Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 02 February 2023
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards a Psychophysiological Approach in Physical Activity, Exercise, and Sports View all 12 articles

Association between motor and math skills in preschool children with typical development: Systematic review

Pedro Flores
Pedro Flores1*Eduarda Coelho,Eduarda Coelho2,3Maria Isabel Mouro-Carvalhal,Maria Isabel Mourão-Carvalhal2,3Pedro Forte,,Pedro Forte1,2,3
  • 1Sports Department, Higher Institute of Education and Sciences of the Douro, Penafiel, Portugal
  • 2Sports Department, University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro, Vila Real, Portugal
  • 3Research Center in Sports, Health and Human Development, Covilhã, Portugal

Mathematics has been the subject in which many school-age children have revealed many difficulties. Research carried out in an attempt to understand the causes of failure in this area pointed to a positive association between mathematical performance and motor performance. Given the importance of math development in future school outcomes, knowing which specific motor components are most associated with math performance can help educators define better strategies for teaching mathematics. In this sense, the present systematic review study aimed to identify the components of motor skills most positively associated with mathematical performance in children with typical development who attend preschool. The PRISMA methodology (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) was used in this study. The databases searched were ERIC, PubMED, SciELO, Scopus and Web of Science. A total of 2,909 articles were identified, of which 18 were included in this systematic review. The main results showed positive associations between fine motor skills, namely fine motor coordination and visuomotor integration, and mathematical performance. The math skill of numerical counting was the most associated with FMS. The main characteristics of the instruments used showed that the tasks of copying figures or drawings are the most used to assess visuomotor integration and the tasks of handling objects with pinch-like movements are the most used to assess fine motor coordination. Given the importance of mathematical performance in future school results, identifying early children with difficulties in fine motor skills will help educators to design better strategies for teaching mathematical skills. In this sense, the need to identify instruments to assess fine motor skills in preschool children with characteristics that facilitate their administration by the educator in the classroom context, i.e., requiring little administration time, not requiring much experience or training, the possibility of being administered to the group/class, few material resources, and the results can be easily interpreted, classified, and associated with mathematical performance.

1. Introduction

The relationship between motor and cognitive development goes back to Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, highlighting the interconnection between individuals and the environment. The process of assimilation and, in particular, accommodation leads to the formation of new and more complex cognitive structures when the child interacts with the environment (Piaget and Inhelder, 1966). According to Wallon (1941), children develop through movement, from action to representation, from physical to cognitive. Pelicier et al. (1996) stated that motor and psychological functions are the two fundamental elements of human behavior. Initially, they develop together, then they specialize and differentiate, although they remain subject to reciprocal interactions (Adolph and Franchak, 2017; Kim et al., 2018). The idea of “learning to learn” suggests that early learning is motor- system centered with brain systems involved in posture, grip, vision, and motor control, and as the child adapts to changes in cognitive and motor skills, he or she develops simultaneously (Adolph, 2005). Today, there is neurophysiological and neuroimaging evidence that the prefrontal cortex, cerebellum and connection structures are co-active in certain cognitive and motor tasks suggesting an interrelationship between motor and cognitive development (Diamond, 2000; Abe and Hanakawa, 2009).

Cross-sectional research has shown positive associations between motor skills and cognitive and academic assessments (Kantomaa et al., 2013; Lopes et al., 2013; Haapala et al., 2014; Diamond, 2015; Geertsen et al., 2016). Additionally, longitudinal studies carried out in typically developing populations have found a relationship between motor development and cognitive development throughout the life cycle. Motor skills acquired at a very early age may be related to cognitive skills during childhood (Michel et al., 2016), adolescence (Cantell et al., 2003), and even adulthood (Kuh et al., 2006). Recent studies have highlighted that motor skills influence academic performance in the early years (Carson et al., 2016; Alvarez-Bueno et al., 2017; Zeng et al., 2017; Macdonald et al., 2018; De Waal, 2019; Duncan et al., 2019; Haapala et al., 2019; Malambo et al., 2022), and are described as one of the criteria for school readiness (Department for Education, 2020; Jones et al., 2021). For this reason, an assessment of motor skills at an early age can help to identify children who are likely to perform poorly in academic skills in advance (Roebers et al., 2014; Cameron et al., 2016; Pitchford et al., 2016; Schmidt et al., 2017; Goodway et al., 2019).

1.1. Motor skills

The term fundamental motor skill was defined by Wickstrom (1977) as a basic motor activity for more advanced and highly specific activities such as running, jumping, and throwing, among others. Currently, the term fundamental motor skill reflects various terminologies that have been used in the literature such as motor proficiency, motor performance, fundamental movement ability, fundamental motor skill, motor skill and motor competence (Robinson et al., 2015).

Various terms have been used to describe fundamental motor skills, such as: gross motor skills (Pang and Fong, 2009; Mostafavi et al., 2013), fundamental motor patterns (Barnett et al., 2012), and fundamental movement skills (Staples and Reid, 2010; Barnett et al., 2015). Similarly, various terminologies have been commonly used to describe fine motor skills, such as: fine motor proficiency, fine motor accuracy, fine motor integration, manual dexterity, or fine motor coordination (Bruininks and Bruininks, 2005) or performance motor skills (Sortwell et al., 2022).

In view of the numerous terminologies found in the literature, the present systematic review is based on the terminologies used in the reviews published by Macdonald et al. (2018), Donnelly et al. (2016) and van der Fels et al. (2015), due to the fact that they present similar objectives. These reviews used the term Motor Skills and the underlying domains as gross motor skills and fine motor skills.

Motor skills (MS) refer to efficient and effective actions resulting from a learning process (Magill, 1984). According to movement control and precision, they are divided into two categories, gross motor skills (GMS) and fine motor skills (FMS). These two categories of motor skills are used in research to analyze the relationship between cognitive and motor development (Grissmer et al., 2010; Gentier et al., 2013; Raisbeck and Diekfuss, 2015; Van der Fels et al., 2015; Oberer et al., 2017; Macdonald et al., 2018; Haywood and Getchell, 2019).

GMS primarily use movements produced by large muscle groups. They include motor skills that imply movement of the body in space, such as walking, running, jumping, sliding, and postural skills, which refer to the ability to keep a controlled position or posture during a specific task or activity (they can be dynamic or static) and manipulative skills used to control objects in actions such as grabbing, hitting, absorbing, lifting, etc., with hands, feet or using other objects for that purpose (Ulrich, 2000; Grissmer et al., 2010; Lopes et al., 2013; D’Hondt et al., 2014; Magistro et al., 2015; Rudd et al., 2015; Chang and Gu, 2018; Haywood and Getchell, 2019). On the other hand, FMS can be defined as movements produced by small muscle groups that involve activities with great precision, implying two distinct capabilities, motor coordination, and visual integration, as well as the integration of both (Carlson et al., 2013). In this context, different types of FMS can be identified depending on the capabilities involved (Kimmel and Ratliff-Schaub, 2011; Carlson et al., 2013). A type of FMS consists of fine motor coordination (FMC), which refers to movements that involve oculus-manual coordination (eye-hand), manual dexterity, motor sequencing, and speed and precision, such as tracing, touching with fingers, building with Legos/blocks, moving coins from one place to another or inserting them into a slot, etc., which can also be called non-graphomotor skills (Davis and Matthews, 2010; Suggate et al., 2018).

Another type of FMS consists of visual motor integration or visuomotor integration (VMI) and spatial or visuospatial integration (VSI), which refers to the organization of small muscle movements of the hand and fingers through the processing of visual and spatial stimuli, more based on synchronized hand-eye movements (Sortor and Kulp, 2003; Carlson et al., 2013; Fuhs et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2018), typically writing, drawing, copying shapes, letters, or other stimuli (Beery and Buktenica, 1997; Grissmer et al., 2010; Roebers et al., 2014; Oberer et al., 2017) that can be called graphomotor skills (Davis and Matthews, 2010; Suggate et al., 2018). Figure 1 summarizes the categories of motor skills.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Summary of motor skills categories (Author’s figure).

1.2. Mathematical performance

The performance of academic skills preferentially values two curricular areas, literacy and mathematics performance (Fernandes et al., 2016; Ribner et al., 2017). These two areas are considered prerequisites for performance in other subjects and, consequently, for academic success (Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD), 2016). However, mathematics plays an important role in the school curriculum and its development. In a modern, technological society, it is seen as a fundamental cognitive attribute, where successful early learning provides not only a framework for later learning (Duncan et al., 2007) but is also an indicator of future academic and professional success (Parsons and Bynner, 2005).

Mathematics is learned by children before school through numbers and quantities (Blair, 2002; McWayne et al., 2004) and the informal knowledge they acquire is often referred to as “basic numerical skills,” being a precondition for mathematical reasoning (Gersten et al., 2005; Jordan et al., 2006). Studies have shown that math skills in preschool education predict performance in reading, math, and science through grade 8 (children between 13 and 14 years old; Claesens and Engel, 2013).

The math skills children acquire in preschool education are important for developing a conceptual understanding of mathematics (Jordan et al., 2009; Siegler et al., 2012), as well as confidence in the ability to engage in activities that support analytical thinking, problem solving and reasoning and argumentation skills (Clements et al., 2004). However, teaching mathematics in preschool education must be related to children’s day-to-day interests, such as playing or exploring everyday situations (Silva et al., 2016).

It is recognized that pre-school children enjoy activities that develop their math skills (Ginsburg et al., 2006). However, most early childhood educators typically place greater emphasis on children’s social–emotional and literacy development and less attention to mathematics (National Research Council, 2009). Many early childhood educators avoid teaching mathematics because of their own negative experiences with it (Ginsburg et al., 2006; Clements and Sarama, 2007).

Fundamental learning for the development of mathematical competences in preschool education consists of two main areas: Numbers (1) and Geometry and Measurement (2). Each of these areas consists of different skills. The area of numbers is subdivided into the core of numbers (counting, cardinality, and identification of numbers), operations (addition and subtraction) and relations (comparing). Regarding the area of geometry and measurement, geometry consists of shapes and space, and measurements of length, area, and volume (National Research Council, 2009; Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Summary of fundamental areas of mathematics learning in preschool education according to the National Research Council, 2009 (Author’s figure).

Numbers are abstractions that apply to a wide range of real and imaginary situations. These do not exist in isolation, but constitute a system of relations and operations by which they can be compared, added, subtracted, multiplied, and divided. These relationships apply to a wide variety of problems (National Research Council, 2009). On the other hand, geometry and measurement provide systems for describing, representing, and understanding the world. Geometry is the study of shapes and spaces (two-dimensional-2-D and three-dimensional – 3-D). Measurement is about determining the size of object shapes (National Research Council, 2009). In this sense, preschool children need to learn the following math skills (National Research Council, 2009):

Core of numbers (cardinality, counting, and identification);

Cardinality – Children learn the concept of cardinality when they understand that adding an object means counting to the next number (Sarnecka and Carey, 2008).

Counting – Counting means listing the count numbers in order, usually starting at 1. It is a way of making a 1 to 1 correspondence between each object (Wynn, 1992).

Number Identification – It is the ability to associate a written number (e.g., 5) with a verbal word (e.g., five; Jordan et al., 2009).

Relationships (comparing);

Higher Institute of Education and Sciences of the Douro, Sports Department, Penafiel, Portugal, Penafiel, Portugal.

Comparing – represents the comparison of quantities of groups of objects using words such as “more/bigger,” “less/smaller” and “equal.” A basic way to compare two quantities of objects is by direct correspondence. If a child has a noticeably larger set of black beads compared to a set of white beads, the child identifies which group has the larger amount of beads. Thus, these skills can be developed separately from other basic math skills, such as counting and cardinality and number identification, because it is not necessary to know the exact number of objects in each group to successfully compare two groups (Traverso et al., 2021).

Operations (addition and subtraction);

Addition and subtraction – refer to basic arithmetic skills such as adding and subtracting and are used to relate quantities. Children are only prepared to develop these skills when they understand the concepts of cardinality and counting. These skills prepare children to develop more complex arithmetic skills such as multiplication and division (Barth et al., 2008; Canobi and Bethune, 2008).

Geometry (shapes and space).

Shapes – Shape is the basic way children learn to name objects (Jones and Smith, 2002). Children have an innate and implicit ability to recognize and match shapes (Anderson, 2000).

