Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 12 May 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Executive Function and Education, volume II: Considerations for Academic Success - Across Contexts and Populations View all 5 articles

Culture, executive functions, and academic achievement

Isu Cho&#x;Isu Cho1Niki Hosseini-Kamkar&#x;Niki Hosseini-Kamkar2Hyun-joo SongHyun-joo Song3J. Bruce Morton
J. Bruce Morton4*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3Department of Psychology, Yonsei University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
  • 4Department of Psychology, Western University, London, ON, Canada

Although it is well known that children of East Asian immigrants show higher academic achievement than native-born North American children, the social-cognitive determinants of this difference remain poorly understood. Given the importance of executive functions (EF) for academic achievement, and evidence that EF develops more quickly in East Asian compared to North American cultures, it is conceivable that differences in academic achievement might be rooted in EF differences between these groups. We examine this possibility by reviewing evidence of cross-cultural differences in EF development but find core concepts and findings limited in several key respects. To address these limitations, we propose a framework for relating EF, culture, and academic achievement that draws on new theoretical ideas about the nature of EF and its relation to social context. We conclude by discussing avenues for future research on the relations between culture, executive functions, and academic achievement.

1. Introduction

Children of East Asian immigrants outperform their native-born North American peers (Kao and Tienda, 2012; Feliciano and Lanuza, 2017; Tran C.D. et al., 2019; Kim, 2021; Xie, 2022) on several measures of academic achievement including the number of years of schooling completed (Keller and Tillman, 2008; Feliciano and Lanuza, 2017), performance on standardized proficiency tests (Glick and White, 2003; Duong et al., 2016), and grades (Hao and Woo, 2012; Kao and Tienda, 2012; Duong et al., 2016). Socio-cognitive factors that contribute to the East Asian advantage in academic achievement, however, remain poorly understood.

One possibility is that the advantage reflects differences in executive functioning (EF), domain-general cognitive processes that are critical for everyday psychological functioning. Across development, individual differences in EF predict academic achievement in math and reading both cross-sectionally (Altemeier et al., 2006; Blair and Razza, 2007; Brock et al., 2009; Mcauley et al., 2010; Willoughby et al., 2012; Thorell et al., 2013; Stipek and Valentino, 2015; Cortés Pascual et al., 2019; Nouwens et al., 2021) and longitudinally (Best et al., 2011; Jacob and Parkinson, 2015; Samuels et al., 2016; Ahmed et al., 2019; Robson et al., 2020; for review, see Zelazo and Carlson, 2020). Moreover, children from East Asian countries outperform North American and European children on a variety of EF measures, including measures of inhibition (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008; Lan et al., 2011; Ellefson et al., 2017; Cho et al., 2021), cognitive flexibility (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Imada et al., 2013; Ellefson et al., 2017), and working memory (Oh and Lewis, 2008; Wang et al., 2016; Ellefson et al., 2017; but see Schirmbeck et al., 2021). This is true both of children born to East Asian immigrants (Cho et al., 2021) as well as children growing up in East Asian countries (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008; Cho et al., 2021). Therefore, while it has not been tested directly, it is at least conceivable that the East Asian advantage in academic achievement stems, at least in part, from early differences in EF.

However, linking sociocultural background, EF, and academic achievement in this way arguably raises more questions than it answers. Why might East Asian children show early advantages in EF? Children from East Asian countries are subject to social and cultural influences that may be different from those experienced by Caucasian children in North America and Europe. For example, East Asian cultures are collectivist in nature because of their pronounced valuation of social harmony and the alignment of personal and communal goals. This contrasts with individualist Caucasian cultures of North America and Europe, which emphasize personal independence and self-expression (Tobin et al., 1989; Nisbett and Miyamoto, 2005; Tran V.C. et al., 2019). Parenting styles also differ across East Asian and Caucasian cultures of North America and Europe. East Asian parents tend to be authoritarian and “effort-oriented,” whereas Caucasian North American and European parents tend to be authoritative and encouraging of independence and personal expression (Stankov, 2010; Hsin and Xie, 2014; Kim, 2021; Boman, 2022). But how might differences in cultural values and socialization practices contribute to differences in children’s EF? Indeed, there is little consensus on whether children’s EF can be shaped by social experiences (for discussion, see Doebel, 2020). Even if we allow for social or cultural influences on children’s EF development, associations between social influences (such as parenting characteristics), and child outcomes can differ markedly across East Asian and Caucasian cultures of North America and Europe. The goal of the current paper is therefore twofold. First, to elaborate on some of the conceptual challenges related to understanding cross-cultural differences in EF development; and second, to propose a theoretical model linking culture, EF, and academic achievement.

