- 1College of Applied Medical Sciences, Prince Sattam Bin Abdulaziz University, Al-Kharj, Saudi Arabia
- 2Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing Department, Mansoura University, Mansoura, Egypt
- 3Department of Pediatric Nursing, Faculty of Nursing, Mansoura University, Mansoura, Egypt
- 4Department of Nursing Administration, Faculty of Nursing, Port Said University, Port Said, Egypt
Background: The prevalence of behavioral problems among preschool children is remarkably increasing in the clinical setting.
Aim: The current study aimed to investigate the association between media exposure and behavioral problems among preschool children.
Methods: This survey study recruited 560 children from 10 nurseries selected randomly, located in El-Mansoura, Egypt. Tools included socio-demographic characteristics the Media Exposure Questionnaire and the Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales (CBRS-48).
Results: The participants were exposed to media for an average of 105.84 min per day and displayed moderate levels in all subscales of CBRS-48. The duration of media exposure was significantly correlated to the hyperactivity index, learning problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problem, anxiety, and psychosomatic problems (r = 0.372, 0.356, 0.323, 0.306, 0.298, 0.291, and 0.255, respectively).
Conclusion: The duration of media exposure was significantly correlated to the subscales of CBRS-48.
Implication for nursing practice: Preschool children should be engaged in concrete activities and social interactions that may lessen negative media effects such as hyperactivity, learning problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problem, anxiety, and psychosomatic problems.
Introduction
Media has become a prominent feature in the lives of children; media exposure among young children also appears to be on the rise. A recent study shows that media exposure in early childhood has increased by 32% in the last two decades (Goode et al., 2019). Nearly all children (96.6%) use mobile devices, and the majority (92.2%) start using media devices before the age of 1 year. By 2 years of age, many children will have used mobile devices on a daily basis. Typically, 72% of preschool children own at least one screen-based electronic device (Bozzola et al., 2018; Dynia et al., 2021).
The extraordinary childhood exposure to media has evoked interest in its potential impact, where the majority of studies suggest its negative relations (Uzundağ et al., 2022). These correlations include aggressive behavior (Delhove and Greitemeyer, 2021), behavioral problems (Mundy et al., 2017; Monteiro et al., 2021), sleep disorders, anxiety and depression (Scott and Woods, 2019), victimization (Kort-Butler and Habecker, 2018), social isolation (Hammad and Alqarni, 2021), reduced prosocial behavior (Zimmerman and Christakis, 2007), attentional problems, and impairment of cognitive abilities (Christakis et al., 2018).
Specifically, previous studies suggested that the amount of early media exposure is more strongly associated with hyperactivity and inattention problems. Khalil et al. (2022), Mistry et al. (2007), and Pagani et al. (2013) detected long-term associations of exposure to media during early childhood to socioemotional difficulties at school entry and attention problems in the first grade. Moreover, Chassiakos et al. (2016) pointed to the negative effects of media exposure on verbal and memory skills in children aged 6 and 7 years for each additional hour of average exposure to media since 3 years of age.
The significant relationship between the duration of media exposure and later behavioral consequences may result from the decreased interactions between caregivers and children (Barr and Linebarger, 2016). Moreover, exposure to media among preschool children may provide them with cognitively passive activities at a time when key experiences for developing attention and behavioral self-regulation are expected to occur (Radesky et al., 2014). As such, scholars recognize sensitive periods during child development, where the specific structures or functions of the human brain are especially vulnerable to particular forms of exposure (Zeanah et al., 2011).
Using data from two time points (child baseline age 2–6 years) and controlling for baseline levels of well-being outcomes, a large cohort study discovered that early exposure to electronic media increased the risk of poorer outcomes in children, but only for some well-being indicators (such as emotional problems), not all of them (e.g., peer problems; Hinkley et al., 2014). Results from a second cohort study that examined the relationship between screen time and behavioral issues at four different ages (ages 3, 5, 7, and 9) revealed that there was a two-way relationship between screen time and internalizing issues (Garfin et al., 2022; Gueron-Sela et al., 2023).
