Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 February 2023
Sec. Educational Psychology

The mediating effect of geospatial thinking on the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography

  • 1College of Geography and Environmental Science, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, Zhejiang, China
  • 2School of Geography and Environment, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
  • 3College of Education and Human Development, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, Zhejiang, China

Purpose: Family environment has the major impact on children’s academic development. The aim of this study was to research the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography. Further, geospatial thinking, as a form of spatial thinking focusing on the scale of the geographical environment, is closely related to family environment and academic achievement in geography. Thus, the study was more specifically to apply a mediation model to explore the potential mediating role of geospatial thinking.

Methods: A total of 1,037 upper-secondary-school students in Western China were surveyed using t the Family Capital Questionnaire and the Geospatial Thinking Test Questionnaire. SPSS (version 26.0) was used for descriptive statistical analysis and correlation analysis. The PROCESS plug-in (version 4.0) was used to test the mediating effect of geospatial thinking.

Results: (1) The correlation analysis showed that family capital has a positive effect on academic achievement in geography and is related to geospatial thinking. Moreover, geospatial thinking exerts a positive effect on academic achievement in geography. (2) The results of mediation analysis indicated that geospatial thinking plays mediating and buffering roles in the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography after controlling for family residence and gender. The direct and indirect effects accounted for 75.32% and 24.68% of the total effect, respectively.

Conclusion: The results indicated that family capital not only affected academic achievement in geography directly but also indirectly through geospatial thinking. This finding provides some ideas for the development of geography education, which suggests that geography educators need to pay more attention to the influence of the family environment on students’ geography learning in curriculum design and teaching processes. Also, the mediating role of geospatial thinking further uncovers the mechanisms underlying the academic achievement in geography. Therefore, it is necessary to focus on both students’ family capital and geospatial thinking in the process of geography learning, and carry out more geospatial thinking training to improve academic achievement in geography.

1. Introduction

Geography education plays a key role in the Framework for 21st Century Learning (Trilling and Fadel, 2009), refer to the development of 21st Century Skills (Sugiyanto et al., 2018). Geography is increasingly recognized as a core subject, because of its relevance to students’ critical thinking and global awareness (Lambert and Jones, 2017). Academic achievement in geography has also received significant global attention as a part of the academic achievement of students in the curriculum. The National Assessment of Education Progress (NAEP) of the United States has repeatedly conducted geography assessments to evaluate trends in students’ academic achievement in geography (Solem et al., 2021), and increased academic achievement in geography helps develop students’ critical, creative thinking (Sugiyanto et al., 2021). In the GeoCapabilities project, sponsored by the European Union’s Comenius Program, academic achievement in geography is linked to imagination and reasoning skills (Lambert and Jones, 2017). In addition, geography education has been linked to the understanding of social and environmental issues, and a number of studies have shown that the development of academic achievement in geography helps students to understand global issues and cultivate human-environment thinking (Israel, 2012; Larsen et al., 2022).

However, there are a number of factors affecting students’ academic achievement in geography, which can be broadly classified into two categories: internal factors (individual student factors) and external factors (e.g., family, school, and social aspects). Research has shown that gender, attitudes to learning and health behaviors can affect academic achievement in geography (Escolano-Pérez and Bestué, 2021). At the same time, individual geospatial thinking is also considered to be an important element in the achievement of the geography profession (Huynh and Sharpe, 2013). Spatial thinking refers to the way of thinking about visualizing and solving problems in space (Nielsen et al., 2011). Geospatial thinking is specialized spatial thinking and has the characteristics of spatial thinking (Verma, 2014). Research found that students with strong geospatial thinking perform better in understanding geographic concepts and are more successful in their geography learning (Klonari and Likouri, 2015; Xie et al., 2022). In addition, the external environment in which students live has also been linked by researchers to academic achievement of geography. It has been found that classroom climate, teachers’ quality, and family educational expectations all influence students’ academic achievement in geography (An et al., 2019; Ozdemir and Ozturk, 2022).

Family is a necessary environment in children’s development and is considered to be an essential ingredient in the development of spatial thinking (Clingan-Siverly et al., 2021). For example, Potter et al. (2013) found that cultural capital in the family (e.g., parents’ educational expectations) influences children’s spatial thinking and the development of related neural networks. As a part of spatial thinking, geospatial thinking is characterized by spatial thinking (Huynh and Sharpe, 2013) and can also be influenced by family (Zhang et al., 2022). Also, family is considered to be a critical factor influencing the academic achievement in geography (An et al., 2019). For example, Solem et al. (2021) identified that the amount of books collected in the home and the education level of the parents predicted students’ geographic achievement. Bravo Sanzana et al. (2017) discovered that family cultural capital has an impact on children’s achievement acquisition in geography. However, existing research does not link family capital, which is defined as the combination of the family economic, cultural, and social capital, to geospatial thinking and academic achievement in geography.

Therefore, in order to clarify the relationship between family capital, geospatial thinking and academic achievement in geography, this study explores the connection between family capital and academic achievement in geography, along with the mediating role of geospatial thinking between the two. Also, the effects of gender and place of residence on academic achievement were controlled in the analysis. In the next section, the definitions of the three variables, the relevant theories, the influencing factors and the links between the variables are presented.

2. Theoretical basis and hypothesis

2.1. Family capital

Family capital is the sum of the resources held by the family, including the economic status, education level, occupation and so on. According to Bourdieu, capital includes economic, cultural and social capital (Bourdieu and Richardson, 1986). Families in different social classes have different capital characteristics that determine the academic achievement of their children (Bourdieu, 1973). Similarly, Coleman describes the main forms of family capital in financial, human and social terms (Coleman, 1990). Specifically, financial capital is the position of wealth in the family (e.g., family income, etc.) that helps to provide resources and opportunities for children’s education. The human capital of the family, including the educational level and cognitive status of the parents, helps to provide a cognitive environment conducive to the intellectual and thinking development of the offspring. Social capital refers to resources that can contribute to the development of children, where the relationship between children and parents is considered to be part of the family’s social capital and this relationship affects children’s growth (Coleman, 1988). In essence, the human, financial and social capital of parents continues to influence the academic status and future achievements of their children through the cultural transmission of habitus, economic resources and interpersonal relationships. Therefore, all three types of capital are vital to the family process over several generations.

Family capital is considered to be an important factor in the health, thinking and educational status of individuals (Weinberg et al., 2019; Wang and Huang, 2021). In terms of personal health, researchers have found that children with poorer family capital exhibit physical disorders, sleep problems (Bøe et al., 2012)and poorer health, and are also more likely to suffer from psychological disorders such as depression (Zhou, 2018). In terms of thinking skills, research has identified that children’s thinking and ability development is influenced by parent–child interactions (Biro et al., 2009; Yuan and Ngai, 2019). At the same time, children with rich family capital have higher levels of creativity, innovation and cognitive ability (Liu et al., 2020; Xu and Pang, 2020). In terms of educational status, Coleman notes that parents’ educational attainment, family book collection, etc., affect their children’s educational achievement (Coleman, 1968). Children with higher family social capital tend to achieve higher levels of educational attainment (Coleman, 1988). A possible explanation for this is that advantaged families use direct resources and indirect cultural transmission to turn family capital advantages into educational opportunity advantages, which influences individual academic achievement (Green et al., 2015). Conversely, children from less privileged families have less access to education, which is detrimental to cognitive development (Brooks-Gunn and Duncan, 1997; Von Stumm et al., 2022).