Space – Space includes two main skills: spatial orientation and spatial visualization of images. Spatial orientation involves knowing where you are and how to move around in the world (Gelman and Williams, 1997). Children learn words such as “beside” and “between.” Later, they learn words referring to frames of reference, such as “in front of,” “behind.” The words “left” and “right” are learned much later, and are a source of confusion for several years (Gopnik and Meltzoff, 1986). In these early years, children can also learn to analyze a route through a space (Wang and Spelke, 2002). Spatial visualization of images is about understanding and performing imagined movements of 2-D and 3-D objects. This requires being able to create a mental image and manipulate it through a close relationship between these two cognitive abilities. Spatial visualization of images has been positively associated with the construction and composition of shapes (Sarama et al., 1996).

Measurement (length, area, and volume).

Length Measurement – quantifies the distance between points in object or space.

Area Measurement – is a quantity of 2-D surface area that is contained within a boundary.

Volume Measurement – volume introduces even more complexity by the addition of a third dimension (3-D), presenting a significant challenge to students’ spatial structuring (Curry and Outhred, 2005).

One way to more formally assess children’s understanding of measurements is through comparison tasks (Mullet and Paques, 1991).

1.3. Relationship between motor and math skills

The literature has pointed out a positive association between mathematics and motor skills (Macdonald et al., 2018) and, according to the theory of “Embodied Cognition” (Embedded Cognition), cognition emerges from the “coupling” (embodied relationship) of the individual with the physical and social context as a result of sensorimotor activity (Smith, 2005). According to this paradigm, body movement causes changes in neural networks, stimulating significant gains in cognition. Within this paradigm, several investigations have highlighted the importance of movement in cognition, particularly in the performance of math skills such as abstract cognitive representations in general, and in the improvement of basic numerical representations in particular (Link et al., 2013). As proposed by Fischer et al. (2018), numbers are embodied concepts and not abstractions dissociated from sensory experiences. In addition, the theory assumes that certain cognitive and motor areas of the brain are activated simultaneously when solving mathematical problems (Fischer and Brugger, 2011).

Much research has been produced in an attempt to demonstrate the association between MH and cognition, but few have considered different categories of motor skills (gross/fine) and different categories of cognitive skills (executive functions/academic success in reading and mathematics; Oberer et al., 2017). Depending on the different categories of variables studied, the results differ and it is not possible to reach conclusive data (Magistro et al., 2015; Veldman et al., 2019). Furthermore, each study analyzed different motor skills and academics, performed them in different ways, or evaluated populations with different characteristics. All these aspects contribute to a disparity of results in this area (Magistro et al., 2015; Veldman et al., 2019). It is, therefore, a complex area of study that does not allow consensual conclusions (Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020). In this sense, it is considered necessary to study the associations between the different categories of specific motor skills and academic skills, in order to contribute to the success of children’s learning.

1.4. Relevance of the study

The subject of mathematics was selected for this study for the following reasons: first, because it is a “universal language” (across all countries); second, because it is the subject in which many school-age children have difficulties in learning, a problem with incidence ranging from 3% to 7% (Shalev et al., 2005; Swanson et al., 2009; Devine et al., 2013, 2018). These difficulties occur more than expected (Koponen et al., 2018; Willcutt et al., 2019) and are already observed at preschool age (Desoete et al., 2012; Devine et al., 2018); third, in light of the priority for children to develop basic numeracy skills upon entry to grade 1 (Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2000; Verdine et al., 2014; Duran et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020); fourth, it is a strong predictor of future academic success (Parsons and Bynner, 2005; Carr and Alexeev, 2011; Fuchs et al., 2016).

Considering the importance of mathematics in future school results, knowing which motor skills can contribute to improving mathematical performance will help educators to select and program more appropriate strategies for teaching and learning math skills.

In this sense, the present systematic review study aims to identify in children with typical development who attend preschool education, the different categories of motor skills that are associated with math skills and the instruments used in investigations carried out with this objective.

It was hypothesized that motor skills positively influence mathematical performance.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Identification of studies

A search was carried out in electronic databases, by two reviewers (PF, PF), according to the PRISMA protocol (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) to identify relevant studies (Moher et al., 2009). The databases searched were ERIC, PubMED, SciELO, Scopus, and Web of Science. The research was conducted on January 15, 2022 and the period was restricted to the last 10 years. The research made no restrictions regarding language. Although the last 5 or 10 years may be well-defined periods for research (Sampaio and Mancini, 2007; Donato and Donato, 2019), they may not, however, be the most appropriate. Thus, we conducted a search in the databases used in this study on systematic reviews published on the subject under study and found that the vast majority were published from 2012 onwards. Thus, the last 10 years were chosen instead of the last 5 years. EndNote was the program used to manage bibliographic references. Keywords were used to identify the relevant literature according to the objective of the study. The research was performed according to the abstract of the articles and the phrase used to detect them in all databases was: ((motor AND (motor AND (proficiency OR competency OR skill* OR development OR ability OR performance OR gross OR fine)) AND (“academic performance” OR “academic achievement” OR “academic grids” OR math* OR numeracy) AND (child* OR preschool*)).

2.2. Selection of studies

After removing duplicates, the titles and abstracts of the remaining studies were read, by two reviewers independently (PF, PF), and selected with reference to the predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria to assess their potential eligibility for this systematic review. Studies in which the abstract clearly indicated that they would be ineligible for inclusion were immediately eliminated, however, those in which there was some doubt as to their eligibility were kept.

Subsequently, the full texts of these articles were obtained to assess eligibility for inclusion in this review by the two reviewers (PF, PF) and in case of doubt the studies were reevaluated together, following inclusion and exclusion criteria. This research followed the PICOS criteria (population, intervention, comparison, outcome, study; Moher et al., 2009): P, preschool children with typical development; intervention, motor skills diagnosis; C, relationship between motor skills and mathematics; O, evidence of the influence of motor skills on mathematical performance; S, cross-sectional or longitudinal studies, with or without intervention, in any language and in any publication format (articles and or papers).

a. Inclusion criteria:

1. The study population should include typically developing children attending preschool education, between the ages of 2.5 and 7 years. Atypical development is defined as the development of children who exhibit early delays, deviations, or disabilities below the desired development for the same age group (Jones et al., 2014; D’Souza and Karmiloff-Smith, 2017; Johnson et al., 2021).

2. Studies should include an association between mathematical performance and at least one specific component of motor skills (GMS and FMS; Grissmer et al., 2010; Gentier et al., 2013; Raisbeck and Diekfuss, 2015; Van der Fels et al., 2015; Oberer et al., 2017; Haywood and Getchell, 2019);

3. The studies should be primary observational studies (longitudinal and transversal);

4. Objective measures should be used to assess the specific components of motor skills and mathematics;

5. Appropriate statistical analyses to report associations should include correlations.

b. Exclusion criteria:

1. Studies with populations of children diagnosed with mental illness, neurological disorders (learning difficulties, motor coordination disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum, and cerebral palsy), and premature children. Studies with premature children who did not develop cerebral palsy were excluded, because among the most frequent problems are those associated with GMS and FMS (de Kieviet et al., 2009; Williams et al., 2010), in which they have been shown to be a negative influence on their academic performance (McHale and Cermak, 1992; Feder et al., 2005; Feder and Majnemer, 2007; Edwards et al., 2011).

2. Secondary studies (non-systematic and systematic reviews, with or without meta-analysis);

3. Studies that did not show associations between math skills and at least one specific component of motor skills (Bruininks and Bruininks, 2005; Carlson et al., 2013);

4. Studies that did not present the results aim to assess the specific components of motor skills and mathematics;

5. Studies that did not include in statistical analyses the correlations between at least one specific component of motor skills and mathematics.

After reading the articles in full, some were excluded because they did not meet all the inclusion criteria. Eligible studies were kept and included for methodological and subsequent quality assessment, data extraction, discussion, and conclusions.

An additional search was conducted in the references of the articles included in the review in order to add relevant articles. After the search, no referenced articles met all the criteria for inclusion in this review.

2.3. Critical assessment of methodological quality

The methodological quality of the studies was assessed by two independent reviewers (EC and IM). In case of disagreement, the studies were jointly reassessed until a consensus was reached regarding the final score.

The Methodological Quality Checklist for studies based on Observational Methodology (MQCOM), intended for studies using observational methodology, was used to assess the methodological quality of the studies. This instrument was firstly designed by Chacón-Moscoso et al. (2018) that determined the primary criteria/dimensions to take into account when reporting research using observational methods and developed a list of metrics to quantify them (Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2015; Portell et al., 2015; Chacón-Moscoso et al., 2016). Recently this instrument was reduced to 16 questions (Chacón-Moscoso et al., 2019), allowing to identify the main methodological quality items needed to conduct studies based on observational methodology and offer the results as a useful tool for authors conducting studies and reviewers making publication decisions.

The MQCOM is composed of 16 items divided into 11 criteria/dimensions: Criterion 1 – Delimitation of objectives is composed of 3 items (Item 1. Reference to observational methodology, specifying whether observation is direct or indirect; Item 2. Delimitation of study objectives; Item 3. Theoretical framework referenced). Criterion/dimension 2 – Observational design, consists of 3 items [Item 4. Observation unit criteria (idiographic: study units are formed by one or more participants if there is a stable link between them; nomothetic: two or more study units); Item 5. Temporal criteria (punctual: one or two observation sessions; follow-up: more than two observation sessions); Item 6. Dimensionality criteria (one-dimensional: one level of response; multidimensional: two or more levels of response)]; Criterion/dimension 3 – Participants/observation units, consists of 1 item [Item 7. Clear specification of inclusion and exclusion criteria for observation units (reasons why some units were chosen in the study and others were not)]. Criterion/dimension 4 – Observation instruments, consists of 2 items [Item 8. Adequacy of the observation instrument (combination of field format with category system, field format, category system, or scale of estimation); Item 9. Codification manual with definition of the categories/behaviors and specification of dimensions (in multidimensional designs)]; Criterion/dimension 5 – Software use, consists of 1 item [Item 10. Software used to register data (SDIS-GSEQ v. 4.2.1./GSEQ 5, LINCE, MATCH VISION STUDIO, Transana, other: specify), control data quality (SDIS-GSEQ v. 4.2.1./GSEQ 5, LINCE, HOISAN, GT, SAS, other: specify)], and analyze data (SDIS-GSEQ, HOISAN, THEME v. 6, R, SAS, other: specify); Criterion/dimension 6 – Data, consists of 1 item [Item 11. Specification of data type as sequential/concurrent (sequential data: behaviors that cannot overlap and belong to a single dimension; concurrent data: behaviors that can co-occur and belong to several dimensions) and event-based/time-based (event-based: the primary parameter used in the record is order of events; time-based: the primary parameter is duration)]; Criterion/dimension 7 – Specification of parameters, consists of 1 item (Item 12. Type of parameters according to given use); Criterion/dimension 8 – Observational sampling, consists of 1 item (Item 13. Delimitation of sessions: clear establishment of criteria (temporal, behavioral, or mixed) for the beginning and the end of sessions within the observation period and of criteria for acceptance of sessions: between-sessions constancy, within-sessions constancy, or temporary disruptions); O Criterion/dimension 9 – Data quality control, consists of 1 item [Item 14. Between-observer reliability (agreement between the records of different observers)/within-observer reliability (agreement between the records of the same observer at two time points)]; Criterion/dimension 10 – Data analysis, consists of 1 item (Item 15. Type of data analysis performed); Criterion/dimension 11 – Interpretation of results, consists of 1 item (Item 16. In the discussion section).

According to Chacón-Moscoso et al. (2019) items are rated between zero and one. The value zero represents, does not comply; The value one represents, meets the criteria; The value 0.5 represents, partially complies. Items 3 and 15 are exceptions, as they still have different intermediate quotations between zero and one (0, 0.33, 0.67, and 1). Item 11 has a rating of nine, not applicable. The minimum value of the MQCOM is zero points and the maximum is 16 points (Chacón-Moscoso et al., 2019).

2.4. Data extraction

Data were independently extracted by two reviewers (PF, PF) and cross-checked, and controversial issues were discussed based on the original text to determine the final outcome. The extracted information included study characteristics: author, year, type of study and country where the study was carried out; main characteristics of the sample (number, sex and age); aim of the study, main results and conclusions; specific components of motor skills associated with different mathematical competences; instruments used to assess motor skills and mathematical competences.