The significance of the study lies in its intention to investigate the increasing prevalence of behavioral problems, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorders, communication disorders, learning problems, aggression, conduct disorders, and anxiety and psychosomatic problems, among preschool children. As such, the current situation has become remarkable in the clinical setting of child psychiatry (Domoff et al., 2019; Coyne et al., 2021). Thus, determining the risk factors for these emerging problems is considered a valuable role in research. Despite growing concerns about the effects of media exposure on young children, to the best of our knowledge, limited research, specifically from non-western culture and targeted preschool children, has been conducted on the specific relationship between media exposure and behavioral problems in preschool-aged children. To address this gap, a study could be conducted to examine the association between specific types of media exposure (e.g., television, tablet use) and behavioral problems in a large sample of preschool-aged children. Therefore, this study sought to answer the following research questions: How much time are preschool children exposed to media? And What is the relationship between media exposure and behavioral problems in preschool-aged children?
Subjects and method
Study design and setting
Current study used the survey study design in accordance with the checklist for reporting of survey studies (CROSS). Ensuing CROSS before and throughout the survey running could support researchers to ensure their survey are satisfactorily reliable, reproducible, and transparent (Sharma et al., 2021). The current study was carried out in nurseries located in El-Mansoura and affiliated with the Ministry of Education in Egypt. According to the Directorate of Education, 33 nurseries are situated throughout the city. The first stage of the study involved a simple random selection of 10 nurseries and during the second stage, students were randomly sampled from each of those nurseries. A total of 70 children were randomly selected using the simple random method from each nursery. Parents were informed about the study formally by letter and informally by verbal messages from the nursery secretaries. A questionnaire and an assessment tool were sent along with the letter, whose subsequent return was considered to be a confirmation of consent after explaining the aim of the study. The response rate of the parents was 80%, during the data collected from September to November 2019. A consultant statistician performed the selection of nurseries and children to ensure randomization.
Participants
The sample size was calculated using OpenEpi, Version 3, an open-source calculator. According to the Directorate of Education, the hypothesized percentage of the outcome factor in the population (p) should be 50% with a precision (d) of 5% given a population size (N) of 3,861. Thus, the minimal required sample size (n) was 350 with a confidence level of 95%. The sample was increased to compensate for the design effect of multistage sampling. Thus, the final sample obtained was 560 children with consenting parents.
Tools for data collection
The first part of the questionnaire collected demographic information, including questions about the age, sex, number of siblings, and birth order of the children and posed questions for the parents regarding their age, level of education, occupation, family residence, and family type.
The second part was intended to measure media exposure and was adopted from Ruangdaraganon et al. (2009) and Taylor et al. (2018). The third part posed questions about the knowledge of parents regarding exposure of their children to media, such as the availability of media devices, duration of media exposure, time allotted for the children’s engagement in other activities (e.g., cooking, baking, playing, and bicycling), time for watching TV, content of media, and monitoring of the contents being watched or played by them. We measured media exposure on a typical day instead of the day before to avoid assessing a day with particularly low or high levels of media exposure compared to the typical exposure of children according to Vandewater and Lee (2009). Furthermore, psychometric properties were assessed to confirm the validity and reliability of the media exposure measure; validity was assessed using the content validity index (CVI), and reliability was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha; (α), CVI = 0.76 and α = 0.80.
To assess behavioral problems, the Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales (CBRS-48) was used. The modified Conner’s rating scale consists of 48 items that aim to assess individuals aged 3–17 years. The items are rated using a four-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 = “Not true at all” to 3 = “Very much true.” Conners CPRS-48 was used to measure six sub-domains, namely, conduct disorders (items 2, 8, 14, 19, 21, 27, 35, and 39), learning problems (items 10, 25, 31, and 37), psychosomatic problems (items 32, 41, 43, and 44), hyperactivity/impulsivity (items 4, 5, 11, and 13), anxiety (items 12, 16, 24, and 47), and item hyperactivity index (items 4, 7, 11, 13, 14, 25, 31, 33, 37, and 38). Because of the aim of the current study was to address children’s behavioral issues, selected items of behavioral issues were used according to the description of the tool subdomains which is drafted with the Arabic version of the tool. The reliability and validity tests of the Arabic version of the Conners CBRS-48, which was developed by Conners, were conducted by Al-Behairy (2011) with positive values, test–retest reliability showed 0.96, 0.89, 0.85, 0.96, 0.90, and 0.98, respectively, for each domain.
A pilot study was conducted on 10 caregivers of children to test the clarity, visibility, and applicability of the tool prior to clinical data collection. The administration of each questionnaire consumed nearly 20 min.