According to the existing literature, most evaluations of family capital similarly identify family income and parental education as key factors (Hanson and Chen, 2007; Xu and Pang, 2020). For example, De Pernillo et al. (2014) used the highest level of parental education, family income, etc., as a basis for judging family capital. The major international education assessment project (PISA) also uses parental education, income and material resources as indicators of family background (Wang and Huang, 2021). This study used the Family Capital survey questions in the PISA 2018 student questionnaire to collect information on the family background such as parents’ education level, occupation, family ownership (e.g., desk, dictionary, etc.) and family book collection.

2.2. Academic achievement in geography

The concept of academic achievement has a broad and a narrow meaning. In a broad sense, academic achievement includes students’ performance in terms of knowledge and skills (Mega et al., 2014). For example, countries such as the United States and Australia have conducted competency assessments to reflect students’ proficiency levels (Collie et al., 2015; Jones and Mueller, 2017). Academic achievement in a narrow sense refers to a students’ examination results (Kristjnsson et al., 2009). A large number of studies have used exam or test scores as a measure of academic achievement (Lüftenegger et al., 2016). The definition adopted in this study is the narrow one, using students’ performance on a geography exam as a criterion for determining academic achievement in geography.

There are series of factors that influence academic achievement in geography, which can be divided into internal and external factors. Internal factors include, for example, individuals’ gender, intelligence, attitude toward learning, etc. (Filgona and Sababa, 2017; Marciano and Camerini, 2021). Filgona and Sababa’s study (2017) indicated that girls performed better than boys in terms of geography. Gil-Espinosa et al. (2019) noted that academic achievement in geography was significantly, although weakly correlated with students’ intelligence. Similar studies have shown that students’ executive functioning, physical activity and gender have a positive impact on academic achievement in geography (Escolano-Pérez and Bestué, 2021). Similarly, students with a positive attitude to learning tend to do better in academic achievement in geography (Díaz-Serrano and Martínez, 2016). External factors refer to family, school, society, etc. Parental educational expectation is considered to be a factor influencing academic achievement in geography (Bravo Sanzana et al., 2017). Besides, there was a link between students’ academic achievement in geography, teacher-student relationships, educational philosophy and the qualifications of geography teachers (Filgona and Sakiyo, 2020; Ho, 2021). Ozdemir and Ozturk (2022) found that students performed better in geography and learning in VR settings. It has also found that social media like Facebook as a teaching tool can improve academic achievement in geography (Al Zboon et al., 2018).

Nevertheless, fewer studies have focused directly on the relationship between academic achievement in geography and family capital, although there is evidence that family capital affects students’ learning status in geography. Research has shown that students with better family capital have more opportunities to go on trips and expeditions (Chiu and Chow, 2015), and such field activities are considered to be valuable geography learning experiences that help to develop interest and geography skills (Rydant et al., 2010; Krakowka, 2012). Interestingly, the study of geography is a lifelong learning process. The more time you have been exposed to geography, the more likely you are to achieve a high level of academic achievement in geography (Downs, 2014). In contrast, children with advantaged family capital are more likely to be exposed to learning tools such as maps and globes both before and during the trip, which increases their exposure to geography and strengthens their understanding of geography knowledge (Bein et al., 2009).

Combined with the overview of family capital, we derive the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1: Family capital positively affects academic achievement in geography.

2.3. Geospatial thinking

Thinking is regarded as an advanced stage of human cognition and process (Zhang, 2002) and is closely related to daily life and learning (Liu et al., 2021). Spatial thinking, as a part of thinking, refers to a combination of an individual’s cognition, skills and performance (Lee and Bednarz, 2012) and emphasizes abstract comparison and analysis of things from a spatial perspective (Hespanha et al., 2009). Learning to Think Spatially, published by the National Academy of Sciences, suggests that spatial thinking is an organic combination of the nature of space, methods of representing spatial information and the process of spatial reasoning (National Research Council, 2006). Nielsen et al. (2011) defined it as a way of thinking about visualizing and solving problems spatially, which in this case includes all space from the microscopic to the planetary scale. Spatial thinking plays an indispensable role in life, scientific research and education. The study found that most people rely on spatial thinking to choose their travel routes and find what they need in shops (Hespanha et al., 2009). At the same time, spatial thinking help develop key competencies (e.g., the ability to think through solutions, make decisions) and performance in subjects such as science and astronomy (Favier and van der Schee, 2014; Cole et al., 2018).

Geospatial thinking has been regarded as a specialized form of spatial thinking (National Research Council, 2006). Scholars consider it as a form of spatial thinking focusing on the scale of the earth, landscape and environment (Bodzin et al., 2014). It is different from spatial thinking, covering all spatial scales, while geospatial thinking is mainly applied in analyzing problems at the earth scale and requires the use of geographical knowledge and technology (Huynh and Sharpe, 2013; Xie et al., 2021). Bednarz (2011)defined geospatial thinking as the knowledge, skills and thinking habits of solving problems by using geographical information (such as maps, etc.) and reasoning process in a specific environment. However, there is a lack of effective ways to assess it (Huynh and Sharpe, 2013). Initially, psychologists developed spatial thinking test questions to support the assessment of geospatial thinking (Kail et al., 1979). However, the tests developed by psychologists are not fully applicable to the evaluation of geospatial thinking (Lee and Bednarz, 2009). In 2003, Lee and Bednarz (2009) designed a test to measure geospatial thinking. They adapted and revised it to update the Geospatial Thinking Test (STAT) instrument in recent years (Lee and Bednarz, 2012). Since then, the test has been widely used (Collins, 2018). Consequently, the geospatial test questionnaire used in this study draws on the Spatial Thinking Aptitude Test (STAT) instrument developed by Lee and Bednarz (2009).

The factors that influence geospatial thinking are more complex. First, individual differences in intelligence, gender and learning ability can affect the level of geospatial thinking (Aliman et al., 2019; Xie et al., 2021). Some studies have shown that males achieve higher scores on geospatial tests than females (Shin et al., 2016). There is also evidence that no significant differences were revealed (Zhang et al., 2022). Collins (2018) stated that students’ academic achievement in geography is related to their level of geospatial thinking. Second, research in the field of brain science and neuroscience have identified the brain has special structures for processing spatial information, which consists of numerous neural systems (Ivanitskii et al., 2015; Demir-Lira et al., 2016), supporting the development of geospatial thinking. Third, environmental factors are also believed to be important in influencing geospatial thinking. One study found that geography textbooks contain review questions about geospatial thinking (Scholz et al., 2014), which can help to improve geospatial thinking. At the same time, the use of paper and digital teaching media is helpful to cultivate students’ geospatial thinking (Collins, 2018). Similarly, the use of web maps in the teaching and learning environment is considered to be an effective way to enhance students’ spatial thinking in geography (Manson et al., 2014).