Considering the methodological heterogeneity among the studies and instruments for the collection of data related to motor skills and mathematics and quality of the studies, it was not possible to carry out a meta-analysis.

3. Results

3.1. Included studies

The total search result in the databases produced 2,909 studies (PubMed: n = 860; Scopus: n = 1,004; Web of Science: n = 778; SciELO: n = 35; ERIC: n = 232), of which 965 were automatically excluded because they were duplicates and, after reading the title and abstract, 1,776 studies were excluded, thus resulting in a total of 168 studies potentially suitable studies. After reading these studies in full, 150 were excluded because they did not meet all inclusion criteria and, so, only 18 were included. After selecting the studies included for the review, a manual search of their bibliographic references was performed, from which no study was obtained, either because they did not meet all the eligibility criteria or because they were already included in the selection. The flowchart according to PRISMA methodology is shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Flowchart of research processes for the inclusion of studies (PRISMA).

Of the 18 studies included in this review, the following procedures were performed: Methodological quality assessment (3.2); Analysis of the main characteristics of the studies (3.3); Association between motor skills and the performance of mathematical competences (3.3); Identification of the instruments used in the studies to assess motor skills and mathematical performance (3.4).

3.2. Methodological quality assessment

With regard to the studies’ rating, observed through MQCOM, on a scale from 0 to 16 points, the studies with the lowest rating obtained 12 points (Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018, 2020; Clark et al., 2021) and the highest rating was obtained by the study of Escolano-Pérez et al. (2020) (Table 1). Regarding the methodological characteristics, all 18 studies make reference to the methodology used (item 1), use observation units with criteria (item 4), fit the observation instruments to the study (item 8), the data and parameters are specified in the study (items 11 and 12), and all studies indicate having performed some inferential analysis to analyze the data. It is noteworthy that all studies perform a good interpretation of the results in the discussion section (item 16), except for the studies of Brock et al. (2018) and Osorio-Valencia et al. (2017) which partially met this criterion. Among all the studies, only one does not partially meet the theoretical framework (item 4) nor does it specify the coding manual with definition of the categories/behaviors and specification of the dimensions (Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017). The major limitation of the studies was the non-use of software for data collection, since only the study by Escolano-Pérez et al. (2020) met this criterion and the study by Clark et al. (2021) partially met it. Probably the design of the studies did not require the use of specific software for data collection, analysis, and interpretation.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Methodological characteristics observed using the Methodological Quality Checklist for studies based on Observational Methodology (MQCOM).

3.3. Characteristics of studies

The 18 studies included in this review were observational studies, 11 (61%) longitudinal and 7 (39%) transversals, which identified associations between motor skills and mathematical performance, using validated and reliable instruments for this purpose, in typically-developed children of both sexes, who attended preschool education. Regarding their country of origin, the majority of studies, 10 (56%), were conducted in the United States (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Verdine et al., 2014; Manfra et al., 2017; Brock et al., 2018; Duran et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Greenburg et al., 2020; Clark et al., 2021), followed by three from Germany (17%; Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018, 2020). The remaining five studies were carried out in different countries, namely Singapore (Khng and Ng, 2021), Spain (Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020), South Africa (De Waal, 2019), Mexico (Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017), and Northeast Pacific (Becker et al., 2014; Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of studies that investigated the relationship between motor skills and mathematic performance in children who attended the preschool education with typical development.

Regarding the sample size, the studies presented a minimum of 33 (Clark et al., 2021) and a maximum of 34.491 (Greenburg et al., 2020), with a total of 43.447 participants. All studies included children of both sexes. Regarding teaching frequency, most studies, 12 (67%), exclusively included children from preschool education, seven transversals (Becker et al., 2014; Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018; De Waal, 2019; Fischer et al., 2020; Clark et al., 2021; Khng and Ng, 2021) and five longitudinal (Verdine et al., 2014; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020). The remaining six studies (33%) were longitudinal and included children from preschool education and later years (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Brock et al., 2018; Duran et al., 2018; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Greenburg et al., 2020; Table 2). Regarding age, in studies that included only preschool education, children were aged between 2.7 (Fischer et al., 2018) and 6.8 (Kim et al., 2018), and in studies that involved children from preschool education and beyond, the minimum age was 4 years and the maximum was up to the 5th grade (children between 10 and 11 years old; Greenburg et al., 2020).

The study conducted by Greenburg et al. (2020) was included in this review as the authors distinguished in their research two specific aspects of FMS (FMC and the integration of motor information with visual (VMI) and spatial (VSI) information), addressing in this study these specific motor skills only as VSI and not VMI skills (Table 2).

3.4. Associations between motor skills and mathematical performance

Regarding the study of the associations between motor skills and mathematics, considering the main conclusions from the 18 studies, only in the study carried out by De Waal (2019) whose objective was to determine if there were correlations between motor skills and the academic performance in preschoolers aged 5–6 years old, there was a moderate and significant correlation between a specific component of GMS, namely balance skills (dynamic and static) and mathematical performance (0.46 > r > 0.23; p < 0.05). On the other hand, FMS have been reported to be associated with mathematical performance in 17 studies (Table 2).

When analyzing the distribution of studies that associated mathematical performance with FMS, namely FMC and VMI, it was found that eight studies reported VMI (Becker et al., 2014; Verdine et al., 2014; Brock et al., 2018; Duran et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020; Khng and Ng, 2021), four studies FMC (Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018, 2020; Clark et al., 2021) and five studies reported both FMS (FMC + VMI; Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Greenburg et al., 2020; Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Distribution of studies associating FMS, FMC and VMI to math skills. FMC, fine motor coordination; FMS, fine motor skills; VMI + FMC, visuomotor integration plus fine motor coordination.

Among the VMI, FMS was the one that stood out the most among the 18 studies, having been reported in 13 (72%), and FMC was associated with mathematical performance only in nine (50%). Thus, among FMS, VMI (more than FMC) was more frequently associated with mathematical performance, and is still reported as an important factor for the diagnosis of learning disabilities in mathematics (Khng and Ng, 2021). However, both VMI and FMC were mentioned as strong predictors of mathematical performance in the present (VMI: Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Becker et al., 2014; Duran et al., 2018; Khng and Ng, 2021; FMC: Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2020) and in the future (VMI: Verdine et al., 2014; Manfra et al., 2017; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Brock et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020; Greenburg et al., 2020; CMF: Manfra et al., 2017; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Greenburg et al., 2020).

In the frequency analysis of the association between MS and math skills (Figure 5 and Tables 3, 4), regarding GMS, namely balance, it was associated with the following math skills: counting, measurement (length), shapes and spatial relations (De Waal, 2019). Regarding FMS, both FMC and VMI were associated with all math skills (Figure 5; Tables 3, 4). However, counting was the math skill that was most frequently reported to be associated with FMS. It was associated with FMC in all nine studies (100%; Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Fischer et al., 2020; Greenburg et al., 2020; Clark et al., 2021) and with VMI (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Verdine et al., 2014; Manfra et al., 2017; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Duran et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020; Greenburg et al., 2020; Khng and Ng, 2021) in nine studies (53%). One can highlight the fact that measurement skills (length), shapes (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Greenburg et al., 2020), cardinality (Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Fischer et al., 2020), and comparing (Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018, 2020; Clark et al., 2021) were reported in four studies. Only in one study number identification (Kim et al., 2018), addition, and subtraction (Suggate et al., 2017) were reported to be associated with FMC. Regarding VMI, except the math skill spatial relations, which was only reported in a single study (Kim et al., 2018), all other skills were reported in four or more studies. Thus, in six studies the number identification skill was reported (Verdine et al., 2014; Duran et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020; Khng and Ng, 2021), addition and subtraction (Becker et al., 2014; Brock et al., 2018; Duran et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Khng and Ng, 2021), and measurement (length; Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Duran et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Greenburg et al., 2020). In five studies, the cardinality (Verdine et al., 2014; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Duran et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020) and shapes skills (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Duran et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Greenburg et al., 2020) were reported. The comparing skill was reported in four studies (Verdine et al., 2014; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Khng and Ng, 2021).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Distribution of studies associating FMS (FMC and VMI) and balance with math skills. FMC, fine motor coordination; VMI, visuomotor integration.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Frequencies of associations between motor skills and mathematics by teaching frequency.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Instruments used in the association between motor skills and math skills.

When analyzing data from studies exclusively involving children who attended preschool, it was found that all math skills were associated with both FMC and VMI: counting – in six studies – (Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Fischer et al., 2020; Clark et al., 2021) and cardinality (Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018; Fischer et al., 2020) and comparing – (Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018, 2020; Clark et al., 2021) in four studies – were the most frequently associated with FMC. Regarding VMI, the math skills most frequently associated with it were counting (Verdine et al., 2014; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020; Khng and Ng, 2021) and number identification (Verdine et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2018; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020; Khng and Ng, 2021), both in five studies, and cardinality (Verdine et al., 2014; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020), comparing (Verdine et al., 2014; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Khng and Ng, 2021), and addition and subtraction (Becker et al., 2014; Cameron et al., 2019; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Khng and Ng, 2021) in four studies.

These results suggest that counting, cardinality, and comparing were the math skills most associated with FMC; and counting, number identification, cardinality, comparing, and addition and subtraction with VMI. However, the math skill of counting was the most commonly associated with FMS (FMC and VMI), as of the 17 studies that analyzed this math skill, it was associated with FMS in 13 (76%; Tables 3, 4).

3.5. Instruments used in the studies to assess motor and math skills

In the 18 studies included, 10 instruments were used to assess motor skills (Table 4).

In the only study that associated GMS with mathematical performance (De Waal, 2019), the Kinder kinetics Screening test was used (Pienaar et al., 2016).

To assess FMS, nine instruments were used. To exclusively assess FMC, three instruments were identified: Grooved Pegboard Test (GPT, Strauss et al., 2006; Clark et al., 2021); Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd edition (MABC-2, Henderson et al., 2007; Fischer et al., 2020); Battery designed to provide an estimate of children’s fine motor skills in preschool (BEFMS, Martzog, 2015). To exclusively assess VMI, four instruments were identified: The Brigance Inventory of Early Development III — Standardized (IED III, French, 2013; Khng and Ng, 2021); Test of Visual-Motor Integration, 6th edition (VMI, Beery and Beery, 2010; Becker et al., 2014; Verdine et al., 2014; Brock et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020); Copy Design Task (CDT, Osborn et al., 1984; Nesbitt et al., 2019); NEuroPSYchological assessment battery, 2nd edition (NEPSY, Korkman et al., 1998; Duran et al., 2018). To simultaneously assess FMC and VMI, three instruments were identified: Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic, 3rd edition (LAP-D, Nehring et al., 1992; Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Greenburg et al., 2020); (NEPSY, Korkman et al., 1998; Kim et al., 2018); Peabody developmental motor scale, 2nd edition (PDMS-2, Folio and Fewell, 2000; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017; Table 4).

For the assessment of mathematical performance, 16 instruments were identified among the 18 studies (Table 4).

Only in one study it was identified: Mathematics Sharing Stories, Cwikla, 2014 (Clark et al., 2021); Dot Counting Task, Clark et al., 2021 (Clark et al., 2021); Test of basic instrumental aspects: reading, writing and numerical concepts (PAIB-1, Galve-Manzano et al., 2009; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2020); Finger-based number representations (Wasner et al., 2015; Fischer et al., 2020); Number tasks (Nosworthy, 2013; Fischer et al., 2020); Foundations for learning: Assessment Framework Grade R (De Waal, 2019); McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA, McCarthy, 2004; Osorio-Valencia et al., 2017); Numerical skills (Dowker, 2008; Suggate et al., 2017); Nonfinger-based numerical skills and Finger-based numerical skills (Crollen et al., 2011; Suggate et al., 2017); Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, 3rd edition (WIAT-III, Wechsler, 2007; Verdine et al., 2014).

It was identified in two studies: Test of Early Mathematics Ability – 3rd edition (TEMA-3, Ginsburg and Baroody, 2003; Duran et al., 2018; Khng and Ng, 2021); Test for diagnosing basic math skills (TEDI-MATH, Kaufmann et al., 2009; Fischer et al., 2018, 2020); KeyMath-3 Diagnostic assessment (KeyMath3, Connolly, 2007; Duran et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2018).

The Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic, 3rd edition (LAP-D, Nehring et al., 1992; Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Greenburg et al., 2020) was identified in three studies.

The Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement (WJ-III, Woodcock et al., 2001; Becker et al., 2014; Brock et al., 2018; Duran et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Nesbitt et al., 2019) was identified in five studies.

Thus, of the seven instruments used to assess VMI, the VMI test (Beery and Beery, 2010) was the most used, as it was reported in five studies. To assess FMC, the most commonly used instrument was the LAP-D (Nehring et al., 1992) in three studies (Table 4).

The main characteristics of the instruments used in the assessment of VMI require tasks of copying geometric figures, letters, numbers or objects, using a sheet of paper and pencil. To assess FMC, the main characteristics of the instruments used allowed the assessment of dexterity and manual speed through tasks that demand the handling and manipulation of objects such as threading beads, placing coins, building blocks, turning cylinders, folding paper or cutting it with scissors.

4. Discussion

The objective of this systematic review was to analyze preschool education children with typical development in the association between motor skills and mathematical performance, identify the math skills involved in this association, as well as the instruments used for this purpose to evaluate both motor skills and mathematics.

Based on the results, in relation to the first objective, there was sufficient evidence to support the associations between FMS, namely FMC and VMI, and mathematical academic performance of the children who attended preschool education with typical development. It is noteworthy that only one study has considered associations between a component of GMS, namely balance, and mathematical academic performance (De Waal, 2019).

Similar results to this study were obtained in a systematic review that sought to report the relationships between motor proficiency and academic performance in mathematics and reading in school-age children and adolescents (Macdonald et al., 2018). The authors concluded that FMS were significantly associated with the performance of math skills, particularly in the early school years, and there was no consistency or sufficient evidence to support associations between the specific components of GMS and academic math performance. Likewise, in a systematic review carried out by Van der Fels et al. (2015), in children aged between 4 and 16 years with typical development, most observational studies (86%) reported significant positive associations between FMS and academic performance, especially in children who attended the preschool education up to the 2nd grade.

Studies have shown that GMS are critical for the development of social skills as well as for physical well-being (Cameron et al., 2016), as they influence children’s level of physical activity and health (Logan et al., 2015; Hamilton and Liu, 2018). On the other hand, FMS have been more strongly associated with academic performance (Cameron et al., 2016) and found to influence children’s ability to perform visual motor integration activities, which is important for classroom activities (Strooband et al., 2020). It has been suggested that, at school age, GMS may be important for social affirmation (Ommundsen et al., 2010) and perceived athletic competence (Piek et al., 2006), and FMS for school readiness (Grissmer et al., 2010) and perceived academic competence (Piek et al., 2006).

Despite different purposes, research has shown that during child development, GMS and FMS seem to have some correlation (Roebers and Kauer, 2009; Cameron et al., 2012; Dayem et al., 2015; Oberer et al., 2017), as higher order neuromotor processes seem to be involved simultaneously in the learning of GMS and FMS (Roebers and Kauer, 2009; Oberer et al., 2017). However, studies that investigated the relationship between GMS and FMS, at different stages of school education, obtained controversial results when comparing measures of individual performance of GMS and FMS (Cameron et al., 2012; Amaro et al., 2014; Dayem et al., 2015; Tortella et al., 2016; Oberer et al., 2017). Cameron et al. (2012) and Oberer et al. (2017) showed a moderate correlation between GMS and FMS. Specifically, Oberer et al. (2017) reported a positive correlation in children aged between 5.6 and 7.25 years, assessing gross and fine motor skills through speed and accuracy tasks. Likewise, the investigation by Cameron et al. (2012) also reported a positive correlation in younger children (3–4 years) between GMS, namely balance and jumping and jumping tasks, and FMS, namely building tasks using blocks and drawing tasks. In addition, Dayem et al. (2015), showed an even greater correlation between GMS (assessed by locomotor tasks, object manipulation, and balance tasks) and FMS (assessed by writing tasks) in children aged 4–6 years. On the other hand, other authors disagree on the positive correlation between GMS and FMS (Souza et al., 2010; Amaro et al., 2014; Tortella et al., 2016). The study carried out by Tortella et al. (2016) reported that there was no correlation between GMS and FMS in preschool education children aged between 5 and 6 years, evaluating GMS through precision, balance, throwing, and walking tasks, while FMS were evaluated using speed and precision tasks, such as block constructions and placing coins. Additionally, Souza et al. (2010), when investigating global motor performance using the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Third Edition), found a weak correlation between GMS and FMS. Furthermore, Amaro et al. (2014) did not reported any correlation between GMS and FMS in children aged between 5 and 10 years when comparing the scores obtained in the “Körperkoordinationtest für kinder” and the Minnesota manual dexterity test. These contrasting results can be attributed to the fact that motor skills do not follow linear development trajectories (Souza et al., 2010; Flatters et al., 2014a). Therefore, it is not surprising that investigating children of different ages can produce different results. In addition, these studies assessed motor skills over short periods using heterogeneous tasks (Sorgente et al., 2021).

However, it is important to note that in the same action it is complex to unequivocally differentiate the involvement of each of the motor skills (GMS and FMS), as they coexist and are fundamental for the efficient performance of the task, as both are related and influence each other (Payne and Isaacs, 2012; Flatters et al., 2014b). In this sense, GMS are influenced and influence FMS, the first being also a good predictor of children’s school learning (Sortor and Kulp, 2003; Beery and Beery, 2006; Henderson et al., 2007; Spanaki et al., 2008; Kambas et al., 2010; Kadkol et al., 2014; Byers et al., 2016; Africa and Deventer, 2017; Bellocchi et al., 2017).

However, there are very few studies in the literature that investigated the association between GMS and academic performance (De Waal, 2019; Sorgente et al., 2021), so it is a topic that can be the subject of further research.

Although in this review only one study associates GMS, namely balance, with mathematical performance (De Waal, 2019), it should also be analyzed. In a study carried out by Vuijk et al. (2011), the authors also found significant correlations between balance and mathematics, however this study was developed in children with learning difficulties at school age. Balance, especially when vision is not used, depends a lot on the effective functioning of the vestibular system involved in the execution of controlled movements (Cheatum and Hammond, 2000). In this sense, problems in the vestibular system not only lead to delays in motor proficiency, but can also negatively affect the performance of activities in the classroom (Cheatum and Hammond, 2000). However, in a recent systematic review, the results concluded that balance did not significantly correlate with mathematical academic performance, as in other GMS considered (Macdonald et al., 2018). Regarding FMS, of the 17 studies that associated these skills with mathematical performance, VMI was the one that stood out the most, being reported in 13 studies (76%), followed by FMC in nine (53%). However, by itself, this result does not mean that the VMI of FMS will be the most predictive of mathematical performance, as studies that included the two FMS concluded that FMC and VMI were both predictors of mathematical performance (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Greenburg et al., 2020), except the study carried out by Fischer et al. (2020), in which FMC was the only one associated with mathematical performance and the studies conducted by Escolano-Pérez et al. (2020) and Osorio-Valencia et al. (2017), in which on the contrary, VMI was the only one associated with mathematical performance. Studies in which only FMC (Suggate et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2018; Clark et al., 2021) or VMI (Becker et al., 2014; Verdine et al., 2014; Brock et al., 2018; Duran et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019; Nesbitt et al., 2019; Khng and Ng, 2021) were analyzed concluded that these motor skills were associated with mathematical performance. Thus, each FMS was used according to the aim of each study. In this sense, although VMI appears as the most frequently associated with mathematical performance, it does not mean a priori that it will have a greater degree of importance than FMC. These results suggest that both FMS (FMC and VMI) are important predictors of mathematical performance depending only on the objectives of the studies and instruments used for this purpose.

VMI involves the integration of visual and motor skills (Sortor and Kulp, 2003; Beery and Beery, 2006) coordinated through the fingers and hands, that is, FMC (Gabbard et al., 2001; Beery and Beery, 2006; Feder and Majnemer, 2007; Bezrukikh and Terebova, 2009; Kambas et al., 2010; Kadkol et al., 2014; Byers et al., 2016). In this sense, VMI implies the mental representation of an image that is replicated by controlling the meticulous movement of the fingers (Carlson et al., 2013). Thus, FMC plays a very important role in school success (Sortor and Kulp, 2003; Roebers et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2015; Fischer et al., 2020) because children with better FMC will be better at handling objects such as pencils or notebooks, which allows them to direct additional attention resources toward learning, rather than focusing them on movements associated with FMC (Kim et al., 2018). FMC can also serve as a fundamental competency by which more complex processes can be built (Sortor and Kulp, 2003), namely for the development of VMI skills (Kim et al., 2018). There is also some evidence that FMC is linked to mathematics through its contribution to the development of VMI (Kim et al., 2018). Thus, a child with good FMC, when performing an academic task, may impose a lower cognitive load compared to a child who still has difficulties in FMC (Luo et al., 2007; Cameron et al., 2015). Cameron et al. (2012) conducted an observational study in classrooms at preschool education and found that 46% of the school day was dedicated to activities involving FMS (FMC and VMI). These activities included tasks such as writing, drawing, using scissors to cut paper, bean counting tasks, and playing with toys like building blocks and Legos. In this sense, FMC and VMI are considered essential for early (Lillard, 2005) and interdependent learning (Kim et al., 2018), proving to be a powerful predictor for school readiness (Bala et al., 2010; Grissmer et al., 2010), adaptation and transition from the preschool education to the 1st grade (Bart et al., 2007) and end of 6th grade (Pagani et al., 2010) and also a great predictor of subsequent academic performance, especially in reading and mathematics (Cameron et al., 2012; Dinehart and Manfra, 2013). On the other hand, low levels of performance in FMS are associated with learning difficulties in the areas of reading, writing, and mathematics (Coetzee and Gerber, 2018).

Another objective of this study was to verify the frequencies with which math skills were associated with MS. Regarding GMS, only balance (static and dynamic) was associated with the math skills of counting, measurement (length), shapes, and space (De Waal, 2019). Despite these data, and as only the study of De Waal (2019) with a relatively small sample (n = 30) associated balance with math skills, it would be too premature to conclude in a sustained way the influence of this HMG on math skills. It is suggested that further studies consider the association between balance and math skills in order to support such conclusions.

Regarding FMS, all math skills proposed for Preschool Education (National Research Council, 2009) were associated with both FMC and VMI. Regardless of the type and characteristics of the studies, counting was the math skill most often associated with FMS, FMC, and VMI.

Counting can be seen as an infinitely long and ordered list of numbers that allows you to quantify what you want. In essence, counting is a way of doing a 1 to 1 correspondence between objects. Generally, each successive counting number describes a quantity that is one more than the previous quantity. Similarly, counting backwards is subtracting one from the previous number (National Research Council, 2009). In this sense, children, in addition to developing the notion of cardinality (Hannula, 2005), are performing addition and subtraction actions that are fundamental for solving problems (Siegler and Shrager, 1984). Children who make frequent counting errors have difficulty calculating (Geary et al., 2007). Since children show a spontaneous tendency to count, educators should take advantage of this to stimulate their practice with the aim of developing not only counting skills but other associated math skills (Hannula, 2005).

Another objective of this review was to describe the instruments used in the studies to assess and associated motor skills with mathematics. Regarding motor skills, in the 18 studies, 10 instruments were used. The study that assessed and associated one of the GMS (balance) with mathematical skills (De Waal, 2019), the Kinder kinetics Screening test was the instrument used (Pienaar et al., 2016). This instrument assesses the basic skills of fundamental movements in children aged between 3 and 6 years. The skills assessed were the fundamental locomotor, balance (dynamic and static), and object manipulation skills (Pienaar et al., 2016).

Regarding FMS, nine instruments were used in the 17 studies analyzed, three to assess FMC exclusively, four to assess VMI and three to assess both FMC and VMI.

In the assessment of FMS, although the LAP-D (Nehring et al., 1992) was the most used (three studies), its characteristics are similar to the other instruments used only in a study each, namely the NEPSY (Korkman et al., 1998), and the MABC-2 (Henderson et al., 2007), the BEFMS (Martzog, 2015), and the PDMS-2 (Folio and Fewell, 2000). Thus, these instruments assess manual dexterity and speed through tasks such as fitting pins, turning cylinders, stringing beads, building with blocks, folding paper, cutting with scissors, and putting coins in a slot.