Ethical considerations
We obtained acceptance to conduct the study from the Faculty of Nursing Ethics Committee, Mansoura University. Moreover, we obtained official approval from the directors of the nurseries and schools. The caregivers of the children were duly informed about the confidentiality of the information collected and that they had the right to withdraw from or refuse to continue the study at any time. Written informed consent was also obtained from the parents regarding data collection and publication.
Statistical analysis
Data was sorted, coded, organized, categorized, and transferred into especially designed formats. Analysis was performed using SPSS version 24 (IBM Corporation, Chicago, IL, United States). Data normality was first tested using the one-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. The categorical variables were described as numerals and percentage, whereas the continuous variables were presented as mean ± SD (standard deviation). The two groups were compared using Student’s t-test. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was employed to compare the means of more than two groups. Pearson’s correlation was used to determine the correlation between continuous data. Furthermore, the results were considered significant, non-significant, and highly significant when the probabilities of error were <5% (p < 0.05), more than 5% (p > 0.05), and <0.1% (p < 0.001), respectively.
Results
The results are presented in Table 1, where the mean age of studied children was 3.98 ± 1.73 and more than half (57.1%) were males. In terms of the number of siblings, 41.4% had one sibling, whereas 38.8% were the oldest children according to order of birth. The age of the mothers was 28 years to <39 years (57.7%) and 18 years to <28 years (32%). Half of the mothers (50.2%) achieved secondary education, whereas 31.6% had completed university education. In terms of employment, 51.6% and 45.2% of the mothers were stay-at-home mothers and government employees, respectively. For the fathers, 72% and 20.2% were aged 28–38 years and 39 to <50 years, respectively. In terms of the level of education, 45.4% and 43.6% of the fathers achieved secondary and university education, respectively, and were employed as government employees (52.7%) and in handicraft professions (29.5%). In terms of the family type, the results demonstrated that the children were divided nearly equally between nuclear and extended family types as well as between urban and rural residences. The majority (95.5%) of the families possessed TVs, smart phones, Internet or Wi-Fi services and/or iPads/tablets with less possession of electronic readers and DVD players.
In Table 2, 25.9%, 25.5%, 20.9%, and 20.5% of the children were exposed to media from 1 h to <2 h, >3 h, <1 h, and from 2 h to <3 h per day, respectively. The mean of the duration of exposure to media was 105.84 min per day. Moreover, 29.6% of the children were engaged in other activities, such as cooking, baking, playing, and bicycling for <1 h. Thus, the mean duration of exposure to other activities was 72.29 min per day. Lastly, 45.5% of the families turned on the TV even if they were not watching.
Table 2. Distribution of the parents’ information about their children’s exposure to entertainment, media materials, and electronic games.
Regarding the type of media that the children were exposed to, 58.1% and 19.8% were exposed to cartoon channels followed by mobile and tablet games, whereas 24.8% of the children had TVs in their rooms. Moreover, 22.7% owned mobile devices or tablets, whereas 17.5% shared a mobile device or tablet with their siblings. Lastly, 62.9% of the parents monitored what their children were watching or playing.
Figure 1 illustrates that the preschool children displayed moderate levels in all subscales of Conners CBRS-48. In particular, 20.9% of the children exhibited severe levels of anxiety, whereas 17.5% showed severe levels of learning problems. Lastly, 19.3% of the children demonstrated mild levels of anxiety and hyperactivity.
Table 3 illustrates that the duration of media exposure was significantly and positively correlated to Conners CBRS-48, the hyperactivity index, learning problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problem, anxiety, and psychosomatic problems (r = 0.372, 0.356, 0.323, 0.306, 0.298, 0.291, and 0.255, respectively). No significant correlation was observed between the duration of media exposure and exposure to other activities where parents engaged with children.
Table 3. Duration of media exposure in correlation with exposure to other activities spent with children and the Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales (CBRS-48; n = 560).
Table 4 clarifies multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) addressing differences across studied variables, accordingly, there is a significant effect of media exposure as independent variable on behavioral problems as dependent variables among studied preschool children, F (Roy’s Largest Root) = 7.466 with significant level p < 0.001.
Table 4. The one-way multivariate analysis of variance (one-way MANOVA) addressing differences across studied variables (n = 560).
