Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 February 2023
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in: Health Psychology View all 10 articles

Does past/current pain change pain experience? Comparing self-reports and pupillary responses

  • Department of Psychology, Chungnam National University, Daejeon, Republic of Korea

Introduction: For decades, a substantial body of research has confirmed the subjective nature of pain. Subjectivity seems to be integrated into the concept of pain but is often confined to self-reported pain. Although it seems likely that past and current pain experiences would interact and influence subjective pain reports, the influence of these factors has not been investigated in the context of physiological pain. The current study focused on exploring the influence of past/current pain on self-reporting and pupillary responses to pain.

Methods: Overall, 47 participants were divided into two groups, a 4°C–10°C group (experiencing major pain first) and a 10°C–4°C group (experiencing minor pain first), and performed cold pressor tasks (CPT) twice for 30 s each. During the two rounds of CPT, participants reported their pain intensity, and their pupillary responses were measured. Subsequently, they reappraised their pain ratings in the first CPT session.

Results: Self-reported pain showed a significant difference (4°C–10°C: p = 0.045; 10°C–4°C: p < 0.001) in the rating of cold pain stimuli in both groups, and this gap was higher in the 10°C–4°C group than in the 4°C–10°C group. In terms of pupillary response, the 4°C–10°C group exhibited a significant difference in pupil diameter, whereas this was marginally significant in the 10°C–4°C group (4°C–10°C: p < 0.001; 10°C–4°C: p = 0.062). There were no significant changes in self-reported pain after reappraisal in either group.

Discussion: The findings of the current study confirmed that subjective and physiological responses to pain can be altered by previous experiences of pain.

1. Introduction

Pain is defined as “an aversive sensory and emotional experience typically caused by, or resembling that caused by, actual or potential tissue injury” (Raja et al., 2020). As noted in this definition, pain is not merely nociception but is an intrinsically complex inner experience consisting of a biopsychosocial process, which could vary according to biological, emotional, cognitive, and contextual factors (Love-Jones, 2019). One factor that influences pain experience is past pain experiences, which are related to the biological and psychosocial responses to pain (Rollman et al., 2004). For example, patients who had experienced pain in the past reported higher pain sensitivity than patients who had a pain-free experience, even if they no longer experienced pain at the moment (Phillips et al., 2022). Additionally, past pain experiences that were highly intense contribute to high reactivity to pain (Rollman et al., 2004). The placebo analgesic effect is altered by past pain experiences as well (Geers et al., 2015). Another factor that affects pain is the current state of pain. When people recall previous pain experiences, their intensity is estimated based on the current pain experience (Haas et al., 2002; Bąbel et al., 2018). In addition, by assimilating it, current pain impacts the perception of past pain (Eich et al., 1985).

It is likely that past and current pain interact with each other and influence the pain experience. However, the relationship between past and current pain experiences has only been investigated in self-reported pain. Since pain is a subjective experience, the most widely used method to assess pain is single-item pain scales such as the visual analog scale (VAS) or the numeric rating scale (NRS) (Hjermstad et al., 2011). Despite these single-item scales showing promising psychometric properties (Bijur et al., 2001; Goddard et al., 2004; Begum and Hossain, 2019), self-reporting pain scales are prone to bias owing to the arbitrary interpretation of self-reporting pain scales (Williams et al., 2000; Tracy, 2017; Johannessen, 2019) and temporal changes (DeLoach et al., 1998; Leino et al., 2011). In addition, the usefulness of these scales is confined to low/high levels of pain due to ceiling and floor effects, making it difficult to measure moderate levels of pain by influencing their reliability and interpretation (Bijur et al., 2001; González-Fernández et al., 2014). Furthermore, because self-reporting scales share considerable variance with factors other than pain (e.g., pain interference and emotional aspects) (Thong et al., 2018), it is unclear which aspects of pain they truly measure. Considering the limitations mentioned above, self-report scales are not precise enough to capture the dynamics of a pain experience influenced by past/current pain, and this leaves some points to be filled in by other ways of measuring pain.

To design an elaborate interpretation of a pain experience, pain-related physiological responses have been suggested as a parameter that allows for the quantification of pain intensity (Tracey et al., 2019). There is an apparent association between nociception and the autonomic nervous system (ANS) (Benarroch, 2006), and it is possible to discriminate between noxious and non-noxious stimuli from ANS parameters to some extent (Ben-Israel et al., 2013). Therefore, many previous studies have attempted to overcome the limitations of self-reporting using ANS parameters (Lin et al., 2018; Subramaniam et al., 2018; Jiang et al., 2019). As the pupillary response is linked to both the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems (Bouffard, 2019), it has been suggested as a useful and informative parameter for ANS activity caused by pain (Cowen et al., 2015; Charier et al., 2017). Furthermore, the pupillary response is coupled with the activity of the locus coeruleus (LC), the brain region involved in the neuromodulation of the noradrenergic system (Larsen and Waters, 2018). The processing and modulation of pain are also involved in the LC (Suárez-Pereira et al., 2021). In addition, LC-mediated stress responses caused by pain can therefore be inferred through the pupillary response (Szabadi, 2012).

Although many studies utilizing pupillary response have confirmed its practicability as a proxy of nociception, these studies have focused on reducing bias and error in measuring the pain experience but overlooked other dimensions that impact the overall pain experience (Charier et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2022). For a comprehensive understanding of the pain experience, it is necessary to move beyond merely pursuing an “objective” pain measurement. To gain a better understanding of the subjective nature of pain, it is plausible to consider a physiological response to pain as an observable proxy, which also reflects the emotional, cognitive, and contextual perspectives of the pain experience. Given the multidimensional aspects of pain, the influence of contextual factors such as past/current pain has yet to be extensively investigated, considering physiological pain as just one component of the total subjective pain experience. By elucidating how the pain response changed in the subjective perception and physiological pain experience depending on the past and current pain experience, it should be possible to gain a better understanding of pain experience.