There are fewer existing studies that focus on the relationship between family capital and geospatial thinking, but it is proved that families have an impact on students’ thinking skills and spatial performance. Researchers have found that the family environment is crucial for children’s development, fostering their neural networks, which contribute to the development of learning and academic skills (Potter et al., 2013; Clingan-Siverly et al., 2021). In addition, there are richer resources in advantaged families to help nurture children’s knowledge base and promote the development of thinking ability (Uhlenberg and Geiken, 2021). What’s more, well educated parents are willing to spend time on constructive activities with their children and use maps, spatial language, etc., in their interactions to promote children’s thinking development (Borriello and Liben, 2018). Based on this, the following hypothesis is proposed:

Hypothesis 2: Family capital positively affects geospatial thinking.

Geography is a subject related to space (IGU CGE, 2019) and geospatial thinking is a crucial thinking skill for learning it. Nazareth et al. (2019) argued that geospatial thinking occupies an imperative place in the professional field of geography. It is worth noting that spatial thinking underpins the practice and theory of geography (Huynh and Sharpe, 2013; Jo and Bednarz, 2014) and contributes to students’ performance in science, astronomy, etc. (Cole et al., 2018). As a result, scholars have worked to develop educators’ abilities to use geospatial technology, defined as a superset of technologies, such as GIS, RS, etc. (Metoyer and Bednarz, 2017). Educators are more willing to teach using spatial technology to promote spatial thinking and develop students’ geospatial thinking (Baker et al., 2015). For example, Carbonell-Carrera and Hess-Medler (2019) used GIS to teach geography in the classroom and found that students’ geospatial thinking was improved. Collins (2018) also found that teaching geography incorporating Google Earth software promoted students’ geospatial thinking. Similar studies have shown that geospatial thinking helps students understand geographical data and influences the acquisition of geospatial knowledge (Perugini and Bodzin, 2020). And beyond that, individuals with strong geospatial thinking tend to succeed in the study (Carbonell-Carrera et al., 2020). In general, students’ geospatial thinking can affect their academic achievement in geography (Aliman et al., 2019). Therefore, we contend as follows:

Hypothesis 3: Geospatial thinking has a positive predictive effect on academic achievement in geography.

Based on the literature and the three hypotheses above, we further propose the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 4: Geospatial thinking acts as a mediator between family capital and academic achievement in geography.

Figure 1 shows a diagram of the mediation model proposed in the four hypotheses that depicts the relationships between the independent, mediator, and dependent variables and two covariates.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The relationships examined in the study.

3. Materials and methods

3.1. Participants and procedures

Public secondary school students in a region of western China, between the ages of 16 and 18, were selected as participants in this study. The survey was conducted by distributing a paper questionnaire completed between 10 and 30 November 2021. First, before completing the questionnaire, the researcher explained the study and the details of the questionnaire to the participating students. Second, with the consent of parents, class teachers and the students themselves, the researcher distributed paper questionnaires to the students and asked them to fill them out truthfully. Finally, we collected the questionnaires and input the data.

After data collection was completed, the researchers tested the validity of the questionnaire and the actual number of valid questionnaires was 1,037. The statistical results are presented in Table 1. Among the interviewees, in terms of gender, 260 (25.0723%) were male and 777 (74.9277%) were female. In terms of residence, 621 (59.8843%) were urban, and 416 (40.1157%) were suburban. In addition, before the research design was finalized, the researchers conducted focal interviews with students about the relationship between family capital, geospatial thinking and academic achievement in geography. Many participants indicated that students with better family capital also had higher academic achievement in geography.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the three variables.

3.2. Materials

The questionnaire used in this study consists of two parts and contains four items: demographic information, academic achievement in geography information, the Family Capital Questionnaire and the Geospatial Thinking Test Questionnaire. In the first part, demographic information was collected, including the gender and residence of the respondents. Second, with the consent of teachers, parents and students, the geographical learning achievements of the students were collected as a representative of academic achievement in geography. As this study was conducted at different school, the results of students’ geography examination were used as a criterion to judge their academic achievement in geography. The scores for each grade of students’ academic performance in geography were therefore divided into six levels: 90 and above (Level 6), 80–89 (Level 5), 70–79 (Level 4), 60–69 (Level 3), 40–59 (Level 2) and below 40 (Level 1). Besides, the researchers contacted geography teachers at the surveyed schools to ensure that the questionnaire was administered 1 week after the midterm exam to guarantee that participants could accurately recall their geography exam results. In addition, the data was rigorously screened by the researchers to eliminate questionable samples and to assure the validity of the sample data.

The second part includes the Family Capital Questionnaire and the Geospatial Thinking Test Questionnaire. The questionnaires used in this study were taken from the English version and were therefore back-translated to improve the quality of the translation (Brislin, 1970). Primarily, the first researcher translated the English questionnaire into Chinese. Then, the second researcher translated them into English. Finally, the third researcher compared the original (English), translated (Chinese) and back-translated versions (English) of the questionnaire to ensure consistency in the meaning expressed in the original English and the translated version. In addition, the questionnaire was adapted and optimized by the researchers before the final questionnaire was accomplished.

3.3. Family capital questionnaire

The Family Capital Questionnaire has been modified based on the Family Background Survey items from the PISA 2018 Student Questionnaire.1 The final questionnaire had six questions, including parental education, parental occupation and family ownership. First, the parents’ education represents the family cultural capital, ranging from 1 (primary school) to 7 (PhD). Second, the parents’ occupation represents the family’s social capital, ranging from 1 (government/authority cadre/civil servant) to 12 (other inconveniently classified occupations). Family ownership points reflect the family economic capital, with points awarded for owning a certain number of items, and no points awarded for not owning items. Then, the two variables were standardized according to existing studies. Second, standardized z-scores were included in the factor analysis (Pokropek et al., 2017). Finally, the total score was used as an indicator of family capital, with higher scores predicting higher levels of family capital.

3.4. Geospatial thinking test questionnaire

The Geospatial Thinking Test questionnaire draws on the Spatial Thinking Aptitude Test (STAT) instrument developed by Bednarz and Lee (2019)2. The questionnaire has 16 questions on directional discrimination, map reading and using, such as: “The closest option to the landform you see is,” “The one that fits the logical operation of the map is,” etc. Students’ ability of geospatial thinking is determined using a scoring system (1 mark for a correct answer, no mark for an incorrect answer) which means that score are positively correlated with geospatial thinking. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was 0.695, indicating that there is a good correlation between the items of the scale (De Vaus, 2002).