2. Conceptual challenges to linking executive functioning development and social experience

East Asian and Caucasian children from North America and Europe show marked differences in EF development and are subject to different cultural and socialization influences. This has led to the idea that cross-cultural differences in EF might be attributable to differences in children’s socio-cultural background (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008). Evidence that East Asian preschoolers, for example, reach developmental EF milestones as much as 6 months earlier than Caucasian preschoolers (Sabbagh et al., 2006), and are socialized from an early age to observe collectivist values, has led some to attribute their precocious EF development to the effect of practicing self-regulation during daily routines (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008; Lan et al., 2011). This seemingly self-evident proposition is not altogether straightforward, however, as it assumes that EF development can vary as a function of social experience generally and practice specifically. This, it turns out, is a matter of considerable debate. On the one hand, interventions that target improving children’s EF and self-regulatory abilities have been designed to enhance their academic achievement (Diamond et al., 2007; Blair and Diamond, 2008; see Mattera et al., 2021 for review), implying that differences in children’s EF might originate, at least in part, from differences in children’s experiences (e.g., training). On the other hand, rigorously designed experimental efforts to improve EF through massive amounts of repeated practice yield no generalizable changes in underlying EF components (Redick et al., 2013; Simons et al., 2016; Kassai et al., 2019; Shepard et al., 2022). These latter findings are important as they directly challenge prevailing practice-based interpretations of the origins of cross-cultural differences in children’s EF (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008).

Some of the tension in this debate appears to hinge on the conceptualization of EF itself (for recent discussions, see Doebel, 2020 and Zelazo and Carlson, 2022). Historically, EF has been defined as a set of domain-general cognitive processes that include working memory (Baddeley, 1992), inhibitory control (Diamond, 2013), and cognitive flexibility or shifting (Garon et al., 2008). To the extent that these processes were thought to operate the same way across different contexts (i.e., domain-general) but combine to support performance of individual tasks, EF came to be viewed as a fixed set of underlying dimensions or “components” of higher-order cognition (Miyake et al., 2000). However, it is precisely when EF is conceptualized and operationalized in this way that it proves to be largely resistant to the effects of practice.

A more recent conceptualization of EF, termed the control skills model, offers a potentially fruitful way forward (for discussion, see Doebel, 2020). On a control skills account, EF consists of a set of skills for achieving goals, skills that depend critically on knowledge, beliefs, values, norms, and preferences for how to use control to achieve goals in particular contexts. This account contrasts with the componential account that views EF as a domain-general process that functions in a stereotyped way across contexts. Imagine, for example, a child who needs to prepare for an upcoming math test but who wants to go out and play with their friends. From the traditional componential definition of EF, achieving control in this situation would involve a general capacity for inhibition, which would allow the child to halt their desire to play and remain focused on their work. From a control skills perspective, on the other hand, achieving control in this situation would involve an appeal to some form of knowledge which would help the child achieve their goal. The child might consider a social norm (e.g., most of my classmates will be studying so I should too), a value (e.g., doing well on the math test is important to me, so I should study), or knowledge about the punishment that would follow from a poor test result (e.g., my parents will withhold my allowance if I do not do well on the math test). Importantly, achieving control in this instance will depend critically on knowledge, values, and norms that are specific to this particular context and that have been acquired through a lifetime of socialization experiences. As it delineates pathways through which culturally unique social experiences could impact children’s EF, a control skills model of EF potentially offers important advantages over traditional componential models of EF for understanding the social origins of EF differences in early development.

However, even if we reconceptualize EF so that pathways linking social experiences and children’s EF can more plausibly be delineated, there are additional conceptual challenges associated with understanding cross-cultural differences in EF development. This has to do with the fact that associations between social influences, such as parenting characteristics, and child outcomes, such as self-regulation and the internalization of social rules, can vary across cultural contexts. Consider, for example, the link between authoritarian parenting styles and child self-regulation. Authoritarian parents demand compliance from their children and endorse the use of power assertion for achieving parenting goals but lack emotional warmth when interacting with their children (Baumrind, 1966, 1967). In Western cultures, authoritarian parenting is associated with lower social adjustment and self-regulation among children, at least as compared to children of authoritative parents (Dornbusch et al., 1987; Milevsky et al., 2007; Murray, 2012). In East Asian cultures, by contrast, authoritarian parenting is not consistently associated with negative child outcomes, but in selected studies, has been associated with child achievement and academic adjustment (Dornbusch et al., 1987; Steinberg et al., 1992; Leung et al., 1998; Chao, 2001; Ang and Goh, 2006; Kim and Park, 2006; Liu and Guo, 2010; Watabe and Hibbard, 2014; Pinquart and Kauser, 2018). Given that the same parenting behaviors can have different implications for children outcomes in different cultural contexts, understanding associations between social influences and children’s EF development will require careful consideration of the cultural meaning of parenting behaviors in different cultural contexts (for full discussion, see Chao, 1994). The critical point here is that the same parenting behaviors can have very different implications for the development of children’s EF when those behaviors are expressed in different cultural contexts. Thus, even if we allow for the possibility that social experience can impact EF development, we need to acknowledge that cultural context can moderate associations between social experiences and child outcomes.