In the assessment of VMI, the most commonly used instrument was the VMI (five studies; Beery and Beery, 2010). Although this instrument was the most commonly used, its tasks are also similar to the other instruments used in the assessment of VMI. The main tasks assessed by these instruments consist of copying geometric figures, letters or numbers, and drawing objects using a sheet of paper and pencil.

The literature has shown that the tasks of copying figures or drawing are the most used tasks to assess VMI, and object handling tasks with pinch-like movements are the most used to assess FMC (Davis and Matthews, 2010; Carlson et al., 2013; Newman and Feinberg, 2015; MacDonald et al., 2016).

The literature shows the importance that the instruments used to assess the development of motor skills have in tracking possible difficulties associated with mathematical performance (Kim et al., 2015). Most of these studies used direct neuropsychological assessments (Kim et al., 2015), such as the NEuroPSYchological Assessment Battery – NEPSY (Korkman et al., 1998) and the Visuomotor Integration test (VMI; Beery and Beery, 2010). However, it is recognized that these instruments are usually time consuming and expensive and are only administered by experts for this purpose (Cameron et al., 2012; Williford et al., 2013). In this sense, the characteristics of the instruments most used for the diagnosis of difficulties associated with mathematical performance may be a limitation in relation to the reality of kindergartens. Thus, the main instruments cited in the literature for the assessment of motor skills associated with mathematical performance practically make it impossible for educators to carry out these assessments on their students (Kim et al., 2015).

Regarding the instruments to assess math skills, in the 18 studies analyzed, 16 different instruments were used. Most studies, 78% (14), used only one instrument, the remaining 22% (4) used more than one, depending on the proposed objective. However, the instrument most used to assess math skills was WJ-III (application problems subscale; Woodcock et al., 2001). The WJ-III battery is considered the most complete to explain intellectual functioning, existing in two versions, the first design to assess cognitive abilities (standard form) and the second to assess academic performance (Mather and Gregg, 2002).

It is noteworthy that in the three studies that used the same instrument, namely the LAP-D, to assess FMS (FMC and VMI) and math skills (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013; Manfra et al., 2017; Greenburg et al., 2020) the result was similar, as FMS were associated with the same math skills: counting, measurement (length), and shapes. The LAP-D is an instrument used to measure children’s academic readiness during preschool education. It includes domains for cognitive development, mathematics, and language, and FMS, which includes tasks to assess FMC and VMI (Nehring et al., 1992).

Similarly, in the three studies that used the same instruments for the assessment of both FMS and math skills, the results were similar, i.e., the studies that used the VMI test (Beery and Beery, 2010) for the assessment of FMS and the WJ-III (Woodcock et al., 2001) for the assessment of math skills recorded a significant association between VMI and the same math skills: addition and subtraction (Becker et al., 2014; Brock et al., 2018; Duran et al., 2018; Cameron et al., 2019).

On the other hand, when different instruments were used in the assessment of motor and math skills, the results were different. This suggests that each instrument should measure exactly what it sets out to measure considering the objectives outlined by the researchers (Roberts and Priest, 2006; Mokkink et al., 2010).

The results of this review may have important implications for the implementation of new strategies for teaching mathematics in preschool, as evidence was sufficient to support the influences of FMS (FMC and VMI) on mathematical academic achievement.

It has also been suggested that math skills are improved primarily from an age-adjusted math teaching intervention, better preparing children for school tasks (Sarama and Clements, 2004). However, cognitive skills arise from experiences, motor skills, and sensory-motor skills (Barsalou, 2010; Fischer, 2012). Most often children learn number skills with the help of objects that are typically small (cubes, buttons, etc.; Sarama and Clements, 2016) which requires precise fine motor handling (Luo et al., 2007). This handling will only be effective if the child is aware of the quantities represented by the object (Sarama and Clements, 2016). If the child has a deficit in FMS, it will be more difficult for the child to assign quantitative meaning to objects because the child will focus more on the fine motor actions than on the quantity of the objects (Carr and Davis, 2001). This finding contributes to support that children with learning disabilities in mathematics exhibit the least proficient FMS (Pieters et al., 2012). Since FMS can be automated (Floyer-Lea and Matthews, 2004), interventions in FMS will be recommended to free up cognitive resources for other learning tasks.

In this sense, the preschool curriculum should include guidelines to promote the development of FMS and thus better prepare children for mathematical learning. But, in order to plan a program to promote the development of FMS, a prior assessment of the child is necessary. However, educators lack training to assess FMS, and thus limitations to design appropriate programs adjusted to the needs (Gehris et al., 2015; Cueto et al., 2017).

5. Conclusion

This systematic review contributes considerably to the literature, as it found only evidence that supports positive associations between FMS, namely FMC and VMI, and mathematical performance in children with typical development in preschool education, with counting being the mathematical skill most associated with FMS (FMC and VMI). The main characteristics of the instruments used showed that the tasks of copying of figures or drawing are the most used to assess VMI and the tasks of handling objects with pinch-like movements are the most used to evaluate FMC. However, it has been recognized that these instruments are usually time consuming and expensive and are generally only administered by experts for this purpose.

Given the importance of FMS in mathematical skills, there is an urgent need to empower educators with tools to enhance the development of FMS in the classroom context. In this sense, early identification of children with difficulties in fine motor skills will help educators to design better strategies for teaching mathematical skills. Since the initial assessment is fundamental to plan an intervention adjusted to the child, it will be necessary to identify instruments with characteristics that allow their application in the classroom context, i.e., that require little administration time, do not require much experience or training, the possibility of being applied to the group/class, few material resources, and the results can be easily interpreted, classified, and associated with mathematical performance.

5.1. Limitations

It is essential to recognize that there were also some limitations in this review. First, it is important to point out that there is an evident gap in the literature of studies that report the association between gross motor skills and mathematical performance in preschool children. Second, there is the considerable heterogeneity of instruments used across studies to assess motor and math skills, making it difficult to compare and clearly interpret the results across studies. Third, the fact that some included studies report covariates that may influence the results, such as demographic factors (for example, socioeconomic status) and cognitive factors (such as executive function and its components). Covariates reported by each eligible study were not discussed as they were beyond the scope of this review.

5.2. Recommendations and implications for future research

In order to allow a more accurate comparison of results between studies in the future, researchers should consider consistent use of valid, reliable, and homogeneous standardized instruments. In addition, studies should control demographic, cognitive and physical confounding factors. Finally, as students with neurodevelopmental disorders attend regular schools, future investigations should also examine the relationships between motor and math skills in this population to inform possible forms of intervention.

5.3. Recommendations and implications for policies and practices

As it is known that children with FMS difficulties may present negative mathematical performance and given the importance of math performance in future school results, early identification of these difficulties will help educators to design better strategies for teaching math skills. However, although the main instruments reported in the literature accurately assess FMS, these instruments tend to be expensive, time-consuming to administer, require individualized assessments, demand a lot of training and experience, and are usually administered by experts. Thus, the characteristics of most instruments for the evaluation of FMS reveal limitations in the view of our kindergartens’ reality. In this way, their administration in the classroom context by educators can be very conditioned taking into account the characteristics of most classes, which are very numerous and with very extensive curricula.

Due to its importance for academic success, it is considered that educators should carry out an assessment of the development of FMS to their students, in order to detect possible problems associated with mathematical performance. Eventually, if the child shows development problems in FMS, he/she should be referred for a specialized evaluation by a technician with qualifications for this purpose. For this reason, there is an urgent need for a new instrument to evaluate FMS in preschool education children with the ability to adjust to the reality of our kindergartens, that is, one that requires less administration time, does not require much experience or specialized training, has the possibility of being administered to the group/class in a classroom school context, requires few material resources and produces results that can be easily interpreted, classified and associated with mathematical performance. In this sense, this instrument can be a starting point for the early detection of FMS deficits and, consequently, the referral of the child for a new reassessment by a qualified professional. Furthermore, if the child has problems at this level, he or she may benefit from a timely intervention by a specialist and, consequently, prevent/reduce his/her difficulties in mathematical performance.

However, despite FMS being significantly associated with math performance, these skills also require the involvement of GMS. In this sense, it would be important to know which GMS can have the most influence on FMS so that a possible motor intervention program is more efficient and more likely to be successful.

For future research, we suggest the identification of instruments to assess fine motor skills in preschool children, with characteristics that allow their administration by the educator in the classroom context.

Author contributions

PFl: conceptualization and writing draft preparation. PFl, EC, MM-C, and PFo: methodology and research. PFl, MM-C, and PFo: formal analysis. EC, MM-C, and PFo: writing revision and editing and supervision. All authors have read and agreed to the version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by national funds (FCT—Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology) under the project UIBD/DTP/04045/2020.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abe, M., and Hanakawa, T. (2009). Functional coupling underlying motor and cognitive functions of the dorsal premotor cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 198, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adolph, K. E. (2005). “Learning to learn in the development of action” in Action as an organizer of learning and development. eds. J. J. Rieser, J. J. Lockman, and C. A. Nelson, The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, vol. 33 (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 91–122.

Google Scholar

Adolph, K. E., and Franchak, J. M. (2017). The development of motor behavior. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 8:e1430. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Africa, E. K., and Deventer, K. J. (2017). A motor-skills programme to enhance visual motor integration on selected pre-school learners. Early Child Dev. Care 187, 1960–1970. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1201478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarez-Bueno, C., Pesce, C., Cavero-Redondo, I., Sánchez-López, M., Garrido-Miguel, M., and Martínez-Vizcaíno, V. (2017). Academic achievement and physical activity: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics 140:e20171498. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-1498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amaro, N., Coelho, L., Cruz, J., Matos, R., and Morouço, P. (2014). Correlation between fine and Gross motor coordination in children. Rev. Saúde Públ. Leiria 48:273.

Google Scholar

Anderson, J. R. (2000). Cognitive psychology and its implications (5th ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Técnicas de análisis en estudios observacionales en ciencias del deporte [Analyses techniques studies in sport science]. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte 15, 13–30. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bala, G., Krneta, Z., and Katiæ, R. (2010). Effects of kindergarten period on school readiness and motor abilities. Coll. Antropol. 34, 61–67.

Google Scholar

Barnett, L. M., Hinkley, T., Okely, A. D., Hesketh, K., and Salmon, J. (2012). Use of electronic games by young children and fundamental movement skills? Percept. Mot. Skills 114, 1023–1034. doi: 10.2466/10.13.PMS.114.3.1023-1034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnett, L. M., Zask, A., Rose, L., Hughes, D., and Adams, J. (2015). Three-year follow-up of an early childhood intervention: what about physical activity and weight status? J. Phys. Act. Health 12, 319–321. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2013-0419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: past, present, and future. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2, 716–724. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bart, O., Hajami, D., and Bar-Haim, Y. (2007). Predicting school adjustment from motor abilities in kindergarten. Infant Child Dev. 16, 597–615. doi: 10.1002/icd.514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barth, H., Beckmann, L., and Spelke, E. S. (2008). Nonsymbolic, approximate arithmetic in children: abstract addition prior to instruction. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1466–1477. doi: 10.1037/a0013046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, D. R., Miao, A., Duncan, R., and McClelland, M. M. (2014). Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor skills and early academic achievement. Early Child. Res. Q. 29, 411–424. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, K. E., and Beery, N. A. (2006). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual motor integration for children and adults: Administration, scoring, and teaching manual, 6th ed. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.

Google Scholar

Beery, K. E., and Beery, N. A. (2010). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual motor integration: Administration, scoring, and teaching manual, 7th ed. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson.

Google Scholar

Beery, K. E., and Buktenica, N. A. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration: Administration, scoring and teaching manual. 4th ed. Parsippany, NJ: Modern Curriculum.

Google Scholar

Bellocchi, S., Muneaux, M., Huau, A., Lévêque, Y., Jover, M., and Ducrot, S. (2017). Exploring the link between visual perception, visual-motor integration, and reading in normal developing and impaired children using DTVP-2. Dyslexia 23, 296–315. doi: 10.1002/dys.1561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bezrukikh, M. M., and Terebova, N. (2009). Characteristics of the development of visual perception in five-to seven year old children. Hum. Physiol. 35, 684–689. doi: 10.1134/S0362119709060048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. Am. Psychol. 57, 111–127. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brock, L. L., Kim, H., and Grissmer, D. W. (2018). Longitudinal associations among executive function, visuomotor integration, and achievement in a high-risk sample. Mind Brain Educ. 12, 23–27. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruininks, R. H., and Bruininks, B. (2005). Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency. 2nd ed. Circle Pines: AGS Publishing.