3.5. Data analysis

This study used SPSS 26.0 software and PROCESS 4.0 plug-in to analyze the data. First, a Harman one-way test was used to test for common method bias before processing the data to ensure the validity of the data analysis (Podsakoff et al., 2003). The results showed that a total of seven factors had eigenvalues greater than 1, with the first factor accounting for only 18.217%, much less than the 40% threshold, so the common method bias problem in this study was small(Li et al., 2020). Second, following the reliability and validity analysis, the mean and standard deviation of the data were calculated using SPSS software to test for trends in the concentration and dispersion of the study data. Then, Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to test the relationship between family capital, academic achievement in geography and geospatial thinking. Finally, a mediation analysis using the PROCESS 4.0 plug-in in SPSS was performed to explore the mediating role of geospatial thinking and to test the four hypotheses of this study.

4. Results

4.1. Descriptive statistics and correlations analyses

The results of the descriptive analysis of family capital, academic achievement in geography and geospatial thinking are shown in Table 1. There is much wider variation in geospatial thinking scores than in both family capital values and academic achievement in geography.

Next, the variables were analyzed for correlation by calculating Pearson correlation coefficients. The results showed (see Table 2) that there was a positive correlation between the three variables. First, there was a significant positive relationship between upper-secondary-school students’ family capital and their academic achievement in geography (r = 0.3860, p < 0.001). Second, there was a significant positive correlation between family capital and geospatial thinking (r = 0.3640, p < 0.001). In addition, there was a significant positive correlation between geospatial thinking and academic achievement in geography (r = 0.3580, p < 0.001).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson’s r for the three variables.

4.2. Mediation analysis

The final hypothesis of this study was to test the mediating role of geospatial thinking. Using the PROCESS plug-in in SPSS (version 4.0), a mediation analysis was conducted with family capital as the independent variable, academic achievement in geography as the dependent variable and geospatial thinking as the mediating variable(Model 4). Furthermore, based on the literature review, gender and household residence were used as control variables in this study, both of which were transformed into dummy variables before being entered into the mediation model.

The results showed (see Table 3) that family capital had a significant positive predictive effect on academic achievement in geography (β = 0.2350, t = 11.4040, p < 0.001), and the prediction remained significant even with the addition of geospatial thinking variable (β = 0.1170, t = 8.4150, p < 0.001). Moreover, family capital was a significant positive predictor of geospatial thinking (β = 0.5610, t = 11.0080, p < 0.001). There was also a significant positive predictive effect of geospatial thinking on academic achievement in geography (β = 0.1030, t = 8.4390, p < 0.001). In addition, both the direct effect of family capital on academic achievement in geography and the mediating effect of geospatial thinking had bootstrap confidence intervals(95%), with no zero between their lower and upper limits (see Table 4). It means that, after controlling for gender and household residence variables, family capital can predict academic achievement in geography directly, and through geospatial thinking indirectly. The direct effect (0.1770) and the indirect one (0.0580) accounted for 75.3191% and 24.6809% of the total effect, respectively.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of mediation analysis for the observed variables.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Total effect, direct effect, and indirect effect among the variables.

As can be seen from Table 3, when exploring the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography, gender has an impact on academic achievement in geography (β = −0.1990, t = −2.7190, p < 0.01). At the same time, even when geospatial thinking was included in the model, gender still significantly influenced academic achievement in geography (β = −0.1950, t = −2.7580, p < 0.01; Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The mediation model showing relationships between Family Capital and Academic Achievement in Geography and the mediating role of Geospatial Thinking.

5. Discussion

5.1. Discussion of the results

In this study, we obtained a mediation model that illustrates the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography and the mediating role of geospatial thinking. At the same time, the results of the survey are consistent with the hypotheses of this study and the findings of previous studies.

First, our findings are consistent with Hypothesis 1 and other similar studies. In this study, family capital and academic achievement in geography were positively correlated. This result suggests that good family capital contributes to academic achievement in geography (Bravo Sanzana et al., 2017) and these parents are more likely to focus on providing their children with rich learning opportunities outside the classroom (Conger et al., 2021). For example, Solem et al. (2021) found that academic achievement in geography increased with parental education and the amount of family books, and that the effect size of family books was consistently larger than that of parental education. A possible explanation for this is that children can gain knowledge from the family book collection (Evans et al., 2010), which contributes to higher academic achievement in geography. Meanwhile, children with poorer family capital tend to play closer to home (Ziviani et al., 2008), or more likely to become addicted to the internet or smartphone (Zhang et al., 2018), reducing the effectiveness of geography learning. While children with better family capital are more likely to engage in outdoor activities with nature, which is conducive to their spatial awareness and academic achievement in geographic (Brookfield, 2022; Mason et al., 2022; Pastor et al., 2022). Similar research has shown that higher levels of family capital are associated with academic achievement in geography (Zhang et al., 2022). In addition, the educational level of parents and the socioeconomic status of the family also significantly predict academic achievement in geography (An et al., 2019). Related studies have found that students’ chances of academic success increase when their parents have high levels of literacy (Cheng and Kaplowitz, 2016). Highly educated parents have a great potential to provide their children with a social environment that is beneficial to learning (Taljūnaitė, 2020). At the same time, they understand how to be successful in school (Roosa et al., 2012), which provides an advantage for children to achieve high levels of academic achievement in geography.

Second, the findings are consistent with Hypothesis 2 and other studies indicating that family capital plays a positive predictive role in geospatial thinking. This finding suggests that higher levels of family capital are more conducive to the development of individual thinking (Goudeau et al., 2017), particularly geospatial thinking (Zhang et al., 2022). Studies have shown that there are significant differences in children’s spatial performance even before they enter formal school, and such differences are closely related to parental spatial language and spatial gestures (Clingan-Siverly et al., 2021). It should be noticed that well educated parents use more spatial relational vocabulary and their children will show the greater capability of spatial thinking (Casasola et al., 2020). This is consistent with other similar studies which showed that students from higher income areas had better spatial performance than students from lower-income areas (Casey et al., 2011). Urban students have better educational backgrounds and opportunities, and their levels of geospatial thinking tests are better than rural students (Tomaszewski et al., 2015). Similarly, students from higher socioeconomic status families performed remarkably better in spatial terms than those from lower socioeconomic status families (Carr et al., 2018). The likely explanation is that families with higher economic and literacy levels have a tendency to provide their children with a rich resources and superior learning conditions (Jin et al., 2017) including books, maps, etc. (Bein et al., 2009), to foster their spatial thinking are developed (Zhang et al., 2019).

Third, the findings validated Hypothesis 3 and other relevant studies, showing that geospatial thinking has a positive impact on academic achievement in geography. Similar studies have shown that there are significant differences in academic achievement in geography between students with different spatial thinking, and students with higher spatial thinking were found to have higher academic achievement in geography (Aliman et al., 2019). Meanwhile, students with strong geospatial thinking perform better in geography (Lee and Bednarz, 2009; Klonari and Likouri, 2015; Gold et al., 2018). A possible explanation for this is that geospatial thinking helps to understand and apply geographical knowledge, geographical data (Perugini and Bodzin, 2020). Also, Carbonell-Carrera et al. (2020) demonstrated that good geospatial thinking facilitates students to apply geographic knowledge in solving geographic problems, which is beneficial to their academic achievement in geography (Huynh and Sharpe, 2013). As previously noted, geospatial thinking is considered an important aspect of geography education (Havelková and Hanus, 2021), and has a positive impact on geography learning (Nielsen et al., 2011). As a result, teachers are more willing to use geospatial technology in the geography classroom to strengthen the training of students’ geospatial thinking and further enhance their academic achievement in geography (Hammond et al., 2018).