3. A theoretical model relating culture, academic achievement, and executive functions

We return now to the question that opened the manuscript, namely whether the East Asian advantage in academic achievement compared to North American peers can be explained, at least in part, by differences in EF? At first blush, the connection seems almost self-evident. After all, early differences in EF predict academic achievement (Best et al., 2011; Jacob and Parkinson, 2015; Samuels et al., 2016; Ahmed et al., 2019; Robson et al., 2020), and children of East Asian origin excel on measures of EF (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008; Lan et al., 2011; Imada et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2016; Ellefson et al., 2017; Cho et al., 2021). However, a closer examination of the hypothesis (i.e., cultural differences in EF → differences in academic achievement) reveals a variety of new questions and conceptual challenges. Why, for example, might children of East Asian origin show an advantage in EF? Although the common assumption is that the advantage takes root in East Asian children’s unique cultural and social environment, conceptually it is not clear how variation in social experience begets variation in EF. Moreover, links between socialization practices (i.e., parenting characteristics) and child outcomes can vary across cultural contexts.

Despite these challenges, we believe that executive functions can, and indeed, should be integrated into models of cultural differences in academic achievement. Doing so will help synthesize findings from the fields of developmental, cross-cultural, and educational psychology, delineate potential pathways linking social experience to EF development and academic achievement, and potentially lead to new testable hypotheses. Here, we propose such an integrative model. A diagram of the model is shown in Figure 1. At the core of the model is the idea that children’s EF be conceptualized as a set of control skills for achieving goals (Doebel, 2020). This represents a departure from the way EF has historically been conceptualized in educational and cross-cultural studies, where the focus has been on underlying EF components such as working memory, inhibitory control, and mental flexibility. The model also draws a distinction between cultural and “social influences” on control skills. “Social influences” refer to the characteristics and behaviors of a child’s key socializing agents, including parents, grandparents, and teachers, whereas culture denotes the broader system of norms and values within which caregiver-child relationships and interactions are embedded. Relatedly, another important feature of the model is that “social influences” are portrayed as a proximal influence on children’s control skills whereas culture moderates the association between socialization factors and children’s control skills. Finally, the model identifies “control skills” as an important predictor of “academic achievement” as the existing evidence on EF and academic achievement suggests that EF predicts academic achievement (Latzman et al., 2010; Samuels et al., 2016; Ahmed et al., 2019; Robson et al., 2020). The model provides a framework for integrating findings from the fields of developmental, cross-cultural, and educational psychology, and helps to resolve important conceptual challenges related to such an integration with the following two main points.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A diagram of the model on culture, academic achievement, and executive functions.

First, the model lays out a conceptually plausible means by which social experiences influence children’s EF, a key challenge in characterizing social determinants of children’s cognitive development. It does so by adopting a control skills characterization of EF in lieu of a more traditional componential model of EF. As discussed, the key shortcoming for the componential view of EF is that there is little empirical evidence for far-transfer effects of training core EF components (e.g., inhibitory control, working memory, and shifting; Redick et al., 2013; Kassai et al., 2019; Shepard et al., 2022), and this contradicts with prevailing practice-based interpretations of the origins of cross-cultural differences in children’s EF. Control skills, by contrast, take root in and are shaped and influenced by the child’s social environment. With this conceptual groundwork in place, the model can accommodate findings concerning the socio-cognitive determinants of the East Asian advantage in academic achievement. One influential study on academic achievement, for example, combined data from two nationally representative longitudinal surveys to test whether East Asian student’s academic advantage relative to Caucasian students is attributable to differences in socio-economic status, intellectual abilities, or work ethic (Hsin and Xie, 2014). By virtue of the longitudinal nature of the data, the researchers were able to show that the emergence of the Asian-Caucasian achievement gap coincided with the divergence in Asian and Caucasian students’ beliefs about the importance of work ethic for academic achievement. Whereas Asian students were more likely to attribute academic success to work ethic, Caucasian students were more likely to attribute success to inborn ability. Finally, Asian students reported greater parental valuation of academic achievement compared to Caucasian students. Although the study was not explicitly framed in terms of EF, the findings are readily accommodated by the proposed control skills model in that differences in socialized beliefs and values contribute to differences in the deployment of control skills in academic settings (e.g., cultural practices of training, parental socialization of self-regulation, and parental valuation of academic effort). This in turn contributes to differences in academic effort and achievement (for related discussion, see Niebaum and Munakata, 2022).