Discussion
The World Health Organization (WHO) recommends limiting preschoolers’ average screen time to 1 hour a day by restricting their access to TVs and other media devices (WHO, 2019). Consistent with those of previous studies, our results suggest that in the majority of homes, these recommendations are far from reality. In fact, several media types are nearly a constant presence in the lives of all children.
The brain of preschool children develops rapidly and is characterized by relative plasticity in response to the environment (Gilmore et al., 2018). The media, nowadays, is considered as one of the elements that influences the children’s cognition (Blumberg et al., 2019). Accordingly, the current study demonstrated that less than one-quarter, more than one-quarter, and more than one-fifth of the preschool children were daily exposed to media from 1 h to <2 h, >3 h, and from 2 h to <3 h, respectively. The mean duration of exposure to media was 105.84 min per day.
The literature provides varied results. For example, Tomopoulos et al. (2007) reported that preschool children were exposed to media for 128.9 min/day, whereas Munzer et al. (2018) stated that the mean screen media exposure was 156 min/day. This result is in agreement with that of Kovačič and Rek (2016), who found that preschool children were exposed to media for 159.85 min/day. Moreover, the mothers in the study of Waters et al. (2016) declared an average of 177 min of electronic media use on weekdays. Moreover, Conners-Burrow et al. (2011) observed that children watched TV for198 min/day.
The study findings showed that the majority of the children were aged more than 5 years and more than half were males. In terms of the number of siblings, 41.4% had one sibling, whereas 38.8% were the oldest children according to order of birth. Half of the mothers achieved secondary education, whereas 31.6% had completed university education. The vast majority of the families possessed TVs, smartphones, Internet or Wi-Fi services and/or iPads/tablets with less possession of electronic readers and DVD players. This suggests that the sample consisted mostly of older children and that there was a relatively equal gender distribution. Also, the studied sample consisted of a mix of children with different family structures and birth order positions. Results suggested that the majority of the mothers were between the ages of 28 and 39, with a smaller proportion being younger, they were with a mix of educational backgrounds, additionally, the vast majority of the families had access to modern technology, which may have implications for their children’s media exposure and behavioral problems.
Variance regarding the duration of media exposure is related to many factors, such as birth order, residence, mothers’ age, her level of education and occupation, culture, content, screen availability, and family economic status (Eales et al., 2021). Studies on different cultures reported that the level of exposure of preschool children to media is higher than that recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics (2001), WHO (2019) and Domoff et al. (2020). Evidently, children are spending considerable amounts of times in front of the screen in a passive manner instead of being physically active. Thus, media devices have become an influential factor in the lives of children (Robinson et al., 2017; Domoff et al., 2020).
The current study revealed that the duration of media exposure is significantly and positively correlated to Conners CBRS-48, the hyperactivity index, learning problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problems, anxiety, and psychosomatic problems. Congruently, a meta-analysis conducted by Nikkelen et al. (2014) noted a significant relationship between media use and behaviors related to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Radesky et al. (2014) found that mild increases in media exposure are associated with problems related to early childhood self-regulation. Moreover, sustained TV exposure is a risk factor for behavioral problems (Mistry et al., 2007). Specifically, Xie et al. (2020) observed that preschool children with a screen time of more than 60 min tend to display more behavioral problems than those with a screen time of <60 min.
Along these arguments, Parkes et al. (2013) reported that watching TV for 3 h or more among 5-year old children predicted conduct problems by the time they reach 7 years compared with watching it <1 h. In addition, other studies addressed the relationship between media content and behavioral problems. For example, Conners-Burrow et al. (2011) confirmed that viewing videos/movies that are rated as inappropriate for young children was associated with increased hyperactivity and aggression scores and low levels in social skills.
In contrast, Conners-Burrow et al. (2011) reported that the duration of media viewing was non-significantly associated with hyperactivity and aggression. Similarly, Zimmerman and Christakis (2007) stated that viewing any content at ages 4–5 years was not associated with attentional problems. Parkes et al. (2013) reported that playing electronic games was not associated with conduct problems, moreover, there were no associations between the type of media and emotional symptoms, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, or prosocial behavior.
The duration and content of media exposure among preschool children widely varied according to culture. Moreover, content considered appropriate for adults may be perceived by children negatively (Guellai et al., 2022). In general, preschool children may imitate what they are exposed to. Simple animations may influence children’s cognition, which is largely undeveloped, causing them to think and act impulsively or aggressively, similar to the characters they see. Moreover, complications may arise when children spend long periods of time in front of screens or are exposed to content that is inappropriate for their age. Finally, reducing the duration of media exposure can provide children with ample opportunities for interaction with peers, parents, and the community (Sanders et al., 2018).