Motivated by this background, this study aimed to investigate the influence of past and current pain by recording self-reported pain and pupillary responses. We manipulated past and current pain experiences using a cold pressor task (CPT) with different intensities of cold pain stimuli. We then compared self-reported and pupillary responses to cold pain stimuli with different experiences of past and current pain. Additionally, we examined whether the appraisal of previous pain could be affected by the current pain experience. Using this approach, we attempted to demonstrate how past pain influences the current pain response, and how the perception of previous pain varies with the current state of pain. Based on previous findings, we hypothesized the following: 1) both self-reported pain and pupil diameter increase as participants experienced longer CPT and both indices would be higher in the 4°C CPT than in the 10°C CPT regardless of the group; 2) in the second CPT, participants who experienced 4°C in the first CPT will report higher pain intensity compared with their counterparts who experienced 10°C in the first CPT, even though they conducted the second CPT at a higher temperature (10°C) than the first CPT (4°C); 3) in the second CPT, pupil diameter will be larger in the 4°C CPT than 10°C CPT irrespective of the temperature of the first CPT; and 4) participants who experience 4°C in the second CPT will downgrade their past pain intensity compared with the participants who experience 10°C in the second CPT.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The participants were recruited from Chungnam National University through a university bulletin board between August 2020 and October 2021. Eligibility criteria were as follows: (1) age over 19 years; (2) never experienced CPT; (3) no substantial pain at baseline (no current pain >3/10 on the VAS when participants arrived at the laboratory); (4) no dermatological problems in the non-dominant hand (e.g., injury on hand); (5) without severe acute pain (e.g., traumatic injury, surgical treatment) or chronic pain history (no experienced pain>3/10 on VAS for more than 3 months, e.g., arthritis, chronic low back pain); (6) no ophthalmological problems (e.g., glaucoma); (7) no neuropsychological/psychiatric problems; (8) no recent use of analgesics; and (9) fluent in the Korean language. A power analysis was conducted using G*power software, version 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007). Based on the power analysis, at least 52 participants for the total sample size were recommended to detect an effect size of 0.20 with a power of 0.80 (α = 0.05). A total of 96 potential participants were enrolled, most of whom were undergraduate students. One participant was excluded because of the recent use of analgesics. Participants were randomly assigned to the 4°C–10°C (n = 47) and 10°C–4°C (n = 48) groups. Ninety-five participants participated in two rounds of CPT. Participants who tolerated CPT for over 30 s were included in the statistical analysis. Of the 95 participants, 47 did not meet this criterion for statistical analysis. The final sample consisted of 47 participants (group 4°C–10°C, n = 23; group 10°C–4°C, n = 24). Another participant was excluded because of declining participation in the CPT. All participants were compensated $8.50. See Figure 1 for the CONSORT flowchart.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. CONSORT flowchart of this study.

2.2. Measures and apparatus

2.2.1. Pain intensity

Pain intensity was measured using the 11-point NRS [i.e., 0, no pain at all; 10, worst pain imaginable (Jensen and Karoly, 2011)]. Therefore, a high score means that the participant experienced high pain intensity.

2.2.2. Pain catastrophizing

Pain catastrophizing was measured using the Korean version of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (K-PCS) (Cho et al., 2013). The K-PCS is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 13 items measuring pain catastrophizing (Sullivan et al., 1995). Each item is rated on a 5-point scale (0 = not at all to 4 = all the time). The total score ranged from 0 to 52, with a high score indicating high pain catastrophizing. The internal consistency of K-PCS was good (Cronbach’s α = 0.88).

2.2.3. Pain anxiety

Pain anxiety was measured using the Korean version of the Pain Anxiety Symptom Scale (KPASS-20) (Cho et al., 2010). The KPASS-20 is a self-reporting questionnaire consisting of 20 items measuring pain-related anxiety symptoms (McCracken et al., 1992). Each item is rated on a 6-point scale (0 = never, 5 = always). The total score ranges from 0 to 100, with a high score representing high pain and anxiety. The internal consistency of the KPASS-20 is good (Cronbach’s α = 0.88).

2.2.4. Positive affect and negative affect

Positive and negative affect were measured using the Korean version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Lim et al., 2010). The PANAS is a self-reporting questionnaire that consists of 10 items measuring positive mood and 10 items measuring negative mood (Schmukle et al., 2002). Each item is rated on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all to 5 = extremely). The total score ranges from 10 to 50 for each subscale. A higher score represents people experiencing a higher level of both positive and negative affects. The internal consistency of the positive affect subscale was good (Cronbach’s α = 0.84). For the negative affect subscale, the internal consistency was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = 0.70).

2.2.5. Cold pressor task

CPT has also been used to induce experimental pain (Mitchell et al., 2004). CPT was conducted using a refrigeration tank (SH-WB-11R-OM) with a water circulation system manufactured by SAMHEUNG ENERGY, Korea. Before beginning, the participants were notified of the overall procedure. To control the baseline hand temperature, participants were asked to immerse their non-dominant hand in a plastic bath filled with warm water (36°C ± 0.5°C) for 2 min. After 2 min, the participants were required to put their hands into a tank filled with cold water maintained at 4°C or 10°C. The participants were blinded to the water temperature. Participants were allowed to withdraw their hand when they felt that they could no longer endure the pain. The upper time limit of CPT immersion was 30 s.

2.2.6. Pupillary response

The pupillary response was measured using a monocular handheld digital infrared pupillometer (PLR™-3000) manufactured by NeurOptics (CA, United States). A pupillometer was used with an eye cup to reduce the impact of ambient light during measurement. The measurements were performed in a quiet room with an illuminance of 795 ± 5 lx. The participants’ right eye was exposed to a white light stimulus of 50 μW to control baseline pupil size, followed by 10 μW light stimulus during measurement, with pupillary response automatically recorded by the pupillometer. The measurement was completed 5 s after the participants withdrew their hands from the cold water.

