pfeifer, C., and Shea, C. (2021). Bimanual coordination associated with left- and right-hand dominance: testing the limb assignment and limb dominance hypothesis. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 1595–1605. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06082-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parncutt, R., Sloboda, J. A., and Clarke, E. F. (1999). Interdependence of right and left hands in sight-read, written, and rehearsed fingerings of parallel melodic piano music. Aust. J. Psychol. 51, 204–210. doi: 10.1080/00049539908255357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peper, C. E., Beek, P. J., and Vanwieringen, P. C. W. (1995). Multifrequency coordination in bimanual tapping - Asymmetrical coupling and signs of supercriticality. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21, 1117–1138. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.5.1117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, M., and Schwartz, S. (1989). Coordination of the two hands and effects of attentional manipulation in the production of a bimanual 2:3 polyrhythm. Aust. J. Psychol. 41, 215–224. doi: 10.1080/00049538908260084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, L. J. (2000). Evolution of hemispheric specialization: advantages and disadvantages. Brain Lang. 73, 236–253. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, L. J. (2002). “Advantages and disadvantages of lateralization,” in Comparative Vertebrate Lateralization, eds L. J. Rogers, and R. Andrew (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Sadato, N., Yonekura, Y., Waki, A., Yamada, H., and Ishii, Y. (1997). Role of the supplementary motor area and the right premotor cortex in the coordination of bimanual finger movements. J. Neurosci. 17, 9667–9674. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-24-09667.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarter, M., Gehring, W. J., and Kozak, R. (2006). More attention must be paid: the neurobiology of attentional effort. Brain Res. Rev. 51, 145–160. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A. (1975). Schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychol. Rev. 82, 225–260. doi: 10.1037/h0076770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schott, G. D., and Wyke, M. A. (1981). Congenital mirror movements. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 44, 586–599. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.44.7.586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serrien, D. J., Nirkko, A. C., and Wiesendanger, M. (2001). Role of the corpus callosum in bimanual coordination: a comparison of patients with congenital and acquired callosal damage. Eur. J. Neurosci. 14, 1897–1905. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01798.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sira, C. S., and Mateer, C. A. (2014). “Frontal lobes,” in Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences, 2nd Edn, eds M. J. Aminoff, and R. B. Daroff (Oxford: Academic Press).

Google Scholar

Summers, J. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Burns, B. D., and Ford, S. K. (1993). Production of polyrhythms. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 19, 416–428. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.2.416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Vugt, F. T., and Altenmüller, E. (2019). On the one hand or on the other: trade-off in timing precision in bimanual musical scale playing. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 216–227. doi: 10.5709/acp-0271-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: piano education, bimanual control, behavioral performance, neural plasticity, handedness

Citation: Pang J, Zhao S, Wang Y, Wang Q and Fang Q (2023) Piano practice with emphasis on left hand for right handers: Developing pedagogical strategies based on motor control perspectives. Front. Psychol. 14:1124508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1124508

Received: 15 December 2022; Accepted: 30 January 2023;
Published: 14 February 2023.

Edited by:

Ahmed Mohamed Fahmy Yousef, Fayoum University, Egypt

Reviewed by:

Chenfan Yang, University of Science Malaysia (USM), Malaysia

Copyright © 2023 Pang, Zhao, Wang, Wang and Fang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qian Wang, yes wangqian0016@qdu.edu.cn; Qun Fang, yes 2020910112@qdu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.