Fourth, the findings are consistent with Hypothesis 4. We found that geospatial thinking can partially and positively mediate the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography. This suggested that family capital not only directly influences academic achievement in geographic, but also influences it indirectly by geospatial thinking (Demetriou et al., 2020), which is in line with similar research findings. Family capital has an impact on children’s cognitive, spatial and psychological development in many ways, and children with low family income have difficulties with language, cognitive development and spatial characteristics (Ip et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2019). Similar studies have shown that children at the age of three are already map readers (Blaut et al., 1970), and this is inextricably linked to the influence of the home environment (Uhlenberg and Geiken, 2021). Meanwhile, children’s cognitive and spatial thinking can influence academic achievement in geography (Wang et al., 2013). Research showed that family socioeconomic environment affects children’s ability to apply basic cognitive skills such as spatial reasoning to academic performance (Casey et al., 2011). Compared with children from wealthier families, less privileged children have less access to spatially stimulating objects and resources (e.g., blocks, puzzles, maps, etc.) (Dearing and Taylor, 2007). These items are effective in enhancing individuals’ geospatial thinking (Collins, 2018), so disadvantaged family capital is detrimental to the acquisition of academic achievement in geography. In conclusion, good family capital contributes to the development of geospatial thinking and related cognitive skills, which have a positive impact on academic achievement in geography.

In this study, geospatial thinking only partially mediates the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography. Analysis of the data showed that family capital had the greatest impact on academic achievement in geography in the model (75.3191%), with geospatial thinking playing only a partially mediating role (24.6809%). In other words, when geospatial thinking are weak, it is still possible for higher family capital to improve students’ academic achievement in geography. Also, students coming from families with lower family capital can also expect some improvement to their academic achievement in geography by enhancing their geospatial thinking.

5.2. Implications

This study provides a new perspective for the study of family capital, geospatial thinking and academic achievement in geography, and has important theoretical and practical significance. Theoretically, this study links family capital with academic achievement in geography, explores the important mediating role of family capital in geospatial thinking, and deepens the research on the impact of family capital on academic achievement in geography. The mediating role of geospatial thinking further uncovers the mechanisms underlying the academic achievement in geography. Students from better family environments are more likely to get a better geospatial thinking and academic achievement in geography, as well as geospatial thinking promotes the development of academic achievement in geography. Therefore,it is necessary to focus on both students’ family capital and geospatial thinking in the process of geography learning, and carry out more geospatial thinking training to improve academic achievement in geography。.

5.3. Limitations and future directions

There are some limitations to this study. First, it has a cross-sectional design. Second, the participants were all from a particular region in western China, which may affect the generalizability of the study results. Future researchers could carry out longitudinal surveys to gather relevant data over a period of time, or conduct in-depth surveys of students from different areas. Furthermore, it is possible to explore which dimension of geospatial thinking mediates the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography. Finally, by analyzing the mechanisms underlying the influence of family capital on geospatial thinking and academic achievement in geography, we can provide a direction for future research on how individuals with disadvantaged family capital improve their academic achievement in geography and geospatial thinking, and effectively help less-privileged students to achieve higher levels of academic achievement in geography.

6. Conclusion

This study explored the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography, and the mediating role of geospatial thinking between the two. The results showed that upper-secondary-school students with better family capital had higher academic achievement in geography. In addition, upper-secondary-school students with stronger geospatial thinking will have higher academic achievement in geography than those who are poorer at it. It is worth noting that despite the mediating role of geospatial thinking, differences in students’ academic achievement in geography are still largely influenced by family capital.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of Zhejiang Normal University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

JG and JZ designed the research. JZ, TS, XL, YX, ZW, and YY carried out the literature search and data analysis. JZ, TS, XL, YX, ZW, YY, and JG wrote the paper. All authors have read and agreed to the submitted version of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Office for Education Science Planning, grant number BAA180017.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the students who completed the questionnaire for their contributions to our research. We would also like to thank those who assisted with language revision.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Al Zboon, M. S., Ghammaz, S. A. D. A., and Al Zboon, M. S. (2018). The impact of the use of YouTube and Facebook on students’ academic achievement in geography course at the University of Jordan for the Bachelor’s degree. Mod. Appl. Sci. 12, 164–174. doi: 10.5539/mas.v12n3p164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aliman, M., Budijanto, S., Astina, I. K., Putri, R. E., and Arif, M. (2019). The effect of Earthcomm learning model and spatial thinking ability on geography learning outcomes. J. Balt. Sci. Educ. 18, 323–334. doi: 10.33225/jbse/19.18.323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

An, G., Wang, J., and Yang, Y. (2019). Chinese parents’ effect on Children’s math and science achievements in schools with different SES. J. Comp. Fam. Stud. 50, 139–161. doi: 10.3138/jcfs.50.2.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, T. R., Battersby, S., Bednarz, S. W., Bodzin, A. M., Kolvoord, B., Moore, S., et al. (2015). A research agenda for geospatial technologies and learning. J. Geogr. 114, 118–130. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2014.950684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bednarz, R., and Lee, J. (2019). What improves spatial thinking? Evidence from the spatial thinking abilities test. Int. Res. Geogr. Environ. Educ. 28, 262–280. doi: 10.1080/10382046.2019.1626124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bein, F. L. (Rick), Hayes, J. J., and Jones, T. G. (2009). Fifteen year follow-up geography skills test administered in Indiana, 1987 and 2002. J. Geogr., 108, 30–36. doi: 10.1080/00221340902886798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biro, M., Smederevac, S., and Tovilovic, S. (2009). Socioeconomic and cultural factors of low scholastic achievement of Roma children. Psihologija 42, 273–288. doi: 10.2298/PSI0903273B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaut, J. M., McCleary, G. S., and Blaut, A. S. (1970). Environmental mapping in young children. Environ. Behav. 2, 335–349. doi: 10.1177/001391657000200305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodzin, A. M., Fu, Q., Kulo, V., and Peffer, T. (2014). Examining the effect of enactment of a geospatial curriculum on students’ geospatial thinking and reasoning. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 23, 562–574. doi: 10.1007/s10956-014-9488-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bøe, T., Hysing, M., Stormark, K. M., Lundervold, A. J., and Sivertsen, B. (2012). Sleep problems as a mediator of the association between parental education levels, perceived family economy and poor mental health in children. J. Psychosom. Res. 73, 430–436. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borriello, G. A., and Liben, L. S. (2018). Encouraging maternal guidance of preschoolers’ spatial thinking during block play. Child Dev. 89, 1209–1222. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourdieu, P. (1973). “Cultural reproduction and social reproduction” in Knowledge, education, and cultural change (London: Routledge)

Google Scholar

Bourdieu, P., and Richardson, J. G. (1986). Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