2.3. Procedure

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Chungnam National University (No. 202001-SB-009-01). Upon arrival at the laboratory at Chungnam National University, participants were randomly assigned to two groups (the 4°C–10°C group or the 10°C–4°C group). Participants then completed informed consent and self-reporting questionnaires (demographic information, K-PCS, KPASS-20, and PANAS). Subsequently, the participants performed the first CPT. Participants reported pain intensity every 10 s, and pupillary responses were measured during the CPT (assessment 1). Participants then took a 15-min break (recovery phase). In doing so, the participants were able to recover from CPT-induced pain. If a participant felt pain even after 15 min, they could take additional time for recovery. The second CPT was then carried out with a self-reported pain rating and pupillary response measurement (assessment 2). The overall pain intensity during CPT was reported at the end of every CPT. The temperature of CPT in assessments 1 and 2 was contingent on the group. The participants who were allocated to the 4°C–10°C group performed the 4°C CPT in assessment 1 and the 10°C CPT in assessment 2. Inversely, participants in the 10°C–4°C group performed the 10°C CPT first and the 4°C CPT later. Subsequently, all participants were asked to reappraise their pain intensity in the first CPT (reappraisal phase). In the reappraisal phase, the 4°C–10°C group reported pain intensity of 4°C CPT and the 10°C–4°C group reported pain intensity of the 10°C CPT. Finally, participants were thoroughly debriefed and compensated.

2.4. Statistical analysis

Data reduction and statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 26 and R version 3.5.2. Independent t-tests and χ2 tests were used to compare between-group differences at the baseline. As a primary analysis to investigate the effect of the group on outcome variables, a three-way (Group × Time × Temperature) mixed-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied. A significant three-way interaction was decomposed using a simple interaction analysis. A simple main analysis was performed if a simple interaction effect showed a significant result. A simple main analysis was used for two-way significant interaction effects. The pupil diameter index was calculated based on a previously described method (Eisenach et al., 2017). Pupil diameters of <20 mm and > 90 mm were considered outliers. Outliers were replaced with the mean pupil diameter of each individual. Data from 1 s to 35 s were included in the analysis. Pain ratings by NRS at each time point were used to assess the pain intensity. Additionally, we explored whether recent pain experiences influenced the rating of previous pain experiences. To compare the reappraised pain rating differences between groups, a two-way (Group × Measurement point) mixed-measures ANOVA was used. All tests were 2-tailed, and p < 0.05 of statistical significance criteria. The results are reported as the mean ± standard deviation in the tables and the mean ± standard error in the figures. The generalized eta squared (ŋG2) was used to calculate the effect size (Bakeman, 2005).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive statistics

The participant characteristics are summarized in Table 1. There was no significant difference between the groups in terms of age (t(45) = −1.21, p = 0.24) or gender (χ2(1) = 0.20, p = 0.77). Pain at baseline (t(45) = −0.83, p = 0.41), pain catastrophizing (t(45) = 0.05, p = 0.96), pain anxiety (t(45) = −5.41, p = 0.59), positive affect (t(45) = −0.18, p = 0.86), and negative affect (t(45) = −0.55, p = 0.59) showed no significant difference between the two groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and group difference for baseline measures.

3.2. Pain intensity

A three-way (Group: 4°C–10°C and 10°C–4°C × Time: 10 s, 20 s, and 30 s × Temperature: 4°C and 10°C) mixed-measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of temperature (F(1, 45) = 79.58, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.11) and time (F(1.32, 59.61) = 248.71, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.38). This result indicated that pain intensity was higher in low-temperature conditions than in high-temperature conditions, and at a later time point than at the early time point in CPT. No significant interaction between group × time × temperature was found (F(1.75, 78.75) = 1.03, p = 0.36, ŋG2 = 0.001). However, the group × temperature (F(1, 45) = 10.51, p = 0.002, ŋG2 = 0.02) and time × temperature interaction (F(1.75, 78.75) = 3.79, p = 0.03, ŋG2 = 0.003) showed a significant effect. Furthermore, the simple main analysis showed a simple main effect of temperature in both groups (4°C–10°C: F(1, 278) = 4.05, p = 0.045, ŋG2 = 0.01; 10°C–4°C: F(1, 278) = 19.40, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.07). In detail, self-reported pain in the 10°C CPT was lower than that in the 4°C CPT in both groups, and this difference was higher in the 10°C–4°C group than in the 4°C–10°C group. In addition, the simple main effect of time was significant at both temperatures (4°C: F(2, 276) = 47.50, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.001; 10°C: F(2, 276) = 36.00, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.01), indicating that pain intensity increased over time at both temperatures, but the effect was higher at 10°C than at 4°C (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Change of pain intensity at different time points and temperatures compared between groups. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

3.3. Pupillary response

A three-way (Group: 4°C–10°C, 10°C–4°C × Time: 1–35 s × Temperature: 4°C, 10°C) mixed-measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of time (F(4.59, 206.45) = 112.91, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.17). This result shows that as the duration of CPT increased, the pupil diameter also increased. However, there was no significant main effect of group (F(1, 45) = 1.24, p = 0.272, ŋG2 = 0.02) or temperature (F(1, 45) = 0.46, p = 0.500, ŋG2 = 0.001). The group × time × temperature interaction was marginally significant (F(6.03, 271.40) = 2.117, p = 0.051, ŋG2 = 0.002) and the group × temperature interaction was also marginally significant (F(1.00, 45.00) = 4.03, p = 0.051, ŋG2 = 0.006). Furthermore, the simple main effect of temperature was significant in the 4°C–10°C group, while it showed a marginally significant difference in the 10°C–4°C group (4°C–10°C: F(1, 3,192) = 13.70, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.004; 10°C-4°C: F(1, 3,192) = 3.48, p = 0.062, ŋG2 = 0.001). However, the effect sizes were weak in both groups. The pupil diameter in the 4°C CPT was higher than that in the 10°C CPT in the 4°C–10°C group. In the 10°C–4°C group, the pupil diameter for the 10°C CPT was higher than that for the 4°C CPT (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Change of pupillary response at different time points and temperatures compared between groups. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