Google Scholar

Byers, A. I., Cameron, C. E., Michelle, K., LoCasale-Crouch, J., and Grissmer, D. W. (2016). What preschool classroom experiences ate associated with whether children improve in visuomotor integration? Early Educ. Dev. 27, 976–1003. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1175243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. E., Brock, L., Hatfield, B., Cottone, E., Rubinstein, E., LoCasale-Crouch, J., et al. (2015). Visuomotor integration and inhibitory control compensate for each other in school readiness. Dev. Psychol. 51, 1529–1543. doi: 10.1037/a0039740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Murrah, W. M., Bell, L. H., Worzalla, S. L., Grissmer, D., et al. (2012). Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Dev. 83, 1229–1244. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01768.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. E., Cottone, E., Murrah, W. M., and Grissmer, D. (2016). How ae motor skills linked to Children’s school performance and academic achievement? Child Dev. Perspect. 10, 93–98. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. E., Kim, H., Ducan, R., Becker, D., and McClelland, M. (2019). Bidirectional and co-developing association of cognitive, mathematics, and literacy skills during kindergarten. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 62, 135–144. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2019.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canobi, K. H., and Bethune, N. E. (2008). Number words in young children’s conceptual and procedural knowledge of addition, subtraction, and inversion. Cognition 108, 675–686. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantell, M., Smyth, M., and Ahonen, T. (2003). Two distinct pathways for developmental coordination disorder: persistence and resolution. Hum. Mov. Sci. 22, 413–431. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2003.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, A. G., Rowe, E. W., and Curby, T. W. (2013). Disentangling fine motor skills’ relation to academic achievement: the differential impact of visual-spatial integration and visual motor coordination. J. Genet. Psychol. 174, 514–533. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2012.717122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, M., and Alexeev, N. (2011). Fluency, accuracy, and gender predict developmental trajectories of arithmetic strategies. J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 617–631. doi: 10.1037/a0023864

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, M., and Davis, H. (2001). Gender differences in arithmetic strategy use: a function of skill and preference. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 26, 330–347. doi: 10.1006/ceps.2000.1059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, V., Hunter, S., Kuzik, N., Wiebe, S. A., Spence, J. C., Friedman, A., et al. (2016). Systematic review of physical activity and cognitive development in early childhood. J. Sci. Med. Sport 19, 573–578. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2015.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chacón-Moscoso, S., Anguera, M. T., Sanduvete-Chaves, S., Losada-López, J. L., Lozano-Lozano, J. A., and Portell, M. (2019). Methodological quality checklist for studies based on observational methodology (MQCOM). Psicothema 31, 458–464. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2019.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chacón-Moscoso, S., Sanduvete-Chaves, S., Anguera, M. T., Losada, J. L., Portell, M., and Lozano-Lozano, J. A. (2018). Preliminary checklist for reporting observational studies in sports areas: content validity. Front. Psychol. 9:291.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chacón-Moscoso, S., Sanduvete-Chaves, S., and Sánchez-Martín, M. (2016). The development of a checklist to enhance methodological quality in intervention programs. Front. Psychol. 7:1811. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M., and Gu, X. (2018). The role of executive function in linking fundamental motor skills and reading proficiency in socioeconomically disadvantaged kindergarteners. Learn. Individ. Differ. 61, 250–255. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheatum, B. A., and Hammond, A. A. (2000). Physical activities for improving children’s learning and behavior: A guide to sensory motor development. Champaign: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Claesens, A., and Engel, M. (2013). How important is where you start? Early mathematics knowledge and later school success. Teach. Coll. Rec. 115, 1–29. doi: 10.1177/016146811311500603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, L., Shelley-Tremblay, J., and Cwikla, J. (2021). Shared developmental trajectories for fractional reasoning and fine motor ability in 4 and 5 year olds. Behav. Sci. 11:26. doi: 10.3390/bs11020026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clements, D. H., and Sarama, J. (2007). “Early childhood mathematics learning” in Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning. ed. F. K. LesterJr. (New York: Information Age), 461–555.

Google Scholar

Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., and DiBiase, A. M. (2004). Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.

Google Scholar

Coetzee, D., and Gerber, B. (2018). Difference between visual-motor integration status of typically developed learners and learners with learning-related problems. S. Afr. J. Res. Sport Phys. Educ. Recreat. 40, 41–52.

Google Scholar

Connolly, A. (2007). KeyMath3 TM diagnostic assessment. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Crollen, V., Mahe, R., Collignon, O., and Seron, X. (2011). The role of vision in the development of finger–number interactions: finger-counting and finger-montring in blind children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109, 525–539. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cueto, S., Prieto, J., Nistal, P., Abelairas-Gómez, C., Barcala, R., and López, S. (2017). Teachers’ perceptions of preschool children’s psychomotor development in Spain. Percept. Mot. Skills 124, 725–739. doi: 10.1177/0031512517705534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curry, M., and Outhred, L. (2005). “Conceptual understanding of spatial measurement” in Building connections: Research, theory, and practice: Proceedings of the 28th annual conference of the mathematics education research Group of Australasia. eds. P. Clarkson, A. Downtown, D. Gronn, M. Horne, A. McDonough, and R. Pierce, et al. (Melbourne, Australia: MERGA), 265–272. Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Pica, P., and Spelke, E.S

Google Scholar

Cwikla, J. (2014). Can kindergartners do fractions? Teach. Child. Math. 20, 354–364. doi: 10.5951/teacchilmath.20.6.0354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Hondt, E., Deforche, B., Gentier, I., Verstuyf, J., Vaeyens, R., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., et al. (2014). A longitudinal study of gross motor coordination and weight status in children. Obesity 22, 1505–1511. doi: 10.1002/oby.20723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Souza, H., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2017). Neurodevelopmental disorders. WIREs. Cogn. Sci. 8:e1398. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. L., and Matthews, R. N. (2010). Review of NEPSY-second edition (NEPSY-II). J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 28, 175–182. doi: 10.1177/0734282909346716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayem, T. S. A. E., Salem, E. E., and Hadidy, E. I. E. (2015). Correlation between Gross motor activities and hand writing skills in elementary school children. Trends Appl. Sci. Res. 10, 259–269. doi: 10.3923/tasr.2015.259.269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Kieviet, J. F., Piek, J. P., Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., and Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Motor development in very preterm and very low-birth-weight children from birth to adolescence: a meta-analysis. JAMA 302, 2235–2242. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Waal, E. (2019). Fundamental movement skills and academic performance of 5- to 6-year-old Preschoolers. Early Childhood Educ. J. 47, 455–464. doi: 10.1007/s10643-019-00936-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Department for Education (2020). Early years foundation stage profile: 2020 handbook ; Department for Education: London, UK.

Google Scholar

Desoete, A., Ceulemans, A., DeWeerdt, F., and Pieters, S. (2012). Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 64–81. doi: 10.1348/2044-8279.002002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine, A., Hill, F., Carey, E., and Szucs, D. (2018). Cognitive and emotional math problems largely dissociate: prevalence of developmental dyscalculia and mathematics anxiety. J. Educ. Psychol. 110, 431–444. doi: 10.1037/edu0000222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine, A., Soltész, F., Nobes, A., Goswami, U., and Szücs, D. (2013). Gender differences in developmental dyscalculia depend on diagnostic criteria. Learn. Instr. 27, 31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 71, 44–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2015). Effects of physical exercise on executive functions: going beyond simply moving to moving with thought. Ann. Sports Med. Res. 2, 1011–1015.

Google Scholar

Dinehart, L., and Manfra, L. (2013). Associations between low-income Children's fine motor skills in preschool and academic performance in second grade. Early Educ. Dev. 24, 138–161. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2011.636729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donato, H., and Donato, M. (2019). Stages for undertaking a systematic review. Acta Medica Port. 32, 227–235. doi: 10.20344/amp.11923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donnelly, J. E., Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D., Etnier, J. L., Lee, S., Tomporowski, P., et al. (2016). Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 1197–1222. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowker, A. (2008). Individual differences in numerical abilities in preschoolers. Dev. Sci. 11, 650–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00713.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, G. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Klebanov, P., Pagani, L. S., Feinstein, L., et al. (2007). School readiness sand later achievement. Dev. Psychol. 43, 1428–1446. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, M., Cunningham, A., and Eyre, E. (2019). A combined movement and story-telling intervention enhances motor competence and language ability in pre-schoolers to a greater extent than movement or story-telling alone. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 25, 221–235. doi: 10.1177/1356336X17715772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duran, C. A. K., Byers, A., Cameron, C. E., and Grissmer, D. (2018). Unique and compensatory associations of executive functioning and visuomotor integration with mathematics performance in early elementary school. Early Child. Res. Q. 42, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, J., Berube, M., Erlandson, K., Haug, S., Johnstone, H., Meagher, M., et al. (2011). Developmental coordination disorder in school-aged children born very preterm and/or at very low birth weight: a systematic review. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 32, 678–687. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31822a396a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolano-Pérez, E., Herrero-Nivela, M. L., and Losada, J. L. (2020). Association between Preschoolers’ specific fine (but not Gross) motor skills and later academic competencies: educational implications. Front. Psychol. 11:1044. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feder, K. P., and Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development, competency, and intervention. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 312–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00312.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feder, K. P., Majnemer, A., Bourbonnais, D., Platt, R., Blayney, M., and Synnes, A. (2005). Handwriting performance in preterm children compared with term peers at age 6 to 7 years. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47, 163–170. doi: 10.1017/S0012162205000307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, V. R., Ribeiro, M. L. S., Melo, T., de Tarso Maciel-Pinheiro, P., Guimarães, T. T., Araújo, N. B., et al. (2016). Motor coordination correlates with academic achievement and cognitive function in children. Front. Psychol. 7:318. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, M. H. (2012). A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied, and situated numerical cognition. Cogn. Process. 13, 161–164. doi: 10.1007/s10339-012-0477-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, M. H., and Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: spatial-numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied cognition. Front. Psychol. 2:260. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, U., Suggate, S. P., Schmirl, J., and Stoeger, H. (2018). Counting on fine motor skills: links between preschool finger dexterity and numerical skills. Dev. Sci. 21:e12623. doi: 10.1111/desc.12623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, U., Suggate, S. P., and Stoeger, H. (2020). The implicit contribution of fine motor skills to mathematical insight in early childhood. Front. Psychol. 11:1143. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flatters, I., Mushtaq, F., Hill, L. J. B., Holt, R. J., Wilkie, R. M., and Williams, M. M. (2014a). The relationship between a child's postural stability and manual dexterity. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 2907–2917. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3947-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flatters, I., Mushtaq, F., Hill, L. J. B., Rossiter, A., Jarrett-Peet, K., Culmer, P., et al. (2014b). Children’s head movements and postural stability as a function of task. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 1953–1970. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3886-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Floyer-Lea, A., and Matthews, P. M. (2004). Changing brain networks for visuomotor control with increased movement automaticity. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2405–2412. doi: 10.1152/jn.01092.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folio, M., and Fewell, R. (2000). Peabody developmental motor scales (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

Google Scholar

French, B. (2013). Brigance inventory of early development (IED III): IED III standardization and validation manual. North Billerica, USA: Curriculum Associates.

Google Scholar

Fuchs, L. S., Gilbert, J. K., Powell, S. R., Cirino, P. T., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., et al. (2016). The role of cognitive processes, foundational math skill, and calculation accuracy and fluency in word-problem solving versus prealgebraic knowledge. Dev. Psychol. 52, 2085–2098. doi: 10.1037/dev0000227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuhs, M. W., Nesbitt, K. T., Farran, D. C., and Dong, N. (2014). Longitudinal associations between executive functioning and academic skills across content areas. Dev. Psychol. 50, 1698–1709. doi: 10.1037/a0036633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabbard, C., Gonçalves, V. M., and Santos, D. C. (2001). Visual-motor integration problems in low birth weight infants. J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 8, 199–204. doi: 10.1023/A:1011373721162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galve-Manzano, J., Ramos-Sánchez, J., Martínez-Arias, R., and Trallero-Sanz, M. (2009). Prueba de Aspectos Instrumentales Básicos: Lectura, Escritura y Conceptos Numéricos. PAIB-1. Madrid: CEPE.