Google Scholar

Bravo Sanzana, M., Salvo, S., Mieres, M., Mansilla, J., and Hederich, C. (2017). Perfiles de desempeño académico: La importancia de las expectativas familiares. Perfiles Latinoamericanos 25, 361–386. doi: 10.18504/pl2550-016-2017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 1, 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brookfield, K. (2022). ‘Nature-enhanced learning’ and geography education. J. Geogr. High. Educ. 46, 327–342. doi: 10.1080/03098265.2021.1926938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks-Gunn, J., and Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. Futur. Child. 7, 55–71. doi: 10.2307/1602387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbonell-Carrera, C., Luis Saorin, J., and Hess-Medler, S. (2020). A geospatial thinking multiyear study. Sustainability 12:4586, 1–15. doi: 10.3390/su12114586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbonell-Carrera, C., and Hess-Medler, S. (2019). 3D landform modeling to enhance geospatial thinking. ISPRS Int. J. Geo Inf. 8:65. doi: 10.3390/ijgi8020065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, M., Alexeev, N., Wang, L., Barned, N., Horan, E., and Reed, A. (2018). The development of spatial skills in elementary school students. Child Dev. 89, 446–460. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casasola, M., Wei, W. S., Suh, D. D., Donskoy, P., and Ransom, A. (2020). Children’s exposure to spatial language promotes their spatial thinking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 1116–1136. doi: 10.1037/xge0000699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. M., Dearing, E., Vasilyeva, M., Ganley, C. M., and Tine, M. (2011). Spatial and numerical predictors of measurement performance: the moderating effects of community income and gender. J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 296–311. doi: 10.1037/a0022516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, S.-T., and Kaplowitz, S. A. (2016). Family economic status, cultural capital, and academic achievement: the case of Taiwan. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 49, 271–278. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2016.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, M. M., and Chow, B. W.-Y. (2015). Classmate characteristics and student achievement in 33 countries: classmates’ past achievement, family socioeconomic status, educational resources, and attitudes toward reading. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 152–169. doi: 10.1037/a0036897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clingan-Siverly, S., Nelson, P. M., Göksun, T., and Demir-Lira, Ö. E. (2021). Spatial thinking in term and preterm-born preschoolers: relations to parent–child speech and gesture. Front. Psychol. 12:651678. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M., Cohen, C., Wilhelm, J., and Lindell, R. (2018). Spatial thinking in astronomy education research. Physical review. Phys. Educ. Res. 14:010139. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.14.010139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleman, J. S. (1968). Equality of educational opportunity. Equity Excell. Educ. 6, 19–28. doi: 10.1080/0020486680060504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social Capital in the Creation of human capital. Am. J. Sociol. 94, S95–S120. doi: 10.1086/228943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleman, J. S. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Collie, R. J., Martin, A. J., Malmberg, L.-E., Hall, J., and Ginns, P. (2015). Academic buoyancy, student’s achievement, and the linking role of control: a cross-lagged analysis of high school students. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 85, 113–130. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, L. (2018). The impact of paper versus digital map technology on students’ spatial thinking skill acquisition. J. Geogr. 117, 137–152. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2017.1374990

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conger, R. D., Martin, M. J., and Masarik, A. S. (2021). Dynamic associations among socioeconomic status (SES), parenting investments, and conscientiousness across time and generations. Dev. Psychol. 57, 147–163. doi: 10.1037/dev0000463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Pernillo, M., Rivas, S., Fuentes, L., Antillon, F., and Barr, R. D. (2014). Measurement of socio-economic status in families of children with cancer in Guatemala: socio-economic status in children with cancer. Pediatr. Blood Cancer 61, 2071–2073. doi: 10.1002/pbc.25060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vaus, D. A. (2002). Surveys in social research (5th). London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Dearing, E., and Taylor, B. A. (2007). Home improvements: within-family associations between income and the quality of children’s home environments. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 28, 427–444. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2007.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demetriou, A., Kazi, S., Makris, N., and Spanoudis, G. (2020). Cognitive ability, cognitive self-awareness, and school performance: from childhood to adolescence. Intelligence 79:101432. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2020.101432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demir-Lira, Ö. E., Prado, J., and Booth, J. R. (2016). Neural correlates of math gains vary depending on parental socioeconomic status (SES). Front. Psychol. 7:892. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Serrano, J., and Martínez, P. M. (2016). Geography in the third cycle of primary education: learning styles and academic performance. Boletín de La Asociación de Geógrafos Españoles 2016, 447–469. doi: 10.21138/bage.2347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downs, R. M. (2014). Time and practice: learning to become a geographer. J. Geogr. 113, 189–197. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2014.881408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolano-Pérez, E., and Bestué, M. (2021). Academic achievement in Spanish secondary school students: the inter-related role of executive functions, physical activity and gender. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 1816–1841. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, M. D. R., Kelley, J., Sikora, J., and Treiman, D. J. (2010). Family scholarly culture and educational success: books and schooling in 27 nations. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 28, 171–197. doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2010.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Favier, T. T., and van der Schee, J. A. (2014). The effects of geography lessons with geospatial technologies on the development of high school students’ relational thinking. Comput. Educ. 76, 225–236. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filgona, J., and Sababa, L. K. (2017). Effect of gender on senior secondary school students’ academic achievement in geography in Ganye educational zone, Nigeria. Eur. J. Educ. Stud. 3, 394–410. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.400596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filgona, J., and Sakiyo, J. (2020). Teachers’ academic qualification as a predictor of attitude and academic achievement in geography of senior secondary school students in Adamawa state, Nigeria. Int. J. Res. Rev. 7, 190–215. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/346952062_Teachers%27_Academic_Qualification_as_a_Predictor_of_Attitude_and_Academic_Achievement_in_Geography_of_Senior_Secondary_School_Students_in_Adamawa_State_Nigeria

Google Scholar

Gil-Espinosa, F. J., Chillón, P., and Cadenas-Sanchez, C. (2019). General intelligence was associated with academic achievement but not fitness in adolescents after one year. Acta Paediatr. 108, 896–902. doi: 10.1111/apa.14521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, A. U., Pendergast, P. M., Ormand, C. J., Budd, D. A., and Mueller, K. J. (2018). Improving spatial thinking skills among undergraduate geology students through short online training exercises. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 40, 2205–2225. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2018.1525621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goudeau, S., Autin, F., and Croizet, J.-C. (2017). Etudier, Mesurer et Manipuler la Classe Sociale en Psychologie Sociale: Approches Economiques, Symboliques et Culturelles [studying, measuring and manipulating social class in social psychology: economic, symbolic and cultural approaches]. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 30, 1–19. doi: 10.5334/irsp.52

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, W., Gannaway, D., Sheppard, K., and Jamarani, M. (2015). What’s in their baggage? The cultural and social capital of Australian students preparing to study abroad. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 34, 513–526. doi: 10.1080/07294360.2014.973381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, T. C., Bodzin, A., Anastasio, D., Holland, B., Popejoy, K., Sahagian, D., et al. (2018). “You know you can do this, right?”: developing geospatial technological pedagogical content knowledge and enhancing teachers’ cartographic practices with socio-environmental science investigations. Cartogr. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 45, 305–318. doi: 10.1080/15230406.2017.1419440