In conclusion, the current study revealed that preschool children are generally exposed to media for an average of 105.84 min/day. Moreover, the duration of media exposure is significantly and positively correlated to Conners CBRS-48, hyperactivity index, learning problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problems, anxiety, and psychosomatic problems. Therefore, the study highlights the need to engage preschool children in concrete activities, familial and social interactions that may lessen negative media effects such as hyperactivity, learning problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problem, anxiety, and psychosomatic problems. As such, future studies should examine other data in detail to explore the risks of various forms of screen time and media content, including exposure to screen violence. Lastly, longitudinal studies are required to examine the long-term effect of media exposure on preschool children particularly after pandemic of COVID-19. Studies may assess the effect of pandemic on exposing preschool children to media besides its consequences.
What is already known on this topic:
• Increased exposure to media has been linked with behavioral problems, although previous findings are inconsistent.
• The majority of studies have focused on watching TV only and overlooked other types of media that have currently emerged and invaded children’s lives.
• Few studies have examined media exposure within specific age groups, such as preschool children.
What this study adds:
• The preschool children were exposed to media for an average of 105.84 min/day.
• The preschool children displayed moderate levels in all subscales of Conners CBRS-48.
• The duration of media exposure is significantly and positively correlated to Conners CBRS-48, hyperactivity index, learning problems, hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problem, and anxiety and psychosomatic problems.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Nursing, Mansoura University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.
Author contributions
MZ and MA planned the study. DA and MZ analyzed the literature. MZ, SH, and DA were major contributors in writing the results section. MZ, HE-G, and MA were major contributors in writing the discussion section. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the parents of the respondents for their cooperation. We are grateful to the directors of the 10 nurseries and the Directorate of Education in El-Mansoura.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Al-Behairy, A. (2011). Conner’s’ rating scales: Conner’s’ teacher rating scales and Conner’s’ parent rating scales (Arabic version). Cairo: Anglo-Egyptian Bookshop.
American Academy of Pediatrics (2001). American Academy of Pediatrics: children, adolescents, and television. Pediatrics 107, 423–426. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.2.423
Barr, R., and Linebarger, D. N. (2016). Media exposure during infancy and early childhood: The effects of content and context on learning and development. New York, NY: Springer International Publishing Available at: https://books.google.com.eg/books?id=QrOVDQAAQBAJ.
Blumberg, F. C., Deater-Deckard, K., Calvert, S. L., Flynn, R. M., Green, C. S., Arnold, D., et al. (2019). Digital games as a context for Children’s cognitive development: research recommendations and policy considerations. Soc. Policy Rep. 32, 1–33. doi: 10.1002/sop2.3
Bozzola, E., Spina, G., Ruggiero, M., Memo, L., Agostiniani, R., Bozzola, M., et al. (2018). Media devices in pre-school children. Ital. J. Pediatr. 44:69. doi: 10.1186/s13052-018-0508-7
Chassiakos, Y. L. R., Radesky, J., Christakis, D., Moreno, M. A., Cross, C., Council on Communications and Media, et al. (2016). Children and adolescents and digital media. Pediatrics 138:e20162593. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2593
Christakis, D. A., Ramirez, J. S. B., Ferguson, S. M., Ravinder, S., and Ramirez, J. M. (2018). How early media exposure may affect cognitive function: a review of results from observations in humans and experiments in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 115, 9851–9858. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1711548115
Conners-Burrow, N. A., Mckelvey, L. M., and Fussell, J. J. (2011). Social outcomes associated with media viewing habits of low-income preschool children. Early Educ. Dev. 22, 256–273. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2011.550844
Coyne, S. M., Shawcroft, J., Gale, M., Gentile, D. A., Etherington, J. T., Holmgren, H., et al. (2021). Tantrums, toddlers and technology: temperament, media emotion regulation, and problematic media use in early childhood. Comput. Hum. Behav. 120:106762:106762. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106762