Google Scholar

Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., and Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Dev. 78, 1343–1359. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01069.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geertsen, S., Thomas, R., Larsen, M., Dahn, I., Andersen, J., Krause-Jensen, M., et al. (2016). Motor skills and exercise capacity are associated with objective measures of cognitive functions and academic performance in preadolescent children. PLoS One 11:e0161960. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehris, J. S., Gooze, R. A., and Whitaker, R. C. (2015). Teachers’ perceptions about children’s movement and learning in early childhood education programmes. Child Care Health Dev. 41, 122–131. doi: 10.1111/cch.12136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, R., and Williams, E. M. (1997). “Enabling constraints for cognitive development and learning: domain specificity and epigenesis” in Cognition, perception, and language, volume 2: Handbook of child psychology. eds. D. Kuhn and R. Siegler. 5th ed (New York: Wiley), 575–630.

Google Scholar

Gentier, I., D’Hondt, E., Shultz, S., Deforche, B., Augustijn, M., Hoorne, S., et al. (2013). Fine and gross motor skills differ between healthy-weight and obese children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 4043–4051. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gersten, R., Jordan, N. C., and Flojo, J. R. (2005). Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. J. Learn. Disabil. 38, 293–304. doi: 10.1177/00222194050380040301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsburg, H. P., and Baroody, A. (2003). Test of early mathematics ability (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro Ed.

Google Scholar

Ginsburg, H. P., Goldberg Kaplan, R., Cannon, J., Cordero, M. L., Eisenband, J. G., Galanter, M., et al. (2006). “Helping early childhood educators to teach mathematics” in Critical issues in early childhood professional development. eds. M. Zaslow and I. Martinez-Beck (Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. Inc.), 171–202.

Google Scholar

Goodway, J., Ozmun, J., and Gallahue, D. (2019). Understanding motor development: infants, children, adolescents, adults, 8th ed. Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learning.

Google Scholar

Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1986). “Words, plans, things, and locations: interactions between semantic and cognitive development in the one-word stage” in The development of word meaning. eds. S. A. KuczajII and M. D. Barrett (Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag), 199–223.

Google Scholar

Greenburg, J. E., Carlson, A. G., Kim, H., Curby, T. W., and Winsler, A. (2020). Early visual-spatial integration skills predict elementary school achievement among low-income, ethnically diverse children. Early Educ. Dev. 31, 304–322. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2019.1636353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., and Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: two new school readiness indicators. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1008–1017. doi: 10.1037/a0020104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haapala, E. A., Lintu, N., Väistö, J., Tompuri, T., Soininen, S., Viitasalo, A., et al. (2019). Longitudinal associations of fitness, motor competence, and adiposity with cognition. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51, 465–471. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haapala, E. A., Poikkeus, A. M., Tompuri, T., Kukkonen-Harjula, K., Leppänen, P. H., Lindi, V., et al. (2014). Associations of motor and cardiovascular performance with academic skills in children. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 46, 1016–1024. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M., and Liu, T. (2018). The effects of an intervention on the gross and fine motor skills of Hispanic pre-K children from low ses backgrounds. Early Childhood Educ. J. 46, 223–230. doi: 10.1007/s10643-017-0845-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannula, M. M. (2005). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in the development of early mathematical skills. Turku, Finland: University of Turku.

Google Scholar

Haywood, K. M., and Getchell, N. (2019). Life span motor development, 7th ed. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Henderson, S., Sugden, E., Barnett, A., and Smits Engelsman, C. (2007). Movement assessment battery for Children-2. USA, Pearson.

Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H., Charman, T., Pickles, A., and Jones, E. J. H. (2021). Annual research review: anterior modifiers in the emergence of neurodevelopmental disorders (AMEND)—a systems neuroscience approach to common developmental disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 62, 610–630. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, E. J. H., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T., and Johnson, M. H. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: a review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 39, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D., Innerd, A., Giles, E. L., and Azevedo, L. B. (2021). The association between physical activity, motor skills and school readiness in 4–5-year-old children in the northeast of England. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:11931. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182211931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. S., and Smith, L. B. (2002). How children know the relevant properties for generalizing object names. Dev. Sci. 5, 219–232. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Nabors Oláh, L., and Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: a longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Dev. 77, 153–175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00862.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., and Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 45, 850–867. doi: 10.1037/a0014939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadkol, M. S., Parkar, S. R., and Chadha, N. (2014). Sensory processing and visuo motor integration in preschoolers: unserstanding the link. Indian J. Physiother. Occup. Ther. 8:1. doi: 10.5958/j.0973-5674.8.2.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambas, A., Fatouros, Y., Christoforidis, C., Venetsanou, F., Papageorgiou, P., Giannakidou, D., et al. (2010). The effects of pshychomotor intervention, on visual-motor control as a graphomotor aspect in preschool age. Eur. Psychomotricity J. 3, 54–61.

Google Scholar

Kantomaa, M. T., Stamatakis, E., Kankaanpää, A., Kaakinen, M., Rodriguez, A., Taanila, A., et al. (2013). Physical activity and obesity mediate the association between childhood motor function and adolescents' academic achievement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 1917–1922. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214574110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, L., Nuerk, H., Graf, M., Krinzinger, H., Delazer, M., and Willmes, K. (2009). TEDI-MATH. Test for assessment of numerical and calculation skills from preschool to third grade. Bern: Hans Huber.

Google Scholar

Khng, K. H., and Ng, E. L. (2021). Fine motor and executive functioning skills predict maths and spelling skills at the start of kindergarten: a compensatory account. Infancia y Aprendizaje 44, 675–718. doi: 10.1080/02103702.2021.1897232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H., Duran, C. A. K., Cameron, C. E., and Grissmer, D. (2018). Developmental relations among motor and cognitive processes and mathematics skills. Child Dev. 89, 476–494. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H., Murrah, W. M., Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Cottone, E. A., and Grissmer, D. (2015). Psychometric properties of the teacher-reported motor skills rating scale. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 33, 640–651. doi: 10.1177/0734282914551536

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmel, S. R., and Ratliff-Schaub, K. (2011). “Growth and development” in Textbook of family medicine. ed. R. E. Rakel. 8th ed (Philadelphia, PA: Saunders Elsevier), 421–441.

Google Scholar

Koponen, T., Aro, M., Poikkeus, A., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M., Ahonen, T., et al. (2018). Comorbid fluency difficulties in reading and math: longitudinal stability across early grades. Except. Child. 84, 298–311. doi: 10.1177/0014402918756269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korkman, M., Kirk, U., and Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. New York, NY: Psychological Corp.

Google Scholar

Kuh, D., Hardy, R., Butterworth, S., Okell, L., Richards, M., Wadsworth, M., et al. (2006). Developmental origins of midlife physical performance: evidence from a British birth cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 164, 110–121. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lillard, A. (2005). Montessori: the science behind the genius. Oxford University Press, New York.

Google Scholar

Link, T., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Fischer, U., and Nuerk, H. (2013). Walk the number line an embodied training of numerical concepts. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2, 74–84. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, S. W., Webster, E. K., Getchell, N., Pfeiffer, K. A., and Robinson, L. E. (2015). Relationship between fundamental motor skill competence and physical activity during childhood and adolescence: a systematic review. Kinesiology Rev. 4, 416–426. doi: 10.1123/kr.2013-0012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes, L., Santos, R., Pereira, B., and Lopes, V. P. (2013). Associations between gross motor coordination and academic achievement in elementary school children. Hum. Mov. Sci. 32, 9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2012.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Z., Jose, P. E., Huntsinger, C. S., and Pigott, T. D. (2007). Fine motor skills and mathematics achievement in east Asian American and European American kindergartners and first graders. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 25, 595–614. doi: 10.1348/026151007X185329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, M., Lipscomb, S., McClelland, M. M., Duncan, R., Becker, D., Anderson, K., et al. (2016). Relations of preschoolers’ visual-motor and object manipulation skills with executive function and social behavior. Res. Quartrely Exercise Sport 87, 396–407. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2016.1229862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macdonald, K., Milne, N., Orr, R., and Pope, R. (2018). Relationships between motor proficiency and academic performance in mathematics and reading in school-aged children and adolescents: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:1603. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15081603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magill, R. A. (1984). Aprendizagem motora: conceitos e aplicações. São Paulo, Edgard Blucher.

Google Scholar

Magistro, D., Bardaglio, G., and Rabaglietti, E. (2015). Gross motor skills and academic achievement in typically developing children: the mediating effect of ADHD related behaviours. Cogn. Brain Behav. 19, 149–163.

Google Scholar

Malambo, C., Nová, A., Clark, C., and Musálek, M. (2022). Associations between fundamental movement skills, physical fitness, motor competency, physical activity, and executive functions in pre-school age children: a systematic review. Children 9:1059. doi: 10.3390/children9071059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manfra, L., Squires, C., Dinehart, L., Bleiker, C., Hartman, S., and Winsler, A. (2017). Preschool writing and premathematics predict grade 3 achievement for low-income, ethnically diverse children. J. Educ. Res. 110, 528–537. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2016.1145095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martzog, P. (2015). Fine motor skills and cogntive development in preschool children (1st ed.). Marburg, Germany: Tectum.

Google Scholar

Mather, N., and Gregg, N. (2002). Assessment with the Woodcock-Johnson III Washington, DC: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

McCarthy, D. (2004). Escalas Mc Carthy de Aptitudes y Psicomotricidad para niños 7 edición, revisada. Madrid: Publicaciones de Psicología Aplicada.

Google Scholar

McHale, K., and Cermak, S. A. (1992). Fine motor activities in elementary school: preliminary findings and provisional implications for children with fine motor problems. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 46, 898–903. doi: 10.5014/ajot.46.10.898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McWayne, C. M., Fantuzzo, J. W., and McDermott, P. A. (2004). Preschool competency in context: an investigation of the unique contribution of child competencies to early academic success. Dev. Psychol. 40, 633–645. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, G., Campbell, J., Marcinowski, E., Nelson, E., and Babik, I. (2016). Infant hand preference and the development of cognitive abilities. Front. Psychol. 7:410. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D., The PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic Reviewsand meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mokkink, L., Terwee, C., Patrick, D., Alonso, J., Stratford, P., Knol, D., et al. (2010). The COSMIN study reached international consensus on taxonomy, terminology, and definitions of measur ment properties for health-related patient-reported outcomes. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 63, 737–745. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mostafavi, R., Ziaee, V., Akbari, H., and Haji-Hosseini, S. (2013). The effects of SPARK physical education program on fundamental motor skills in 4–6 year-old children. Iran. J. Pediatr. 23, 216–219.

Google Scholar

Mullet, E., and Paques, P. (1991). The height + width = area of a rectangle rule in five-year olds: effects of stimulus distribution and graduation of the response scale. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 52, 336–343. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(91)90067-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Research Council (2009). Mathematics learning in early childhood: Paths toward excellence and equity. The National Academies Press: Washington, DC.

Google Scholar

Nehring, A., Nehring, E., Bruni, J., and Randolph, P. (1992). LAP-D: Learning accomplishment profile-diagnostic standardized assessment. Lewisville, NC: Kaplan Press.

Google Scholar

Nesbitt, K. T., Fuhs, M. W., and Farran, D. C. (2019). Stability and instability in the co-development of mathematics, executive function skills, and visual-motor integration from prekindergarten to first grade. Early Child. Res. Q. 46, 262–274. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, J., and Feinberg, M. P. (2015). Move to learn. Early Childhood Education. Lewisville, North Carolina: Gryphon House, Inc.

Google Scholar

Nosworthy, N. (2013). An investigation of the association between arithmetic achievement and symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude processing in 5–9 year-old children: Evidence from a paper-and-pencil test. London, ON: University of Western Ontario.

Google Scholar

Oberer, N., Gashaj, V., and Roebers, C. M. (2017). Motor skills in kindergarten: internal structure, cognitive correlates and relationships to background variables. Hum. Mov. Sci. 52, 170–180. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ommundsen, Y., Gundersen, K. A., and Mjaavatn, P. E. (2010). Fourth graders' social standing with peers: a prospective study on the role of first grade physical activity, weight status, and motor proficiency. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 54, 377–394. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2010.493344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) (2016). Preliminary reflections and research on knowledge, skills, attitudes and values necessary for 2030. The future of education and skills: Education 2030. Available at: https://www.oecd.org/education/2030-project/about/documents/

Google Scholar

Osborn, A., Butler, N., and Morris, A. (1984). The social life of Britain’s five year olds: a report of the child health and education study. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London, England.