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, M. D., and Chen, E. (2007). Socioeconomic status and substance use behaviors in adolescents: the role of family resources versus family social status. J. Health Psychol. 12, 32–35. doi: 10.1177/1359105306069073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Havelková, L., and Hanus, M. (2021). Upper-secondary students’ strategies for spatial tasks. J. Geogr. 120, 176–190. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2021.1981979

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hespanha, S. R., Goodchild, F., and Janelle, D. G. (2009). Spatial thinking and Technologies in the Undergraduate Social Science Classroom. J. Geogr. High. Educ. 33, S17–S27. doi: 10.1080/03098260903033998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, L.-C. (2021). ‘Our students do not get that equal chance’: teachers’ perspectives of meritocracy. Camb. J. Educ. 51, 173–193. doi: 10.1080/0305764X.2020.1796927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huynh, N. T., and Sharpe, B. (2013). An assessment instrument to measure geospatial thinking expertise. J. Geogr. 112, 3–17. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2012.682227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IGU CGE. (2019). 2016 international charter on geographical education. Available at: https://www.igu-cge.org/2016-charter/

Google Scholar

Ip, P., Rao, N., Bacon-Shone, J., Li, S. L., Ho, F. K., Chow, C., et al. (2016). Socioeconomic gradients in school readiness of Chinese preschool children: the mediating role of family processes and kindergarten quality. Early Child. Res. Q. 35, 111–123. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Israel, A. L. (2012). Putting geography education into place: what geography educators can learn from place-based education, and vice versa. J. Geogr. 111, 76–81. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2011.583264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanitskii, A. M., Portnova, G. V., Martynova, O. V., Maiorova, L. A., Fedina, O. N., and Petrushevskii, A. G. (2015). Brain mapping in verbal and spatial thinking. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 45, 146–153. doi: 10.1007/s11055-015-0052-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, X., Liu, H., and Liu, L. (2017). Family education support to rural migrant children in China: Evidence from Shenzhen. Eurasian Geogr. Econ. 58, 169–200. doi: 10.1080/15387216.2017.1339621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jo, I., and Bednarz, S. W. (2014). Dispositions toward teaching spatial thinking through geography: conceptualization and an exemplar assessment. J. Geogr. 113, 198–207. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2014.881409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. H., and Mueller, C. E. (2017). The relationship among achievement goals, standardized test scores, and elementary students’ focus in school. Psychol. Sch. 54, 979–990. doi: 10.1002/pits.22040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kail, R., Carter, P., and Pellegrino, J. (1979). The locus of sex differences in spatial ability. Percept. Psychophys. 26, 182–186. doi: 10.3758/BF03199867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klonari, A., and Likouri, A. A. (2015). The relation of multiple intelligences and spatial perception with performance in geography education. GI_Forum 1, 359–362. doi: 10.1553/giscience2015s359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krakowka, A. R. (2012). Field trips as valuable learning experiences in geography courses. J. Geogr. 111, 236–244. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2012.707674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kristjnsson, I. L., Sigfsdttir, I. D., Allegrante, J. P., and Helgason, S. R. (2009). Adolescent health behavior, contentment in school, and academic achievement. Am. J. Health Behav. 33, 69–79. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.33.1.7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, D., and Jones, M. (2017). Debates in geography education (2nd). London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Larsen, T., Gerike, M., and Harrington, J. (2022). Human-environment thinking and K-12 geography education. J. Geogr. 121, 34–46. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2021.2005666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., and Bednarz, R. (2009). Effect of GIS learning on spatial thinking. J. Geogr. High. Educ. 33, 183–198. doi: 10.1080/03098260802276714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., and Bednarz, R. (2012). Components of spatial thinking: evidence from a spatial thinking ability test. J. Geogr. 111, 15–26. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2011.583262

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Gao, H., and Xu, Y. (2020). The mediating and buffering effect of academic self-efficacy on the relationship between smartphone addiction and academic procrastination. Comput. Educ. 159:104001, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.104001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Z., Li, S., Shang, S., and Ren, X. (2021). How do critical thinking ability and critical thinking disposition relate to the mental health of university students? Front. Psychol. 12:704229. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.704229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, T., Zhang, X., and Jiang, Y. (2020). Family socioeconomic status and the cognitive competence of very young children from migrant and non-migrant Chinese families: the mediating role of parenting self-efficacy and parental involvement. Early Child. Res. Q. 51, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lüftenegger, M., Klug, J., Harrer, K., Langer, M., Spiel, C., and Schober, B. (2016). Students’ achievement goals, learning-related emotions and academic achievement. Front. Psychol. 7:603. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manson, S., Shannon, J., Eria, S., Kne, L., Dyke, K., Nelson, S., et al. (2014). Resource needs and pedagogical value of web mapping for spatial thinking. J. Geogr. 113, 107–117. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2013.790915

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marciano, L., and Camerini, A. L. (2021). Recommendations on screen time, sleep and physical activity: associations with academic achievement in Swiss adolescents. Public Health 198, 211–217. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2021.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, L., Ronconi, A., Scrimin, S., and Pazzaglia, F. (2022). Short-term exposure to nature and benefits for students’ cognitive performance: a review. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 34, 609–647. doi: 10.1007/s10648-021-09631-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mega, C., Ronconi, L., and De Beni, R. (2014). What makes a good student? How emotions, self-regulated learning, and motivation contribute to academic achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 106, 121–131. doi: 10.1037/a0033546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metoyer, S., and Bednarz, R. (2017). Spatial thinking assists geographic thinking: evidence from a study exploring the effects of geospatial technology. J. Geogr. 116, 20–33. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2016.1175495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Research Council. (2006). Learning to think spatially: GIS as a support system in K-12 education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Google Scholar

Nazareth, A., Newcombe, N. S., Shipley, T. F., Velazquez, M., and Weisberg, S. M. (2019). Beyond small-scale spatial skills: navigation skills and geoscience education. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 4:17. doi: 10.1186/s41235-019-0167-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, C. P., Oberle, A., and Sugumaran, R. (2011). Implementing a high school level geospatial technologies and spatial thinking course. J. Geogr. 110, 60–69. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2011.534171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozdemir, D., and Ozturk, F. (2022). The investigation of Mobile virtual reality application instructional content in geography education: academic achievement, presence, and student interaction. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 38, 1487–1503. doi: 10.1080/10447318.2022.2045070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pastor, D., Ballester-Ferrer, J. A., Carbonell-Hernández, L., Baladzhaeva, S., and Cervello, E. (2022). Physical exercise and cognitive function. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:9564. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19159564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perugini, S., and Bodzin, A. M. (2020). Using web-based GIS to assess students’ geospatial knowledge of hurricanes and spatial habits of mind. J. Geogr. 119, 63–73. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2019.1710764