3.4. Pain reappraisal

A two-way (Groups: 4°C–10°C, 10°C–4°C × Measurement point: overall, reappraisal) repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of the group (F(1, 88) = 19.78, p < 0.001, ŋG2 = 0.18), indicating that participants who had experienced 4°C CPT first rated overall and reappraised pain intensity higher than participants who had experienced 10°C CPT first. This result showed that participants reported higher pain on the 4°C CPT than on the 10°C CPT after reappraisal. There were no significant main effects of time (F(1, 88) = 0.59, p = 0.443, ŋG2 = 0.007) and group × time interaction (F(1, 88) = 2.19, p = 0.143, ŋG2 = 0.02). These results revealed that a recent experience of pain did not change the reappraised pain intensity (Figure 4; Table 2).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interaction plot of overall and reappraisal with pain intensity between groups. The result of the 4°C–10°C group depicts the NRS score for the first 4°C CPT, and the 10°C–4°C group depicts the NRS score for the first 10 CPT. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means and Standard deviations for pain intensity and pupil diameter.

4. Discussion

In this study, we investigated the pattern of subjective and physiological responses to cold pain stimuli and whether they were influenced by past or current pain experiences. As the limitations of self-reporting have increased, there have been numerous attempts to investigate pain responses in terms of biological parameters (Laycock and Bantel, 2016; Subramaniam et al., 2018). However, these studies have rarely considered contextual changes in pain. By understanding the influence of past and current pain experiences, we attempted to integrate the contextual perspective into pain research. Consistent with previous studies (Tassorelli et al., 1995), both self-reported pain experiences and pupil diameter increased in participants as the duration of the cold stimuli increased, regardless of temperature. We also found a main effect of temperature in self-reporting, where participants reported higher pain intensity during the 4°C CPT experiment than in the 10°C CPT experiment. Unlike self-reporting, the temperature of CPT had no significant effect on pupil diameter. Based on these results, we can assume that the subjective perception of pain is affected by both time and temperature, but that the physiological response is altered by time only.

We also confirmed that the influence of past/current pain has some differences in self-reporting and pupillary responses. Regarding self-reported pain, both groups showed lower pain intensity during the 10°C CPT than during the 4°C CPT, but the pain rating of the 10°C CPT in the 10°C–4°C group was lower than that in the 4°C–10°C group. These results suggest that, despite the same intensity of noxious stimuli, contextual factors can change self-reported pain intensity. The influence of past/current pain on self-reporting might reveal a different magnitude because the participants interpreted the NRS anchors differently. Although anchors of NRS exist (Hrvatin and Puh, 2021), the meaning of the numbers could vary according to one’s personal interpretation (Robinson-Papp et al., 2015). In particular, the label of the upper limit in NRS is prone to bias from past pain experiences because the meaning of 10 is infinite with no boundary (Walton et al., 2018). Participants in this study comprised young adults who had no history of severe pain, so the first CPT was more likely to manipulate their interpretation of the label of “worst pain imaginable.” The low intensity of the first 10°C CPT could make the 10°C–4°C group interpret the upper label of NRS as a less painful experience relative to the 4°C–10°C group, which experienced a more painful first CPT. This may contribute to a distinct perception between 10°C and 4°C cold pain stimuli by interpreting 4°C as much closer to the worst pain they can imagine than 10°C.

The results of the pupillary response revealed a different pattern from that of self-reported pain. We found that the group effect was significant in the pupillary response, indicating that the first experience of CPT alters the pupillary response in the second CPT. Contrary to our hypothesis that temperature influences pupil diameter regardless of the previous pain experience, the difference in pupil diameter was distinct, whereas the effect size was weak for both groups. It is noteworthy that the pupil diameter not only varied depending on the temperature but also on previous experiences of pain.

One possible explanation for these results is that the emotional response to the second CPT changed due to the experience of the prior CPT. Pain is usually accompanied by high emotional arousal, such as anxiety (Kapoor et al., 2016; Michaelides and Zis, 2019), pain anxiety (Cimpean and David, 2019), and fear of pain (Martinez-Calderon et al., 2019), which contribute to a worsening of the pain experience. Since the pupil dilates not only in response to nociception but also to emotional arousal (Bradley et al., 2008), emotional arousal could be the reason why the pupil dilated in different patterns. For instance, in the 4°C–10°C group, it is conceivable that participants experienced more aversive pain in their first CPT than in the 10°C–4°C group. High emotional arousal in the first pain experience may change the perception of the subsequent pain and bring out different magnitudes of pupillary response in the 4°C–10°C group by altering the emotional pain experience. Given that emotion is one of the major components of pain experience, the demand for emotion regulation may influence the pupillary response during the experience of pain. Emotion regulation can be reflected in the pupillary response because it is highly related to ANS and LC activity (Kinner et al., 2017; Ferencova et al., 2021). Previous experiences with different levels of pain may demand a different level of emotion regulation, which could lead to alterations in pupillary responses to subsequent pain.

Cognitive factors are also involved in pupillary response to pain. As the pupillary response is linked to cognitive aspects through the LC-norepinephrine system (van der Wel and van Steenbergen, 2018), pupil diameter could be affected by cognitive factors. A previous study demonstrated that the expectation of pain can alter the subsequent pupillary response (Eisenach et al., 2017). Even though the participants experienced the same intensity of 50°C, when participants anticipated lower pain (47°C), their pupil diameter was smaller than when they expected the correct intensity of pain they received (50°C) (Eisenach et al., 2017). In the current study, we also identified a similar pattern of pupil dilation; whereas the pupil diameter in the first 4°C CPT was the highest among all measurements, the pupil diameter was dilated less in the 4°C CPT after experiencing the 10°C CPT. Participants may have had a smaller pupil diameter because they anticipated less intensity of pain based on previous pain experience.