Google Scholar

Osorio-Valencia, E., Torres-Sánchez, L., López-Carrillo, L., Rothenberg, S. J., and Schnaas, L. (2017). Early motor development and cognitive abilities among Mexican preschoolers. Child Neuropsychol. 24, 1015–1025. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2017.1354979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagani, L., Fitzpatrick, C., Archambault, I., and Janosz, M. (2010). School readiness and later achievement: a French Canadian replication and extension. Dev. Psychol. 46, 984–994. doi: 10.1037/a0018881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pang, A. W. Y., and Fong, D. T. P. (2009). Fundamental motor skill proficiency of Hong Kong children aged 6–9 years. Res. Sports Med. 17, 125–144. doi: 10.1080/15438620902897516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, S., and Bynner, J. (2005). Does numeracy matter more? London: National Research and Development Centre for Adult Literacy and Numeracy.

Google Scholar

Payne, V. G., and Isaacs, L. D. (2012). Human motor development. US: Jones And Bartlett Publishers Inc., McGraw Hill – Connect Learn Succeed.

Google Scholar

Pelicier, Y., Porot, A., and Sutter, J. (1996). Manuel Alphabétique de Psychiatrie Clinique et Thérapeutique. Paris: PUF.

Google Scholar

Piaget, J., and Inhelder, B. (1966). La psychologie de l’enfant. Presses Universitaires de France, Paris, France.

Google Scholar

Piek, J. P., Baynam, G. B., and Barrett, N. C. (2006). The relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self-perceptions and self-worth in children and adolescents. Hum. Mov. Sci. 25, 65–75. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pienaar, A. E., Van Reenen, I., and Weber, A. M. (2016). Sex differences in fundamental movement skills of a selected group of 6-year-old south African children. Early Child Dev. Care 186, 1994–2008. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1146263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pieters, S., Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., Vanderswalmen, R., and Van Waelvelde, H. (2012). Behind mathematical learning disabilities: what about visual perception and motor skills? Learn. Individ. Differ. 22, 498–504. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.03.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitchford, N. J., Papini, C., Outhwaite, L. A., and Guillford, A. (2016). Fine motor skills predict maths ability better than they predict reading ability in the early primary school years. Front. Psychol. 7:783. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portell, M., Anguera, M. T., Chacón-Moscoso, S., and Sanduvete-Chaves, S. (2015). Guidelines for reporting evaluations based on observational methodology (GREOM). Psicothema 27, 283–289. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2014.276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raisbeck, L. D., and Diekfuss, J. A. (2015). Fine and gross motor skills: the effects on skill-focused dual-tasks. Hum. Mov. Sci. 43, 146–154. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribner, A. D., Willoughby, M. T., and Blair, C. B., The Family Life Project Key Investigators (2017). Executive function buffers the association between early math and later academic skills. Front. Psychol. 8:869. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Pianta, R. C., and Cox, M. J. (2000). Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Child. Res. Q. 15, 147–166. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(00)00049-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, P., and Priest, H. (2006). Reliability and validity in research. Nurs. Stand. 20, 41–45. doi: 10.7748/ns.20.36.41.s58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, L. E., Stodden, D. F., Barnett, L. M., Lopes, V. P., Logan, S. W., Rodrigues, L. P., et al. (2015). Motor competence and its effect on positive developmental trajectories of health. Sports Med. 45, 1273–1284. doi: 10.1007/s40279-015-0351-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roebers, C. M., and Kauer, M. (2009). Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Dev. Sci. 12, 175–181. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00755.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roebers, C. M., Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Michel, E., and Jäger, K. (2014). The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children's transition to school: a latent variable approach. Hum. Mov. Sci. 33, 284–297. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudd, J. R., Barnett, L. M., Butson, M. L., Farrow, D., Berry, J., and Polman, R. C. J. (2015). Fundamental movement skills are more than run, throw and catch: the role of stability skills. PLoS One 10:e0140224. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampaio, R. F., and Mancini, M. C. (2007). Estudos de revisão sistemática: Um guia para síntese criteriosa da evidência científica. Rev. Bras. Fisioter. 11, 83–89. doi: 10.1590/S1413-35552007000100013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarama, J., and Clements, D. H. (2004). Building blocks for early childhood mathematics. Early Child. Res. Q. 19, 181–189. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarama, J., and Clements, D. H. (2016). “Physical and virtual manipulatives: what is “concrete”?” in International perspectives on teaching and learning mathematics with virtual manipulatives (mathematics education in the digital era, 7). ed. P. S. Moyer-Packenham (Cham, Switzerland: Springer International), 71–93.

Google Scholar

Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., and Vukelic, E. B. (1996). “The role of a computer manipulative in fostering specific psychological/mathematical processes” in Proceedings of the 18th annual meeting of the North America chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of mathematics education (2). eds. E. Jakubowski, D. Watkins, and H. Biske (Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education), 567–572.

Google Scholar

Sarnecka, B. W., and Carey, S. (2008). How counting represents number: what children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition 108, 662–674. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M., Egger, F., Benzing, V., Jäger, K., Conzelmann, A., Roebers, C., et al. (2017). Disentangling the relationship between children’s motor ability, executive function and academic achievement. PLoS One 12:e0182845. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalev, R. S., Manor, O., and Gross-Tsur, V. (2005). Developmental dyscalculia: a prospective six-year follow-up. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47, 121–125. doi: 10.1017/S0012162205000216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., Duncan, G. J., Davis-Kean, P. E., Duckworth, K., Claessens, A., Engel, M., et al. (2012). Early predictors of high school mathematics achievement. Psychol. Sci. 23, 691–697. doi: 10.1177/0956797612440101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., and Shrager, J. (1984). “Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: how do children know what to do?” in The origins of cognitive skills. ed. C. Sophian (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 229–293.

Google Scholar

Silva, I., Marques, L., Mata, L., and Rosa, M. (2016). Orientações Curriculares para a Educação Pré-Escolar. Lisboa, Portugal: Ministério da Educação/Direção-Geral da Educação.

Google Scholar

Smith, L. (2005). Cognition as a dynamic system: principles from embodiment. Dev. Rev. 25, 278–298. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorgente, V., Cohen, E. J., Bravi, R., and Minciacchi, D. (2021). Crosstalk between Gross and fine motor domains during late childhood: the influence of Gross motor training on fine motor performances in primary school children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:11387. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182111387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sortor, J. M., and Kulp, M. T. (2003). Are the results of the beery-buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration and its subtests related to achievement test scores? Optom. Vis. Sci. 80, 758–763. doi: 10.1097/00006324-200311000-00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sortwell, A., Behringer, M., Granacher, U., Trimble, K., Forte, P., Neiva, H. P., et al. (2022). Advancing sports science and physical education research through a shared understanding of the term motor performance skills: a scoping review with content analysis. Int. J. Kinesiology Sports Sci. 10, 18–27. doi: 10.7575/aiac.ijkss.v.10n.3p.18

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souza, C. T., Santos, D. C., Tolocka, R. E., Baltieri, L., Gibim, N. C., and Habechian, F. A. (2010). Assessment of global motor performance and gross and fine motor skills of infants attending day care centers. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 14, 309–315.

Google Scholar

Spanaki, I., Nikolopoulos, A., Skordilis, E., and Koutsouki, D. (2008). Motor and graphomotor performance of students aging from 5 to 10 years. Eur. Psychomotricity J. 1, 38–45.

Google Scholar

Staples, K. L., and Reid, G. (2010). Fundamental movement skills and autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 209–217. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0854-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, P., Strauss, E., Sherman, N., Sherman, E., and Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary ; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK. ISBN 978-0-19-515957.

Google Scholar

Strooband, K. F., de Rosnay, M., Okely, A. D., and Veldman, S. L. (2020). Systematic review and meta-analyses: motor skill interventions to improve fine motor development in children aged birth to 6 years. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 41, 319–331. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suggate, S., Pufke, E., and Stoeger, H. (2018). Do fine motor skills contribute to early reading development? J. Res. Read. 41, 1–19. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suggate, S., Stoeger, H., and Fischer, U. (2017). Finger-based numerical skills link fine motor skills to numerical development in preschoolers. Percept. Mot. Skills 124, 1085–1106. doi: 10.1177/0031512517727405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, H. L., Jerman, O., and Zheng, X. (2009). Math disabilities and reading disabilities: can they be separated? J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 27, 175–196. doi: 10.1177/0734282908330578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tortella, P., Haga, M., Loras, H., Sigmundsson, H., and Fumagalli, G. (2016). Motor skill development in Italian pre-school children induced by structured activities in a specific playground. PLoS One 11:e0160244. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traverso, L., Tonizzi, I., Usai, M. C., and Viterbori, P. (2021). The relationship of working memory and inhibition with different number knowledge skills in preschool children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 203, 105014–105021. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, D. A. (2000). Test of Gross motor development, 2nd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Google Scholar

Van der Fels, I. M., Te Wierike, S. C., Hartman, E., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Smith, J., and Visscher, C. (2015). The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4-16 year old typically developing children: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 18, 697–703. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veldman, S. L. C., Santos, R., Jones, R. A., Sousa-Sá, E., and Okely, A. D. (2019). Associations between gross motor skills and cognitive development in toddlers. Early Hum. Dev. 132, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2019.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdine, B. N., Irwin, C. M., Golinkoff, R. M., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2014). Contributions of executive function and spatial skills to preschool mathematics achievement. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 126, 37–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuijk, P. J., Hartman, E., Mombarg, R., Scherder, E., and Visscher, C. (2011). Associations between academic and motor performance in a heterogeneous sample of children with learning disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 44, 276–282. doi: 10.1177/0022219410378446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallon, H. (1941). L'évolution psychologique de l'enfant. A. Colin, Paris, reed. 1974.

Google Scholar

Wang, R. F., and Spelke, E. S. (2002). Human spatial representation: insights from animals. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 376–382. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01961-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wasner, M., Moeller, K., Fischer, M. H., and Nuerk, H. C. (2015). Related but not the same: ordinality, cardinality and 1-to-1 correspondence infinger-based numerical representations. J. Cogn. Psychol. 27, 426–441. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.964719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2007). “Wechsler individual achievement test – Australian standardised edition” in Pearson Clinical and Talent Assessment. 2nd edn. London, UK: Pearson Assessment / Pearson Education Limited.

Google Scholar

Wickstrom, R. L. (1977). Fundamental motor patterns. (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.

Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., McGrath, L. M., Pennington, B. F., Keenen, J. M., DeFries, J. C., Olson, R. K., et al. (2019). Understanding comorbidity between specific learning disabilities. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2019, 91–109. doi: 10.1002/cad.20291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J., Lee, K. J., and Anderson, P. J. (2010). Prevalence of motor skill impairment in preterm children who do not develop cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 52, 232–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03544.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williford, A. P., Downer, J. T., and Hamre, B. K. (2013). “Virginia kindergarten readiness project—phase 1 report: concurrent validity of teaching strategies GOLD,” in Research report prepared for Elevate Early Education (E3). Retrieved from: http://www.e3va.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/02/VKRP-Validity-Report_2014.pdf

Google Scholar

Woodcock, R., McGrew, K., and Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement. Riverside, Itasca, IL.

Google Scholar

Wynn, K. (1992). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cogn. Dev. 24, 220–251. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(92)90008-P

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, N., Ayyub, M., Sun, H., Wen, X., Xiang, P., and Gao, Z. (2017). Effects of physical activity on motor skills and cognitive development in early childhood: a systematic review. Biomed. Res. Int. 2017, 1–13. doi: 10.1155/2017/2760716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: children, preschool, motor skills, math skills, mathematical performance

Citation: Flores P, Coelho E, Mourão-Carvalhal MI and Forte P (2023) Association between motor and math skills in preschool children with typical development: Systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1105391. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1105391

Received: 22 November 2022; Accepted: 10 January 2023;
Published: 02 February 2023.

Edited by:

Ana Filipa Silva, Instituto Politécnico de Viana do Castelo, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Francisco Tomás González-Fernández, University of Granada, Spain
Elena Escolano-Pérez, University of Zaragoza, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Flores, Coelho, Mourão-Carvalhal and Forte. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pedro Flores, ✉ pedro.flores@iscedouro.pt

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.