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pokropek, A., Borgonovi, F., and McCormick, C. (2017). On the cross-country comparability of indicators of socioeconomic resources in PISA. Appl. Meas. Educ. 30, 243–258. doi: 10.1080/08957347.2017.1353985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potter, D., Mashburn, A., and Grissmer, D. (2013). The family, neuroscience, and academic skills: An interdisciplinary account of social class gaps in children’s test scores. Soc. Sci. Res. 42, 446–464. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2012.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roosa, M. W., O’Donnell, M., Cham, H., Gonzales, N. A., Zeiders, K. H., Tein, J.-Y., et al. (2012). A prospective study of Mexican American adolescents’ academic success: considering family and individual factors. J. Youth Adolesc. 41, 307–319. doi: 10.1007/s10964-011-9707-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rydant, A. L., Shiplee, B. A., Smith, J. P., and Middlekauff, B. D. (2010). Applying sequential fieldwork skills across two international field courses. J. Geogr. 109, 221–232. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2010.502945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholz, M. A., Huynh, N. T., Brysch, C. P., and Scholz, R. W. (2014). An evaluation of university world geography textbook questions for components of spatial thinking. J. Geogr. 113, 208–219. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2013.872692

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, E. E., Milson, A. J., and Smith, T. J. (2016). Future teachers’ spatial thinking skills and attitudes. J. Geogr. 115, 139–146. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2015.1100654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solem, M., Vaughan, P., Savage, C., and De Nadai, A. S. (2021). Student- and school-level predictors of geography achievement in the United States, 1994–2018. J. Geogr. 120, 201–211. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2021.2000009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiyanto, S., Maryani, E., and Ruhimat, M. (2018). A preliminary study on developing geography literacy based on social studies learning model to improve teachers 21st century skills. IOP Conf. Ser. Earth Environ. Sci. 145:012062. doi: 10.1088/1755-1315/145/1/012062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiyanto, S., Maryani, E., and Ruhimat, M. (2021). Development of social teaching materials based on geography literacy for increasing, critical, and creative thinking skills. IOP Conf. Ser. Earth Environ. Sci. 683:012042. doi: 10.1088/1755-1315/683/1/012042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taljūnaitė, M. (2020). Inheritance of social Capital in the Family: the case of multi-child families. Filosofija. Sociologija 31, 349–356. doi: 10.6001/fil-soc.v31i4.4350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomaszewski, B., Vodacek, A., Parody, R., and Holt, N. (2015). Spatial thinking ability assessment in Rwandan secondary schools: baseline results. J. Geogr. 114, 39–48. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2014.918165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trilling, B., and Fadel, C. (2009). 21st century skills: learning for life in our times. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Available at: http://dspace.vnbrims.org:13000/xmlui/bitstream/handle/123456789/4208/21st%20Century%20Skills%20Learning%20for%20Life%20in%20Our%20Times.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y

Google Scholar

Uhlenberg, J. M., and Geiken, R. (2021). Supporting young Children’s spatial understanding: examining toddlers’ experiences with contents and containers. Early Childhood Educ. J. 49, 49–60. doi: 10.1007/s10643-020-01050-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verma, K. (2014). Geospatial thinking of undergraduate students in public universities in the United States. Ph.D. Thesis. San Marcos, TX: Texas State University.

Google Scholar

Von Stumm, S., Cave, S. N., and Wakeling, P. (2022). Persistent association between family socioeconomic status and primary school performance in Britain over 95 years. Npj Sci. Learn. 7:4. doi: 10.1038/s41539-022-00120-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., and Huang, C. (2021). Family capital, learning engagement, and students’ higher education gains: an empirical study in mainland China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:11571. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182111571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D. Y., Lee, M.-H., and Sun, C.-T. (2013). Effects of thinking style and spatial ability on anchoring behavior in geographic information systems. Educ. Technol. Soc. 16, 1–13. Available at: https://www.jstor.org/stable/jeductechsoci.16.3.1

Google Scholar

Weinberg, D., Stevens, G. W. J. M., Duinhof, E. L., and Finkenauer, C. (2019). Adolescent socioeconomic status and mental health inequalities in the Netherlands, 2001–2017. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:3605. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16193605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, S., Zeng, S., Liu, L., Wei, H., Xu, Y., and Lu, X. (2022). Predicting geospatial thinking ability for secondary school students based on the decision tree algorithm in mainland China. TED EĞİTİM VE BİLİM 47. doi: 10.15390/EB.2022.10367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, S., Zheng, X., Sun, Y., Wan, J., and Lu, X. (2021). The factors and mechanisms that influence geospatial thinking: a structural equation modeling approach. J. Geogr. 120, 165–175. doi: 10.1080/00221341.2021.1967423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., and Pang, W. (2020). Reading thousands of books and traveling thousands of miles: diversity of life experience mediates the relationship between family SES and creativity. Scand. J. Psychol. 61, 177–182. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, R., and Ngai, S. S. (2019). Generating identity capital from family capital and personal agency: a mediation analysis. J. Adult Dev. 26, 71–80. doi: 10.1007/s10804-018-9306-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L. F. (2002). Thinking styles and modes of thinking: implications for education and research. J. Psychol. 136, 245–261. doi: 10.1080/00223980209604153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Hu, B. Y., Ren, L., Huo, S., and Wang, M. (2019). Young Chinese Children’s academic skill development: identifying child-, family-, and school-level factors: young Chinese Children’s academic skill development. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2019, 9–37. doi: 10.1002/cad.20271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Liang, X., Su, T., Li, X., Ge, J., An, Z., et al. (2022). The mediating effect of geospatial thinking on the relationship between family capital and sense of place. Front. Psychol. 13:918326. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.918326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Qin, X., and Ren, P. (2018). Adolescents’ academic engagement mediates the association between internet addiction and academic achievement: the moderating effect of classroom achievement norm. Comput. Hum. Behav. 89, 299–307. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Q. (2018). Association between family socioeconomic status and depressive symptoms among Chinese adolescents_ evidence from a national household survey. Psychiatry Res. 259, 81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziviani, J., Wadley, D., Ward, H., Macdonald, D., Jenkins, D., and Rodger, S. (2008). A place to play: socioeconomic and spatial factors in children’s physical activity. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 55, 2–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1630.2006.00646.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: family capital, academic achievement in geography, geospatial thinking, mediating effect, upper-secondary-school students

Citation: Zhang J, Su T, Liang X, Xu Y, Wang Z, Yu Y and Ge J (2023) The mediating effect of geospatial thinking on the relationship between family capital and academic achievement in geography. Front. Psychol. 14:1067198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1067198

Received: 11 October 2022; Accepted: 02 February 2023;
Published: 17 February 2023.

Edited by:

Antonio P. Gutierrez de Blume, Georgia Southern University, United States

Reviewed by:

Carlos Carbonell Carrera, University of La Laguna, Spain
Bob Sharpe, Wilfrid Laurier University, Canada

Copyright © 2023 Zhang, Su, Liang, Xu, Wang, Yu and Ge. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jiahao Ge, ✉ jiahaogeedu@foxmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.