Second, the model draws a distinction between social and cultural influences on EF with different roles. Social influences refer to the characteristics and behaviors of caregivers (e.g., parents and teachers), and they can impact children’s control skills directly. Cultural influences, by contrast, refer to the broader system of norms and values within which caregiver-child relationships are embedded and moderate the association between social influences and children’s EF. As such, the model can accommodate evidence that the same class of parenting attitudes and behaviors (i.e., authoritarian parenting) can have different associations with child outcomes in different cultural backgrounds (Dornbusch et al., 1987; Leung et al., 1998; Chao, 2001; Kim and Park, 2006; Murray, 2012; Pinquart and Kauser, 2018). In North America, restrictive and controlling parenting behaviors are often rooted in negative views of children, such as notions of original sin, and motivated by an interest in breaking the will of the child (Smuts and Hagen, 1985). In East Asia, by contrast, restrictive and controlling parenting behaviors can be viewed positively, as they are thought to reflect a commitment to family socialization (e.g., “ga-jung-kyo-yuk” in Korean; see Choi et al., 2013), training (e.g., “chiao shun” in Chinese), and governance (e.g., “guan” in Chinese). This form of parenting is considered important in East Asian cultures so that children can learn their position in society and help to maintain social harmony. Thus, parenting practices with firm control, training, and governance can appear controlling from a Western point of view, but in an East Asian cultural context, they can signal care, love, and concern for a child’s education, which is somewhat different from a solely “authoritarian” parenting styles (Leung et al., 2010; Huang and Gove, 2015). Whether culture moderates the association between parenting behaviors and children’s EF as predicted by the model has not been investigated directly but is certainly conceivable. Indeed, some have speculated that collectivist cultures make it easier for Asian-American parents to inculcate values related to academic achievement and a strong work ethic, precisely because in collectivist cultures, there is a shared expectation that children will conform to family expectations (for discussion, see Hsin and Xie, 2014). To summarize, these examples illustrate the idea that culture is as a set of beliefs and values that are jointly held by caregivers and children so that it should be understood in the corresponding cultural settings (Chao, 1994; Ang and Goh, 2006; Huang and Gove, 2015; Cheung and Lim, 2022). By having two separate entities (i.e., social influences as a variable influencing children’s EF and culture as a moderating variable between social influences and children’s EF), the model has merit in that it cannot only accommodate the findings on possible difference in psychological interpretation of certain socialization practices depending on cultures, but also allow our better understanding of how they impact children’s EF.

4. Implications for future research

The proposed model is an integration of ideas about the nature of EF, cultural differences in socialization experiences (e.g., parenting), and academic achievement. It is, of course, possible to imagine more basic models in which caregiver socialization impacts children’s academic achievement directly without any intervening influence of EF. Parents may encourage their children to expend greater effort on their studies, for example, which may have a direct influence on children’s achievement. Although greatly simplified, we feel that such models do not provide sufficient conceptual tools to delineate how children achieve success academically. Parental valuation of their children’s hard-work and attention to academic matters is undoubtedly important for children’s achievement in academic settings, but only to the extent these parenting strategies shape children’s goals (e.g., “My goal is to do well at school”) and engage their children’s motivation to achieve these goals (e.g., “I am prepared to work very hard to do well at school”). As such, it is important to include child characteristics of some kind in models relating parenting behaviors and children’s academic achievement. We believe control skills is a useful construct in this regard. The ultimate value of the model though is that it helps to identify research questions and generate predictions for future studies in this area.

First and foremost, the model raises questions about the measurement of EF. How, for example, should EF be measured in cross-cultural studies involving children? Historically, EF has been measured through the administration of age-appropriate working memory, flexibility, or inhibitory control tasks. This makes sense from a componential standpoint, as these tasks have been designed to yield relatively pure measures of underlying EF components or processes. From a control skills perspective, however, control is not founded on underlying domain-general components and the assumption that laboratory tasks generate pure measures of EF is considered suspect. From this standpoint, EF should be measured with procedures with greater ecological validity than standard “laboratory measures,” procedures such as non-computerized tasks that use real objects and scenarios that are reminiscent of the school context (for fuller discussion, see Doebel, 2020). It may also be promising to examine parent–child interactions and/or teacher-child interactions in the context of these procedures to shed light on how caregivers support children’s developing control skills. To date, however, ecologically valid procedures for examining control skills in academic contexts have not been developed or validated. The development of such measures therefore constitutes an important avenue for future work.

A related issue concerns the measurement of culture. Despite the number of studies that have examined cross-cultural differences in EF and academic achievement, there is little consensus about how culture should be measured. In fact, many such studies do not measure culture at all: participants are simply grouped according to their self-reported ethnicity (i.e., East Asian, Caucasian) or national origin (e.g., China, United States) on the assumption that any individual from a particular region or country possesses a particular set of cultural values (e.g., collectivist and individualist). This is problematic in a number of ways, not least of which that attributing effects to unmeasured independent variables seems decidedly risky from a scientific standpoint. A larger issue is that culture in regions like East Asia and North America is regionally quite varied and, in some cases, undergoing rapid change. To assume that all individuals within a region share the same cultural orientation is at odds with the complexity of cultural life in these regions. Indeed, studies that have measured culture directly do not find that individuals from East Asian countries are equally collectivistic, nor necessarily less individualistic than individuals from North America. For example, when comparing individuals from two putatively collectivist countries (i.e., Japan and South Korea), individuals from Japan typically view themselves as an indivisible part of larger social contexts (i.e., highly collectivistic), whereas individuals from South Korea typically view themselves as autonomous agents capable of influencing social contexts (i.e., relationalism; bi-directional dynamics between the self and contexts; see Park and Han, 2018 for discussion). A second study that compared EF in children from Canada and South Korea found that parents of South Korean preschoolers reported being more individualist and less collectivist in their values compared to parents of Canadian preschoolers (Cho et al., 2021). Findings from these studies are at odds with common assumptions about cultural orientations in East Asia and North America and underscore the importance of measurement of culture in cross-cultural research on EF.