Delhove, M., and Greitemeyer, T. (2021). Violent media use and aggression: two longitudinal network studies. J. Soc. Psychol. 161, 697–713. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2021.1896465
Domoff, S. E., Borgen, A. L., and Radesky, J. S. (2020). Interactional theory of childhood problematic media use. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Technol. 2, 343–353. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.217
Domoff, S. E., Harrison, K., Gearhardt, A. N., Gentile, D. A., Lumeng, J. C., and Miller, A. L. (2019). Development and validation of the problematic media use measure: a parent report measure of screen media ‘addiction’ in children. Psychol. Pop Media Cult. 8, 2–11. doi: 10.1037/ppm0000163
Dynia, J. M., Dore, R. A., Bates, R. A., and Justice, L. M. (2021). Media exposure and language for toddlers from low-income homes. Infant Behav. Dev. 63:101542. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101542
Eales, L., Gillespie, S., Alstat, R. A., Ferguson, G. M., and Carlson, S. M. (2021). Children’s screen and problematic media use in the United States before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Child Dev. 92, e866–e882. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13652
Garfin, D. R., Holman, E. A., Fischhoff, B., Wong-Parodi, G., and Silver, R. C. (2022). Media exposure, risk perceptions, and fear: Americans’ behavioral responses to the Ebola public health crisis. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 77:103059. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2022.103059
Gilmore, J. H., Knickmeyer, R. C., and Gao, W. (2018). Imaging structural and functional brain development in early childhood. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 123–137. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2018.1
Goode, J. A., Fomby, P., Mollborn, S., and Limburg, A. (2019). Children’s technology time in two US cohorts. Child Indic. Res. 13, 1–26.
Guellai, B., Somogyi, E., Esseily, R., and Chopin, A. (2022). Effects of screen exposure on young children’s cognitive development: a review. Front. Psychol. 13, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.923370
Gueron-Sela, N., Shalev, I., Gordon-Hacker, A., Egotubov, A., and Barr, R. (2023). Screen media exposure and behavioral adjustment in early childhood during and after COVID-19 home lockdown periods. Comput. Hum. Behav. 140:107572. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107572
Hammad, M. A., and Alqarni, T. M. (2021). Psychosocial effects of social media on the Saudi society during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One 16:e0248811. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248811
Hinkley, T., Verbestel, V., Ahrens, W., Lissner, L., Molnár, D., and Moreno, L. A. Idefics Consortium. (2014). Early childhood electronic media use as a predictor of poorer well-being: a prospective cohort study. JAMA pediatrics 168, 485–492.
Khalil, A. H., Zayed, A. M., Amer, A., and Baz, H. (2022). Relation between exposure to different multimedia programs and presentation of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Egypt. J. Otolaryngology 38:5. doi: 10.1186/s43163-021-00197-4
Kort-Butler, L. A., and Habecker, P. (2018). Framing and cultivating the story of crime: the effects of media use, victimization, and social networks on attitudes about crime. Crim. Justice Rev. 43, 127–146. doi: 10.1177/0734016817710696
Kovačič, A., and Rek, M. (2016). Factors increasing media exposure of preschool children. New Educ. Rev. 45, 249–258. doi: 10.15804/tner.2016.45.3.20
Mistry, K. B., Minkovitz, C. S., Strobino, D. M., and Borzekowski, D. L. G. (2007). Children’s television exposure and behavioral and social outcomes at 5.5 years: does timing of exposure matter? Pediatrics 120, 762–769. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3573
Monteiro, R., Rocha, N. B., and Fernandes, S. (2021). Are emotional and behavioral problems of infants and children aged younger than 7 years related to screen time exposure during the coronavirus disease 2019 confinement? An exploratory study in Portugal. Front. Psychol. 12:590279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.590279
Mundy, L. K., Canterford, L., Olds, T., Allen, N. B., and Patton, G. C. (2017). The association between electronic media and emotional and behavioral problems in late childhood. Acad. Pediatr. 17, 620–624. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2016.12.014
Munzer, T. G., Miller, A. L., Peterson, K. E., Brophy-Herb, H. E., Horodynski, M. A., Contreras, D., et al. (2018). Media exposure in low-income preschool-aged children is associated with multiple measures of self-regulatory behavior. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 39, 303–309. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000560