Regarding the reappraisal of the previous pain experience, self-reported pain was still higher in the 4°C CPT than in the 10°C CPT after reappraisal, even though they had a distinct recent pain experience. Although a previous study suggested that the relief of current pain alters the perception of the previous pain experience (Smith and Safer, 1993), our findings demonstrate that people can calibrate their previous pain experience accurately regardless of the current state of pain. This was in line with the results of another study, which confirmed that the rating of past pain was explained mainly by the pain intensity at the time it occurred, rather than the current pain (Daoust et al., 2017). Likewise, current pain might have had little influence on the change in the appraisal of previous pain experiences in this study. In addition, another study showed that the association between past and current pain could be weak; only a small proportion (26.5%) of individuals without pain used past pain experiences in current pain appraisal (Staud et al., 2010). Contrary to the common assumption that a patient’s report is an unreliable source of a pain experience (Marty et al., 2009; Dorfman et al., 2016), these results suggest that it is more accurate than expected, while the risk of being biased by contextual factors remains (Jensen et al., 2008; Boring et al., 2022).

These findings have several clinical implications. First, they suggest that self-reported pain scales have a potential risk of bias depending on previous pain. This implies that patients’ self-reporting of their pain could be perceived differently due to their past pain experiences. As the unreliability of a patient’s reporting of pain could lead to miscommunication between patients and clinicians and improper interventions, further investigation is needed to find solutions for these problems and address them accordingly (Seers et al., 2018). Past pain experience could be a point for adjusting the patient’s self-reporting to be more reliable. In addition, applying self-reporting pain scales with additional aids would be a way to solve related problems. For example, providing a clinician’s interpretation of self-reported pain facilitates communication easier (Bakshi et al., 2021). Training using a fixed anchor of the pain experience and a reminder card could also be beneficial for enhancing the accuracy of the pain rating (Smith et al., 2016).

Second, the pupillary response patterns identified in this study highlight the importance of proper pain management. The low intensity of previous pain leads to a decreased subsequent physiological pain response, even for the high-intensity pain stimuli. Based on this result, we can infer that relieving pain could change physiological responses to pain in the future. Likewise, previous pain experiences cause psychological symptoms and elevated pain sensitivity in the chronic pain population, whereas patients with no recent pain experience show low pain sensitivity and few psychological symptoms (Phillips et al., 2022). Given that our results underpin the influence of a previous pain experience, it also supports that timely clinical interventions that alleviate the current state of pain have not only immediate clinical benefits but also long-term benefits by preventing the exacerbation of pain and psychological problems caused by a previous pain experience.

Third, our findings support the concept of pain as a multidimensional experience, underscoring the necessity of multimodal pain assessment. Multimodal pain assessment integrates various components of pain; its benefits far outweigh single-scale pain assessment by offering a comprehensive understanding of pain (Wideman et al., 2019). One factor restricting multimodal pain assessment is the absence of valid tools to measure the various dimensions of pain (Jaaniste et al., 2019). Although some physiological parameters have been suggested, they have technical problems for use in daily practice because of inconvenient assessment equipment, such as electroencephalography (EEG) or electromyography (EMG) (Cowen et al., 2015). Pain parameters that can be assessed using easy-to-use devices are required (Wagemakers et al., 2019). The pupillary response not only reflects both physiological and psychological perspectives of pain but also provides convenience and information about real-time ANS activity (Joshi and Barreto, 2008; Maruthy et al., 2020). In this regard, integrating pupillometry into pain assessment would promote multimodal assessment and tailored treatment of pain. Our results reveal fundamental knowledge about the response pattern of the pupil and self-reporting of pain in different contexts and their contribution to the application of multimodal pain assessment.

Despite these implications, a few limitations of this study remain. First, the participants in our study consisted of a homogenous population of young, healthy Asian adults without a history of severe pain. In addition, we excluded participants who endured less than 30 s in two rounds of CPT. Therefore, our data could not report the response patterns of people with low pain tolerance. Overall, future studies should consider various demographic factors (Fillingim, 2017) and individual differences related to pain (Coghill, 2010). Second, because our results were obtained from a small sample size, further studies should validate this result with a larger sample size. Third, the study design excluded individuals with chronic pain, limiting the sample to participants with acute pain conditions. Chronic pain disorders are frequently accompanied by dysregulation of ANS activity (Tracy et al., 2016) and analgesia, which change the pupillary response (Charier et al., 2019). Further investigations are warranted to generalize our results to other patients with chronic pain. Finally, because the main aim of this study was to explore the influence of past and current pain on pain responses, we did not examine their causal relationship. Given that the subjective and physiological experiences of pain may impact each other (Caceres and Burns, 1997; Mischkowski et al., 2019), further investigations are required to identify the specific underlying mechanism and causal relationships between them.

Here, we tried to broaden our understanding of the influence of past and current pain experiences. Our findings verified that self-reported pain and physiological response to pain experience varied according to contextual differences and represented different response patterns. Through replication of this work in chronic pain conditions, it would be possible to provide a basis for applying multimodal assessment extensively and assure the prospective benefits of using pupillary response in pain assessment. In addition, this approach could contribute to effective and pragmatic pain assessments in clinical settings.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institute Review Board at Chungnam National University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

HY contributed to study design, data collection, organizing database, statistical analysis, and writing manuscript. YC participated in data collection and study design. SC supervised the whole process of the study and writing. All authors have contributed to the manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2022S1A5A2A03050752) and Young Investigator Grant of Korean Health Psychology Association.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Bąbel, P., Bajcar, E., Śmieja, M., Adamczyk, W., Świder, K., Kicman, P., et al. (2018). Pain begets pain. When marathon runners are not in pain anymore, they underestimate their memory of marathon pain––a mediation analysis. Eur. J. Pain 22, 800–809. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behav. Res. Methods 37, 379–384. doi: 10.3758/bf03192707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakshi, S. G., Rathod, A., and Salunkhe, S. (2021). Influence of interpretation of pain scores on patients’ perception of pain: a prospective study. Indian J. Anaesth. 65, 216–220. doi: 10.4103/ija.ija_130_21

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begum, M. R., and Hossain, M. A. (2019). Validity and reliability of visual analogue scale (VAS) for pain measurement. J. Med. Case Reports Rev. 2, 394–402.

Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. (2006). Pain-autonomic interactions. Neurol. Sci. 27, s130–s133. doi: 10.1007/s10072-006-0587-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Israel, N., Kliger, M., Zuckerman, G., Katz, Y., and Edry, R. (2013). Monitoring the nociception level: a multi-parameter approach. J. Clin. Monit. Comput. 27, 659–668. doi: 10.1007/s10877-013-9487-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bijur, P. E., Silver, W., and Gallagher, E. J. (2001). Reliability of the visual analog scale for measurement of acute pain. Acad. Emerg. Med. 8, 1153–1157. doi: 10.1111/j.1553-2712.2001.tb01132.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boring, B. L., Ng, B. W., Nanavaty, N., and Mathur, V. A. (2022). Over-rating pain is overrated: a fundamental self-other bias in pain reporting behavior. J. Pain 23, 1779–1789. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2022.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouffard, M. A. (2019). The pupil CONTINUUM. Lifelong Learn. Neurol. 25, 1194–1214. doi: 10.1212/con.0000000000000771

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., and Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology 45, 602–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caceres, C., and Burns, J. W. (1997). Cardiovascular reactivity to psychological stress may enhance subsequent pain sensitivity. Pain 69, 237–244. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(96)03289-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charier, D., Vogler, M.-C., Zantour, D., Pichot, V., Martins-Baltar, A., Courbon, M., et al. (2019). Assessing pain in the postoperative period: analgesia nociception IndexTM versus pupillometry. Br. J. Anaesth. 123, e322–e327. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.09.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charier, D. J., Zantour, D., Pichot, V., Chouchou, F., Barthelemy, J.-C. M., Roche, F., et al. (2017). Assessing pain using the variation coefficient of pupillary diameter. J. Pain 18, 1346–1353. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, S., Kim, H.-Y., and Lee, J.-H. (2013). Validation of the Korean version of the pain catastrophizing scale in patients with chronic non-cancer pain. Qual. Life Res. 22, 1767–1772. doi: 10.1007/s11136-012-0308-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, S., Lee, S.-M., McCracken, L. M., Moon, D.-E., and Heiby, E. M. (2010). Psychometric properties of a Korean version of the pain anxiety symptoms Scale-20 in chronic pain patients. Int. J. Behav. Med. 17, 108–117. doi: 10.1007/s12529-010-9080-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cimpean, A., and David, D. (2019). The mechanisms of pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety in acute pain. Health Psychol. Open 6:2055102919865161. doi: 10.1177/2055102919865161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coghill, R. C. (2010). Individual differences in the subjective experience of pain: new insights into mechanisms and models. Headache: J. Head Face Pain 50, 1531–1535. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2010.01763.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowen, R., Stasiowska, M. K., Laycock, H., and Bantel, C. (2015). Assessing pain objectively: the use of physiological markers. Anaesthesia 70, 828–847. doi: 10.1111/anae.13018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daoust, R., Sirois, M.-J., Lee, J. S., Perry, J. J., Griffith, L. E., Worster, A., et al. (2017). Painful memories: reliability of pain intensity recall at 3 months in senior patients. Pain Res. Manag. 2017:5983721. doi: 10.1155/2017/5983721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLoach, L. J., Higgins, M. S., Caplan, A. B., and Stiff, J. L. (1998). The visual analog scale in the immediate postoperative period: intrasubject variability and correlation with a numeric scale. Anesth. Analg. 86, 102–106. doi: 10.1213/00000539-199801000-00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorfman, D., George, M. C., Robinson-Papp, J., Rahman, T., Tamler, R., and Simpson, D. M. (2016). Patient reported outcome measures of pain intensity: do they tell us what we need to know? Scand. J. Pain 11, 73–76. doi: 10.1016/j.sjpain.2015.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eich, E., Reeves, J. L., Jaeger, B., and Graff-Radford, S. B. (1985). Memory for pain: relation between past and present pain intensity. Pain 23, 375–380. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(85)90007-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenach, J. C., Curry, R., Aschenbrenner, C. A., Coghill, R. C., and Houle, T. T. (2017). Pupil responses and pain ratings to heat stimuli: reliability and effects of expectations and a conditioning pain stimulus. J. Neurosci. Methods 279, 52–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferencova, N., Visnovcova, Z., Olexova, L. B., and Tonhajzerova, I. (2021). Eye pupil–a window into central autonomic regulation via emotional/cognitive processing. Physiol. Res. 70, S669–S682. doi: 10.33549/physiolres.934749 Actions