Beyond issues of measurement, the proposed model raises questions about the relationship between parenting, academic achievement, and culture. According to the model, parenting style relates differently with child outcomes in East Asian and Caucasian samples because of the moderating influence of culture. But what aspect of culture exerts this moderating influence? The most common assumption is that East Asian and Caucasian cultures are best distinguished in terms of collectivist (or Confucian) vs. individualist values, respectively. While these value systems differ markedly with respect to the relative importance of the individual vs. the group, they are extremely general and not specific to the caregiver-child relationship or child education. Indeed, country-level individualism–collectivism does not always correspond to individual-level individualism–collectivism (see Li et al., 2018). Although at the group-level, East Asian cultures can be collectivistic and Western cultures can be individualistic, caregiver-child relationships at the level of the individual may differ from the group-level norms. Furthermore, the “East Asian” advantage in academic achievement, at least in the North American context, is not specific to children of parents from East Asian countries known for their Confucian value orientation — it also extends to children of parents from South Asian countries whose cultures are not distinctly Confucian (for discussion, see Hsin and Xie, 2014). Moving forward, it may therefore be more profitable to examine values more specifically related to parenting, child behavior, and school achievement and that more directly shape how caregivers and children interpret various social interactions related to children’s intellectual development and academic performance.

Finally, the proposed model raises questions about cultural differences in EF early in development and their implications for later academic achievement. The proposed model moves away from a componential view of EF in favor of a control skills framework, in part because EF components are not amenable to change through practice in the same way as control skills. But then why do children from East Asian countries typically outperform same-aged Caucasian children from North America or Europe on laboratory (i.e., componential) measures of flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory? One possibility is that cross-cultural differences in children’s EF reflect differences in socio-cultural dynamics of psychological testing procedures rather than differences in underlying EF components. In studies of young children, testing is always done in person with an experimenter. However, children are not randomly assigned to experimenters in cross-cultural studies — it is not feasible. Instead, experimenters from one country typically test all children in their country and experimenters from the comparison country test children in their country. Consequently, cross-cultural differences in EF might reflect differences in the meaning of psychological testing both for the children being tested as well as the experimenters who are conducting the testing, and/or the different dynamics between the children and experimenters across countries. Development of control skills measures (e.g., measures controlling for or at least minimizing the possibly different impact of experimenters on children across countries) may help to test these possibilities. Assuming that control skills measures are eventually developed, one final set of questions concerns whether componential measures or control skills measures would have a stronger predictive relationship with academic achievement. Interestingly, although cross-cultural differences in EF have been documented empirically, it is not clear whether these differences predict differences in academic achievement. The proposed model predicts that control skills measures (i.e., ecologically valid control skills measures in academic contexts) would be more strongly associated with academic achievement than componential measures of EF, and that cross-cultural differences in EF would predict differences in academic achievement. Indeed, evidence that differences in effort are more predictive of academic achievement than differences in intellectual proficiency (Hsin and Xie, 2014) is consistent with this prediction. Direct tests of these predictions however await future testing.

5. Caveats and limitations

There are of course a number of caveats and limitations of the proposed model that need to be discussed. First, there are obviously many other factors influencing the development of children’s EF that are not represented in the model, such as genetic, demographic, neurophysiological, and lifestyle (e.g., diet, sleep, exercise, etc.) factors to name but a few. Moreover, it is conceivable that “social influences,” the one factor referenced in the model, is at least partially confounded with these unreferenced variables (e.g., Schmitt et al., 2019). Comprehensively assessing biological, lifestyle, and social influences is clearly important if we are to understand the unique impact of factors such as parenting attitudes and behaviors on EF development. At the same time, measuring and modeling such a broad range of measures in an adequately sized international sample is beyond the scope of most research groups, but it would require the coordinated efforts of a large international consortium [for examples, see the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study, the Generation R study, and Growing Up in Singapore Toward healthy Outcome (GUSTO) birth cohort study].