Nikkelen, S. W. C., Valkenburg, P. M., Huizinga, M., and Bushman, B. J. (2014). Media use and ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 50, 2228–2241. doi: 10.1037/a0037318
Pagani, L. S., Fitzpatrick, C., and Barnett, T. A. (2013). Early childhood television viewing and kindergarten entry readiness. Pediatr. Res. 74, 350–355. doi: 10.1038/pr.2013.105
Parkes, A., Sweeting, H., Wight, D., and Henderson, M. (2013). Do television and electronic games predict children’s psychosocial adjustment? Longitudinal research using the UK millennium cohort study. Arch. Dis. Child. 98, 341–348. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2011-301508
Radesky, J. S., Silverstein, M., Zuckerman, B., and Christakis, D. A. (2014). Infant self-regulation and early childhood media exposure. Pediatrics 133, e1172–e1178. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2367
Robinson, T. N., Banda, J. A., Hale, L., Lu, A. S., Fleming-Milici, F., Calvert, S. L., et al. (2017). Screen media exposure and obesity in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 140, S97–S101. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758K
Ruangdaraganon, N., Chuthapisith, J., Mo-suwan, L., Kriweradechachai, S., Udomsubpayakul, U., and Choprapawon, C. (2009). Television viewing in Thai infants and toddlers: impacts to language development and parental perceptions. BMC Pediatr. 9:34. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-9-34
Sanders, W., Parent, J., and Forehand, R. (2018). Parenting to reduce child screen time: a feasibility pilot study. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatri. 39, 46–54. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000501
Scott, H., and Woods, H. C. (2019). Understanding links between social media use, sleep and mental health: recent progress and current challenges. Current Sleep Med. Rep. 5, 141–149. doi: 10.1007/s40675-019-00148-9
Sharma, A., Minh Duc, N. T., Luu Lam Thang, T., Nam, N. H., Ng, S. J., Abbas, K. S., et al. (2021). A consensus-based checklist for reporting of survey studies (CROSS). J. Gen. Intern. Med. 36, 3179–3187. doi: 10.1007/s11606-021-06737-1
Taylor, G., Monaghan, P., and Westermann, G. (2018). Investigating the association between children’s screen media exposure and vocabulary size in the UK. J. Child. Media 12, 51–65. doi: 10.1080/17482798.2017.1365737
Tomopoulos, S., Valdez, P. T., Dreyer, B. P., Fierman, A. H., Berkule, S. B., Kuhn, M., et al. (2007). Is exposure to media intended for preschool children associated with less parent-child shared reading aloud and teaching activities? Ambul. Pediatr. 7, 18–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2006.10.005
Uzundağ, B. A., Altundal, M. N., and Keşşafoğlu, D. (2022). Screen Media Exposure in Early Childhood and Its Relation to Children’s Self-Regulation. Human Behav. Emerging Technol.
Vandewater, E. A., and Lee, S.-J. (2009). Measuring children’s media use in the digital age: issues and challenges. Am. Behav. Sci. 52, 1152–1176. doi: 10.1177/0002764209331539
Waters, N. E., Domoff, S. E., and Tang, S. (2016). “Parenting of preschool children’s media use in the home,” in Socializing children through language. eds. P. Davis-Kean and S. Tang (Cambridge, MA: Elsevier, Inc.), 111–146.
WHO (2019). Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep, vol. 4. Geneva: World Health Organization Available at: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/325147/WHO-NMH-PND-2019.4-eng.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y%0Ahttp://www.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664%0A.
Xie, G., Deng, Q., Cao, J., and Chang, Q. (2020). Digital screen time and its effect on preschoolers’ behavior in China: results from a cross-sectional study. Ital. J. Pediatr. 46:9. doi: 10.1186/s13052-020-0776-x
Zeanah, C. H., Gunnar, M. R., McCall, R. B., Kreppner, J. M., and Fox, N. A. (2011). Sensitive periods. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 76, 147–162. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00631.x
Keywords: media exposure, behavioral problems, preschool children, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, learning disabilities, anxiety
Citation: Zoromba MA, Abdelgawad D, Hashem S, El-Gazar H and Abd El Aziz MA (2023) Association between media exposure and behavioral problems among preschool children. Front. Psychol. 14:1080550. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1080550
Edited by:
Giorgia Bussu, Uppsala University, SwedenReviewed by:
Regina Ahn, University of Miami, United StatesEmma L. Axelsson, The University of Newcastle, Australia
Benedetta Ragni, Libera Università Maria SS. Assunta, Italy
Copyright © 2023 Zoromba MA, Abdelgawad D, Hashem S, El-Gazar H and Abd El Aziz MA. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Mohamed A. Zoromba, zromba2010@mans.edu.eg