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fillingim, R. B. (2017). Individual differences in pain: understanding the mosaic that makes pain personal. Pain 158, S11–S18. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geers, A. L., Fowler, S. L., Wellman, J. A., Helfer, S. G., Close, S., and France, C. R. (2015). Prior experience with a pain stimulus as a predictor of placebo analgesia. J. Behav. Med. 38, 136–142. doi: 10.1007/s10865-014-9586-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goddard, G., Karibe, H., and McNeill, C. (2004). Reproducibility of visual analog scale (VAS) pain scores to mechanical pressure. Cranio 22, 250–256. doi: 10.1179/crn.2004.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Fernández, M., Ghosh, N., Ellison, T., McLeod, J. C., Pelletier, C. A., and Williams, K. (2014). Moving beyond the limitations of the visual analog scale for measuring pain: novel use of the general labeled magnitude scale in a clinical setting. Amer. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 93, 75–81. doi: 10.1097/phm.0b013e31829e76f7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haas, M., Nyiendo, J., and Aickin, M. (2002). One-year trend in pain and disability relief recall in acute and chronic ambulatory low back pain patients. Pain 95, 83–91. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00377-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hjermstad, M. J., Fayers, P. M., Haugen, D. F., Caraceni, A., Hanks, G. W., Loge, J. H., et al. (2011). Studies comparing numerical rating scales, verbal rating scales, and visual analogue scales for assessment of pain intensity in adults: a systematic literature review. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 41, 1073–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2010.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hrvatin, I., and Puh, U. (2021). Measurement properties of the numerical pain rating scale in patients with musculoskeletal impairments of the limbs–a systematic literature review. Sloven. Med. J. 90, 1–9. doi: 10.6016/ZdravVestn.3108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaaniste, T., Noel, M., Yee, R. D., Bang, J., Tan, A. C., and Champion, G. D. (2019). Why unidimensional pain measurement prevails in the pediatric acute pain context and what multidimensional self-report methods can offer. Children 6:132. doi: 10.3390/children6120132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, M. P., and Karoly, P. (2011). “Self-report scales and procedures for assessing pain in adults” in Handbook of Pain Assessment. eds. D. C. Turk and R. Melzack (New York: The Guilford Press), 135–151.

Google Scholar

Jensen, M. P., Mardekian, J., Lakshminarayanan, M., and Boye, M. E. (2008). Validity of 24-h recall ratings of pain severity: biasing effects of “peak” and “end” pain. Pain 137, 422–427. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, M., Mieronkoski, R., Syrjälä, E., Anzanpour, A., Terävä, V., Rahmani, A. M., et al. (2019). Acute pain intensity monitoring with the classification of multiple physiological parameters. J. Clin. Monit. Comput. 33, 493–507. doi: 10.1007/s10877-018-0174-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johannessen, L. E. (2019). The commensuration of pain: how nurses transform subjective experience into objective numbers. Soc. Sci. Med. 233, 38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.05.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, V., and Barreto, A. (2008). Real-Time Measurement of Pupil Diameter for Quantitative Monitoring of the Autonomic Nervous System in Biomedical Engineering Recent Developments. eds. H. Nazeran, M. Goldman and Schoephoerster (Medical and Engineering Publishers), 47–50.

Google Scholar

Kapoor, S., White, J., Thorn, B. E., and Block, P. (2016). Patients presenting to the emergency department with acute pain: the significant role of pain catastrophizing and state anxiety. Pain Med. 17, 1069–1078. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnv034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinner, V. L., Kuchinke, L., Dierolf, A. M., Merz, C. J., Otto, T., and Wolf, O. T. (2017). What our eyes tell us about feelings: tracking pupillary responses during emotion regulation processes. Psychophysiology 54, 508–518. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, R. S., and Waters, J. (2018). Neuromodulatory correlates of pupil dilation. Front. Neural Circuits 12:21. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laycock, H., and Bantel, C. (2016). Objective assessment of acute pain. J.Anesth. Clin. Res. 7:1000630. doi: 10.4172/2155-6148.1000630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leino, K., Kuusniemi, K., Lertola, K., and Olkkola, K. (2011). Comparison of four pain scales in patients with hip fracture or other lower limb trauma. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 55, 495–502. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-6576.2010.02373.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, Y.-J., Yu, B.-H., Kim, D.-K., and Kim, J.-H. (2010). The positive and negative affect schedule: psychometric properties of the Korean version. Psychiatry Investig. 7, 163–169. doi: 10.4306/pi.2010.7.3.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y., Wang, L., Xiao, Y., Urman, R.D., Dutton, R., and Ramsay, M. (2018). Objective pain measurement based on physiological signals, in: Proceedings of the International Symposium on Human Factors and Ergonomics in Health Care SAGE Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA.

Google Scholar

Love-Jones, S. J. (2019). “Pain as a subjective, multidimensional experience” in Pain (New York: Springer), 141–144.

Google Scholar

Martinez-Calderon, J., Flores-Cortes, M., Morales-Asencio, J. M., and Luque-Suarez, A. (2019). Pain-related fear, pain intensity and function in individuals with chronic musculoskeletal pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Pain 20, 1394–1415. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2019.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marty, M., Rozenberg, S., Legout, V., Durand-Zaleski, I., Moyse, D., Henrotin, Y., et al. (2009). Influence of time, activities, and memory on the assessment of chronic low back pain intensity. Spine 34, 1604–1609. doi: 10.1097/brs.0b013e3181a15359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maruthy, K., Padmavathi, R., Sowjanya, B., and MaheshKumar, K. (2020). Quantitative determination of pupil by dynamic pupillometry using infrared videography–role in evaluation of autonomic activity. Clin. Epidemiol. Glob. Health 8, 728–732. doi: 10.1016/j.cegh.2020.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCracken, L. M., Zayfert, C., and Gross, R. T. (1992). The pain anxiety symptoms scale: development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain 50, 67–73. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(92)90113-p