A second limitation of the model is that it does not explicitly consider the difference between East Asian children growing up in East Asian countries and children of East Asian immigrants growing up in Europe or North America. Most cross-cultural studies of EF development compare Caucasian children residing in North America/Europe and East Asian children residing in East Asian countries (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008; Lan et al., 2011; Ellefson et al., 2017; but see Yang et al., 2011 and Cho et al., 2021 for exception including East Asian immigrant children in North America). The strongest empirical evidence for the “East Asian” advantage in academic achievement, however, comes from national longitudinal studies that compare East Asian immigrant and Caucasian non-immigrant students on a common set of academic outcome measures (Hsin and Xie, 2014; Feliciano and Lanuza, 2017). International comparisons of academic achievement can be more challenging as there are fewer common outcome measures available and a greater potential for unmeasured sources of variability between samples. That said, comparisons of academic achievement of East Asian immigrants and Caucasians (North American or European) confound immigrant status with socio-cultural factors specific to the East Asian community. This is an important limitation of the current model given that the immigrant experience itself profoundly impacts the social dynamics of the family and children’s achievement in academic settings (Fuligni, 1997). Whether socio-cultural influences unique to East Asia can be distinguished from the effects of immigration of course remains unclear, and it is possible that the current model could be adapted to accommodate both influences. Given the current state of the literature, however, it was not possible to explicitly distinguish between the effects of culture and immigration on children’s academic achievement.

6. Summary and conclusion

In the proposed model, culture acts as a moderating variable that modifies caregiver socialization on children’s cognition, rather than a fixed variable that directly influences child cognition without considering the possibility of different context-specific social influences on child cognition. Although it is possible that culture itself can be an antecedent variable that influences both socializing agents and children’s control skills, the proposed model can accommodate evidence that the same class of parenting attitudes are associated with different child outcomes—presumably because the same class of parenting attitudes are interpreted differently by children in different cultural contexts. The proposed model is a first-step in elucidating how socializing agents, culture, EF control skills, and academic achievement are related to each other with a more nuanced explanation, rather than simply assuming that culture impacts EF and ultimately influences academic achievement. A consideration of cultural factors in understanding links between EF and education is imperative for revealing whether observed effects are universal or culture specific. These considerations are particularly timely given that the majority of existing research in the field of psychology is based on WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) participants (see Gutchess and Rajaram, 2022 for theoretical review). Therefore, the proposed model may provide further insights into the underlying the relationship between socializing agents, culture, EF control skills, and academic achievement in diverse contexts.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

JBM, IC, and NH-K contributed to conception and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by funding from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (grant number: R3353A12; awarded to JBM).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahmed, S. F., Tang, S., Waters, N. E., and Davis-Kean, P. (2019). Executive function and academic achievement: longitudinal relations from early childhood to adolescence. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 446–458. doi: 10.1037/edu0000296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altemeier, L., Jones, J., Abbott, R. D., and Berninger, V. W. (2006). Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders. Dev. Neuropsychol. 29, 161–173. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ang, R. P., and Goh, D. H. (2006). Authoritarian parenting style in Asian societies: a cluster-analytic investigation. Contemp. Fam. Ther. 28, 131–151. doi: 10.1007/s10591-006-9699-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science 255, 556–559. doi: 10.1126/science.1736359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Dev. 37, 887–907. doi: 10.2307/1126611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genet. Psychol. Monogr. 75, 43–88.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., and Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 899–911. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 78, 647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boman, B. (2022). Educational achievement among East Asian schoolchildren 1967–2020: A thematic review of the literature. Int. J. Educ. Res. 3:100168. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedro.2022.100168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., and Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of “hot” and “cool” executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Child. Res. Q. 24, 337–349. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Dev. 65, 1111–1119. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00806.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Dev. 72, 1832–1843. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, H. S., and Lim, E. (2022). A scoping review of Singapore parenting: culture-general and culture-specific functions of parenting styles and practices. Infant Child Dev. 31:e2312. doi: 10.1002/icd.2312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, I., Park, J., Song, H. J., and Morton, J. B. (2021). Disentangling language status and country-of-origin explanations of the bilingual advantage in preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 212:105235. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, Y., Kim, Y. S., Kim, S. Y., and Park, I. J. (2013). Is Asian American parenting controlling and harsh? Empirical testing of relationships between Korean American and Western parenting measures. Asian Am. J. Psychol. 4, 19–29. doi: 10.1037/a0031220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortés Pascual, A., Moyano Muñoz, N., and Quilez Robres, A. (2019). The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:1582. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., and Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science 318, 1387–1388. doi: 10.1126/science.1151148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 942–956. doi: 10.1177/1745691620904771

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dornbusch, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Leiderman, P. H., Roberts, D. F., and Fraleigh, M. J. (1987). The relation of parenting style to adolescent school performance. Child Dev. 58:1244. doi: 10.2307/1130618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duong, M. T., Badaly, D., Liu, F. F., Schwartz, D., and McCarty, C. A. (2016). Generational differences in academic achievement among immigrant youths: a meta-analytic review. Rev. Educ. Res. 86, 3–41. doi: 10.3102/0034654315577680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellefson, M. R., Ng, F. F. Y., Wang, Q., and Hughes, C. (2017). Efficiency of executive function: a two-generation cross-cultural comparison of samples from Hong Kong and the United Kingdom. Psychol. Sci. 28, 555–566. doi: 10.1177/0956797616687812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feliciano, C., and Lanuza, Y. R. (2017). An immigrant paradox? Contextual attainment and intergenerational educational mobility. Am. Sociol. Rev. 82, 211–241. doi: 10.1177/0003122416684777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuligni, A. J. (1997). The academic achievement of adolescents from immigrant families: the role of family background, attitudes, and behavior. Child Dev. 68, 351–363. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01944.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., and Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 134, 31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glick, J. E., and White, M. J. (2003). The academic trajectories of immigrant youths: analysis within and across cohorts. Demography 40, 759–783. doi: 10.1353/dem.2003.0034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutchess, A., and Rajaram, S. (2022). Consideration of culture in cognition: how we can enrich methodology and theory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1-18. doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02227-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, L., and Woo, H. S. (2012). Distinct trajectories in the transition to adulthood: are children of immigrants advantaged? Child Dev. 83, 1623–1639. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01798.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsin, A., and Xie, Y. (2014). Explaining Asian Americans’ academic advantage over whites. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 8416–8421. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1406402111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, G. H. C., and Gove, M. (2015). “Confucianism, Chinese families, and academic achievement: exploring how Confucianism and Asian descendant parenting practices influence children’s academic achievement” in Science education in East Asia. ed. M. S. Khine (Cham: Springer), 41–66.

Google Scholar

Imada, T., Carlson, S. M., and Itakura, S. (2013). East-west cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Dev. Sci. 16, 198–208. doi: 10.1111/desc.12016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, R., and Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school-based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement: a review. Rev. Educ. Res. 85, 512–552. doi: 10.3102/0034654314561338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kao, G., and Tienda, M. (2012). “Optimism and achievement: the educational performance of immigrant youth” in The New Immigration. eds. C. Suarez-Orozco, M. Suarez-Orozco, and D. B. Qin-Hilliard (New York: Routledge), 345–358.

Google Scholar

Kassai, R., Futo, J., Demetrovics, Z., and Takacs, Z. K. (2019). A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near-and far-transfer effects among children’s executive function skills. Psychol. Bull. 145, 165–188. doi: 10.1037/bul0000180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, U., and Tillman, K. H. (2008). Post-secondary educational attainment of immigrant and native youth. Soc. Forces 87, 121–152. doi: 10.1353/sof.0.0104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. (2021). Home educational contexts of Asian American children: disentangling the effects of structural and cultural factors. Early Child. Res. Q. 54, 307–320. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, U., and Park, Y. S. (2006). Indigenous psychological analysis of academic achievement in Korea: the influence of self-efficacy, parents, and culture. Int. J. Psychol. 41, 287–291. doi: 10.1080/00207590544000068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., and Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: a cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 677–692. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latzman, R. D., Elkovitch, N., Young, J., and Clark, L. A. (2010). The contribution of executive functioning to academic achievement among male adolescents. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 32, 455–462. doi: 10.1080/13803390903164363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, K., Lau, S., and Lam, W. L. (1998). Parenting styles and academic achievement: A cross-cultural study. Merrill-Palmer Q. 44, 157–172.

Google Scholar

Leung, N. A., Wong, S., Wong, I., and McBride-Chang, C. (2010). Filial piety and psychosocial adjustment in Hong Kong Chinese early adolescents. J. Early Adolesc. 30, 651–667. doi: 10.1177/0272431609341046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. B., Vazsonyi, A. T., and Dou, K. (2018). Is individualism-collectivism associated with self-control? Evidence from Chinese and US samples. PLoS One 13:e0208541. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, M., and Guo, F. (2010). Parenting practices and their relevance to child behaviors in Canada and China. Scand. J. Psychol. 51, 109–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00795.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattera, S., Rojas, N. M., Morris, P. A., and Bierman, K. (2021). Promoting EF with preschool interventions: lessons learned from 15 years of conducting large-scale studies. Front. Psychol. 12:640702. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.640702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcauley, T., Chen, S., Goos, L., Schachar, R., and Crosbie, J. (2010). Is the behavior rating inventory of executive function more strongly associated with measures of impairment or executive function? J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 495–505. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milevsky, A., Schlechter, M., Netter, S., and Keehn, D. (2007). Maternal and paternal parenting styles in adolescents: associations with self-esteem, depression and life-satisfaction. J. Child Fam. Stud. 16, 39–47. doi: 10.1007/s10826-006-9066-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, A. (2012). The relationship of parenting style to academic achievement in middle childhood. Ir. J. Psychol. 33, 137–152. doi: 10.1080/03033910.2012.724645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niebaum, J. C., and Munakata, Y. (2022). Why doesn’t executive function training improve academic achievement? Rethinking individual differences, relevance, and engagement from a contextual framework. J. Cogn. Dev. 1-19:2160723. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2022.2160723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., and Miyamoto, Y. (2005). The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 467–473. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nouwens, S., Groen, M. A., Kleemans, T., and Verhoeven, L. (2021). How executive functions contribute to reading comprehension. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 169–192. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, S., and Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Dev. 79, 80–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01112.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J., and Han, G. (2018). “Collectivism and the development of indigenous psychology in South Korea” in Asia-Pacific Perspectives on Intercultural Psychology. eds. W. W. Li, D. Hodgetts, and K. H. Foo (London: Routledge), 53–74.