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michaelides, A., and Zis, P. (2019). Depression, anxiety and acute pain: links and management challenges. Postgrad. Med. 131, 438–444. doi: 10.1080/00325481.2019.1663705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischkowski, D., Palacios-Barrios, E. E., Banker, L., Dildine, T. C., and Atlas, L. Y. (2019). Pain or nociception? Subjective experience mediates the effects of acute noxious heat on autonomic responses-corrected and republished. Pain 160, 1469–1481. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, L. A., MacDonald, R. A., and Brodie, E. E. (2004). Temperature and the cold pressor test. J. Pain 5, 233–237. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, A. E., Bick, B. L., Faghih, M., Yadav, D., Drewes, A. M., Singh, V. K., et al. (2022). Pain sensitivity and psychiatric comorbidities in chronic pancreatitis patients with and without pain: past experience matters. Gastro Hep. Adv. 1, 796–802. doi: 10.1016/j.gastha.2022.04.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raja, S. N., Carr, D. B., Cohen, M., Finnerup, N. B., Flor, H., Gibson, S., et al. (2020). The revised IASP definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises. Pain 161, 1976–1982. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson-Papp, J., George, M. C., Dorfman, D., and Simpson, D. M. (2015). Barriers to chronic pain measurement: a qualitative study of patient perspectives. Pain Med. 16, 1256–1264. doi: 10.1111/pme.12717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rollman, G. B., Abdel-Shaheed, J., Gillespie, J. M., and Jones, K. S. (2004). Does past pain influence current pain: biological and psychosocial models of sex differences. Eur. J. Pain 8, 427–433. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmukle, S. C., Egloff, B., and Burns, L. R. (2002). The relationship between positive and negative affect in the positive and negative affect schedule. J. Res. Pers. 36, 463–475. doi: 10.1016/s0092-6566(02)00007-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seers, T., Derry, S., Seers, K., and Moore, R. A. (2018). Professionals underestimate patients' pain: a comprehensive review. Pain 159, 811–818. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Amtmann, D., Askew, R. L., Gewandter, J. S., Hunsinger, M., Jensen, M. P., et al. (2016). Pain intensity rating training: results from an exploratory study of the ACTTION PROTECCT system. Pain 157, 1056–1064. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, W. B., and Safer, M. A. (1993). Effects of present pain level on recall of chronic pain and medication use. Pain 55, 355–361. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(93)90011-d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staud, R., Robinson, M. E., and Price, D. D. (2010). Do past pain events systematically impact pain ratings of healthy subjects or fibromyalgia patients? J. Pain 11, 142–148. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suárez-Pereira, I., Llorca-Torralba, M., Bravo, L., Camarena-Delgado, C., Soriano-Mas, C., and Berrocoso, E. (2021). The role of the locus Coeruleus in pain and associated stress-related disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 91, 786–797. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.11.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Subramaniam, S. D., Doss, B., Chanderasekar, L. D., Madhavan, A., and Rosary, A. M. (2018). Scope of physiological and behavioural pain assessment techniques in children–a review. Healthc. Technol. Lett. 5, 124–129. doi: 10.1049/htl.2017.0108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, M. J., Bishop, S. R., and Pivik, J. (1995). The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol. Assess. 7, 524–532. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szabadi, E. (2012). Modulation of physiological reflexes by pain: role of the locus coeruleus. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:94. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tassorelli, C., Micieli, G., Osipova, V., Rossi, F., and Nappi, G. (1995). Pupillary and cardiovascular responses to the cold-pressor test. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 55, 45–49. doi: 10.1016/0165-1838(95)00026-t

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thong, I. S., Jensen, M. P., Miró, J., and Tan, G. (2018). The validity of pain intensity measures: what do the NRS, VAS, VRS, and FPS-R measure? Scand J Pain 18, 99–107. doi: 10.1515/sjpain-2018-0012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracey, I., Woolf, C. J., and Andrews, N. A. (2019). Composite pain biomarker signatures for objective assessment and effective treatment. Neuron 101, 783–800. doi: 10.1515/sjpain-2018-0012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracy, L. M. (2017). Psychosocial factors and their influence on the experience of pain. Pain Rep. 2:e602. doi: 10.1097/pr9.0000000000000602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracy, L. M., Ioannou, L., Baker, K. S., Gibson, S. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., and Giummarra, M. J. (2016). Meta-analytic evidence for decreased heart rate variability in chronic pain implicating parasympathetic nervous system dysregulation. Pain 157, 7–29. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Wel, P., and van Steenbergen, H. (2018). Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 2005–2015. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1432-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagemakers, S. H., van der Velden, J. M., Gerlich, A. S., Hindriks-Keegstra, A. W., van Dijk, J. F., and Verhoeff, J. J. (2019). A systematic review of devices and techniques that objectively measure patients' pain. Pain Physician 1, 1–13. doi: 10.36076/ppj/2019.22.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, D. M., Elliott, J. M., Salim, S., and Al-Nasri, I. (2018). A reconceptualization of the pain numeric rating scale: anchors and clinically important differences. J. Hand Ther. 31, 179–183. doi: 10.1016/j.jht.2017.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Guo, Y., Dalip, B., Xiao, Y., Urman, R. D., and Lin, Y. (2022). An experimental study of objective pain measurement using pupillary response based on genetic algorithm and artificial neural network. Appl. Intell. 52, 1145–1156. doi: 10.1007/s10489-021-02458-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wideman, T. H., Edwards, R. R., Walton, D. M., Martel, M. O., Hudon, A., and Seminowicz, D. A. (2019). The multimodal assessment model of pain: a novel framework for further integrating the subjective pain experience within research and practice. Clin. J. Pain 35, 212–221. doi: 10.1097/ajp.0000000000000670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A. C. D. C., Davies, H. T. O., and Chadury, Y. (2000). Simple pain rating scales hide complex idiosyncratic meanings. Pain 85, 457–463. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00299-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain, past pain experience, current pain experience, reappraisal, self-report, pupillary response

Citation: Yoo H, Cho Y and Cho S (2023) Does past/current pain change pain experience? Comparing self-reports and pupillary responses. Front. Psychol. 14:1094903. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1094903

Received: 10 November 2022; Accepted: 18 January 2023;
Published: 17 February 2023.

Edited by:

Changiz Mohiyeddini, Oakland University William Beaumont School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Timothy Joseph Meeker, Morgan State University, United States
Fausta Lui, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Yoo, Cho and Cho. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sungkun Cho, ✉ sungkunc@cnu.ac.kr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.