Google Scholar

Pinquart, M., and Kauser, R. (2018). Do the associations of parenting styles with behavior problems and academic achievement vary by culture? Results from a meta-analysis. Cult. Divers. Ethn. 24, 75–100. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 359–379. doi: 10.1037/a0029082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robson, D. A., Allen, M. S., and Howard, S. J. (2020). Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 146, 324–354. doi: 10.1037/bul0000227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., and Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: a comparison of Chinese and US preschoolers. Psychol. Sci. 17, 74–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samuels, W. E., Tournaki, N., Blackman, S., and Zilinski, C. (2016). Executive functioning predicts academic achievement in middle school: a four-year longitudinal study. J. Educ. Res. 109, 478–490. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2014.979913

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schirmbeck, K., Rao, N., Wang, R., Richards, B., Chan, S. W., and Maehler, C. (2021). Contrasting executive function development among primary school children from Hong Kong and Germany. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 36, 923–943. doi: 10.1007/s10212-020-00519-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, S. A., Korucu, I., Purpura, D. J., Whiteman, S., Zhang, C., and Yang, F. (2019). Exploring cross-cultural variations in the development of executive function for preschoolers from low and high socioeconomic families. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 43, 212–220. doi: 10.1177/0165025418785469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepard, E., Sweeney, C., Thompson, L., Jacobs, S., Grimm, J., and Weyandt, L. L. (2022). Effectiveness of executive functioning training among heterogeneous adolescent samples: a systematic review. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child, 1–17. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2022.2106436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186. doi: 10.1177/1529100616661983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smuts, A. B., and Hagen, J. W. (1985). History of the family and of child development: introduction to part 1. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 50, (4–5, Serial No. 211).

Google Scholar

Stankov, L. (2010). Unforgiving Confucian culture: A breeding ground for high academic achievement, test anxiety and self-doubt? Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 555–563. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Dornbusch, S. M., and Darling, N. (1992). Impact of parenting practices on adolescent achievement: authoritative parenting, school involvement, and encouragement to succeed. Child Dev. 63, 1266–1281. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01694.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stipek, D., and Valentino, R. A. (2015). Early childhood memory and attention as predictors of academic growth trajectories. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 771–788. doi: 10.1037/edu0000004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorell, L. B., Veleiro, A., Siu, A. F., and Mohammadi, H. (2013). Examining the relation between ratings of executive functioning and academic achievement: findings from a cross-cultural study. Child Neuropsychol. 19, 630–638. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.727792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobin, J. J., Wu, D. Y., and Davidson, D. H. (1989). Preschool in Three Cultures: Japan, China, and the United States. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press

Google Scholar

Tran, C. D., Arredondo, M. M., and Yoshida, H. (2019). Early executive function: the influence of culture and bilingualism. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 22, 714–732. doi: 10.1017/S1366728918000160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tran, V. C., Lee, J., and Huang, T. J. (2019). Revisiting the Asian second-generation advantage. Ethn. Racial Stud. 42, 2248–2269. doi: 10.1080/01419870.2019.1579920

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Devine, R. T., Wong, K. K., and Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind and executive function during middle childhood across cultures. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 149, 6–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watabe, A., and Hibbard, D. R. (2014). The influence of authoritarian and authoritative parenting on Children's academic achievement motivation: a comparison between the United States and Japan. North Am. J. Psychol. 16, 359–382.

Google Scholar

Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., and Greenberg, M. (2012). The measurement of executive function at age 5: psychometric properties and relationship to academic achievement. Psychol. Assess. 24, 226–239. doi: 10.1037/a0025361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, Y. (2022). A bamboo ceiling in the classroom? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 119:e2203850119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.220385011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S., Yang, H., and Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: dissociating culture and language. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 14, 412–422. doi: 10.1017/S1366728910000611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., and Carlson, S. M. (2020). The neurodevelopment of executive function skills: implications for academic achievement gaps. Psychol. Neurosci. 13, 273–298. doi: 10.1037/pne0000208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., and Carlson, S. M. (2022). Reconciling the context-dependency and domain-generality of executive function skills from a developmental systems perspective. J. Cogn. Dev. 1-19, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2022.2156515

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: culture, executive functioning, academic achievement, control skills, model, social influences

Citation: Cho I, Hosseini-Kamkar N, Song H and Morton JB (2023) Culture, executive functions, and academic achievement. Front. Psychol. 14:1100537. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1100537

Received: 16 November 2022; Accepted: 12 April 2023;
Published: 12 May 2023.

Edited by:

Mariëtte Huizinga, VU Amsterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Elke Emmers, University of Hasselt, Belgium
Qiong Zhang, Zhejiang University, China

Copyright © 2023 Cho, Hosseini-Kamkar, Song and Morton. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: J. Bruce Morton, Ym1vcnRvbjNAdXdvLmNh

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.